Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

The hector G-Protein Coupled Receptor Is Required in a Subset of fruitless Neurons for Male Courtship Behavior

  • Yuanli Li,

    Affiliation Department of Biology and Biochemistry, University of Houston, Houston, Texas, United States of America

  • Valbona Hoxha,

    Affiliation Department of Biology and Biochemistry, University of Houston, Houston, Texas, United States of America

  • Chamala Lama,

    Affiliation Department of Biology and Biochemistry, University of Houston, Houston, Texas, United States of America

  • Bich Hien Dinh,

    Affiliation Department of Biology and Biochemistry, University of Houston, Houston, Texas, United States of America

  • Christina N. Vo,

    Affiliation Department of Biology and Biochemistry, University of Houston, Houston, Texas, United States of America

  • Brigitte Dauwalder

    bdauwalder@uh.edu

    Affiliation Department of Biology and Biochemistry, University of Houston, Houston, Texas, United States of America

Abstract

Male courtship behavior in Drosophila melanogaster is controlled by two main regulators, fruitless (fru) and doublesex (dsx). Their sex-specific expression in brain neurons has been characterized in detail, but little is known about the downstream targets of the sex-specific FRU and DSX proteins and how they specify the function of these neurons. While sexual dimorphism in the number and connections of fru and dsx expressing neurons has been observed, a majority of the neurons that express the two regulators are present in both sexes. This poses the question which molecules define the sex-specific function of these neurons. Signaling molecules are likely to play a significant role. We have identified a predicted G-protein coupled receptor (GPCR), CG4395, that is required for male courtship behavior. The courtship defect in the mutants can be rescued by expression of the wildtype protein in fru neurons of adult males. The GPCR is expressed in a subset of fru-positive antennal glomeruli that have previously been shown to be essential for male courtship. Expression of 4395-RNAi in GH146 projection neurons lowers courtship. This suggests that signaling through the CG4395 GPCR in this subset of fru neurons is critical for male courtship behavior.

Introduction

One critical aspect of understanding how complex behaviors are regulated is to understand the circuits and molecules that are required to generate and display the behavior. Sexual behavior offers an excellent model for the study of complex behaviors because it is both hard-wired and responsive to environmental cues. Courtship behavior in male fruit flies consists of well-defined courtship steps that can easily be studied to examine the effect of mutants.

Sex-specific behaviors in Drosophila are under the control of the same master upstream regulators that also regulate somatic sex [1], [2], [3], [4], [5]. They control the sex specific generation of two key male regulators, fruitless (fruM) and doublesex (dsxM). The male-specific fruM and dsxM proteins are both critically required for normal male courtship behavior [6], [7], [8], [9]. Detailed insight has been gained into the expression pattern of these genes and their regulation by the sex-determination mechanisms, and details are starting to emerge on the sex-specific circuits these neurons may form. While an increasing number of differences are being discovered between males and females in the number of fru and dsx neurons and the connections they make, it is also evident that many of the neurons that express fruM and dsxM and form the male circuits are not absent in females [6], [7], [8], [9], [10], [11], [12], [13], [14], [15], [16], [17], [18], [19], [20]. Thus, it is likely that it is the expression of sex specific molecules in these neurons, or the sex-specific nature of their input and/or output that is critical for the observed sex specific behavior. Exploring which genes are expressed in these neurons and how this determines their function and their connections is therefore a critical next step in trying to understand how mating behavior is regulated. Both Fru and Dsx are transcription factors that in microarray studies have been shown to control a large number of genes [21], [22]. However, little is known to date about the significance of these genes, in which cells they might be expressed and what biological role they might play. Given the crucial importance of communication among neurons in circuits, we hypothesized that signaling molecules play an important role in the function of male specific circuits. We describe here the identification of a novel putative G-protein coupled receptor (GPCR), encoded by the gene CG4395, that is required for male courtship. Mutants for the gene, which we named hector (hec), have courtship defects that can be rescued by expression of the wildtype protein in fru neurons. Co-localization studies of fru and hec suggest that the 4395 GPCR is required in a subset of glomeruli in the antennal lobes.

Results

CG4395 mutants have reduced male courtship

To examine whether CG4395 has a role in male courtship behavior, we tested two different pBac insertions, PBac(f06077) and PBac(f04274). CG4395 is located at 11D4. The insertions are located in the last intron of the gene, 4 bp apart, but in different orientations. In order to control for genetic background, we outcrossed the mutants to a cantonized w1118 strain for ten generations. Since the gene is located on the X chromosome, we crossed the mutants to Canton-S wildtype in reciprocal crosses and tested male progeny with or without the mutant chromosome. We observed a significant drop in the courtship index (CI) for both mutants (Figure 1A). The CI expresses the fraction of time a male spends performing any of the steps of the courtship ritual during the observation period. Although courtship of the mutant males is quantitatively reduced, it is not absent. To assess whether particular steps of courtship are affected in the mutants, we quantified latency (time to first orientation towards the female), fraction of wing extension during the overall courtship time, and the number of attempted copulations (Table 1). Mutant males are capable of all steps of courtship but perform them less frequently and seem to lack “motivation” to court. Control short-term activity assays did not show a difference among the genotypes tested (Figure 1B), indicating that the observed courtship phenotype was not caused by general sluggishness or sickness. To further assess the mutant phenotype, we performed additional courtship assays in red light (Figure 1C–E). Under these conditions, pheromonal input plays a major role in mate recognition. Without the visual input, courtship indices are generally lower, also in the control flies. The mutants show a similarly reduced defect in comparison to control males as in white light (Figure 1C). The same is true when they are paired with a matured virgin female (Figure 1D) (young virgin females were used in the other courtship assays). To further examine the effect of the mutation on male courtship behavior, we tested male-male courtship. Mutant males did not exhibit increased male-male courtship when compared to wildtype males and desat1573 mutant males whose male-male courtship phenotype has previously been described [23] (Figure 1E). Thus, mutant males appear to be able to distinguish between males and females.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Mutations in the CG4395 GPCR reduce male courtship.

Graphs show the courtship index CI (fraction of time males spend courting during the observation period) ± SEM of the indicated genotypes, or the performance of males in a control activity assay (# of line crossings ± SEM). N = 10. Data were analyzed by ANOVA followed by Tukey Kramer multiple comparisons. * p<0.001). CG4395 mutants PBac(f06077) and PBac(f04274) have a reduced CI in comparison to the control (A), but no defects in activity (B). A similar reduction in courtship is also observed when the assays are performed in red light with either a young or aged virgin female (C, D). Mutant males do not court other males, in contrast to desat11537 males (E). Expression of 4395-RNAi using the promoter construct 4395-Gal4 reduces the CI to the levels observed in the PBac mutants in a wildtype as well as a 4395 mutant background (F); activity in the mutants is not different from the control (G). The PBac(f06077) mutant phenotpye can be rescued by expressing wildtype 4395 using the 4395-Gal4 driver and two independent UAS-4395 responder lines (lines 1 and 2) (H). (I) Conditional rescue in adult males. UAS-4395 expression is restricted by the presence of tubP-Gal80ts at 18°C (induction −). Placement of 5 day old males at 32°C for 40 hours (induction +) releases the inhibition and leads to expression of 4395. Adult induction of 4395 leads to the rescue of the mutant phenotype.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0028269.g001

To ascertain that the mutant phenotype we observed is due to the disruption of the 4395 gene, we performed rescue experiments by expressing the wildtype gene in the mutants using the Gal4/UAS system. We created 4395-Gal4 transgenic flies by placing 4.7 kb of the 4395 promoter region upstream of Gal4 coding sequences. This fragment contains 1.2 kb upstream of exon 1, the non-coding exon 1, intron 1 and exon 1 right up to the translation start. UAS-4395 transgenic lines were produced by insertion of the 4395 ORF downstream of UAS. PBac(f06077) mutant males carrying 4395-Gal4 and UAS-4395 were tested in courtship. The two independent UAS-4395 lines tested were capable of completely rescuing the mutant phenotype (p<0.001) (Figure 1H). When 4395-Gal4 was used to express a UAS-4395-RNAi transgene we observed a reduction in courtship that was indistinguishable from the PBac(f06077) mutants, and which could not be lowered further when 4395-RNAi was expressed in a PBac(f06077) mutant background (p<0.001). Locomotion activity was not affected in these mutants either (Figure 1 F, G). We conclude from these experiments that the CG4395 GPCR, which we named “hector (hec)”, is required for male courtship behavior. We next asked whether hector is required during development or whether it has a physiological role in adult courting males. We used the Gal80ts conditional expression system [24] to examine whether expression of hector only in adult mature males was capable of rescuing the mutant phenotype. At 18°C, Gal80ts represses 4395-Gal4. Thus, UAS-4395 is not expressed. Upon transfer of the flies to 32°C, Gal80ts is inactivated, 4395-Gal4 is active and leads to the expression of the UAS-4395 rescue transgene. Virgin males grown at 18°C were collected and aged individually at 18°C for seven days. They were then placed at 32°C for two days, allowed to adjust to room temperature for one hour and tested for courtship. Control animals were continuously kept at 18°C, allowed to adjust to room temperature for one hour, and tested in parallel to induced flies. We observed complete rescue in flies in which 4395 was induced in adult males (p<0.001), indicating that the gene plays a physiological, rather than a developmental, role in normal courtship (Figure 1 I). This rescue was dose-dependent, as induction for one day only led to partial rescue (data not shown).

CG4395 is expressed in several brain areas

To establish where hector is expressed, we crossed 4395-Gal4 flies to UAS-lacZnls and UAS-GFPnls reporter lines in which the reporters are located to the nucleus, and examined expression by immunohistochemistry. We observed fairly widespread expression in adult male brains. Prominent expression was observed in what appeared to be mushroom body (mb) cell bodies (Figure 2A). To further characterize this expression, we performed colocalization of 4395-Gal4 driven β-Gal with Dco, the catalytic subunit of PKA which is preferentially expressed in mushroom bodies [25]. As previously reported, Dco is present in mb cell bodies and processes. We observed co-staining in a subset of Kenyon cells. However, hector is only expressed in a subset of the Dco expressing cells, and not all 4395-Gal4 expressing cells in the dorsal brain area are mushroom body cells (Figure 2B, C).

thumbnail
Figure 2. CG4395-Gal4 expression in male brains.

(A). Z-stack of anti-β-Gal staining of isolated male brains of 4395-Gal4/UAS-lacZnls. (B,C). Brains of 4395-Gal4/UAS-lacZnls males were double-stained with anti-β-Gal (red) and anti-Dco (green). Dco is present in the cytoplasm of the cell bodies of the mushroom bodies and projections of the mushroom bodies [24]. Optical sections are shown. Anti-β-Gal staining is seen in a number of mushroom body cell bodies (arrowhead), but absent from others (arrow). A magnification of (B) is shown in (C). (D–I). Brains of 4395-Gal4/UAS-mcD8::GFP males were double-stained with anti-GFP (green) and nc82 to visualize neurons (magenta). Projections can be seen in the mushroom bodies (D, Z-stack), the central complex (E, Z-stack) and a subset of glomeruli in the antennal lobes (F, selected optical section). Staining was observed in glomeruli DA1, VA1v, VA1d, VA6, DM3 and DL3 (F). (G–I) To visualize mb projections, double staining was performed with anti-GFP and anti-Dco (G, merged), the single channels are shown in (H) and (I); Z stacks are shown. Abbreviations: (nls) nuclear localization signal; (mcD8::GFP) membrane bound GFP that visualizes neuronal extensions. (mb) mushroom bodies; (ca) calyx of the mushroom bodies; α, β γ: α, β, γ lobes of the mushroom bodies.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0028269.g002

To further visualize the mushroom body neurons that express 4395-Gal4, we crossed 4395-Gal4 to UAS-mcD8::GFP, thus expressing membrane bound GFP in hector neurons. GFP was visualized by anti-GFP antibody staining and co-localization with Dco was examined. We observed expression of 4395-Gal4 in the α, β and γ lobes of the mushroom bodies, but not the α′ and β′ lobes (Figure 2 D, G–I)). Consequently, 4395-Gal4 expressing neurons form a subset of the neurons seen in the peduncle. 4395-Gal4 neurons were further found to project to the wheel of the mushroom bodies and make additional contacts with mushroom body neurons. In addition to expression in the mushroom bodies, prominent labeling of central complex neurons was observed (Figure 2 E). Expression was observed in the ellipsoid body (mainly in the interior layers R1 and R2, but not R4), the fan-shaped body, the noduli and the lateral triangles, the dendrites of ellipsoid body neurons, as well as the protocerebral bridge. Additional expression is visible in the median bundle and a number of isolated larger cells. 4395-Gal4 projections were also observed in a subset of glomeruli in the antennal lobes. Staining was present in the glomeruli DM3, DL3, DA1, VA2a, VA1v and V6 (Figure 2 F). Labeled cell bodies close by suggest that the 4395-Gal4 neurons might be projection neurons.

hector is required in a subset of fru cells

Which of the cells expressing 4395-Gal4 might be the relevant cells for the role of hector in male courtship? While 4395-Gal4 expression in the mushroom bodies is prominent, it has previously been shown that the mb are not required for courtship behavior per se [26]. We confirmed this by expressing 4395-RNAi in the mb using the well-characterized mb-driver P247-Gal4. Like 4395-Gal4, P247 is predominantly expressed in the α, β and γ lobes [27]. As expected, these males did not show courtship defects (Figure 3C).

thumbnail
Figure 3. The sexual identity of 4395 Neurons is important for courtship.

(A, B) Feminization of 4395-Gal4 expressing cells by the expression of UAS-traF reduces male courtship (A), indicating that these cells are sexually dimorphic. In contrast, conditional feminization in adult 5-day old males does not affect male courtship (B). (C). Expression of 4395-RNAi in the mushroom bodies using the P247-Gal4 driver does not affect courtship. N = 10.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0028269.g003

The central complex is implicated in the control of locomotion. Since the hec f06077 and hecf04274 mutants are normal in an activity assay and can perform all steps of courtship, a reduction of hector in the central complex is probably not a major contributor to the courtship defects we observed.

The glomeruli of the antennal lobe in which 4395-Gal4 projections are found include the glomeruli that have been shown to contain fru expressing projection neurons (DA1, VA1v, VL2a and V6) and have been implicated in male courtship [28], [29]. They may therefore represent hector expressing cells within the antennal lobe that are important for male mating behavior.

Is the sexual identity of 4395-Gal4 expressing cells important for male courtship behavior? We used 4395-Gal4 to express traF, the key female regulator protein, to selectively feminize these cells in an otherwise normal male [30]. Males with feminized 4395-Gal4 cells had a significant decrease in their courtship index (Figure 3A). Conditional feminization only in adult flies did not affect male courtship, indicating that the relevant cells are sexually determined prior to adulthood (Figure 3B).

These data demonstrate that the 4395-Gal4 cells that are important for courtship are sexually determined. This finding prompted us to examine the genetic relationship between fruitless, a major courtship regulator, and hector. When the hector mutation was placed in a heterozygous fru mutant background, we observed a significantly lower courtship index than in the hector mutants alone (p<0.001; Figure 4A). Heterozygous fru males do not have courtship defects. This indicates that the two genes are playing a role in the same overall genetic pathway that regulates normal male courtship behavior. This could indicate that hector is expressed in fru neurons, or/and that the two kinds of neurons interact. To examine the two possibilities, we used fru-Gal4 to express 4395-RNAi, thus lowering hector in fru cells. We found that this led to a reduction of courtship to the levels observed in the hector PBac mutants, indicating the importance of signaling through the hector GPCR in fru neurons (p<0.001), Figure 4B). Again, the additional presence of the PBacf06077 hector mutation in fru-Gal4/4395-RNAi flies did not further lower courtship. The mutants showed normal activity in a short term activity assay (Figure 4C).

thumbnail
Figure 4. CG4395 is required in fru neurons for male courtship.

Graphs show the courtship index CI (fraction of time males spend courting during the observation period) ± SEM of the indicated genotypes, or the performance of males in a control activity assay (# of line crossings ± SEM). N = 10. Data were analyzed by ANOVA followed by Tukey Kramer multiple comparisons. * p<0.001). (A) A mutation in CG4395 and fru interact genetically. The CI of PBacf06077; fru4/+ males is significantly lower than the CI of PBacf06077 alone. fru4/+ males have a normal CI. (B) Expression of 4395-RNAi by fru-Gal4 reduces the CI to the level of the PBacf06077 mutants. The mutants have activity levels that are not different from the controls (C). (D,E) The PBacf06077 mutant phenotype can be rescued by expression of wildtype UAS-4395 by fru-Gal4 (D). (E) Conditional adult expression of UAS-4395 by fru-Gal4 rescues the mutant phenotype. UAS-4395 expression is restricted by the presence of tubP-Gal80ts at 18°C (induction −). Placement of 5 day old males at 32°C for 16 hours (induction +) releases the inhibition and leads to expression of 4395.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0028269.g004

To further test the hypothesis that hector is a downstream effector in fru neurons, we examined whether hector mutants could be rescued by the expression of hec protein only in fru neurons. As shown in Figure 4D, this was indeed the case. Furthermore, like in the case of the rescue with the 4395-Gal4 driver, conditional rescue in adult males using fru-Gal4 completely rescued the mutant phenotype (Figure 4E). Taken together these findings demonstrate that the hector GPCR is required in fru neurons of adult males for normal male courtship behavior.

Based on these findings, we suggest that 4395-Gal4 expressing cells that also express fru are likely to be the cells that require hector for courtship. To identify these cells, we generated flies that express a UAS-reporter under the control of 4395-Gal4 at the same time with a lexAoperator-reporter driven by fruP1LexA [31]. We chose two kinds of reporters: One set with a nuclear localization signal, and the other expressing membrane-bound reporters in order to visualize neuronal projections. The results are shown in Figure 5. In the mushroom body area, numerous cells express both 4395-Gal4 and fru (Figures 5 A–F). In contrast to 4395-Gal4, fru is also expressed in the α′ and β′ lobes (Figures 5 L, L′, M, N). In order to assess the nature of cells that express both 4395-Gal4 and fru in the mushroom body area, we performed triple staining with anti-Dco. A representative optical section is shown in Figures 5 G–K. The cells co-expressing 4395-Gal4 and fru were found to be mushroom body neurons. We did not observe cells that co-expressed fru and 4395-Gal4 in the central complex. Staining in the antennal lobes was much weaker for both fru and 4395-Gal4 than in the other brain cells. fru expression was seen in glomeruli DA1 and VA1v (Figure 5Q), but presumably staining was too weak to see expression in VL2a and VA6, where its expression has also been reported [29]. hector expression was observed in the same subset of glomeruli already described in Figure 2 (Figure 5P) . This confirms expression of 4395-Gal4 in fru+ glomerular neurons of the antenna lobes (figure 5O) and suggests that hector may be required for male courtship in these cells. To further examine this possibility, we used GH146-Gal4 to express 4395-RNAi. GH146-Gal4 labels a large subset of projection neurons in the antennal glomeruli. Silencing of GH146 neurons has been shown to affect courtship and these neurons have been shown to be part of the sexually dimorphic neuronal pathway that responds to cis vaccenyl acetate (cVA) [18], [32], [33]. The GH146-Gal4/4395-RNAi males showed significantly reduced courtship in white light as well as red light (Figure 6A, B)). This indicates a role for CG4395 in a subset of GH146 projection neurons. However, the effect was not as pronounced as in fru-Gal4/4395-RNAi flies, suggesting an additional role for CG4395 outside of GH146 projection neurons, or a different strength of the two drivers. Using a GH146Q/UASQ-tomato reporter line, we did find co-staining of GH146Q and CG4395-Gal4 (Figure 6C), as expected from our results. In order to examine whether CG4395's main function might be in olfaction, we removed the third antennal segment, which contains the olfactory receptors, in control CS flies and in CG4395 mutant flies and examined their courtship in red light (Figure 6D). Courtship in all flies was reduced, as expected. The CI of the mutant flies was still significantly lower than in wildtype, arguing that CG4395 also has a role outside of olfaction.

thumbnail
Figure 5. Localization of cells that express both fru and 4395-Gal4.

(A–F) Brains of adult UAS-lacZnls/lexAop::GFPnls; 4395-Gal4/fruP1LexA males were double-stained with anti-β-Gal (magenta) and anti-GFP (green) to identify cells expressing both fru and hector. Although this GFP carries a nuclear localization signal (nls), diffusion of the protein is also seen into the projections. Co-staining is observed in numerous cell bodies in the mb area. (A, D: merged images, B, C, E, F: single images). A–C show the Z-stack of a whole brain, D–F show a representative magnified optical section in the area of the mb calyx. (G–K) Triple staining of UAS-lacZnls/lexAop::GFPnls; 4395-Gal4/fruP1LexA males with anti-β-Gal (blue), anti-GFP (green) and anti Dco (red) shows that cells that express both fru and hector are mushroom body cells. Co-staining is shown in G, single channels are shown in M, N, O. (L–Q) Brains of adult UAS-mcD8::GFP/4395-Gal4; lexAop-myr-mCherry/fruP1LexA were co-stained with anti-GFP (magenta) and anti-RFP (green). The projections of the neurons can be seen. In contrast to 4395-Gal4, fru, in addition to expression in the α, β and γ lobes, is also expressed in the α′ and β′ lobes. (L′) shows a magnification at the level of the β and γ lobes that visualizes overlap as well as close proximity of the labeled projections. (O–Q) Colocalization of fru and 4395-Gal4 staining in glomeruli of the antennal lobes. 4395-Gal4 projections are visible in all fru positive glomeruli.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0028269.g005

thumbnail
Figure 6. Reduction of CG4395 RNA in GH146 neurons reduces courtship.

Graphs show the courtship index CI (fraction of time males spend courting during the observation period) ± SEM of the indicated genotypes, N = 10. Expression of 4395-RNAi in GH146 projection neurons reduces male courtship in both white and red light (A, B). (C) Optical sections showing co-expression of UAS-mcD8::GFP/4395-Gal4 (magenta) and UASQ-tomato::GH146-Q (green) in antennal glomeruli. (D) CG4395f06077 mutant males missing the third antennal segment court less than control males.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0028269.g006

Discussion

Our data demonstrate a role for the CG4395 “hector” encoded GPCR in the regulation of Drosophila male courtship behavior. While hector mutant males are capable of performing all steps of courtship, the probability that they maintain and re-initiate courtship is low, resulting in a significantly reduced courtship index. Our experiments show that the courtship defect can be rescued by conditional expression of the gene in adult males. This demonstrates that the gene is required in adult males for normal courtship, thus indicating a physiological role for the protein in mating behavior. Based on its similarity to GPCRs, the most likely role for hector is in signaling processes that mediate the behavior. The hector gene codes for an orphan predicted neuropeptide GPCR that is related to the class of secretin-like receptors [34]. These include the mammalian calcitonin gene-related peptide (CGRP) receptors that bind Calcitonin (32aa), Calcitonine-related peptide and Amylin [35]. Among other things, Amyilin has been shown to regulate feeding related changes [36]. Interestingly, Takeout, a secreted protein that is required for male courtship has, in addition to its role in courtship, been reported to play a role in feeding behavior in response to starvation [37], [38].

hector is expressed in numerous cells in the fly brain. While a number of them remain unidentified, dominant expression is observed in the neurons of three defined brain structures: The mushroom bodies, the central complex and, at lower levels, in a subset of glomeruli in the antennal lobes. A large number of hector expressing cells are part of the mushroom body neurons that form the α, β and γ lobes. The mushroom bodies play a central role in learning and memory [39]. However, they have been shown to not be required for basic male courtship behavior [26]. We have confirmed that expression of hector in the mushroom bodies is not required for male courtship: Expression of 4395 RNAi by the P247-Gal4, a driver expressed predominantly in the α, β and γ lobes of the mushroom bodies [27], did not affect male courtship, whereas expression of the same 4395-RNAi by 4395-Gal4 lowered male courtship to the levels observed in the PBac mutants. These data demonstrate that expression of hector in the mushroom bodies is not required for male courtship behavior, in agreement with previous findings that these structures are dispensable for basic courtship.

Expression of hector is also observed in a number of structures of the central complex. The central complex plays a crucial role in the control of locomotion [40]. However, our short term activity assays which reflect motor behavior did not indicate locomotion defects in the hector mutants we examined. Furthermore, we did not observe co-expression of hector and fru in central complex neurons (see below).

Our initial genetic interaction studies indicated that hector and fru act in the same overall pathways that regulate male courtship behavior. Remarkably, when we expressed hector in fru neurons by using the fru-Gal4 driver, this expression was sufficient to completely rescue the hector mutant phenotype, indicating that hector is required in fru neurons. Furthermore, expression of 4395-RNAi in fru neurons using fru-Gal4 leads to a mutant phenotype that is indistinguishable from the ones observed in the pBac mutants. This implies a crucial role for hector in the control of male courtship behavior in a subset of fru neurons that co-express fru-Gal4 and 4395-Gal4. When we examined the co-expression of fru and hector we observed extensive co-labeling in the lateral protocerebrum. Not all hector expressing cells also express fru, and only a subset of fru cells expresses hector. Triple labeling experiments with anti-Dco, a mushroom body marker [25], demonstrated that the cells in the lateral protocerebrum that express both fru and hector are mushroom body neurons. As described above, mushroom bodies are not required for male courtship. This implies that cells outside the mushroom bodies, where hector and fru are co-expressed, are the cells that are required for male courtship.

While we observed a few additional isolated cells that express both, the only other neurons where we have observed hector expression where fru is also expressed are neurons in a subset of glomeruli in the antennal lobes. Although expression of our reporters in the antennal lobes was much weaker than in other parts of the brain, we have observed hector projections in glomeruli DM3, DL3, DA1, VA2a, VA1v and V6. This includes all of the glomeruli that have been shown to be innervated by both fru ORN and fru projection neurons (DA1, VA1v, VL2a and V6, [28], [29]. The best characterized among them is the DA1 glomerulus. It is innervated by the olfactory receptor neuron Or67d and has been shown to be involved in the perception of 11-cis-vaccenyl acetate (cVA) in both males and females [41], [42], [43]. cVA acts as a repellant in males, and an attractant in females. Or67d mutant males show increased courtship towards other males, but maintain courtship towards females. In contrast to the findings with Or67d mutants, we have not observed male-male courtship in hector mutants. We have also not observed an increase in latency, i.e. the time to first orientation towards the female, which is thought to be an indicator of impairment in perceiving pheromonal cues. Significantly, Stockinger et al. (2005) have shown that silencing of all of the fru projection neurons in the antennal glomeruli resulted in male-female courtship defects that were as strong as when they silenced all fru-expressing neurons [29]. This result implies an important role for these glomeruli in male-female courtship. Given the presence of hector projections in these glomeruli, and the results we obtained using fru-Gal4 driven rescue as well as hector knockdown by RNAi, we propose that the phenotypes we observe are in large parts caused by the absence of hector signaling in these glomerular fru neurons. Hence, signaling through the hector GPCR is likely a crucial component of male courtship in this subset of fru neurons. The observed courtship reduction in GH146/4395-RNAi males suggests that some of these neurons are projection neurons.

The hector protein is one of the few molecules identified that are specifically and functionally required in fru neurons. In support of this, we also find that the feminization of hector expressing cells leads to courtship defects, demonstrating the sexually dimorphic nature of these neurons. However, feminization of these neurons in adult males does not affect courtship. This is in agreement with earlier genetic studies that suggested that the basic competence for male courtship is established in late pupal stages [44], [45]. hector does not appear to be required to establish the male characteristics of fru neurons during development, since conditional expression of hector in adult males using the 4395-Gal4 as well as the fru-Gal4 driver was capable of completely rescuing the mutant phenotype. These results indicate that hector signaling is required to mediate adult male-specific signaling that is required for normal courtship. These signals may come from the fru ORN that project to these glomeruli, or from interneurons or other projection neurons. A fairly large number of neurotransmitters and neuropeptides has been found in the antennal lobes [46], and it remains to be seen what the ligand is for hector, and what signaling pathways lie downstream.

Materials and Methods

Fly strains

All flies strains were reared on standard corn meal/sugar-based medium at room temperature, except for Gal80ts flies that were grown at 18°C and induced as adults at 32°C as indicated. The CG4395 mutants PBacCG4395[f04274] and PBacCG4395[f06077] were obtained from the Exelixis Drosophila Stock Collection at the Harvard Medical School. traF is w1118; P{UAS-tra.F}20J7 [30]. w1118(CS) and w; GH146-Gal4 were a gift from Gregg Roman, University of Houston. fru-Gal4 [12] was a gift from Barry Dickson, IMP, Austria. fruP1.lexA [31] was a gift from David Mellert and Bruce Baker, Janelia farms. LexAop-myr Cherry [47] was a gift from Soeren Diegelmann and Matthias Landgraf, Cambridge and Erin Savner, Columbia University. UAS-4395 RNAi is VDRC stock no. 7223. desat11573 [23] was a gift from Jean-François Ferveur, Université de Bourgogne, Dijon, France.

w*; P{tubP-GAL80ts}20; TM2/TM6B, Tb1(stock no. 7019), w1118; P{UAS-lacZ.NZ}J312 (stock no. 3956), y1 w*; P{UAS-mCD8::GFP.L}LL5 (stock no. 5137), w1118; P{UAS-GFP.nls}14, w1118; lexA-2×hrGFP.nls (chrs 2, stock no. 29954), w1118; UAS-lacZ,NZ 20b (chrs 2, stock no. 3955), and w1118; {GH146-QF.P}53 P{QUAS-mtdTomato-3×HA}24A (stock no. 30037) [48] were obtained from the Bloomington stock center.

Courtship assay and Short term activity assay

Assays were performed as described [37]. Tested males were 4–6 days old. For experiments with mature virgin females, females were collected 2–3 hours after eclosion and aged in groups for 4 days before the courtship assay. Male–male courtship was performed as described in [23]. For experiments with removed antennae, antennae of newly eclosed virgin males were removed with a razor blade. The flies were then put in individual vials and tested for courtship 4–5 days later.

Immunohistochemistry

The brains of 4–5 day-old male flies were dissected in 1× PBS and fixed in freshly prepared 4% paraformaldehyde in PBSH (PBS containing 1 M NaCl) for 20 min at room temperature. All subsequent procedures were performed at room temperature, except for antibody incubations. Brains were washed three times in 1× PBHS/0.5% Triton for 15 min each and then washed three times in 0.1 M Tris-HCl/0.3 M NaCl (pH 7.4), containing 0.5% Triton X-100 (TNT) for 15 min each. Blocking was performed in TNT solution containing 4% normal goat serum (blocking buffer) for 1.5 h. The primary antibody was applied in blocking solution and incubated overnight at 10°C. The brains were rinsed six times for 15 min each and five times for 30 min each in TNT. The secondary antibody was diluted in blocking solution and incubated overnight at 10°C. Brains were washed six times for 15 min each. Fluorescently labeled brains were mounted with Vectashield mounting medium (Vector Laboratories, CA). For double-staining experiments, antibody stainings were performed sequentially using the procedure described above.

Antibodies used

Chicken anti-β-galactosidase (abcam ab9361) 1∶500; Mouse anti-β-galactosidase (Sigma G8021) 1∶100; Rabbit anti-β-Galactosidase; Rabbit anti-RFP (abcam ab62341) 1∶100, Rabbit anti-GFP (Invitrogen 673782) 1∶100; Mouse anti-GFP (Roche 11814460001) 1∶50, Mouse nc82 (Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank, Univ. of Iowa) 1∶20, Rabbit anti-Dco (Skoulakis et al., 1993) 1∶100, Alexa Fluor 635 goat anti-mouse (Invitrogen A31575) 1∶200, DyLight 405 goat anti-chicken (Jackson Immuno Research 103-475-155) 1∶200, Alexa Fluor 555 goat anti-rabbit (Invitrogen A21429) 1∶200.

Confocal microscopy

Fluorescent preparations were viewed using an Olympus FV1000 confocal microscope, Images were processed using Adobe Photoshop.

Generation of the 4395-Gal4 and UAS-4395 transgenes

4395-Gal4: 4.7 kb immediately upstream of the CG4395 translation initiation site, including 1.2 kb of upstream region, plus the non-coding exon 1 and intron 1 were amplified from CS genomic DNA and inserted into the pPTGAL transformation vector [49] as a BamH1/XbaI fragment. The primers used for PCR amplification were 5′-CGGGATCCGTGACTAACTCTTGTCAACGGCTAATGC-3′ and 5′-GCTCTAGA GTTGCCGATGGAACTGATGTCGC-3′, with unique 5′ restriction sites (underlined).

UAS4395: The open reading frame of 4395 was amplified by RTPCR from Canton-S head RNA isolated with RNAzol (Invitrogen). cDNA was prepared using the Superscript II kit (Invitrogen).The fragment was cloned into the pPUAST transformation vector. All constructs were verified by sequencing. Transgenic lines were established by Rainbow Transgenic Flies, Inc., CA, in a w1118 background, by P-element mediated transformation.

Statistical analysis

Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed, followed by Tukey Kramer pairwise comparisons using JMP (version 8.0.2 for Windows, SAS Institute, Inc.,). or Instat (version 3 for Macintosh, GraphPad Software , Inc.) software.

Acknowledgments

We thank Gregg Roman for critical reading of the manuscript and for flies, Makis Skoulakis for anti-Dco antibodies, and Barry Dickson, David Mellert, Bruce Baker, Soeren Diegelman and Matthias Landgraf for flies.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: YL VH CL BD. Performed the experiments: YL VH CL BHD CNV BD. Analyzed the data: YL VH CL BHD BD. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: YL VH BD. Wrote the paper: YL BD. Revising manuscript: VH CL.

References

  1. 1. Billeter JC, Rideout EJ, Dornan AJ, Goodwin SF (2006) Control of male sexual behavior in Drosophila by the sex determination pathway. Curr Biol 16: R766–776.
  2. 2. Dauwalder B (2008) Systems Behavior: Of Male Courtship, the Nervous System and Beyond in Drosophila. Current Genomics 9: 517–524.
  3. 3. Manoli DS, Meissner GW, Baker BS (2006) Blueprints for behavior: genetic specification of neural circuitry for innate behaviors. Trends Neurosci 29: 444–451.
  4. 4. Villella A, Hall JC (2008) Neurogenetics of courtship and mating in Drosophila. Adv Genet 62: 67–184.
  5. 5. Yamamoto D (2007) The neural and genetic substrates of sexual behavior in Drosophila. Adv Genet 59: 39–66.
  6. 6. Kimura K, Hachiya T, Koganezawa M, Tazawa T, Yamamoto D (2008) Fruitless and doublesex coordinate to generate male-specific neurons that can initiate courtship. Neuron 59: 759–769.
  7. 7. Rideout EJ, Billeter JC, Goodwin SF (2007) The sex-determination genes fruitless and doublesex specify a neural substrate required for courtship song. Curr Biol 17: 1473–1478.
  8. 8. Rideout EJ, Dornan AJ, Neville MC, Eadie S, Goodwin SF (2010) Control of sexual differentiation and behavior by the doublesex gene in Drosophila melanogaster. Nat Neurosci 13: 458–466.
  9. 9. Sanders LE, Arbeitman MN (2008) Doublesex establishes sexual dimorphism in the Drosophila central nervous system in an isoform-dependent manner by directing cell number. Dev Biol 320: 378–390.
  10. 10. Billeter JC, Villella A, Allendorfer JB, Dornan AJ, Richardson M, et al. (2006) Isoform-specific control of male neuronal differentiation and behavior in Drosophila by the fruitless gene. Curr Biol 16: 1063–1076.
  11. 11. Cachero S, Ostrovsky AD, Yu JY, Dickson BJ, Jefferis GS (2010) Sexual dimorphism in the fly brain. Curr Biol 20: 1589–1601.
  12. 12. Demir E, Dickson BJ (2005) fruitless splicing specifies male courtship behavior in Drosophila. Cell 121: 785–794.
  13. 13. Dornan AJ, Gailey DA, Goodwin SF (2005) GAL4 enhancer trap targeting of the Drosophila sex determination gene fruitless. Genesis 42: 236–246.
  14. 14. Kimura K, Ote M, Tazawa T, Yamamoto D (2005) Fruitless specifies sexually dimorphic neural circuitry in the Drosophila brain. Nature 438: 229–233.
  15. 15. Koganezawa M, Haba D, Matsuo T, Yamamoto D (2010) The shaping of male courtship posture by lateralized gustatory inputs to male-specific interneurons. Curr Biol 20: 1–8.
  16. 16. Lee G, Foss M, Goodwin SF, Carlo T, Taylor BJ, et al. (2000) Spatial, temporal, and sexually dimorphic expression patterns of the fruitless gene in the Drosophila central nervous system. J Neurobiol 43: 404–426.
  17. 17. Lee G, Hall JC, Park JH (2002) Doublesex gene expression in the central nervous system of Drosophila melanogaster. J Neurogenet 16: 229–248.
  18. 18. Ruta V, Datta SR, Vasconcelos ML, Freeland J, Looger LL, et al. (2010) A dimorphic pheromone circuit in Drosophila from sensory input to descending output. Nature 468: 686–690.
  19. 19. Taylor BJ (1989) Sexually dimorphic neurons in the terminalia of Drosophila melanogaster: I. Development of sensory neurons in the genital disc during metamorphosis. J Neurogenet 5: 173–192.
  20. 20. Yu JY, Kanai MI, Demir E, Jefferis GS, Dickson BJ (2010) Cellular organization of the neural circuit that drives Drosophila courtship behavior. Curr Biol 20: 1602–1614.
  21. 21. Arbeitman MN, Fleming AA, Siegal ML, Null BH, Baker BS (2004) A genomic analysis of Drosophila somatic sexual differentiation and its regulation. Development 131: 2007–2021.
  22. 22. Goldman TD, Arbeitman MN (2007) Genomic and functional studies of Drosophila sex hierarchy regulated gene expression in adult head and nervous system tissues. PLoS Genet 3: e216.
  23. 23. Marcillac F, Grosjean Y, Ferveur JF (2005) A single mutation alters production and discrimination of Drosophila sex pheromones. Proc Biol Sci 272: 303–309.
  24. 24. McGuire SE, Roman G, Davis RL (2004) Gene expression systems in Drosophila: a synthesis of time and space. Trends Genet 20: 384–391.
  25. 25. Skoulakis EM, Kalderon D, Davis RL (1993) Preferential expression in mushroom bodies of the catalytic subunit of protein kinase A and its role in learning and memory. Neuron 11: 197–208.
  26. 26. Kido A, Ito K (2002) Mushroom bodies are not required for courtship behavior by normal and sexually mosaic Drosophila. J Neurobiol 52: 302–311.
  27. 27. Zars T, Fischer M, Schulz R, Heisenberg M (2000) Localization of a short-term memory in Drosophila. Science 288: 672–675.
  28. 28. Manoli DS, Foss M, Villella A, Taylor BJ, Hall JC, et al. (2005) Male-specific fruitless specifies the neural substrates of Drosophila courtship behaviour. Nature.
  29. 29. Stockinger P, Kvitsiani D, Rotkopf S, Tirian L, Dickson BJ (2005) Neural circuitry that governs Drosophila male courtship behavior. Cell 121: 795–807.
  30. 30. Ferveur JF, Stortkuhl KF, Stocker RF, Greenspan RJ (1995) Genetic feminization of brain structures and changed sexual orientation in male Drosophila [see comments]. Science 267: 902–905.
  31. 31. Mellert DJ, Knapp JM, Manoli DS, Meissner GW, Baker BS (2009) Midline crossing by gustatory receptor neuron axons is regulated by fruitless, doublesex and the Roundabout receptors. Development 137: 323–332.
  32. 32. Heimbeck G, Bugnon V, Gendre N, Keller A, Stocker RF (2001) A central neural circuit for experience-independent olfactory and courtship behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98: 15336–15341.
  33. 33. Datta SR, Vasconcelos ML, Ruta V, Luo S, Wong A, et al. (2008) The Drosophila pheromone cVA activates a sexually dimorphic neural circuit. Nature 452: 473–477.
  34. 34. Hewes RS, Taghert PH (2001) Neuropeptides and neuropeptide receptors in the Drosophila melanogaster genome. Genome Res 11: 1126–1142.
  35. 35. Poyner DR, Sexton PM, Marshall I, Smith DM, Quirion R, et al. (2002) International Union of Pharmacology. XXXII. The mammalian calcitonin gene-related peptides, adrenomedullin, amylin, and calcitonin receptors. Pharmacol Rev 54: 233–246.
  36. 36. Lutz TA (2009) Roles of amylin in satiation, adiposity and brain development. Forum Nutr 63: 64–74.
  37. 37. Dauwalder B, Tsujimoto S, Moss J, Mattox W (2002) The Drosophila takeout gene is regulated by the somatic sex-determination pathway and affects male courtship behavior. Genes Dev 16: 2879–2892.
  38. 38. Sarov-Blat L, So WV, Liu L, Rosbash M (2000) The Drosophila takeout gene is a novel molecular link between circadian rhythms and feeding behavior. Cell 101: 647–656.
  39. 39. Skoulakis EM, Grammenoudi S (2006) Dunces and da Vincis: the genetics of learning and memory in Drosophila. Cell Mol Life Sci 63: 975–988.
  40. 40. Strauss R, Heisenberg M (1993) A higher control center of locomotor behavior in the Drosophila brain. J Neurosci 13: 1852–1861.
  41. 41. Ferveur JF (2005) Cuticular hydrocarbons: their evolution and roles in Drosophila pheromonal communication. Behav Genet 35: 279–295.
  42. 42. Ha TS, Smith DP (2006) A pheromone receptor mediates 11-cis-vaccenyl acetate-induced responses in Drosophila. J Neurosci 26: 8727–8733.
  43. 43. Kurtovic A, Widmer A, Dickson BJ (2007) A single class of olfactory neurons mediates behavioural responses to a Drosophila sex pheromone. Nature 446: 542–546.
  44. 44. Arthur BI Jr, Jallon JM, Caflisch B, Choffat Y, Nothiger R (1998) Sexual behaviour in Drosophila is irreversibly programmed during a critical period. Curr Biol 8: 1187–1190.
  45. 45. Belote JM, Baker BS (1987) Sexual behavior: its genetic control during development and adulthood in Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 84: 8026–8030.
  46. 46. Carlsson MA, Diesner M, Schachtner J, Nassel DR (2010) Multiple neuropeptides in the Drosophila antennal lobe suggest complex modulatory circuits. J Comp Neurol 518: 3359–3380.
  47. 47. Diegelmann S, Bate M, Landgraf M (2008) Gateway cloning vectors for the LexA-based binary expression system in Drosophila. Fly (Austin) 2: 236–239.
  48. 48. Potter CJ, Tasic B, Russler EV, Liang L, Luo L (2010) The Q system: a repressible binary system for transgene expression, lineage tracing, and mosaic analysis. Cell 141: 536–548.
  49. 49. Sharma Y, Cheung U, Larsen EW, Eberl DF (2002) PPTGAL, a convenient Gal4 P-element vector for testing expression of enhancer fragments in drosophila. Genesis 34: 115–118.