Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Visual Personal Familiarity in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment

  • Luisa Jurjanz ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Luisa Jurjanz, Markus Donix

    luisa.jurjanz@uniklinikum-dresden.de (LJ); markus.donix@uniklinikum-dresden.de (MD)

    Affiliation Division of Old Age Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Carl Gustav Carus, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany

  • Markus Donix ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Luisa Jurjanz, Markus Donix

    luisa.jurjanz@uniklinikum-dresden.de (LJ); markus.donix@uniklinikum-dresden.de (MD)

    Affiliations Division of Old Age Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Carl Gustav Carus, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany, German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, Dresden, Germany

  • Eva C. Amanatidis,

    Affiliation Division of Old Age Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Carl Gustav Carus, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany

  • Shirin Meyer,

    Affiliation Division of Old Age Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Carl Gustav Carus, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany

  • Katrin Poettrich,

    Affiliation Division of Old Age Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Carl Gustav Carus, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany

  • Thomas Huebner,

    Affiliations Division of Old Age Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Carl Gustav Carus, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany, Department of Psychology, Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany

  • Damaris Baeumler,

    Affiliation Division of Old Age Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Carl Gustav Carus, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany

  • Michael N. Smolka,

    Affiliations Division of Old Age Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Carl Gustav Carus, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany, Department of Psychology, Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany

  • Vjera A. Holthoff

    Affiliations Division of Old Age Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Carl Gustav Carus, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany, German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, Dresden, Germany

Abstract

Background

Patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment are at high risk for developing Alzheimer's disease. Besides episodic memory dysfunction they show deficits in accessing contextual knowledge that further specifies a general concept or helps to identify an object or a person.

Methodology/Principal Findings

Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, we investigated the neural networks associated with the perception of personal familiar faces and places in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment and healthy control subjects. Irrespective of stimulus type, patients compared to control subjects showed lower activity in right prefrontal brain regions when perceiving personally familiar versus unfamiliar faces and places. Both groups did not show different neural activity when perceiving faces or places irrespective of familiarity.

Conclusions/Significance

Our data highlight changes in a frontal cortical network associated with knowledge-based personal familiarity among patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. These changes could contribute to deficits in social cognition and may reduce the patients' ability to transition from basic to complex situations and tasks.

Introduction

Familiarity describes the initial ‘feeling of knowing’ that immediately arises at the moment we recognize someone or something we were previously exposed to. For example, unexpectedly encountering a person we already met before, could elicit a feeling of familiarity although we may not be able to remember any specific details about this person [1]. This type of familiarity is based on repeated perception. In contrast, personal familiarity implies the availability of contextual knowledge which would individuate a stimulus [2]. In this case, recognizing an object or a person elicits multifaceted information, such as semantic knowledge (e.g., where a person lives or works, or what the person's intentions and feelings are), our emotional response towards the stimulus, or autobiographical episodes that come to our mind. The ability to not just recognize someone or something as already known, but to identify a stimulus as personally familiar based on contextual information and emotional response, is essential for our everyday functioning.

The potential influence of personal familiarity on object use and person recognition plays a substantial role in treatment and care of patients with pathological cognitive decline. For example, Giovannetti et al. [3] showed that patients with dementia performed significantly better in identifying personal objects versus unfamiliar analogs, or generating specific information and gestures for them. A familiar environment reduces wandering behavior [4], and patients may show impaired functional task performance in an unfamiliar environment, but the same skills could be preserved in a familiar environment [5]. Most importantly, the close relationship to a familiar person is associated with improved psychological wellbeing and better problem solving abilities [6], as well as slower cognitive decline [7].

In this study we investigated patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI). Although these patients are not demented, they are at high risk for developing Alzheimer's Disease (AD), with annual conversion rates of 10–12% [8]. According to the diagnostic criteria, aMCI patients present with memory complaint, which is preferably corroborated by an informant, objective memory deficits beyond what is expected for their age, relatively preserved general cognition, and intact activities of daily living [9]. Studies investigating familiarity in aMCI patients are rare and focus on experimentally learned (perceptual) familiarity using visual and auditory stimuli. This perceptual familiarity appears to be unimpaired among aMCI patients [10], [11], although this may depend on how familiarity is investigated [1]. There is no study specifically investigating personal familiarity in aMCI patients. Recent behavioral data suggest that aMCI patients may have difficulties in accessing episodic memory details [12], and in forming associations between different types of information [13]. As outlined above, both processes are essential for experiencing personal familiarity.

Studies aimed at investigating the neural correlates underlying familiarity are rare, and they usually focus on young, healthy people. In functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies, familiar stimuli have been shown to activate medial posterior brain regions across various stimulus modalities, such as faces, places or voices [14], [15], [16], [17]. This suggests the existence of a unique brain network involved in familiarity perception, which is relatively independent from the stimulus modality [14], [18]. Activity in the posterior cingulate cortex and the precuneus can be demonstrated when we perceive experimentally learned stimuli [2], [15], [19], so it does not depend on the availability of background knowledge surrounding a stimulus. Studies specifically investigating the neural networks associated with personal (knowledge-based) familiarity revealed additional activity in medial prefrontal, anterior cingulate and posterior temporal areas [2], [20]. These areas are known to be associated with social cognition [21], the representation of the mental states of others [22], and self-referential processing [23]. The activity pattern may also reflect the richness of available episodic and semantic information associated with personal familiarity, as well as social attachment and emotional response [18], [20].

Recent structural and functional imaging studies investigating MCI patients suggest complex anatomical and functional changes in brain regions that are associated with familiarity [24], [25], [26], [27]. There is, for example, an increased beta amyloid deposition in the prefrontal cortex [24], [28], indicating that neural changes are not restricted to the medial temporal lobe, where earliest AD-associated neuropathology is known to arise [29]. In a recent positron emission tomography (PET) study, Fouquet et al. [30] demonstrated significant reduction in medial prefrontal and anterior cingulate glucose metabolism among aMCI subjects later converting to AD. The posterior cingulate cortex is also one of the brain regions showing hypometabolism in MCI [31], [32]. However, using fMRI, Ries et al. [33] demonstrated that healthy control subjects and aMCI patients showed similar posterior cingulate activity when they perceived self-associated information rather than experimentally learned information. This suggests that although metabolic changes in the posterior cingulate cortex may occur early in the course of pathologic cognitive decline, this does not necessarily reflect functional impairment in a specific situation.

To reveal the neural networks associated with personal familiarity, in this study, photographs of personally familiar faces (spouse or children) and places (from the participants' own homes), as well as unfamiliar faces and places, were presented to aMCI patients and healthy elderly control participants during fMRI scanning. We predicted that in line with the ability to recognize the visual stimulus as familiar, both participant groups would engage the posterior cingulate/precuneus region irrespective of stimulus type (face/place), when perceiving familiar versus novel context. We further hypothesized that associated with impairment in accessing rich contextual details for the familiar stimulus, aMCI patients would exhibit reduced activity in prefrontal cortical areas.

Methods

Subjects

All aMCI subjects were recruited through the University's Memory Clinic. Control participants responded to public advertisements. The experiments were done in accord with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975. The Ethics Committee of Dresden University's Medical Faculty ‘Carl Gustav Carus’, Dresden, Germany approved the study and written informed consent was obtained. Twelve aMCI subjects meeting Petersen et al. [9] criteria participated in the study. All aMCI subjects had subjective memory complaints, were not demented and reported normal activities of daily living. The aMCI patients showed memory impairment, which was defined as a performance of one standard deviation below age-adjusted normative data in at least one of the tests of verbal or non-verbal long-term memory (CVLT [34], WMS-R [35]) at the single subject level. Performance in other cognitive domains was within the age-adjusted normal range (immediate recall or working memory: WMS-R [35]; language: AAT, COWAT [36], [37], processing speed and attention: Trailmaking Test A and B [38]. Diagnoses were established by a clinician after clinical patient evaluation and the neuropsychological examination shown in Table 1. Standard laboratory testing and structural brain scans complemented the diagnostic procedures to rule out conditions that would have explained the cognitive impairment otherwise. All aMCI subjects were classified as single-domain type [9].

Twelve cognitively healthy subjects participated as control subjects. These subjects performed within the normal range in all neuropsychological tests. The healthy subjects also served as participants in a previous study investigating familiarity effects in normal aging [39]. In both groups, only subjects free of white matter lesions or focal white matter lesions only (ARWMC-scale [40], score<2 points) and free of focal lesions in grey matter were included. All subjects were right-handed. Exclusion criteria were education less than eight years, history of alcohol or substance abuse, head trauma, psychiatric or neurological disorder preceding MCI onset, or major systemic disease affecting brain function. All study participants were free of any medication aimed at stabilizing or enhancing cognitive functioning.

Image preparation

For familiar faces, we obtained photographs of each participant's close relatives (spouse or children) with a digital camera. Each relative was photographed from five different angles (left side; 45° left, frontal, 45° right, right side). Images were digitally manipulated to ensure similar head size, luminance, and background. Pictures of unfamiliar faces were obtained from family members of the clinical staff. Familiar and unfamiliar face stimuli were matched for gender and approximate age. Images of familiar places were taken of the participants' homes. We obtained photographs of rooms rather than of single furniture. Pictures of unfamiliar places were obtained from the homes of clinical staff members and their relatives.

Experimental design

In order to investigate the neural activity associated with different stimulus modality and personal familiarity, we used a blocked factorial design, presenting images of personally familiar faces and places, and unfamiliar faces and places during an fMRI experiment. We utilized the same experimental procedure as in our previous study investigating familiarity effects in healthy aging [39]. Briefly, five individual stimuli of one of the four conditions (familiar face = FF, unfamiliar face = UF, familiar place = FP, unfamiliar place = UP) were blocked together (stimulus onset-time 5s). Each block's image showed the same stimulus but photographed from different angles to avoid habituation effects. Images were presented in counterbalanced order within and between subjects for both familiarity and stimulus modality. To ensure alertness, and to test whether participants would correctly recognize familiar and unfamiliar stimuli each block contained a question stimulus in response of which the subjects were asked to press the correct button (“if the stimulus presented was familiar press the button in your left hand/if unfamiliar press the button in your right hand”). Experimental conditions were separated by intervals lasting 9s, during which the participants focused at a fixation cross. We performed a total of three experimental runs, each consisting of 8 stimulus blocks. Given this design, each condition was presented six times (twice per run) in the experiment. Across these six presentations, stimulus images were not repeated; therefore the participant did not see the same image twice throughout the experiment. We used a 3T MRI scanner (Trio; Siemens AG, Erlangen, Germany). fMRI images were acquired with an EPI pulse sequence using BOLD contrast: TR = 1.95 s, TE = 25 ms, α = 80°, 34 transversal slices acquired in descending order, orientated axially parallel to the ac-pc line, thickness 3 mm (1 mm gap), FOV = 220 mm, voxel size 3.44×3.44×4 mm. We collected 547 volumes for each subject. Stimuli were presented using bi-screen goggles, placed next to the subject's eyes below the head coil (VisuaStim Digital, Resonance Technology Inc., Northridge, CA, USA). Task presentation and behavioral response recording was performed with Presentation® software (Version 9.9, Neurobehavioral Systems Inc., Albany, CA, USA). High-resolution anatomic images were also acquired using a T1-weighted 3-D magnetization-prepared, rapid acquisition gradient echo (MPRAGE) pulse sequence: TR = 1.9 s, TE = 2.26 ms, FOV = 256 mm, 176 slices, voxel size 1×1×1 mm3.

Image processing and statistical analysis

Image processing and statistical calculations were performed using MATLAB (The Mathworks Inc., Natick MA, USA) and statistical parametric mapping software (SPM5, Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, UK). The first five EPI images were discarded to allow the MRI signal to reach a steady state. To correct for head movement we spatially realigned individual data to the first volume. We used a standard EPI template (MNI brain) for normalization. After resampling to achieve 3×3×3 mm isotropic voxels we smoothed the functional data using an isotropic Gaussian kernel of 10 mm FWHM. At the single subject level, we modelled all four conditions of the paradigm in the context of a general linear model (GLM). We also modelled the question stimulus, the subjects' response (button presses) and feedback separately from the rest condition (focusing on a fixation cross). We used a flexible factorial modelling procedure for second level analyses in a 2*2*2 factorial design, investigating the factors stimulus type (face/place), familiarity (familiar/unfamiliar), and group (control/aMCI). After examining the factors' main effects, we investigated all two-way interactions (group*familiarity, group*stimulus type and familiarity*stimulus type). In case of significant interactions we additionally calculated the respective simple main effects (e.g. effect of familiarity in both groups). Although our groups did not differ in mean age or education status, we additionally investigated whether modelling age and education as covariates would change our findings. Voxels in MNI-space were considered statistically significant at a threshold of p<0.05 (corrected at cluster level) using a height threshold of p<0.001 uncorrected, corresponding to T = 3.28 and a cluster size of at least 30 activated voxels. Sociodemographic data and neuropsychological scores were compared using two-tailed t-tests.

Results

fMRI

Given our hypotheses, we were specifically interested in examining a possible interaction between the factors group and familiarity. We detected significant main effects for familiarity but not for group (Table 2), as well as a significant interaction between both factors. Group comparison (interaction group*familiarity) revealed lower right prefrontal cortical activity among aMCI subjects when compared to control participants for familiar versus unfamiliar stimuli (Contrast (FF+FP)-(UF+UP), Table 3, Figure 1). Group comparison for the reverse effect (controls<aMCI) did not yield a significant finding. To further explore the different patterns of brain activation between controls and aMCI patients we calculated the effect of familiarity within both groups (simple main effects familiarity). Among control subjects, familiar compared to unfamiliar stimuli, irrespective of stimulus type (contrast (FF+FP)-(UF+UP)), elicited substantially more brain activity, primarily in frontal, anterior and posterior cingulate, as well as temporal areas. aMCI subjects showed a bilateral activation in the precuneus for this contrast, extending into the right posterior cingulate cortex (Table 3, Figure 2). In both groups, there was no brain region showing less activity associated with familiar compared to unfamiliar stimuli.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Between-group comparison: personal familiarity irrespective of stimulus type.

The figure shows brain areas with relative decrease in neural activity among aMCI patients when compared to control subjects, associated with familiar>unfamiliar stimulus content irrespective of stimulus type. The two local maxima (indicated by crosshair positions) are superimposed on a sagittal single subject brain section provided by SPM5. Both maxima are part of the same cluster (for details see Table 3). The histograms display percentage BOLD signal change for the local maximum as a function of the experimental conditions (mean and 90% confidence interval). CF = controls familiar, CU = controls unfamiliar, MF = aMCI familiar, MU = aMCI unfamiliar.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0020030.g001

thumbnail
Figure 2. Within-group effect of personal familiarity irrespective of stimulus type.

The figure shows brain areas with relative increase in neural activity for both subject groups when perceiving familiar>unfamiliar stimulus content irrespective of stimulus type. The local maxima are superimposed on a rendered standard single subject brain provided by SPM5. See Table 3 for exact coordinates. R = right, L = left, A = anterior, P = posterior.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0020030.g002

thumbnail
Table 3. Relative increases in brain activity associated with personal familiarity.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0020030.t003

The factor stimulus type was of no primary interest for our main hypothesis. However, investigating main effects (Table 2) for stimulus type (irrespective of familiarity, contrast (FF+UF)−(FP+UP)), we found that subjects showed more brain activity in bilateral temporal areas when perceiving faces compared to places. Contrariwise presentation of places elicited more brain activity in occipital brain regions. Investigating the interaction terms stimulus type*group and stimulus type*familiarity did not reveal brain regions showing significantly greater or reduced neural activity associated with one of the conditions. Modelling age and education as covariates did not change the pattern of our significant group findings.

Post-scanning debriefing

In a post-scanning debriefing, individual stimuli used during the scan were again presented on a computer screen. Both participant groups did not significantly differ in their ability to correctly categorize familiar and unfamiliar stimuli.

Discussion

In this study we demonstrated that aMCI patients compared to healthy elderly subjects showed lower activity in right prefrontal brain regions when perceiving personally familiar faces and places. Within-group comparison revealed that control participants activated a large neural network including frontal, posterior cingulate and temporal cortices for personally familiar versus unfamiliar stimuli, whereas aMCI patients showed activity in the bilateral precuneus and right posterior cingulate cortex only. These differences in neural activity occurred irrespective of visual stimulus modality (face/place), and despite the fact that both groups did not show neural activity differences when perceiving faces or places per se, irrespective of familiarity.

Personal familiarity associated with close family members and one's own home arises from years of interaction and exposure. The recollection of specific knowledge and experiences associated with a familiar stimulus has been shown to recruit brain regions involved in social cognition and episodic memory [2], [21], [41]. For example, Cloutier at al. [2] demonstrated that the extensive information surrounding a familiar face stimulus is preferentially associated with neural activity in medial prefrontal cortex. Within this region, the anterior cingulate and paracingulate cortices, and the anterior frontal poles play a major role in episodic memory retrieval [41], self-reflection [23], and making inferences about others' thoughts [42].

Throughout the literature there is a main focus on the medial temporal lobe with respect to patients suffering from pathologic cognitive decline. Our data contribute to the emerging evidence that changes in frontal cortical functioning are also involved relatively early in the course of cognitive impairment. Due to its late myelination in brain development, the frontal cortex is susceptible to myelin damage [43]. Elevated beta amyloid levels in the prefrontal cortex of aMCI patients [24] could contribute to myelin toxicity [44]. AD patients show functional disconnection, particularly between medial temporal and frontal cortical areas [45], [46]. This model of disease pathophysiology is supported by brain metabolic changes, such as reduced frontal cortical glucose metabolism among aMCI subjects later converting to AD [30], [47]. Our fMRI results indicate reduced prefrontal cortical activity associated with a personally familiar stimulus in aMCI patients. Previous data show that aMCI patients are impaired in accessing specific memory details and forming associations between different types of information [12], [13]. The lower frontal brain activity may reflect impairment in connecting rich background information with a familiar stimulus. Although this will require future investigations, our data are in line with the notion that cognitive impairment in aMCI patients is more complex than episodic memory retrieval deficit detected by standard neuropsychological testing. Reduced frontal cortical activity could reflect subtle changes in working-memory capacity [13] and executive functioning [48]. However, it needs to be mentioned that there are conflicting data whether cognitively impaired patients at risk for AD would show increased [49], [50] or decreased [51], [52] frontal cortical activity during memory tasks. These differences could reflect different stages of cognitive impairment [53], or they could be task-associated [54].

In contrast to prefrontal cortical activity, we did not detect a group difference in the posterior cingulate cortex when the subjects perceived personally familiar stimuli. The posterior cingulate/precuneus region is closely associated with perceptual familiarity, irrespective of whether or not there is any knowledge available that would further individuate the perceptually familiar stimulus [2], [15], [19]. A number of studies show preserved perceptual familiarity recognition in aMCI [10], [11], [55], contrary to the patients' declining memory recollection abilities. The brain network involved in familiarity recognition seems to be relatively independent from the stimulus modality [15], [16], [17], [18]. It is therefore interesting that a personally familiar environment is particularly helpful in dementia care and therapy [3], [5], [7], since a demented person may not have access to semantic facts or episodic memories surrounding a familiar face or object. Besides the availability of such contextual knowledge, this could also be due to the emotional salience of a stimulus, which is known to influence familiarity-associated neural activity in the posterior cingulate cortex [20], [56]. Other investigations demonstrated that the degree of self-relevance of a familiar stimulus may also modulate the activity in the posterior cingulate/precuneus region [33], [57].

With respect to activation laterality, it should be highlighted that our group difference associated with personal familiarity was detected in the right prefrontal cortex. Whereas left more than right prefrontal regions are involved in episodic memory encoding, the opposite pattern has been described for episodic memory retrieval [58]. Thus, our data could suggest an early impairment in accessing semantic and episodic information associated with a familiar stimulus among aMCI patients, which would be in line with the existing literature [12], [59]. However, this has to be interpreted with caution. Aging itself could influence hemispheric lateralization processes [60] and frontal cortical involvement in general [61]. We previously demonstrated that frontal cortical activity associated with perceiving a personally familiar face or place, did not change with age [39]. In this study we would therefore not expect familiarity-related hemispheric lateralization effects to be aging-associated. It remains a possibility that right prefrontal cortices may be preferentially involved in encoding pictorial rather than verbal information [62], which could have contributed to a lateralization effect.

It is a limitation of this study that we were not able to directly investigate the participants' performance of retrieving detail-rich contextual information on the behavioral level. However, we previously showed that aMCI patients retrieve autobiographical events with fewer details when compared to healthy subjects [12]. Finally our data may be susceptible to false negative findings due to the small sample size. We reanalyzed our data using a height threshold of p<0.05, uncorrected. This did not change the pattern of brain regions for which we found a significant group difference.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: VAH LJ KP MD. Performed the experiments: LJ MD SM DB ECA. Analyzed the data: LJ MD TH MNS. Wrote the paper: LJ MD VAH. Supervision: MNS VAH.

References

  1. 1. Ally BA, Gold CA, Budson AE (2009) An evaluation of recollection and familiarity in Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment using receiver operating characteristics. Brain Cogn 69: 504–513.
  2. 2. Cloutier J, Kelley WM, Heatherton TF (2010) The influence of perceptual and knowledge-based familiarity on the neural substrates of face perception. Soc Neurosci:. pp. 1–13.
  3. 3. Giovannetti T, Sestito N, Libon DJ, Schmidt KS, Gallo JL, et al. (2006) The influence of personal familiarity on object naming, knowledge, and use in dementia. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 21: 607–614.
  4. 4. Hong GR, Song JA (2009) Relationship between familiar environment and wandering behaviour among Korean elders with dementia. J Clin Nurs 18: 1365–1373.
  5. 5. Liu L, Gauthier L, Gauthier S (1991) Spatial disorientation in persons with early senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. Am J Occup Ther 45: 67–74.
  6. 6. Burgener S, Twigg P (2002) Relationships among caregiver factors and quality of life in care recipients with irreversible dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 16: 88–102.
  7. 7. Norton MC, Piercy KW, Rabins PV, Green RC, Breitner JC, et al. (2009) Caregiver-recipient closeness and symptom progression in Alzheimer disease. The Cache County Dementia Progression Study. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 64: 560–568.
  8. 8. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, et al. (1999) Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol 56: 303–308.
  9. 9. Petersen R (2004) Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med 256: 183–194.
  10. 10. Serra L, Bozzali M, Cercignani M, Perri R, Fadda L, et al. (2010) Recollection and familiarity in amnesic mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology 24: 316–326.
  11. 11. Anderson ND, Ebert PL, Jennings JM, Grady CL, Cabeza R, et al. (2008) Recollection- and familiarity-based memory in healthy aging and amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology 22: 177–187.
  12. 12. Donix M, Brons C, Jurjanz L, Poettrich K, Winiecki P, et al. (2010) Overgenerality of autobiographical memory in people with amnestic mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer's disease. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 25: 22–27.
  13. 13. Kessels RP, Meulenbroek O, Fernandez G, Olde Rikkert MG (2010) Spatial Working Memory in Aging and Mild Cognitive Impairment: Effects of Task Load and Contextual Cueing. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn:. pp. 1–19.
  14. 14. Sugiura M, Shah NJ, Zilles K, Fink GR (2005) Cortical representation of personally familiar objects and places: Functional organization of the human posterior cingulate cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 17: 183–198.
  15. 15. Gobbini MI, Haxby JV (2006) Neural response to the visual familiarity of faces. Brain Res Bull 71: 76–82.
  16. 16. Shah NJ, Marshall JC, Zafiris O, Schwab A, Zilles K, et al. (2001) The neural correlates of person familiarity. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study with clinical implications. Brain 124: 804–815.
  17. 17. Epstein RA, Higgins JS, Jablonski K, Feiler AM (2007) Visual scene processing in familiar and unfamiliar environments. J Neurophysiol 97: 3670–3683.
  18. 18. Sugiura M, Sassa Y, Watanabe J, Akitsuki Y, Maeda Y, et al. (2009) Anatomical segregation of representations of personally familiar and famous people in the temporal and parietal cortices. J Cogn Neurosci 21: 1855–1868.
  19. 19. Kosaka H, Omori M, Iidaka T, Murata T, Shimoyama T, et al. (2003) Neural substrates participating in acquisition of facial familiarity: an fMRI study. NeuroImage 20: 1734–1742.
  20. 20. Gobbini MI, Leibenluft E, Santiago N, Haxby JV (2004) Social and emotional attachment in the neural representation of faces. NeuroImage 22: 1628–1635.
  21. 21. Adolphs R (2009) The social brain: neural basis of social knowledge. Annu Rev Psychol 60: 693–716.
  22. 22. Gallagher HL, Frith CD (2003) Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’. Trends Cogn Sci 7: 77–83.
  23. 23. Fossati P, Hevenor SJ, Graham SJ, Grady C, Keightley ML, et al. (2003) In search of the emotional self: an fMRI study using positive and negative emotional words. Am J Psychiatry 160: 1938–1945.
  24. 24. Devanand DP, Mikhno A, Pelton GH, Cuasay K, Pradhaban G, et al. (2010) Pittsburgh compound B (11C-PIB) and fluorodeoxyglucose (18 F-FDG) PET in patients with Alzheimer disease, mild cognitive impairment, and healthy controls. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 23: 185–198.
  25. 25. Poettrich K, Weiss PH, Werner A, Lux S, Donix M, et al. (2009) Altered neural network supporting declarative long-term memory in mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol Aging 30: 284–298.
  26. 26. Wang L, Goldstein FC, Veledar E, Levey AI, Lah JJ, et al. (2009) Alterations in cortical thickness and white matter integrity in mild cognitive impairment measured by whole-brain cortical thickness mapping and diffusion tensor imaging. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 30: 893–899.
  27. 27. Westman E, Simmons A, Zhang Y, Muehlboeck JS, Tunnard C, et al. (2010) Multivariate analysis of MRI data for Alzheimer's disease, mild cognitive impairment and healthy controls. NeuroImage.
  28. 28. Rombouts SA, Barkhof F, Goekoop R, Stam CJ, Scheltens P (2005) Altered resting state networks in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer's disease: an fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 26: 231–239.
  29. 29. Braak H, Braak E (1991) Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 82: 239–259.
  30. 30. Fouquet M, Desgranges B, Landeau B, Duchesnay E, Mezenge F, et al. (2009) Longitudinal brain metabolic changes from amnestic mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer's disease. Brain 132: 2058–2067.
  31. 31. Nestor PJ, Fryer TD, Ikeda M, Hodges JR (2003) Retrosplenial cortex (BA 29/30) hypometabolism in mild cognitive impairment (prodromal Alzheimer's disease). Eur J Neurosci 18: 2663–2667.
  32. 32. Mosconi L, Perani D, Sorbi S, Herholz K, Nacmias B, et al. (2004) MCI conversion to dementia and the APOE genotype: a prediction study with FDG-PET. Neurology 63: 2332–2340.
  33. 33. Ries ML, Schmitz TW, Kawahara TN, Torgerson BM, Trivedi MA, et al. (2006) Task-dependent posterior cingulate activation in mild cognitive impairment. NeuroImage 29: 485–492.
  34. 34. Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA (1987) California Verbal Learning Test, Adult Version. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.
  35. 35. Härting C (2000) WMS-R. Wechsler Gedächtnistest- revidierte Fassung. Deutsche Adaptation der revidierten Fassung der Wechsler Memory Scale. Bern: Verlag Hans Huber.
  36. 36. Huber W, Poeck K, Willmes K (1984) The Aachen Aphasia Test. Adv Neurol 42: 291–303.
  37. 37. Loonstra AS, Tarlow AR, Sellers AH (2001) COWAT metanorms across age, education, and gender. Appl Neuropsychol 8: 161–166.
  38. 38. Gordon NG (1972) The Trail Making Test in neuropsychological diagnosis. J Clin Psychol 28: 167–169.
  39. 39. Donix M, Petrowski K, Jurjanz L, Huebner T, Herold U, et al. (2010) Age and the neural network of personal familiarity. PLoS One 5: e15790.
  40. 40. Wahlund LO, Barkhof F, Fazekas F, Bronge L, Augustin M, et al. (2001) A new rating scale for age-related white matter changes applicable to MRI and CT. Stroke 32: 1318–1322.
  41. 41. Cabeza R, Nyberg L (2000) Imaging cognition II: An empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. J Cogn Neurosci 12: 1–47.
  42. 42. Amodio DM, Frith CD (2006) Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 7: 268–277.
  43. 43. Choi SJ, Lim KO, Monteiro I, Reisberg B (2005) Diffusion tensor imaging of frontal white matter microstructure in early Alzheimer's disease: a preliminary study. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 18: 12–19.
  44. 44. Bartzokis G (2004) Age-related myelin breakdown: a developmental model of cognitive decline and Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging 25: 5–18; author reply 49-62.
  45. 45. Villain N, Fouquet M, Baron JC, Mezenge F, Landeau B, et al. (2010) Sequential relationships between grey matter and white matter atrophy and brain metabolic abnormalities in early Alzheimer's disease. Brain 133: 3301–3314.
  46. 46. Allen G, Barnard H, McColl R, Hester AL, Fields JA, et al. (2007) Reduced hippocampal functional connectivity in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 64: 1482–1487.
  47. 47. Drzezga A, Lautenschlager N, Siebner H, Riemenschneider M, Willoch F, et al. (2003) Cerebral metabolic changes accompanying conversion of mild cognitive impairment into Alzheimer's disease: a PET follow-up study. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging 30: 1104–1113.
  48. 48. Werner P, Rabinowitz S, Klinger E, Korczyn AD, Josman N (2009) Use of the virtual action planning supermarket for the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment: a preliminary study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 27: 301–309.
  49. 49. Bookheimer SY, Strojwas MH, Cohen MS, Saunders AM, Pericak-Vance MA, et al. (2000) Patterns of brain activation in people at risk for Alzheimer's disease. N Engl J Med 343: 450–456.
  50. 50. Woodard JL, Seidenberg M, Nielson KA, Antuono P, Guidotti L, et al. (2009) Semantic memory activation in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Brain 132: 2068–2078.
  51. 51. Petrella JR, Krishnan S, Slavin MJ, Tran TT, Murty L, et al. (2006) Mild cognitive impairment: evaluation with 4-T functional MR imaging. Radiology 240: 177–186.
  52. 52. Mandzia JL, McAndrews MP, Grady CL, Graham SJ, Black SE (2009) Neural correlates of incidental memory in mild cognitive impairment: an fMRI study. Neurobiol Aging 30: 717–730.
  53. 53. Celone KA, Calhoun VD, Dickerson BC, Atri A, Chua EF, et al. (2006) Alterations in memory networks in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease: an independent component analysis. J Neurosci 26: 10222–10231.
  54. 54. Trivedi MA, Murphy CM, Goetz C, Shah RC, Gabrieli JD, et al. (2008) fMRI activation changes during successful episodic memory encoding and recognition in amnestic mild cognitive impairment relative to cognitively healthy older adults. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 26: 123–137.
  55. 55. Westerberg CE, Paller KA, Weintraub S, Mesulam MM, Holdstock JS, et al. (2006) When memory does not fail: familiarity-based recognition in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology 20: 193–205.
  56. 56. Maddock RJ (1999) The retrosplenial cortex and emotion: new insights from functional neuroimaging of the human brain. Trends Neurosci 22: 310–316.
  57. 57. Cavanna AE, Trimble MR (2006) The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 129: 564–583.
  58. 58. Habib R, Nyberg L, Tulving E (2003) Hemispheric asymmetries of memory: the HERA model revisited. Trends Cogn Sci 7: 241–245.
  59. 59. Estevez-Gonzalez A, Garcia-Sanchez C, Boltes A, Otermin P, Pascual-Sedano B, et al. (2004) Semantic knowledge of famous people in mild cognitive impairment and progression to Alzheimer's disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 17: 188–195.
  60. 60. Cabeza R (2002) Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model. Psychol Aging 17: 85–100.
  61. 61. Davis S, Dennis N, Daselaar S, Fleck M, Cabeza R (2007) Qué PASA? The Posterior-Anterior Shift in Aging. Cereb Cortex.
  62. 62. Kelley WM, Miezin FM, McDermott KB, Buckner RL, Raichle ME, et al. (1998) Hemispheric specialization in human dorsal frontal cortex and medial temporal lobe for verbal and nonverbal memory encoding. Neuron 20: 927–936.