Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Genetic and Environmental Influences on Infant Growth: Prospective Analysis of the Gemini Twin Birth Cohort

  • Laura Johnson,

    Affiliations Cancer Research UK Health Behavior Research Centre, Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London, London, United Kingdom, Department of Public Health and Primary Care, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom

  • Clare H. Llewellyn,

    Affiliation Cancer Research UK Health Behavior Research Centre, Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London, London, United Kingdom

  • Cornelia H. M. van Jaarsveld,

    Affiliation Cancer Research UK Health Behavior Research Centre, Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London, London, United Kingdom

  • Tim J. Cole,

    Affiliation MRC Centre of Epidemiology for Child Health, Institute of Child Health, University College London, London, United Kingdom

  • Jane Wardle

    j.wardle@ucl.ac.uk

    Affiliation Cancer Research UK Health Behavior Research Centre, Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London, London, United Kingdom

Abstract

Objective

Infancy is a critical period during which rapid growth potentially programs future disease risk. Identifying the modifiable determinants of growth is therefore important. To capture the complexity of infant growth, we modeled growth trajectories from birth to six months in order to compare the genetic and environmental influences on growth trajectory parameters with single time-point measures at birth, three and six months of age.

Methods

Data were from Gemini, a population sample of 2402 UK families with twins. An average 10 weight measurements per child made by health professionals were available over the first six months. Weights at birth, three and six months were identified. Longitudinal growth trajectories were modeled using SITAR utilizing all available weight measures for each child. SITAR generates three parameters: size (characterizing mean weight throughout infancy), tempo (indicating age at peak weight velocity (PWV)), and velocity (reflecting the size of PWV). Genetic and environmental influences were estimated using quantitative genetic analysis.

Results

In line with previous studies, heritability of weight at birth and three months was low (38%), but it was higher at six months (62%). Heritability of the growth trajectory parameters was high for size (69%) and velocity (57%), but low (35%) for tempo. Common environmental influences predominated for tempo (42%).

Conclusion

Modeled growth parameters using SITAR indicated that size and velocity were primarily under genetic influence but tempo was predominantly environmentally determined. These results emphasize the importance of identifying specific modifiable environmental determinants of the timing of peak infant growth.

Introduction

Infancy is a critical period during which growth patterns may program lifelong risk of obesity and chronic disease [1], [2], [3], [4], [5]. Normal infant growth consists of an initial fall in weight after birth followed by increasing weight gain to a peak at six weeks (peak weight velocity (PWV)) after which the rate of weight gain declines to a plateau around six months [6]. Both weight velocity and age at PWV (an indicator of growth tempo) vary between infants. Similar to other tempo indicators (e.g. adiposity rebound, puberty onset, peak height velocity), earlier PWV is associated with a higher risk of adult disease [2], [7], [8], [9]; suggesting that an accelerated tempo of development is detrimental to longer-term health. Rapid weight gain is a well-studied risk factor for obesity [10], but weight gain based on just two weight measurements cannot estimate age at PWV or characterize variations in velocity throughout infancy, which may be crucial to the development of chronic disease.

Quantitative genetic studies of single time-point weight measures between birth and three months indicate that variation in weight is primarily attributable to the environment; with just 10–44% explained by genetic influences [11], [12], [13], [14], [15], [16], [17], [18], whereas from five months onwards genes play a larger role (66–90% heritability) [13], [18], [19]. A summary of growth velocity can be captured by using repeated measures of weight throughout infancy and mathematically modeling growth trajectories using one of a range of infant growth curve models [20], [21], [22], [23]. Two studies modeled infant growth velocity (using a polynomial of degree 4 model or the Count model) based on an average of 12 weight measures between birth and 2.5 years in 681 children from 169 families [24], and in a large sample of twins (n = 3477 pairs) [22]. The heritability of growth velocity was estimated as 28% in the family study but 63% in the twin sample [22], [24].

No studies to date have characterized the heritability of the timing of PWV, nor has any study established whether the heritability of modeled growth parameters, which may be more reliable because they use data from multiple time-points, is higher than single time-point measures of weight. SuperImposition by Translation And Rotation (SITAR) is a novel method of modeling growth that estimates three parameters: size, velocity and tempo (equivalent to age at PWV) [25]. In this study we assessed the contribution of genetic and environmental factors to single measurements of weight at birth, three and six months and the three SITAR growth parameters in a large twin birth cohort.

Methods

Ethics Statement

Parents provided informed written consent for their family to participate in the study and ethical approval was granted by the University College London Committee for the Ethics of non–National Health Service Human Research. All aspects of data collection and storage were in accordance with the standards stipulated by this body.

Data came from Gemini [26], a birth cohort initiated in 2007 to investigate genetic and environmental influences on appetite, activity and growth from birth to 5 years. All families in England and Wales with live twin births between March and December 2007 (n = 6754) were eligible for recruitment and were asked by the Office of National Statistics for consent to be contacted by the research team. A total of 3435 families (51%) agreed to be contacted, of whom 2402 (70% of those contacted and 39% of all eligible families) returned a baseline questionnaire. Sex and gestational age were maternally reported. A validated questionnaire [27] established the zygosity of same-sex twin pairs as monozygotic (MZ) or dizygotic (DZ) (opposite-sex twins are all DZ). Children in England and Wales are measured regularly from birth by health professionals and the values are recorded in a personal health record kept by the parents. Parents were asked to photocopy the relevant pages of their children's health records or transcribe the measurements into the questionnaire. Parents returned the weight data when the twins were on average 8 (SD 2) months old, with a median of 10 (Inter-Quartile Range (IQR) 7) weight measurements per child recorded between birth and a median of 6.6 (IQR 3.1) months old.

Statistical analyses

Continuous variables are described between twin pairs by calculating the sample mean and SD of the means within each twin pair. Analogously, the within-pair SD was calculated as the SD of the difference within each twin pair. Categorical variables are described with frequencies and percentages. Descriptive analyses were performed in SPSS v15 (SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL).

Infant growth modeling.

Weight growth curves were analyzed using the SuperImposition by Translation and Rotation (SITAR) method [25]. SITAR is a shape invariant model with random effects, originally used for infant growth modeling by Beath [28]. It involves the estimation of an average growth curve for the sample, plus a set of three parameters for each individual that together transform the average curve to match the individual's growth. The average curve is fitted as a cubic spline and individual parameters are estimated as mixed-model random effects based on at least one weight measure per child. Growth curves for children with fewer measures of weight are therefore ‘shrunk’ towards the average curve because of the reduced information. Size is an up/down shift of the average curve, indicating whether the infant is bigger or smaller than average (see figure 1, panel A). Tempo is a left/right shift of the average curve, indicating whether the timing of PWV is earlier or later than average (figure 1, panel B). Velocity is a shrinking/stretching of the age scale, because shrinking the age scale makes the curve steeper (increasing velocity) and stretching the scale creates a shallower slope (reducing velocity) - effectively this is a rotation of the growth curve indicating the rate at which ‘growth time’ passes for an individual relative to the average (figure 1, panel C). The SITAR analysis was done using the nlme library [29] in the statistical package R [30].

thumbnail
Figure 1. Average weight and weight velocity curves for extremesa of size (A), tempo (B) and velocity (C) in infancy.

a Extreme groups were based on tertiles (T1, T2, T3) of SITAR parameters such that ‘small’ children were in T1 for size and T2 for tempo and velocity and ‘big’ children were in T3 for size and T2 for tempo and velocity; ‘early’ children were in T1 for tempo and T2 for size and velocity and ‘late’ children were in T3 for tempo and T2 for size and velocity; ‘slow’ children were in T1 for velocity and T2 for tempo and size and ‘fast’ children were in T3 for velocity and T2 for tempo and size.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0019918.g001

Separate SITAR models were fitted for first- and second-born twins, and the parameter estimates were compared within twin pairs. The modeling involved weight in kg and postmenstrual age (weeks). Size was measured in kg, tempo in weeks and velocity in fractional units, which multiplied by 100 correspond to a percentage of mean velocity [31]. Negative values represent smaller/earlier/slower and positive values larger/later/faster than average.

Single time-point measures of weight at birth, three and six months were identified for quantitative genetic analysis. Weight ‘at three months’ was defined as weight measured between two and four months closest to three months and weight ‘at six months’ was weight measured between five and seven months closest to six months (exact age was recorded). Weight standard deviation scores (SDS) at birth, three and six months were calculated adjusting for age, sex and gestational age based on the British 1990 growth reference [32], [33]. Change in weight SDS between birth and, three or six months was calculated by subtracting weight SDS at the earlier from weight SDS at the later time-point.

Heritability analyses.

Heritability was estimated using intra-class correlation (ICC) coefficients and quantitative genetic analysis. ICCs assess similarity within and between twin pairs; a higher ICC in MZ than DZ twin pairs indicates greater genetic influence. All heritability analyses were adjusted for age and sex using the residual method, a standard practice because age and sex are perfectly correlated within same-sex pairs, which mimics and inflates common environmental variation.

Quantitative genetic analysis provides robust estimates of genetic and environmental influences by partitioning trait variation into an additive genetic component (A; which makes more genetically related children more similar), a common environment component (C; environmental factors that make children in the same family more similar) and a unique environment component (E; which makes children in the same family more different but also includes measurement error) as well as generating confidence intervals. Using maximum-likelihood structural equation modelling, trait variance is partitioned based on expected covariance structures between MZ and DZ twins using the following assumptions: 1) MZs share 100% of their DNA (so their coefficient of genetic relatedness is 1); 2) DZs share 50% of their segregating genes (so their coefficient of genetic relatedness is 0.5); 3) MZs and DZs have the same common environmental exposures (fixing the covariance of the C component is 1 for both types of twins); 4) each twin's unique environment is uncorrelated with their sibling. More constrained sub-models dropping A or C or both were examined, but in all cases the full ACE model fitted best (as judged by the Bayesian Information Criterion and change in -2LL χ2 tests of sub-models producing a p<0.05) so only ACE model results are presented. Sex differences in A, C and E were also investigated using a sex-limitation model, but none were significant.

All available data on each weight/growth variable were used for the genetic analyses, but single time-point data had some missing cases, which meant that the sample size varied. Estimates of size, tempo and velocity were available for 2340 twin pairs, while weight SDS data were available for 2322 twin pairs at birth, 2110 pairs at three months and 1717 pairs at six months. Data on 2099 pairs were available for SDS change from birth to three months and 1633 pairs for SDS change from birth to six months. Twin analyses were carried out using Mx Maximum-Likelihood Structural Equation Modeling Software (version 32; Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA).

Results

Descriptive characteristics of the Gemini sample are displayed in Table 1. As expected of twins, they were born earlier and smaller than the British 1990 reference singleton children. MZ twins were also born earlier than DZ twins, which explained their lower weight at birth, three and six months. The SITAR model for first-born twins (based on 21,617 weights from 2340 individuals) had a residual SD of 0.16 kg compared with a residual SD of 0.69 kg for the population growth curve with no random effects. Thus the addition of the SITAR parameters to the model reduced the variance by 94%; providing a much better fit to the data. Results for the second-born twins were effectively the same as for the first-born. The random effect parameters had means of zero (by definition) and SDs of 0.68 kg for size, 3.7 weeks for tempo and 24% for velocity, which means that 95% of infants had sizes within 1.36 kg of mean size, tempos within 7.4 weeks of mean tempo, and velocities within 48% of mean velocity [31].

thumbnail
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of weight and weight change in infancy in the Gemini cohort.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0019918.t001

The size parameter was positively correlated with weight at birth, three and six months (r = 0.39, r = 0.41, r = 0.69 respectively). Velocity was associated with greater changes in weight SDS from birth to three (r = 0.68) and birth to six (r = 0.70) months. A later PWV was associated with longer gestation (r = 0.23) and lower weight SDS at birth, three and six months (r = −.022, r = −0.56, r = −0.22, respectively); conversely those who were younger at PWV tended to be larger at all ages.

ICCs and results of the quantitative twin analyses for weight at birth, three and six months, alongside SITAR growth parameters are shown in Table 2. ICCs for all measures were higher for MZ than DZ twins indicating genetic influence on infant growth; but the difference varied across growth indicators. The twin analyses indicated that heritability varied from just 38% at birth and three months to 62% at six months. Weight SDS change between birth and three months was 35% heritable, increasing to 57% for change in weight SDS from birth to six months. Unique environmental influences (which include measurement error) explained most of the variation in birth weight, while common environment effect was strongest for change in weight SDS from birth to three months (Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Intra-class correlations and heritability of weight, weight change and SITAR growth parameters from birth to six months in Gemini.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0019918.t002

The heritability of the SITAR growth parameters was moderate for size (69%) and velocity (57%) but low for tempo (35%) (Table 2). The common environment effect was highest for tempo (42%) and lowest for size (11%). Environmental influences unique to each twin explained a similar amount of variation for all three SITAR parameters.

Discussion

In this exploration of the genetic architecture of early infant growth, we observed differences in the heritability of early and later weight, as well as between the three modeled growth parameters (size, tempo and velocity). Like weight at six months, size and velocity were highly heritable features of growth trajectories. However, tempo showed a smaller genetic effect and a stronger influence of the common environment. In the light of evidence that peak weight velocity may be an important determinant of later health, support for the influence of childhood environments provides a valuable starting point for investigations of the environmental determinants of infant growth.

Birth weight, weight at three months, and weight SDS change from birth to three months, all had low heritability in the present analysis, which is in line with previous studies [11], [12], [13], [14], [15], [16], [18]. Our results indicate that environmental variation in birth weight is predominantly owing to factors unique to each child. A Norwegian family-based study of birth weight observed a similar pattern, with estimates of 15% for the common vs. 32% for the unique environment effect [17]. Unique environmental influences may be higher for birth weight because placental factors affecting nutrient transfer (such as chorionicity, placental fusion and central vs. peripheral insertion of the umbilical cord) can create differences even between MZ twins who share the same genes and placenta [34], [35]. Accounting for unique variation in placental factors in twin analyses has been shown to increase the estimated heritability of birth weight [14].

Between birth and three months, growth rates change rapidly and therefore weight at a single time-point during this period may not reflect size as reliably as at later points when growth rates stabilize [36]. Lower reliability reduces estimates of heritability and common environment and inflates the unique environment effect, which could contribute to the apparent low heritability of weight in early life. However, the first three months of life has also been identified as a critical period when growth is nutrition dependent [37], and because nutrition is primarily an environmental exposure (albeit affected by infant appetite [38]), this could offer an alternative explanation for the low heritability for immediate postnatal growth; consistent with the moderate shared environment effect observed in this analysis.

By six months there was evidence for a stronger genetic influence on weight. Studies of human growth and body composition [39] indicate that immediate postnatal weight gain is predominantly composed of fat mass, but by six months, gain in lean mass makes a larger contribution, as indicated by increased height growth after six months [40]. Adult height is 80% heritable [41] and therefore if weight in later infancy has a greater contribution from height gain, then this may partly explain the rise in heritability observed during this period.

Modeling growth using all available weight measures using SITAR generates three variables that indicate individual variation from the average growth curve for the group. Heritability of the size parameter was 69%; similar to results for weight measures taken after 5 months [13], [18], [19] and to our estimate for weight SDS at six months. The higher heritability may also reflect improved reliability of ‘size’ (which summarizes many weights) compared with single measures of weight. Our estimate of 57% heritability for velocity was similar to the estimate for change in weight SDS from birth to six months in the present study, and also to velocity derived from a different model of growth in twins during the first two years [22], although higher than was found in a family-based study [24]. There is evidence that some obesity genes are associated with both weight (a proxy for size) and weight gain (a proxy for velocity) in early infancy [42], [43], [44]. Multivariate heritability models investigating the genetic correlation between size, velocity and tempo could add further insight into the physiological control of growth in infancy.

We are not aware of any previous studies that have examined the heritability of the timing of PWV (tempo in the SITAR model). Our results showed low heritability for tempo compared with size and velocity. Previous studies of the magnitude of PWV have shown associations with an earlier onset of puberty (a indicator of tempo in later childhood) [45], suggesting that infant and later childhood growth tempo are linked. Earlier maternal menarche has also been associated with more rapid growth in their infants; linking tempo across generations [46]. However, our estimate of heritability for tempo in infancy was lower than has been observed for the timing of puberty [47], [48], [49]; supporting different biological processes. The evidence that tempo is under primarily environmental influence is important in the light of evidence that growth and health risk are programmed [50]. It highlights the need for studies that directly assess the potential environmental exposures.

The present study is strengthened by the use of data from a large, population-based sample with multiple weight measures made by health professionals, which have been shown to be accurate compared with clinic measures [36]. SITAR modeling averages information across measurement occasions and therefore minimizes measurement error as well as summarizing growth over time, allowing for more reliable estimates. There are also limitations. Although the Gemini sample is reasonably representative of families in England and Wales [26], higher SES families are over-represented, as observed in other cohort studies [51], [52]. Twins also grow differently from singletons during infancy [53], although there is no evidence to indicate differences in the underlying physiology of growth. The twin method also makes several assumptions [54], e.g. that MZ and DZ twins have equal sharing of common environmental exposures, but even if the equal environments assumption is violated, the effect on heritability estimates is relatively small and unlikely to alter the qualitative conclusion that genetic influence on tempo is lower than on size or velocity. Zygosity was estimated using a validated parent-report questionnaire that has previously demonstrated 96% accuracy for classifying twins correctly compared with DNA testing [27].

Conclusions

Using modeled growth parameters, we have shown that size and velocity are highly heritable whereas tempo (age at PWV) is largely explained by environmental factors. A better appreciation of the environmental determinants of infant growth will help inform the development of effective early interventions to promote healthy weight.

Acknowledgments

We should like to thank the Gemini families who are participating in the study and the Office of National Statistics for their help in recruiting them.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: JW LJ CHMvJ CHL TJC. Analyzed the data: LJ CHL TJC. Wrote the paper: LJ CHL TJC JW CHMvJ. Obtained funding for the study: JW.

References

  1. 1. Botton J, Heude B, Maccario J, Ducimetiere P, Charles M-A, et al. (2008) Postnatal weight and height growth velocities at different ages between birth and 5 y and body composition in adolescent boys and girls. Am J Clin Nut 87: 1760–1768.
  2. 2. Tzoulaki I, Sovio U, Pillas D, Hartikainen A-L, Pouta A, et al. (2010) Relation of Immediate Postnatal Growth With Obesity and Related Metabolic Risk Factors in Adulthood: The Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1966 Study. Am J Epidemiol.
  3. 3. Larnkjaer A, Schack-Nielsen L, Molgaard C, Ingstrup HK, Holst JJ, et al. (2010) Effect of growth in infancy on body composition, insulin resistance, and concentration of appetite hormones in adolescence. Am J Clin Nutr 91: 1675–1683.
  4. 4. Gillman MW (2008) The first months of life: a critical period for development of obesity. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 87: 1587–1589.
  5. 5. Chomtho S, Wells JCK, Williams JE, Davies PSW, Lucas A, et al. (2008) Infant growth and later body composition: evidence from the 4-component model. Am J Clin Nut 87: 1776–1784.
  6. 6. Butte NF (2005) Energy requirements of infants. Public Health Nutrition 8: 953–967.
  7. 7. Williams SM, Goulding A (2008) Patterns of Growth Associated With the Timing of Adiposity Rebound. Obesity 17: 335–341.
  8. 8. Pierce MB, Leon DA (2005) Age at menarche and adult BMI in the Aberdeen Children of the 1950s Cohort Study. Am J Clin Nut 82: 733–739.
  9. 9. Sandhu J, Ben-Shlomo Y, Cole TJ, Holly J, Davey Smith G (2006) The impact of childhood body mass index on timing of puberty, adult stature and obesity: a follow-up study based on adolescent anthropometry recorded at Christ's Hospital (1936-1964). Int J Obes 30: 14–22.
  10. 10. Ong KK, Loos RJF (2006) Rapid infancy weight gain and subsequent obesity: Systematic reviews and hopeful suggestions. Acta Paediatrica 95: 904–908.
  11. 11. Pietilainen KH, Kaprio J, Rasanen M, Rissanen A, Rose RJ (2002) Genetic and Environmental Influences on the Tracking of Body Size from Birth to Early Adulthood. Obesity 10: 875–884.
  12. 12. Beardsall K, Ong KK, Murphy N, Ahmed ML, Zhao JH, et al. (2009) Heritability of childhood weight gain from birth and risk markers for adult metabolic disease in prepubertal twins. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 94: 3708–3713.
  13. 13. Dubois L, Girard M, Girard A, Tremblay R, Boivin M, et al. (2007) Genetic and environmental influences on body size in early childhood: a twin birth-cohort study. Twin Res Hum Genet 10: 479–485.
  14. 14. Gielen M, Lindsey PJ, Derom C, Smeets HJ, Souren NY, et al. (2008) Modeling genetic and environmental factors to increase heritability and ease the identification of candidate genes for birth weight: a twin study. Behav Genet 38: 44–54.
  15. 15. Vlietinck R, Derom R, Neale MC, Maes H, van Loon H, et al. (1989) Genetic and environmental variation in the birth weight of twins. Behav Genet 19: 151–161.
  16. 16. Whitfield JB, Treloar SA, Zhu G, Martin NG (2001) Genetic and non-genetic factors affecting birth-weight and adult Body Mass Index. Twin Res 4: 365–370.
  17. 17. Lunde A, Melve KK, Gjessing HK, Skjaerven R, Irgens LM (2007) Genetic and Environmental Influences on Birth Weight, Birth Length, Head Circumference, and Gestational Age by Use of Population-based Parent-Offspring Data. American Journal of Epidemiology 165: 734–741.
  18. 18. Levine RS, Hennekens CH, Jesse MJ (1987) Genetic variance of weight and length in infant twins. Am J Epidemiol 126: 929–935.
  19. 19. Demerath EW, Choh AC, Czerwinski SA, Lee M, Sun SS, et al. (2007) Genetic and environmental influences on infant weight and weight change: the Fels Longitudinal Study. Am J Hum Biol 19: 692–702.
  20. 20. Karlberg J, Engstrom I, Karlberg P, Fryer JG (1987) Analysis of linear growth using a mathematical model. I. From birth to three years. Acta Paediatr Scand 76: 478–488.
  21. 21. Jenss RM, Bayley N (1937) A mathematical method for studying the growth of a child. Human Biology 9: 556–563.
  22. 22. van Dommelen P, de Gunst MC, van der Vaart AW, Boomsma DI (2004) Genetic study of the height and weight process during infancy. Twin Res 7: 607–616.
  23. 23. Count EW (1943) Growth patterns of the human physique: an approach to kinetic anthropometry. Human Biology 15: 1–32.
  24. 24. Livshits G, Peter I, Vainder M, Hauspie R (2000) Genetic analysis of growth curve parameters of body weight, height and head circumference. Ann Hum Biol 27: 299–312.
  25. 25. Cole TJ, Donaldson MDC, Ben-Shlomo Y (2010) SITAR–a useful instrument for growth curve analysis. Int J Epidemiol dyq115.
  26. 26. van Jaarsveld CHM, Johnson L, Llewellyn C, Wardle J (2010) Gemini: A UK twin birth cohort with a focus on early childhood weight trajectories, appetite and the family environment. Twin Research and Human Genetics 13: 72–78.
  27. 27. Price TS, Freeman B, Craig I, Petrill SA, Ebersole L, et al. (2000) Infant zygosity can be assigned by parental report questionnaire data. Twin Res 3: 129–133.
  28. 28. Beath KJ (2007) Infant growth modelling using a shape invariant model with random effects. Statistics in Medicine 26: 2547–2564.
  29. 29. Pinheiro J, Bates D, DebRoy S, Sarkar D, R Core Team (2009) nlme: linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. R package version 3.1-92 ed.
  30. 30. R Development Core Team. (2009) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
  31. 31. Cole TJ (2000) Sympercents: symmetric percentage differences on the 100 loge scale simplify the presentation of log transformed data. Statistics in Medicine 19: 3109–3125.
  32. 32. Freeman JV, Cole TJ, Chinn S, Jones PR, White EM, et al. (1995) Cross sectional stature and weight reference curves for the UK, 1990. Archives of Disease in Childhood 73: 17–24.
  33. 33. Cole TJSoftware for LMS method: LMSGrowth PC. Available: http://homepage.mac.com/tjcole/FileSharing1.html. Accessed 2011 May 9 2009.
  34. 34. Loos RJ, Derom C, Derom R, Vlietinck R (2005) Determinants of birthweight and intrauterine growth in liveborn twins. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 19: Suppl 115–22.
  35. 35. van Baal CG, Boomsma DI (1998) Etiology of individual differences in birth weight of twins as a function of maternal smoking during pregnancy. Twin Res 1: 123–130.
  36. 36. Howe LD, Tilling K, Lawlor DA (2009) Accuracy of height and weight data from child health records. Arch Dis Child 94: 950–954.
  37. 37. Tse WY, Hindmarsh PC, Brook CGD (1989) The Infancy-Childhood-Puberty Model of Growth: Clinical Aspects. Acta Paediatrica 78: 38–43.
  38. 38. Drewett RF, Kasese-Hara M, Wright C (2003) Feeding behaviour in young children who fail to thrive. Appetite 40: 55–60.
  39. 39. Veldhuis JD, Roemmich JN, Richmond EJ, Rogol AD, Lovejoy JC, et al. (2005) Endocrine Control of Body Composition in Infancy, Childhood, and Puberty. Endocr Rev 26: 114–146.
  40. 40. Botton J, Heude B, Maccario J, Ducimetiere P, Charles MA, et al. (2008) Postnatal weight and height growth velocities at different ages between birth and 5 y and body composition in adolescent boys and girls. Am J Clin Nut 87: 1760–1768.
  41. 41. Silventoinen K, Sammalisto S, Perola M, Boomsma DI, Cornes BK, et al. (2003) Heritability of adult body height: a comparative study of twin cohorts in eight countries. Twin Res 6: 399–408.
  42. 42. Lopez-Bermejo A, Petry CJ, Diaz M, Sebastiani G, de Zegher F, et al. (2008) The association between the FTO gene and fat mass in humans develops by the postnatal age of two weeks. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 93: 1501–1505.
  43. 43. Petry CJ, Lopez-Bermejo A, Diaz M, Sebastiani G, Ong KK, et al. (2010) Association between a common variant near MC4R and change in body mass index develops by two weeks of age. Horm Res Paediatr 73: 275–280.
  44. 44. Elks CE, Loos RJF, Sharp SJ, Langenberg C, Ring SM, et al. (2010) Genetic Markers of Adult Obesity Risk Are Associated with Greater Early Infancy Weight Gain and Growth. PLoS Med 7: e1000284.
  45. 45. Silva IdS, De Stavola BL, Mann V, Kuh D, Hardy R, et al. (2002) Prenatal factors, childhood growth trajectories and age at menarche. Int J Epidemiol 31: 405–412.
  46. 46. Ong KK, Northstone K, Wells JCK, Rubin C, Ness AR, et al. (2007) Earlier Mother's Age at Menarche Predicts Rapid Infancy Growth and Childhood Obesity. PLoS Med 4: e132.
  47. 47. Wehkalampi K, Silventoinen K, Kaprio J, Dick JM, Rose RJ, et al. (2008) Genetic and environmental influences on pubertal timing assessed by height growth. Am J Hum Biol 20: 417–423.
  48. 48. Silventoinen K, Haukka J, Dunkel L, Tynelius P, Rasmussen F (2008) Genetics of Pubertal Timing and Its Associations With Relative Weight in Childhood and Adult Height: The Swedish Young Male Twins Study. Pediatrics 121: e885–e891.
  49. 49. van den Berg SM, Boomsma DI (2007) The familial clustering of age at menarche in extended twin families. Behav Genet 37: 661–667.
  50. 50. Gillman MW (2008) The first months of life: a critical period for development of obesity. Am J Clin Nutr 87: 1587–1589.
  51. 51. ALSPACRepresentativeness of the ALSPAC cohort. Available: http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/sci-com/resource/represent/. Accessed 2011 May 9 2010.
  52. 52. Plewis I (2007) Non-Response in a Birth Cohort Study: The Case of the Millennium Cohort Study. International Journal of Social Research Methodology 10: 325–334.
  53. 53. van Dommelen P, de Gunst MC, van der Vaart AW, van Buuren S, Boomsma D (2008) Growth references for height, weight and body mass index of twins aged 0 to 2.5 years. Acta Paediatr 97: 1099–1104.
  54. 54. Boomsma D, Busjahn A, Peltonen L (2002) Classical twin studies and beyond. Nat Rev Genet 3: 872–882.