Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Unstable Maternal Environment, Separation Anxiety, and Heightened CO2 Sensitivity Induced by Gene-by-Environment Interplay

  • Francesca R. D'Amato,

    Affiliation CNR, Cell Biology and Neurobiology Institute, Roma, Italy

  • Claudio Zanettini ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Claudio Zanettini, Valentina Lampis

    Affiliation CNR, Cell Biology and Neurobiology Institute, Roma, Italy

  • Valentina Lampis ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Claudio Zanettini, Valentina Lampis

    Affiliation Academic Centre for the Study of Behavioural Plasticity, Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan, Italy

  • Roberto Coccurello,

    Affiliation CNR, Cell Biology and Neurobiology Institute, Roma, Italy

  • Tiziana Pascucci,

    Affiliations Santa Lucia Foundation, European Centre for Brain Research (CERC), Rome, Italy, Department of Psychology, University “La Sapienza”, Rome, Italy

  • Rossella Ventura,

    Affiliations Santa Lucia Foundation, European Centre for Brain Research (CERC), Rome, Italy, Department of Biomedical Science and Technology, Università dell' Aquila, Coppito, L'Aquila, Italy

  • Stefano Puglisi-Allegra,

    Affiliations Santa Lucia Foundation, European Centre for Brain Research (CERC), Rome, Italy, Department of Psychology, University “La Sapienza”, Rome, Italy

  • Chiara A. M. Spatola,

    Affiliation Academic Centre for the Study of Behavioural Plasticity, Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan, Italy

  • Paola Pesenti-Gritti,

    Affiliation Academic Centre for the Study of Behavioural Plasticity, Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan, Italy

  • Diego Oddi,

    Affiliation CNR, Cell Biology and Neurobiology Institute, Roma, Italy

  • Anna Moles,

    Affiliations CNR, Cell Biology and Neurobiology Institute, Roma, Italy, Genomnia, Lainate, Italy

  • Marco Battaglia

    marco.battaglia@unisr.it

    Affiliations Academic Centre for the Study of Behavioural Plasticity, Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan, Italy, Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Istituto Scientifico San Raffaele, Milan, Italy

Abstract

Background

In man, many different events implying childhood separation from caregivers/unstable parental environment are associated with heightened risk for panic disorder in adulthood. Twin data show that the occurrence of such events in childhood contributes to explaining the covariation between separation anxiety disorder, panic, and the related psychobiological trait of CO2 hypersensitivity. We hypothesized that early interference with infant-mother interaction could moderate the interspecific trait of response to CO2 through genetic control of sensitivity to the environment.

Methodology

Having spent the first 24 hours after birth with their biological mother, outbred NMRI mice were cross-fostered to adoptive mothers for the following 4 post-natal days. They were successively compared to normally-reared individuals for: number of ultrasonic vocalizations during isolation, respiratory physiology responses to normal air (20%O2), CO2-enriched air (6% CO2), hypoxic air (10%O2), and avoidance of CO2-enriched environments.

Results

Cross-fostered pups showed significantly more ultrasonic vocalizations, more pronounced hyperventilatory responses (larger tidal volume and minute volume increments) to CO2-enriched air and heightened aversion towards CO2-enriched environments, than normally-reared individuals. Enhanced tidal volume increment response to 6%CO2 was present at 16–20, and 75–90 postnatal days, implying the trait's stability. Quantitative genetic analyses of unrelated individuals, sibs and half-sibs, showed that the genetic variance for tidal volume increment during 6%CO2 breathing was significantly higher (Bartlett χ = 8.3, p = 0.004) among the cross-fostered than the normally-reared individuals, yielding heritability of 0.37 and 0.21 respectively. These results support a stress-diathesis model whereby the genetic influences underlying the response to 6%CO2 increase their contribution in the presence of an environmental adversity. Maternal grooming/licking behaviour, and corticosterone basal levels were similar among cross-fostered and normally-reared individuals.

Conclusions

A mechanism of gene-by-environment interplay connects this form of early perturbation of infant-mother interaction, heightened CO2 sensitivity and anxiety. Some non-inferential physiological measurements can enhance animal models of human neurodevelopmental anxiety disorders.

Introduction

The term ‘separation anxiety’ applies comprehensively to multiple forms of distress reactions displayed by mammals during postnatal development in conjunction with events of separation from a caregiver [1]. Childhood separation anxiety disorder (SAD) - an extreme human manifestation within this interspecies' propensity - predicts heightened risk for panic disorder (PD) in early adulthood [2], and both PD and SAD [3] share a trait of oversensitivity to higher-than-normal CO2 concentrations in inhaled air. Relatively specific responses to CO2-enriched air mixtures have been described in controlled studies of PD and SAD. These responses consist of both stronger emotional reactions (e.g. panic anxiety), and altered respiratory parameters (i.e., wider tidal volume enhancements and fluctuations, and heightened minute ventilation) [4][6], compared to those seen in control subjects.

Whilst PD is part of the DSM-IV anxiety disorders, the bulk of time-honoured data from clinical observation and empirical research indicates that panic attacks should not be equated with fear responses. Clinical panic attacks are typically spontaneous and unpredictable, and characterised by prominent physical symptoms such as dyspnea, rather than by cognitive symptoms [7]. Another physical symptom, frequently reported during spontaneous and CO2-provoked panic attacks, is dizziness [8], which may be substantiated in the vestibular dysfunctions often present among people with PD [9]. Endocrinological data contribute to strengthening the view that panic is not a typical emergency fear response. Heightened cortisol levels in spontaneous and CO2-provoked attacks [10], [11] have been found to reflect anticipatory anxiety/individual differences in emotionality, rather than the diagnostic category of PD per se. Thus, inasmuch as panic attacks occur in the absence of cues of external danger and are triggered by heightened CO2 concentrations, they are better seen as inner unconditioned false alarms of biological origin. According to this model, panic attacks derive from a deranged suffocation detector [4] via pathophysiological mechanisms that differ from those underlying general or anticipatory anxiety. Accordingly, most people at the onset of PD are no more anxious/apprehensive/avoidant than people in the general population [12] , and their cortisol levels are within the range of normality [4], [13]. However, after having experienced one or more panic attacks, subjects with PD develop a form of avoidance towards places (such as subways or cinemas) where they believe they will experience dyspnea/panic [14], and also become less explorative towards novel, open spaces, behavioural characteristics collectively named ‘agoraphobia’ [15].

According to twin studies, shared genetic determinants appear to be the major underlying cause of the developmental continuity of childhood SAD into adult PD, and of the association of both disorders with altered sensitivity to CO2 [16], [17]. Moreover, a host of events implying unstable parental environment and separation during childhood (encompassing, e.g, parental military service, job relocation, separation, divorce, death, etc.) can account for a significant additional proportion of the covariation between SAD, CO2 sensitivity and PD [17]. Thus, in addition to genetic determinants, environmental risk factors affect the liability to these traits, and Ogliari et al. [18] showed that several life events that influence the susceptibility to PD also predict heightened CO2 reactivity. There is now initial evidence that genetic and environmental determinants may not simply add, but also interact, to influence human responses to CO2. By modelling the effects of life events in young adult twins, Spatola et al. [19] recently found that adversities that take place within the childhood-adolescence window of risk moderate the genetic variance for CO2 sensitivity, as assessed by a CO2 challenge provocation test. Such a form of gene-by-environment interplay is consistent with a diathesis-stress model, and points towards gene-by-environment interactions [20], [21] that, while rooted in early life, can exert their effect also in early adulthood.

However, the connections between early perturbations of the offspring-caregiver relationships, separation anxiety, panic, and altered respiratory physiology are still to be clarified.

While the study of human subjects is necessarily limited by the ‘natural experiment’ approach, the fact that all mammals show similar physiological responses to heightened CO2 concentrations (i.e., hyperventilation and increased arousal/anxiety) can be exploited to disentangle some of the questions that pertain to the human SAD-PD developmental continuum. By capitalizing on physiological responses to heightened CO2 concentrations that are relevant to both animal behaviour and the human SAD-PD continuum, one can tackle these questions by fully experimental approaches within the context of animal models [14], [22].

Indeed, higher-than-normal environmental concentrations of CO2 constitute an aversive stimulus for many species. In the C. Elegans [23] and the Drosophila [24], [25], CO2 elicits innate responses of avoidance. In man, heightened CO2 concentrations induce hyperventilation, subjective air hunger and anxiety [18], [26], [27] by activating the ventral medulla and subsequently the pons, midbrain, limbic and paralimbic areas, parahyppocampalgyrus, and the anterior insula [28]. Recent data [29] also show that the amygdala is itself a chemosensor that initiates fear responses under hypercarbia and acidosis.

While all mammals respond similarly to the unconditioned suffocative stimulus of heightened CO2 concentration by increasing ventilation, vigilance and eventually by displaying anxious/avoidant behaviour [30], individuals within the same species differ widely from each other in the intensity of these responses, partially due to genetic factors [8], [31], [32]. Moreover, different types of early experiences –including environmental adversities not primarily associated with breathing- may affect the plasticity of the mammalian respiratory control system [33].

To sum up the background of this study, seven interrelated points appear fundamental: 1) SAD and PD are on a developmental and pathophysiological continuum, as SAD often precedes PD, and both conditions are associated with CO2 hypersensitivity; 2) hypersensitivity to CO2 can be indexed via respiratory parameters and/or exaggerated anxiety responses to heightened CO2 concentrations in inhaled air; 3) the phenotypes of hypersensitivity to CO2, SAD, and PD share a genetic background; 4) in man, childhood separation from caregivers/unstable parental environment and early life adversities appear to enhance the risk for SAD/PD/CO2 hypersensitivity; 5) human responses to heightened CO2 concentrations may be in part influenced by complex causal mechanisms, whereby the degree of sensitivity to early environmental adversities appears to be under genetic control; 6) rodents are prone to separation anxiety and respond to heightened CO2 concentrations similarly to man, i.e., by incrementing ventilation and arousal/anxiety; 7) also similarly to man, the increase in ventilation under heightened CO2 concentrations among rodents yields a degree of interindividual variance, which can be amenable to quantitative genetic estimations.

We speculated that early environmental adversities may moderate a proportion of genetic liability to CO2 sensitivity through gene-by-environment interplay mechanisms, and that such mechanisms could be substantiated in man and animals. Inasmuch as CO2 sensitivity represents a valid endophenotype [34][37] that shares part of the liability with human PD and SAD [17], and since CO2 sensitivity is interspecific, animal models of CO2 responses can be used as a proxy of a human psychiatric disorder to study gene-environment interplay [38].

To investigate the relationships that link early interference to infant-mother interactions, separation anxiety, and CO2 sensitivity, we focused on the ventilatory response to heightened CO2 concentrations in outbred mice repeatedly cross-fostered during the first postnatal days. This approach, largely based on respiratory measurements, permits the circumventing of the difficulties that arise from making inferences about an animal's emotional state. Moreover, the laboratory context avoids the gene-by-environment correlations that hamper research on gene-by-environment interplay in man.

By implementing a repeated cross-fostering procedure, we sought to address three main questions that pertain to the human SAD-PD developmental continuum: 1) can this form of perturbation of infant-mother relationship alter the pattern of individual reactivity to inhaled CO2? 2) is the alteration in sensitivity to CO2 specific and stable? 3) can this type of early manipulation act as an enhancer of individual differences, so that it can reveal mechanisms of genetic control of sensitivity to the environment?

Materials and Methods

Animals

NMRI outbred mice (Harlan, Italy) were used in all experiments. Mice were mated when they were twelve weeks old. Mating protocol consisted in housing two females with one male in transparent high temperature polysufone cages (26.7×20.7×14.0 cm) with water and food available ad libitum. Room temperature (21±1°C) and a 12∶12 h light dark cycle (lights on at 07.00 a.m.) were kept constant. After 15 days males were removed and pregnant females were isolated in clean cages, and inspected twice a day for live pups. For the first postnatal day (PND0) litters were left with the biological mother.

Postnatal treatment: Repeated Cross-fostering (RCF) Procedure

The Repeated Cross Fostering procedure (RCF) is a new experimental rearing protocol devised to interfere with infant-mother interaction in the first days of life, thus predisposing offspring to separation anxiety without inducing neglect from caregivers. This was based upon the knowledge that when mouse pups are cross-fostered to adoptive lactating dams, they are usually well accepted and nurtured [39], and on the fact that an adoption procedure carried out in the first postnatal days has a low impact on offspring's HPA functioning [40].

Having spent the first postnatal day (PND0) with the biological mother, on PND1 litters were culled to eight pups (4 males and 4 females) and assigned to experimental Repeated Cross Fostering (RCF), or control (CT) treatment. Unlike the ‘classical’ cross-fostering procedures [41], RCF pups changed caregiver every 24 hours: 4 times in the PND1-PND4 time interval by following a rotation scheme, each dam shifted to 4 different litters and each litter was shifted to 4 different dams (see also Figure S1). The procedure consisted of first removing the mother from the cage, then removing its entire litter, and immediately introducing this litter into the home-cage of a different dam whose pups had just been removed. The RCF pups were then semi-covered with the home-cage bedding of the adoptive mother, which was then reintroduced in the cage and left with this litter for 24 hours. The entire procedure lasted about 30 seconds and took place every day between 10.30 and 11.00 am. This was repeated daily, four times (PND1 to PND4), until reaching the fourth adoptive mother, with which pups remained until weaning (PND0: biological mother, PND1-PND4: adoptive mother 1 to 4- Figure S1). Adoptive dams were lactating females with pups of the same age as fostered litters. This repeated change of caregiver was aimed at interfering with the formation of the infant-mother relationship [42], and to approximate parental instability, a risk factor for internalising disorders, SAD, PD and CO2 hypersensitivity in man [17], [43], [44].

Control litters were collected daily and reintroduced to their home-cage, covered with home-cage bedding and had their biological mothers returned within 30 sec, from PND1 to PND4. Animals were weaned when 28 days old and then separated by sex and left in cages with littermates.

Table S1 shows the body weights of RCF and CT individuals during development and in adulthood, as well as their basal body temperature at PND20, measured at a fixed time of day with an infrared body thermometer (153 IRB, Bioseb), in accordance to previously published methods [45].

Maternal Behaviour

Maternal behaviour was observed daily from PND1 to PND7 by an observer unaware of the litter's status (RCF/CT) in two daily sessions (12.00–12.30 and 16.00–16.30), the first session taking place one hour after the cross fostering procedure on PND1-PND4. Maternal behaviour: a) NURSING, including the arched-back and blanket postures, and b) GP/L: grooming and licking pups [46] was monitored with an instantaneous sampling method (one sampling every 2 min), for a total of 16 sampling points/session. The analyses of maternal behaviour were based on the observation of NURSING and GP/L on 10 litters of RCF, and 8 litters of CT pups.

Offspring behaviour

Pups' behaviour was evaluated at: a) PND8, by measuring ultrasonic (USVs) distress vocalizations emitted during isolation, and: b) PND10, by measuring the pups' ability to orient towards and approach maternal/home-cage beddings' [47], [48] odour cues (HOMING behaviour, vide infra). The assessments of USVs were preceded by transfer of the home-cages into the experimental room at 14.30 of PND8. On PND8, after 1 hour of acclimatization, the mother was removed and transferred into a clean cage, while the offspring was left in the home cage standing on a warm plate set at the temperature of 35,5°C to prevent cooling. Each pup was individually placed for 5 minutes into a beaker containing (i) own-cage bedding (USVs-own) or (ii) clean bedding (USVs-clean) and the vocalizations were recorded. No more than 1 pup/litter/condition was employed and pups were gender-matched for a total of 31 RCF and 44 CT pups. Ultrasonic vocalizations were recorded using an UltraSoundGate Condenser Microphone (CM16, Avisoft Bioacoustics, Berlin, Germany) lowered 1 cm above the top of the isolation beaker containing the pup. The microphone was sensitive to frequencies of 15–180 kHz with a flat frequency response (±6 dB) between 25–140 kHz. It was connected via an UltraSoundGate USB Audio device to a personal computer, where acoustic data were recorded as wav files at 250,000 Hz in 16 bit format. Sound files were transferred to SasLab Pro (version 4.40; AvisoftBioacoutics) for sonographic analysis and a fast Fourier transformation was conducted (512 FFT-length, 100% frame, Hamming window and 75% time window overlap). Spectrograms were produced at 488 Hz of frequency resolution and 0.512 ms of time resolution. To detect ultrasonic vocalizations, an automatic threshold-based algorithm and a hold time mechanism (hold time: 20 ms) were used. Signals below 30 kHz were truncated to reduce background noise to 0 dB. Inaccurate detections were adjusted manually by an experienced user before running the automatic parameter analysis. The total number of vocalizations emitted in 5 minutes was measured.

The pups' orientation towards familiar odorous cues (HOMING behaviour) was evaluated on PND10 in 32 RCF and 36 CT pups. The assessments of HOMING were preceded by transfer of the home-cages into the experimental room at 14.30 of PND10. The amount of time spent in their home-cage bedding-scented versus (i) clean, or (ii) bedding from an alien dam's cage portions of the apparatus was recorded in 5 minute test sessions. The ability of pups to orient towards familiar odorous cues was evaluated in a small apparatus (5×33×10 h cm) with a central plexiglas part (5×5 cm, starting point) that separated (with sliding doors) two differently-scented chambers. One of these was covered with pups' home-cage bedding, whilst the other was covered with (i) clean or (ii) home-cage bedding from an alien dam's cage. After 1 minute of habituation in the central part of the maze, the lateral doors were removed and the pups could move freely in the apparatus. The behaviour of pups in the maze was video-recorded for 5 minutes and the time spent in the different compartments was evaluated thereafter. No more than two pups/litter/condition in a gender-matched design were tested in these two HOMING procedures.

Basal Corticosterone levels during development

Dams and pups were sacrificed to measure serum corticosterone basal levels collected via trunk blood samples from 10.00 to 11.00 am. Trunk blood samples were collected after decapitation in RCF and CT offspring (RCF = 16, CT = 14, in a gender-matched design) and their corresponding dams as adoptive or biological mothers (RCF dams = 6, CT dams = 5) before weaning (PND 27–28). After blood centrifugation (20 min, 4°C, 4000 rpm) serum samples were stored at −25°C until assay were conducted. Corticosterone levels were measured using commercially available EIA kits (EIA kit Assay Design, sensitivity 27.0 pg/mL). All corticosterone measures were carried out in duplicate.

Assessments of Face Validity for Human Panic

In order to assess the face validity for human PD/SAD of the current animal model, we capitalised on the following 4 features of human PD, as outlined in the introduction: 1) post-CO2 cortisol levels do not differ between subjects at heightened risk for PD and control subjects [10], [11]; 2) after having experienced one or more panic attacks, subjects with PD avoid places where they anticipate experiencing dyspnea [14]; 3) after having experienced panic attacks, subjects with PD become less explorative towards novel, open spaces and develop agoraphobia [15] 4)vestibular dysfunctions are often described among people with PD [9]. These 4 features of human PD were assessed by proxy among RCF vs. CT adult animals in the 4 following experiments:

1. Corticosterone levels after exposition to CO2-enriched air Six.

The basal serum corticosterone levels obtained from the tail of 6 RCF (2 females 4 males), and 5 CT (2 females, 3 males) individuals (PND70–90) were collected in a litter-balanced design, and compared to corticosterone levels collected in the same individuals by the same tail-cut method after 20 minutes spent an incubator chamber while breathing 6% CO2-enriched air. As described above, after blood centrifugation serum samples were stored at -25°C until assay were conducted using the commercially available EIA kits (EIA kit Assay Design). All corticosterone measures were carried out in duplicate.

2. Avoidance of CO2-enriched environments.

Place avoidance/preference towards a CO2-enriched environment was measured in 70–90 day old CT (N = 15) and RCF (N = 11) naïve male mice in a litter-balanced design, in a ‘place conditioning’ apparatus [49] consisting of two differently-cued chambers connected by a central alley. On day 1 (pretest), the mouse was introduced in the central alley and left free to explore the entire apparatus. During the following 8 days (conditioning), each mouse was confined daily (for 20 min) in one of the two chambers, while the apparatus was introduced into an incubator with either room air or 6% CO2-enriched air, on alternative days. For each animal, over the 8 training days, one of the two chambers was consistently paired with 6% CO2-enriched air and the other one with normal air. Testing was conducted on day 10. Animals were placed in the central alley of the apparatus and left free to explore the chambers for 10 min. Pretest and test sessions were videotaped, and subsequently an experienced observer unaware of the treatment conditions recorded the time (in seconds) spent in the different compartments with dedicated software (Smart, Panlab). Place-preference scores were calculated as: [(the amount of time spent in the CO2-paired compartment)/(amount of time spent in both compartments)]×100.

3. Effects of CO2 exposure on exploratory behaviour.

Animals (78 individuals, 70–90 PND, gender- and litter-matched) for this experiment were first isolated for 24 hrs in a clean cage with a sliding door, with food and water available. Then subjects were exposed either to room air (RCF: n = 21, CT: n = 20) or to a 20% CO2-enriched environment (RCF: n = 20, CT: n = 17) for 2 minutes. Immediately thereafter, each animal was allowed to enter a free exploratory apparatus (70×90 cm) connected with the home cage, by leaving the sliding door open for 10 minutes. Each session was video-recorded, and later the percentage of time spent in the centre of the arena (30×50 cm) during exploration of the apparatus was measured by a dedicated software (Smart, Panlab).

4. Evaluation of vestibular function among RCF and CT individuals.

Thirty-seven RCF and 32 CT gender and litter-matched individuals (PND70–90) were assessed for their performance at the Rotarod test [50] as a proxy of balance. After training for three sessions in the preceding day, mice had to maintain balance upon an accelerating rotating rod (four trials, from 4 to 40 rpm in 300 seconds), the dependent variable being each subject's latency to fall from the rod [50].

Respiratory Responses

We took into account the following respiratory parameters: tidal volume (i.e. the volume of air displaced between normal inspiration and expiration,TV), respiratory frequency (i.e., the number of breaths an individual takes per minute, f), and minute volume (MV, which is obtained by multiplying TV by f).

On PNDs 16–20, sixty-four pups were tested for their respiratory responses. Thirty-six RCF pups belonging to 14 litters, and 28 CT pups belonging to 12 litters were tested; each subject was exposed to only 1 air mixture condition (air/6%CO2/10%O2). For each litter a maximum of 2 pups (one male and one female) were exposed to the same air mixture (air/6%CO2/10%O2). We used an unrestrained plethysmograph (PLY4211, Buxco Electronics, Sharon CT) carrying two separate Plexiglass chambers of 450 ml, allowing for the parallel assessment of two animals/session.

Before recording, each subject was closed in the chamber for an acclimatisation of 40 minutes without any air mixture being administered. Subsequently the recording of respiratory parameters started under air condition (baseline) for 20 minutes. Next, the first challenge began: this lasted 20 min and could consist of any of the following three conditions: 1) normal air (20%O2); 2) 10%O2, 3) 6%CO2. A 20 min recovery period (air) followed, then the second challenge (20 min with the same gas mixture as employed in the 1st challenge) took place. A 2nd recovery period (air) of the same duration of the 1st recovery followed, which ended the trial and the recording time. A complete session thus lasted 140 minutes per animal (Figure 1).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Scheme of the respiratory protocol.

During ‘baseline’ and ‘recovery’ periods, subjects inhaled normal air. During ‘challenge’ periods subjects were exposed to one type of air mixture: 6% CO2-enriched air, or 10%O2 air, or normal air.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018637.g001

Similarly, on PND 75–90, seventy-six adult mice were tested with the same device and procedures, except that only normal air (20%O2) or 6%CO2 were employed for adults, given the negative results (see results section) with 10%O2- and the positive results with 6%CO2 stimuli in pups. A with the pups, a maximum of 2 adult subjects (one male and one female) per litter were tested under one air mixture (air/6%CO2). Of the 76 adult mice that underwent the respiratory challenges, 33 had previously been exposed to an air/6%CO2 challenge as pups: for their challenge in adulthood we used the same air mixture they had been exposed to as pups.

Preliminary tests by general linear models were run to assess whether during the first 20 minutes of pre-challenge baseline recording with air, the TV, f and MV parameters differed between RCF and CT pups and adults, and no significant differences were found. Since TV changes are a major physiological strategy to reduce blood P CO2, we capitalized on the mean percentage of TV increment (ΔTV%) from baseline to air/6%CO2/10%O2 as a reference measure to compare animals' respiratory responses to the air mixtures in the experiments. We also preliminarily tested the effects of: weight, sex, and chamber (there were 2 separate plethysmographic chambers) on the respiratory responses of RCF and CT pup and adult subjects separately, by regression procedures. For adult mice, we also tested the effect of previous participation to the challenge as pups. None of these four predictors influenced the respiratory measurements under air/6%CO2/10%O2. However, for adult mice, regression of ‘previous participation to the respiratory challenge as pup’ upon the ΔTV% to 6%CO2 provided a r = 0.18, F1,74 = 2,44, which approached significance: p = 0.12. This variable was therefore added as covariate in the analyses of adult respiratory responses.

The effects of treatment (air vs. 6%CO2 vs. 10%O2 in pups, and air vs. 6%CO2 in adults) and postnatal manipulation (RCF vs. standard rearing in CT) in the two exposure challenges were then tested by repeated-measures ANOVA; upon confirmation of significant main effects, differences among individual means were analyzed with post-hoc Tukey's HSD test.

Since the respiratory outcomes in the first and second challenge were highly correlated for all parameters (mean r = 0.82, p = 0.000001, in pups and adults), for the sake of conciseness we only show the results with the first respiratory challenge in pups and adults.

Quantitative Genetic Investigation of Individual Differences for CO2 Sensitivity

In quantitative genetics, the variance observed for a given phenotype in a group can be partitioned into a proportion attributable to genetic factors, and another proportion attributable to environmental factors, provided that the degree of genetic relatedness among individuals in the study group is known. Thus, depending on circumstances, one can derive from the phenotypic measurements of human monozygotic and dizygotic twin pairs (additive genetic correlation: 1 and 0.5, respectively), or from the phenotypic measurements of unrelated, half-sib and full-sib animals (additive genetic correlation: 0, 0.25 , and 0.5, respectively), the ratio of genetic variance to phenotypic variance or heritability (h2) [51]. Gene-by-environment interplay (GXE) mechanisms assume that genetic variance changes as a function of environmental exposure [20], [21]. Likewise, in a typical stress-diathesis GXE model, one may observe that the heritability for the trait under study increases as a function of environmental adversities.

To investigate the nature of the proportion of variance of the respiratory response to CO2 associated with RCF, we crossed 8 unrelated naïve sires with 16 unrelated naïve dams in a 1 sire/2 dam breeding design, yielding litters of 8 pups/dam in full-sib/half-sib degrees of relatedness. Eight litters were exposed to the RCF procedure, while the other eight were reared normally, yielding 64 RCF and 64 CT offspring within a full-sib/half-sib (fs/hs) design [51], whereby for each sire both litters were assigned to the RCF- or CT post-natal condition, as appropriate to conduct variance component analysis, nested ANOVA and heritability estimates [51]. Pups in the fs/hs design were assessed only for respiratory phenotypes.

Animal Care and Statistical Analyses

Unless otherwise specified, all animals took part in only one of the different experiments outlined in this paper, so that they were all naïve individuals. All experiments were conducted under license from the Italian Health Department and in accordance with Italian regulations on the use of research animals (legislation DL 116/92) and NIH guidelines on animal care.

Data were analysed using general linear model approaches, nested ANOVA or variance component analysis, as appropriate.

The variability among dams in providing NURSING and GP/L to fostered (RCF) pups was assessed by two separate ANOVA (Factor: dam) of maternal behaviour collected from PND1 to PND7.

We tested whether the degree of variability in receiving NURSING and GP/L among RCF litters could be assumed as homogeneous by the Levene test (factor : litter).

Similarly, we assessed whether the degree of variability in receiving NURSING and GP/L could be comparable between RCF and CT pups (factor : post-natal treatment) by the Levene test.

Finally, to dissociate the role played by maternal care received by the “final” adoptive mother from PND5 onward from the effects exerted by changes and variation in care received across the 4 cross fostering days (PND1-PND4), we compared the means and variances for NURSING and GP/L in 10 RCF litters by dividing the periods of maternal care into 2 periods of respectively 4 days (PND1–4) and 3 days (PND5–7), as factors.

To ensure sufficient statistical power, the number of subjects in the experiments was determined on the basis of pilot studies carried out in pups and adults. For all figures, bars on histograms indicate standard errors, for all experiments significance was set at p≤0.05.

Results

Maternal Behaviour

Nursing decreased significantly in time (F6,119 = 3.48, p<0.0035, Figure 2A) across the PND1-PND7 time span, but neither post-natal treatment, nor time-by-post-natal treatment yielded significant effects (respectively: F1,124 = 0.85 p = NS; F6,112 = 1.27 p = NS); consistent with these data, there was no significant difference between RCF and CT mice for weight, measured at different stages of development from PND8 through PND90 (see also Table S1).

thumbnail
Figure 2. Maternal care and offspring behavioural indices in standard rearing (CT) vs. repeated cross-fostering (RCF) conditions.

Sum of 2 daily observations of maternal behaviour: A) nursing behaviour, encompassing ‘arch-back’+‘blanket’ postures, and B) grooming/licking (GP/L) behaviour towards adoptive (RCF n = 10) and own (CT n = 8) litter, measured during PND1–PND7. Nursing decreased significantly in time, and was comparable in RCF and CT pups across the PND1–PND7 time span. Grooming/licking (GP/L) did not vary significantly in time, and RCF and CT pups received comparable GP/L (see Methods and Results sections for details). Pups' behaviour: C) Mean number of ultrasonic vocalisations (USVs) emitted by 8-day old RCF and CT pups. Pups were isolated and exposed for 5′ to fresh clean bedding (clean) and own-cage bedding (own). ANOVA showed that the postnatal treatment (RCF vs. CT) yielded a significant effect (F1,73 = 4.24, p = 0.04) while the condition (‘clean’ vs. ‘own’ bedding) did not exert a significant effect (F1,73 = 0.84, p = NS); there was no significant postnatal treatment-by-condition effect (F1,71 = 1.29, p = NS). D) Percentage of time spent during 5 minutes by pups in a compartment containing own-cage vs. fresh clean bedding (own vs. clean), or own-cage vs. an alien dam's bedding (own vs. alien dam). RCF pups spent less time in the compartment with own-cage bedding than controls in both conditions (F1,64 = 7.46, p<0.01).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018637.g002

Grooming/licking (GP/L) did not vary significantly in time (F6,119 = 1.5 p = NS, Figure 2B), and neither post-natal treatment (F1,124 = 0.12 p = NS) nor the interaction of post-natal treatment-by-time yielded significant effects (F6,112 = 1.33 p = NS).

By ANOVA (factor: dam) we found that NURSING did not differ significantly among dams who took care of the RCF pups (F9,30 = 1.45 p = NS) as ‘adoptive mothers’, but GP/L differed significantly among these dams (F9,30 = 2.90 p = 0.01). However, when we estimated the differences in variance between 10 RCF litters (estimated across 7 days and 4 different dams/litter) for the amount of received care (factor: litter) by Levene test, we found significant differences neither for NURSING (Levene's 9,60 = 1.14, p = NS) nor for GP/L (Levene's 9,60 = 1.46, p = NS). Thus, while there was a certain degree of variability for maternal GP/L towards the fostered pups that was attributable to dams as individuals, the differences in variance of received care among RCF litters from PND1 to PND7 were not significant. This implies that although each RCF litter received care from 4 different dams (i.e., 4 different ‘foster mothers’) in the PND1-PND4 period, the amount of variability of GP/L and NURSING could be assumed as homogeneous among RCF litters.

Similarly, the total variance of NURSING and GP/L did not differ between RCF and CT pups: when we assessed by the Levene test whether the degree of variability in receiving NURSING and GP/L could be comparable between RCF and CT pups (factor: post-natal treatment), we found no significant differences (NURSING Levene's1,124 = 2.86, p = NS; GP/L Levene's1,124 = 0.53, p = NS). This implies that (inasmuch as the variances of NURSING and GP/L could be assumed as sufficient indicators of postnatal treatment) RCF and CT pups were exposed to similar amounts and variability of postnatal care, the only difference between the two types of postnatal treatment being the intrinsic instability of the caregiver.

Finally, when we compared the means and variances for NURSING and GP/L among RCF pups during the PND1-PND4 period, as opposed to the following PND5-PND7 period (factor: PND1–4 vs. PND5–7), we found that the means and variances for both indexes could be assumed as equal (NURSING: Levene's1,68 = 2.43, p = NS; GP/L: Levene's1,68 = 0.27, p = NS; NURSING: ANOVA F1,68 = 1.86, p = NS; GP/L ANOVA F1,68 = 1.03, p = NS). This implies that within the RCF group, NURSING and GP/L were quantitatively similar when pups were exposed to changing caregivers, and in the following first 3 days of stable motherhood.

Offspring's Behaviour

At PND8, during two different isolation paradigms (conditions: a) ‘clean bedding’ and b) ‘own cage bedding’, see also methods) we observed an effect of postnatal treatment upon the isolation distress calls in the form of more USVs emitted by RCF pups. Figure 2C (see also caption) shows that there was no ‘postnatal treatment-by-condition’ interaction. The lower RCF vs. CT difference in isolation distress calls during the ‘clean bedding’ condition (Figure 2C) may indicate a ‘maximum stimulation’ effect induced by the absence of any odour in this specific experimental condition. Consistently, a milder stimulation condition (isolation in ‘own cage bedding’) appeared to unmask the RCF-CT differences more sharply. Sex of subjects did not yield a significant effect, alone or in interaction with the other independent variables.

At PND10, when RCF pups were tested for their preference between own-cage bedding as an alternative to: a) clean bedding, or: b) to an alien dam's bedding (Figure 2D), they showed consistently reduced preference for their own bedding compared to CT pups (Figure 2D). Sex of subjects did not yield a significant effect, alone or in interaction with the other independent variables.

Corticosterone basal levels in RCF and CT Individuals

The mean (±SE) basal serum concentration of corticosterone did not differ in lactating RCF and CT dams (ng/ml: 45.48±17.79 vs 53.02±11.87 respectively; F1,9 = 0.11, p = NS). Likewise, variance components analyses showed that serum concentrations of corticosterone at PND27–28 did not differ amongst RCF (n = 16 subjects belonging to 4 sibships, n/ml 62.70±3.86) vs. CT subjects (n = 14 subjects belonging to 4 sibships, ng/ml 70.27±4.25; fixed: maternal effect F1,6 = 1.38, p = 0.28: random: sibship effect F6,22 = 1.36, p = 0.27).

Assessments of Face Validity for Human Panic (1–4)

1 Corticosterone levels after exposition to CO2-enriched air.

Figure 3A shows that after 20 minutes of 6% CO2 breathing the serum concentration of corticosterone was heightened in a similar fashion among RCF and CT subjects.

thumbnail
Figure 3. Behavioural and Endocrinological Phenotypes among RCF and CT subjects.

A: Serum concentration of corticosterone. In both RCF and CT subjects corticosterone was significantly heightened (F1,9 = 49.71 p = 0.0001) after 20 minutes of 6% CO2 breathing compared to the basal values obtained in room air breathing; neither ‘postnatal treatment’ (RCF vs. CT) nor ‘postnatal treatment-by-air mixtures’ revealed differences for corticosterone serum concentrations (respectively F1,9 = 0.2 p = NS, and F1,7 = 0.08, p = NS). B: Place avoidance/preference towards environments with heightened CO2 concentration (6% CO2 air mixture): during the first five minutes of the test session, RCF individuals showed significantly higher tendency to avoid the compartment that had been previously paired with 6% CO2. (ANOVA-R : postnatal manipulation x time interval effect: F1,22 = 4.51, p<0.05, Tukey HSD post-hoc test p<0.02). C: Free exploratory test. The percentage of time spent at the centre of an arena was significantly influenced by the interaction of postnatal treatment-by-air mixtures (F1,74 = 4.03, p = 0.048) whereby RCF subjects spent significantly less time than CT subjects after exposure to 20% CO2. Neither the ‘postnatal treatment’ (RCF/CT), nor the ‘air mixtures’ (normal air/20% CO2) variables showed significant effects alone (respectively F1,76 = 1.90 p = NS, F1,76 = 0.13 p = NS). D: Latency to fall from the Rotarod. Analysis of variance showed that the performance at the Rotarod was significantly worse among RCF than CT subjects (F1,67 = 5.08 p = 0.03), in that the former showed significantly shorter latency of fall from the rod.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018637.g003

2 Avoidance of CO2-enriched environments.

Adult (PND 70–90) RCF mice exposed to a place conditioning protocol displayed an immediate avoidant response to environments associated with heightened CO2 concentration, compared to CT mice, as shown in Figure 3B.

3 Free exploratory test after exposition to CO2-enriched air.

A free exploratory test showed that after exposure to 20% CO2, RCF subjects have a significant reduction of the percentage of time spent at the centre of an arena compared to CT subjects (Figure 3C). After being exposed to room air, on the contrary, RCF and CT subjects did not differ significantly for this behaviour. Sex of subjects did not yield a significant effect, alone or in interaction with the other independent variables.

4 Balance test (Rotarod) in RCF and CT individuals.

The performance at the Rotarod shown in Figure 3D was significantly worse among RCF than CT subjects, in that the former showed significantly shorter latency of fall from the rod, which suggests impaired balance. Sex of subjects did not yield a significant effect.

Respiratory Parameters in RCF and CT Individuals

The RCF procedure was associated with higher mean percent increment of tidal volume from baseline (ΔTV%) during 6%CO2. At PND16–20, the RCF pups showed one-and-a-half times the ΔTV% increase shown by CT pups when they were exposed to 6% CO2-enriched air mixture, but no difference of ΔTV% in response to hypoxic air (10% O2), or normal air, compared to CT pups (Figure 4A). The TV increase determined higher MV amongst RCF than CT pups in response to 6%CO2 (mean MV during 1st 6%CO2 challenge, respectively: ml/min 53.10±18.22 vs.40.98±12.65, p = 0.009), whereas the mean respiratory frequency (f) did not differ significantly in RCF and CT pups during air, 10%O2, or 6%CO2 conditions. The RCF procedure also appeared to affect individual sensitivity to 6%CO2 in a stable way, in that adult mice (age 75–90 days) that had experienced postnatal RCF, but no other adverse event thereafter, showed higher ΔTV% than CT mice in response to 6%CO2 (Figure 4B). For both pups and adult mice, the respiratory response to 6%CO2 was more marked during the 1st challenge than during the 2nd challenge, as shown by a significant effect of ‘time’ in the general ANOVA-R models, but differences between RCF and CT animals remained consistent and significant within the first- and second challenge recordings.

thumbnail
Figure 4. Respiratory responses to air, 10%O2, or 6%CO2 in CT and RCF subjects at different ages.

Percentage of tidal volume changes from baseline (ΔTV%) for: a) 16–20 day-old pups in response to normal air, 10% O2, or 6% CO2. The ANOVA-R carried out on two consecutive respiratory challenges (as depicted in Figure 1) indicated a significant effect of: 1) treatment (type of air mixture): F2,58 = 91.30, p = 0.000001, 2) time: F1,58 = 4.34, p<0.05, and 3) an interaction effect of postnatal manipulation-by-type of air mixture: F2,58 = 9.99, p<0.0002); Tukey HSD post-hoc test p<0.001; b) 75–90 day-old adult mice in response to normal air or 6% CO2. The ANOVA-R carried out on two consecutive respiratory challenges (as depicted in Figure 1) indicated a significant effect of: 1) treatment (type of air mixture): F1,71 = 184.83, p = 0.00001, 2) time: F1,72 = 35.12, p = 0.00001 and 3) an interaction effect of postnatal manipulation-by-type of air mixture F1,71 = 6.60, p = 0.012). Tukey HSD post-hoc test p<0.001. Sample sizes varied between 9 and 13 animals per group. Only the responses to the first of two consecutive challenges performed for each subject with the same air mixture (air/10% O2/6% CO2) are shown in Figure 4 for the sake of conciseness.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018637.g004

Individual Differences for CO2 Responsiveness and Gene-Environment Interplay

Among the unrelated individuals which underwent the respiratory challenges shown in Figure 4, the mean variance of ΔTV% during 6%CO2 challenges was 57.75 for CT (n = 8) and 124.58 for RCF (n = 13) pups, and: 113.06 for CT (n = 17) and 224.62 for RCF (n = 19) adult subjects (Figure 4). Thus, the RCF procedure appeared to act not only as an enhancer of the mean physiological response to 6%CO2, but it also acted as a trigger to disclose individual differences for the response to CO2 amongst unrelated individuals.

On the basis of this datum, and to explore the nature of the proportion of variance of the respiratory response to CO2 that appeared to be associated with the RCF procedure, we relied on quantitative genetic analyses of data obtained from the full sib/half-sib (fs/hs) design, as outlined in the Methods section.

Table 1 shows the ΔTV% mean increments during 6%CO2 for RCFfs/hs and CTfs/hs subjects at PND 16–20. According to nested ANOVA, this response to 6% CO2-enriched air was significantly influenced by both postnatal treatment (RCF vs. CT) and by the degree of genetic relatedness, i.e., a ‘sibship’ factor. Consistent with this result, Variance Component Analysis showed significantly higher genetic variance (Va) for the ΔTV% response to 6% CO2-enriched air among RCFfs/hs than CTfs/hs individuals, and almost double heritability, as estimated from half-sibs' correlations [51], for ΔTV% response to 6% CO2-enriched air among RCFfs/hs compared to CTfs/hs subjects. The 0.21 heritability we found for ΔTV% among CTfs/hs is close to the 0.24 heritability value reported for TV increase under continuous respiration of 7% CO2-enriched air mixtures in normally-reared rats [52]. The significant difference between the RCFfs/hsVa and the CTfs/hsVa for ΔTV% response to 6% CO2-enriched air, and the sizable increase in heritability, indicate the presence of genetic control of sensitivity to the environment [53] evoked by the RCF procedure.

thumbnail
Table 1. Tidal Volume percent increment (ΔTV%) in response to 6% CO2 in RCF and normally-reared (CT) pups at postnatal day 16–20: Mean Values, Genetic Variance, and Heritability figures estimated from unrelated, half-sib and full-sib individuals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018637.t001

Discussion

Our results show that the respiratory reactivity to CO2-enriched air can be modified by a form of environmental adversity that is not primarily associated with breathing, namely repeated cross-fostering during the first postnatal days. While the RCF protocol used in this study did not interfere with the pups' normal development, as shown by comparable weights and body temperatures in RCF and CT mice, it may have interfered with the formation of infant-mother selective bond. Accordingly, our behavioural data show that while the RCF procedure did not evoke a response of neglect from adoptive mothers, it induced measurable behavioural distress -such as higher number of separation calls, typically interpreted as sign of separation anxiety [54]- amongst RCF pups. The RCF also possibly altered the ability to orient and approach maternal cues among these pups. More importantly for the aims of this investigation, the RCF procedure appeared to impact upon sensitivity to 6% CO2-enriched air selectively (as responses to 10%O2 and normal air were unaffected by the RCF), and stably from childhood into early adulthood (as responses to 6% CO2 were similar in pups and in adult RCF subjects). Such specificity of effect conforms with the notion that the regulatory mechanisms of hypercapnic and hypoxic ventilatory responses are functionally separated and genetically dissociated in mice [31]. Moreover, these results point towards an effect of RCF upon the central, more than the peripheral chemoceptors, since the former are much more sensitive to changes in P CO2 (monitored as [H+]), than changes in P O2. The ‘classical’ maternal separation protocol (e.g., 3 hours/day for 10 consecutive days) has been reported to influence the respiratory responses in rats [55]. However these effects are less specific, in that both the responses to hypoxia and CO2 are altered, and less straightforward to interpret, as opposite patterns of ventilatory response heightened CO2 have been observed in male and female rats [54]. Unlike what we observed in our RCF mice via the corticosterone data, in rats the 3 hours/day for 10 consecutive days procedure of maternal separation enhances the basal hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis function [56].

The RCF procedure appeared to act not only as an enhancer of the mean physiological response to CO2. It also acted as a trigger to disclose individual differences for the predisposition to vary the response to CO2. Our data show significant differences in genetic variance and in heritability between RCF and CT subjects. This indicates that mechanisms of genetic control of sensitivity to the environment are operant here, in the absence of the gene-environment correlations that often complicate the interpretation of heritability variation in man [20], [57]. In other words, the RCF procedure brought about a proportion of diversity for CO2 sensitivity that was ultimately attributable to genetic effects. One may speculate on variations in gene expressions as one likely molecular explanation of our quantitative genetics results, and genomics approaches would now be needed to further explore this paradigm. The results of molecular genetic analyses in this mouse model of separation anxiety could in turn kindle new molecular genetic approaches to human PD and SAD.

Turning to behavioural variables, while the separation calls in RCF and CT pups showed that our procedure evoked more separation anxiety amongst the former [54], it is tempting to relate the avoidance towards CO2-enriched environments shown by RCF mice to the avoidant/escape behaviour that people with PD display towards crowded, closed environments, where they fear they might experience smothering sensations and panic [14]. Likewise, previous exposure to heightened CO2 concentrations reduced free exploratory behaviour significantly more among the RCF than the CT subjects.

Two further findings appear to establish a parallel between the RCF mice and humans at heightened risk for the SAD-PD continuum. First, we found similar post-CO2 corticosterone concentrations among RCF and CT mice, and cortisol levels following spontaneous and CO2-provoked attacks do not seem to bear strongly upon the diagnosis of PD in man [4], [10], [11], [13]. Second, RCF mice performed significantly worse than CT at the Rotarod. Consistent with this finding, vestibular dysfunctions are found more often among people with PD than among healthy controls [9], with dizziness being frequently reported in spontaneous and CO2-provoked panic attacks in man [8]. On the other hand, we did not find significant sex-related differences in our respiratory and behavioural tests. This is a possibly relevant discrepancy compared to human data, whereby women typically respond more than men to CO2 stimulation and have higher prevalence of PD [8].

In conclusion, inasmuch as the genetic determinants that promote overreaction to heightened CO2 concentrations and naturally-occurring panic attacks in man coincide [16], the adoption of objective respiratory responses, more than the inferential assessment of emotionality or behaviour, is a viable strategy for laboratory animal models of human PD. By these same strategies the developmental pathways of continuity from childhood separation anxiety into adult panic disorder, and the association of both conditions with altered sensitivity to CO2, can be further clarified, and GXE mechanisms explored in man and animal. Animal laboratory investigations of the mechanisms by which environmental adversities -including childhood unstable parental environment/separation from caregivers- and genetic factors impinge upon CO2 sensitivity can set a new basis for future, better-tailored genetic approaches to human neurodevelopmental anxiety disorders [20], [58]. Our findings further support the investigation of the precise causal mechanisms that connect environmental adversities occurring in sensitive periods of development to health status in childhood and early adulthood [59].

Supporting Information

Table S1.

Body Weight and Temperature of RCF and CT pups at different developmental stages.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018637.s002

(DOC)

Acknowledgments

The help of C Cinque and S Quaresima in corticosterone assay measurements is gratefully acknowledged.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: MB FRD AM. Performed the experiments: CZ VL RC DO. Analyzed the data: MB FRD CAMS PP-G CZ VL DO. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: TP RV SP-A. Wrote the paper: MB FRD. Revision of the final version of the manuscript: CAMS PP-G VL.

References

  1. 1. MacLean PD (1985) Brain evolution relating to family, play, and the separation call. Arch Gen Psychiatry 42: 405–417.
  2. 2. Battaglia M, Bertella S, Politi E, Bernardeschi L, Perna G, et al. (1995) Age at onset of panic disorder: influence of familial liability to the disease and of childhood separation anxiety disorder. Am J Psychiatry 152: 1362–1364.
  3. 3. Klein RG (1995) Is panic disorder associated with childhood separation anxiety disorder? Clin Neuropharmacol 18: S7–S14.
  4. 4. Klein DF (1993) False suffocation alarms, spontaneous panic, and related conditions. Arch Gen Psychiatry 50: 306–317.
  5. 5. Pine DS, Klein RG, Coplan JD, Papp LA, Hoven CW, et al. (2000) Differential carbon dioxide sensitivity in childhood anxiety disorders and non ill comparison group. Arch Gen Psychiatry 57: 960–967.
  6. 6. Preter M, Klein DF (2008) Panic, suffocation false alarms, separation anxiety and endogenous opioids. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 32: 603–612.
  7. 7. Anderson DJ, Noyes R Jr, Crowe RR (1984) A comparison of panic disorder and generalized anxiety disorder. Am J Psychiatry 141: 572–575.
  8. 8. Battaglia M, Ogliari A, Harris J, Spatola CA, Pesenti-Gritti P, et al. (2007) A genetic study of the acute anxious response to carbon dioxide stimulation in man. J Psychiatr Res 41: 906–917.
  9. 9. Jacob RG, Furman JM, Durrant JD, Turner SM (1996) Panic,agoraphobia and vestibular dysfunction. Am J Psychiatry 4: 503–512.
  10. 10. van Duinen MA, Schruers KR, Maes M, Griez EJ (2007) CO2 challenge induced HPA axis activation in panic. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 10: 797–804.
  11. 11. Terleph TA, Klein RG, Roberson-Nay R, Mannuzza S, Moulton JL 3rd, et al. (2006) Stress responsivity and HPA axis activity in juveniles: results from a home-based CO2 inhalation study. Am J Psychiatry 163: 738–740.
  12. 12. Brandes M, Bienvenu OJ (2006) Personality and anxiety disorders. Curr Psychiatry Rep 8: 263–269.
  13. 13. Westberg P, Modigh K, Lisjö P, Eriksson E (1991) Higher postdexamethasone serum cortisol levels in agoraphobic than in nonagoraphobic panic disorder patients. Biol Psy 30: 247–256.
  14. 14. Battaglia M, Ogliari A (2005) Anxiety and panic: from human studies to animal research and back. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29: 169–179.
  15. 15. American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (ed 4). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
  16. 16. Battaglia M, Pesenti-Gritti P, Spatola CA, Ogliari A, Tambs K (2008) A twin study of the common vulnerability between heightened sensitivity to hypercapnia and panic disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 147: 586–593.
  17. 17. Battaglia M, Pesenti-Gritti P, Medland SA, Ogliari A, Tambs K, et al. (2009) A genetically informed study of the association between childhood separation anxiety, sensitivity to CO(2), panic disorder, and the effect of childhood parental loss. Arch Gen Psychiatry 66: 64–71.
  18. 18. Ogliari A, Tambs K, Harris JR, Scaini S, Maffei C, et al. (2010) The relationships between adverse events, early antecedents, and carbon dioxide reactivity as an intermediate phenotype of panic disorder. A general population study. Psychother Psychosom 79: 48–55.
  19. 19. Spatola CAM, Scaini S, Pesenti-Gritti P, Medland SE, Moruzzi S, et al. (2011) Gene-Environment Interactions in Panic Disorder and CO2 Sensitivity: Effects of Life Events Occurring Early in Life. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 156: 79–88.
  20. 20. Rutter M, Moffitt TE, Caspi A (2006) Gene-environment interplay and psychopathology: multiple varieties but real effects. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 47: 226–261.
  21. 21. Purcell S (2002) Variance components models for gene-environment interaction in twin analysis. Twin Res 5: 554–71.
  22. 22. Kas MJ, Fernandes C, Schalkwyk LC, Collier DA (2007) Genetics of behavioural domains across the neuropsychiatric spectrum; of mice and men. Mol Psychiatry 12: 324–330.
  23. 23. Hallem EA, Sternberg PW (2008) Acute carbon dioxide avoidance in Caenorhabditiselegans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105: 8038–8043.
  24. 24. Suh GS, Wong AM, Hergarden AC, Wang JW, Simon AF, et al. (2004) A single population of olfactory sensory neurons mediates an innate avoidance behaviour in Drosophila. Nature 431: 854–859.
  25. 25. Cayirlioglu P, Kadow IG, Zhan X, Okamura K, Suh GS, et al. (2008) Hybrid neurons in a microRNA mutant are putative evolutionary intermediates in insect CO2 sensory systems. Science 319: 1256–1260.
  26. 26. Griez EJ, Colasanti A, van Diest R, Salamon E, Schruers K (2007) Carbon dioxide inhalation induces dose-dependent and age-related negative affectivity. PLoS One 10: e987.
  27. 27. Colasanti A, Salamon E, Schruers K, van Diest R, van Duinen M, et al. (2008) Carbon dioxide-induced emotion and respiratory symptoms in healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology 33: 3103–3110.
  28. 28. Brannan S, Liotti M, Egan G, Shade R, Madden L, et al. (2001) Neuroimaging of cerebral activations and deactivations associated with hypercapnia and hunger for air. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98: 2029–2034.
  29. 29. Ziemann AE, Allen JE, Dahdaleh NS, Drebot II, Coryell MW, et al. (2009) The amygdala is a chemosensor that detects carbon dioxide and acidosis to elicit fear behavior. Cell 139: 1012–21.
  30. 30. Millhorn DE, Eldridge FL (1986) Role of ventrolateral medulla in the regulation of respiratory and cardiovascular systems. J Appl Physiol 61: 1249–1263.
  31. 31. Tankersley CG, Fitzgerald RS, Kleeberger SR (1994) Differential control of ventilation among inbred strains of mice. Am J Physiol 267: R1371–1377.
  32. 32. Gaultier C, Gallego J (2008) Neural control of breathing: insights from genetic mouse models. J Appl Physiol 104: 1522–1530.
  33. 33. Bavis RW, Mitchell GS (2008) Long-term effects of the perinatal environment on respiratory control. J Appl Physiol 104: 1220–1229.
  34. 34. Gottesman II, Gould TD (2003) The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry 160: 636–645.
  35. 35. Gould TD, Gottesman II (2006) Psychiatric endophenotypes and the development of valid animal models. Genes Brain Behav 5: 113–119.
  36. 36. Smoller JW, Tsuang MT (1998) Panic and phobic anxiety: defining phenotypes for genetic studies. Am J Psychiatry 155: 1152–1162.
  37. 37. Fyer AJ, Weissman MM (1999) Genetic linkage study of panic: clinical methodology and description of pedigrees. Am J Med Genet 88: 173–181.
  38. 38. Caspi A, Moffitt TE (2006) Gene-environment interactions in psychiatry: joining forces with neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 7: 583–590.
  39. 39. Shoji H, Kato K (2009) Maternal care affects the development of maternal behavior in inbred mice. Dev Psychobiol 51: 345–57.
  40. 40. Barbazanges A, Vallée M, Mayo W, Day J, Simon H, et al. (1996) Early and later adoptions have different long-term effects on male rat offspring. J Neurosci 16: 7783–7790.
  41. 41. Bartolomucci A, Gioiosa L, Chirieleison A, Ceresini G, Parmigiani S, et al. (2004) Cross fostering in mice: behavioral and physiological carry-over effects in adulthood. Genes Brain Behav 3: 115–122.
  42. 42. Upton KJ, Sullivan RM (2010) Defining age limits of the sensitive period for attachment learning in rat pups. Dev Psychobiol 52: 453–464.
  43. 43. Kendler KS, Neale MC, Kessler RC, Heath AC, Eaves LJ (1992) Childhood parental loss and adult psychopathology in women: a twin study perspective. Arch Gen Psychiatry 49: 109–116.
  44. 44. Forman EM, Davies PT (2003) Family instability and young adolescent maladjustment: the mediating effects of parenting quality and adolescent appraisals of family security. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 32: 94–105.
  45. 45. Warn PA, Brampton MW, Sharp A, Morrissey G, Steel N, et al. (2003) Infrared body temperature measurement of mice as an early predictor of death in experimental fungal infections. Lab Anim 37: 126–131.
  46. 46. Shoji H, Kato K (2006) Maternal behavior of primiparous females in inbred strains of mice: a detailed descriptive analysis. Physiol Behav 89: 320–328.
  47. 47. Moles A, Kieffer BL, D'Amato FR (2004) Deficit in attachment behavior in mice lacking the mu-opioid receptor gene. Science 304: 1983–1986.
  48. 48. Ricceri L, Moles A, Crawley J (2006) Behavioral phenotyping of mouse models of neurodevelopmental disorders: relevant social behavior patterns across the life span. Behav Brain Res 176: 40–52.
  49. 49. Cabib S, Orsini C, Le Moal M, Piazza PV (2000) Abolition and reversal of strain differences in behavioral responses to drugs of abuse after a brief experience. Science 289: 463–465.
  50. 50. Mandillo S, Tucci V, Hölter SM, Meziane H, Banchaabouchi MA, et al. (2008) Reliability, robustness, and reproducibility in mouse behavioralphenotyping: a cross-laboratory study. Physiol Genomics 34: 243–255.
  51. 51. Falconer DS, McKay TFC (1996) Introduction to quantitative genetics. England: Longman Essex.
  52. 52. Han F, Strohl KP (2000) Inheritance of ventilatory behavior in rodent models. Respir Physiol 121: 247–256.
  53. 53. Mather K, Jinks JL (1982) Biometrical genetics: The study of continuous variation. London: Chapman & Hall.
  54. 54. Cryan JF, Holmes A (2005) The ascent of mouse: advances in modelling human depression and anxiety. Nat Rev Drug Discov 4: 775–790.
  55. 55. Genest SE, Gulemetova R, Laforest S, Drolet G, Kinkead R (2004) Neonatal maternal separation and sex-specific plasticity of the hypoxic ventilatory response in awake rat. J Physiol 554: 543–557.
  56. 56. Genest SE, Gulemetova R, Laforest S, Drolet G, Kinkead R (2007) Neonatal maternal separation induces sex-specific augmentation of the hypercapnic ventilatory response in awake rat. J Appl Physiol 102: 1416–1421.
  57. 57. Visscher PM, Hill WG, Wray NM (2008) Heritability in the genomics era: concepts and misconceptions. Nat Rev Genet 9: 255–266.
  58. 58. Ressler KJ, Mayberg HS (2007) Targeting abnormal neural circuits in mood and anxiety disorders: from the laboratory to the clinic. Nat Neurosci 10: 1116–1124.
  59. 59. Shonkoff JP, Boyce WT, McEwen BS (2009) Neuroscience, molecular biology, and the childhood roots of health disparities: building a new framework for health promotion and disease prevention. JAMA 301: 2252–2259.