Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Adult Vampire Bats Produce Contact Calls When Isolated: Acoustic Variation by Species, Population, Colony, and Individual

  • Gerald G. Carter ,

    gcarter@umd.edu

    Affiliations Biological Sciences, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland, United States of America, Organization for Bat Conservation, Bloomfield Hills, Michigan, United States of America

  • Ryane Logsdon,

    Affiliation Biology Department, University of Rochester, Rochester, New York, United States of America

  • Bryan D. Arnold,

    Affiliation Department of Biological Sciences, Murray State University, Murray, Kentucky, United States of America

  • Angelica Menchaca,

    Affiliation Instituto de Ecologia, UNAM, Mexico City, Mexico

  • Rodrigo A. Medellin

    Affiliation Instituto de Ecologia, UNAM, Mexico City, Mexico

Abstract

Background

Bat pups produce individually distinct isolation calls to facilitate maternal recognition. Increasing evidence suggests that, in group-living bat species, adults often use similar calls to maintain contact. We investigated if isolated adults from all three species of the highly cooperative vampire bats (Phyllostomidae: Desmodontinae) would produce vocally distinct contact calls when physically isolated.

Methods/Principal Findings

We assessed variation in contact calls recorded from isolated captive and wild-caught adult common vampire bats (Desmodus rotundus), white-winged vampire bats (Diaemus youngi) and hairy-legged vampire bats (Diphylla ecaudata). We compared species-typical contact call structure, and used information theory and permuted discriminate function analyses to examine call structure variation, and to determine if the individuality of contact calls is encoded by different call features across species and populations. We found that isolated adult vampire bats produce contact calls that vary by species, population, colony, and individual. However, much variation occurred within a single context and individual. We estimated signature information for captive Diaemus (same colony), captive Desmodus (same colony), and wild Desmodus (different colonies) at 3.21, 3.26, and 3.88 bits, respectively. Contact calls from a captive colony of Desmodus were less individually distinct than calls from wild-caught Desmodus from different colonies. Both the degree of individuality and parameters encoding individuality differed between the bats from a single captive colony and the wild-caught individuals from different groups. This result is consistent with, but not sufficient evidence of, vocal convergence in groups.

Conclusion

Our results show that adult vampire bats of all three species produce highly variable contact calls when isolated. Contact calls contain sufficient information for vocal discrimination, but also possess more intra-individual variation than is required for the sole purpose of identifying individuals.

Introduction

Many group-living birds and mammals produce contact calls consisting of a series of harmonically rich, frequency-modulated syllables or notes (e.g. bottlenose dolphins [1], orange-fronted parakeets [2], cotton-top tamarins [3], for other examples see Bradbury & Vehrencamp [4]). Such signals often show “signature” variation [5] and are particularly important for animals that must maintain contact under conditions of low visibility. Echolocating bats live largely in a world of sound. In complete darkness, they perceive their surroundings with biosonar [6][7], track conspecifics by eavesdropping [8][11], identify offspring [5], [12], and communicate using an extensive repertoire of social calls, which have diverse forms and functions within and between species [12][19]. In every bat species studied thus far, non-volant pups produce isolation calls that allow mothers to find and recognize them [12]. Bohn et al. [20][21] illustrated the importance of isolation calls by showing correlated evolution between the hearing sensitivities of bats and the frequency of their species-specific isolation calls.

Analysis of animal signals through information theory (bits) allows the comparison of information content across different species, sample sizes, and signal modalities [5]. Wilkinson [12] showed that the information content of isolation calls correlates with colony size across eight species of bats, suggesting that vocal discrimination of pups in large colonies has driven the evolution of isolation call individuality. An illustrative example is provided by Mexican free-tailed bats, Tadarida brasiliensis, which produced the most variable isolation calls in his study [5], [12]. Female T. brasiliensis raise offspring in what are arguably the densest vertebrate aggregations, where a single cave might be inhabited by more than a million individuals [22], and each lactating female returning from foraging must find her single pup among thousands of others twice per day in total darkness [23]. The estimated complexity of these pup isolation calls at 9 bits [12] allows for 512 unique signature calls. In contrast, the isolation calls of three other species living in smaller colonies contained an estimate of 2 bits or less [12].

Isolation calls are a type of contact call. Hence, adult contact call variation should also correlate with social complexity. In some bat species, adults produce contact calls when held in isolation, searching for roosts, or alone in roosts with conspecifics calling outside [9], [13], [16], [17]. Adult bats can also eavesdrop on conspecific echolocation calls to find foraging or roosting locations, but the acoustic structure and directionality of biosonar pulses makes echolocation less ideal for intentional signaling [24] (but see [10], [25]). Social calls, by contrast, are typically longer in duration and lower in frequency, and hence travel farther with higher fidelity. Contact calls are likely to convey reliable information, such as individual identity or colony membership because they are often used to mediate cooperative interactions [9], [12][13], [18], [26][27].

There is evidence for heritable individual variation in bat social calls [28][29], but vocal learning can add another layer of complexity since contact calls might converge in structure between affiliated individuals [12], [30][35]. In the greater-spear nosed bat Phyllostomus hastatus, for example, newborn pups produce isolation calls with individual signatures [36], but female adults later join stable colonies and learn “screech calls”, which convey group, but not individual, identity [26]. These differences illustrate a match between structure and function, because individually distinct isolation calls allow recognition of each pup from 20 or so others, whereas the group-specific screech calls appear to coordinate group foraging [37].

In contrast to female greater spear-nosed bats, which live in stable social groups, other species demonstrate fission-fusion dynamics where individuals frequently switch roosts in such a way that groups split apart and recombine. Pallid bats (Antrozous pallidus), disc-winged bats (Thyroptera tricolor), noctules (Nyctalus noctula), Bechstein's bats (Myotis bechsteinii), and Natterer's bats (Myotis nattereri) are all species that switch roosts frequently and use contact calls to coordinate reunions at new roosting locations [16][19]. Here, contact calls with individual signatures allow particular individuals to roost together, even after foraging individually [18], [27].

The observation that social calls can mediate parental care [12], colonial roosting [16][18], group foraging [37], and collision avoidance [38] suggests that complex communication might mediate other cooperative social behaviors. Common vampire bats Desmodus rotundus show fission-fusion dynamics [12], [39], and possess the most cooperative social lives known among bats [12], [40][42]. Desmodus in Costa Rica roost in groups of 8–20 adults, which split apart and recombine, due to roost switching [39]. Females maintain long-term roosting affiliations that are largely independent of relatedness and last up to 12 years [12]. This species has been observed to survive 15+ years in the wild [43], with records twice as long in captivity [12], [44]. Such long-term social bonds involve cooperative behaviors such as allogrooming and regurgitated food sharing [39][41]. Unlike Desmodus, the social structures of the other two vampire species, the white-winged vampire bat (Diaemus youngi) and hairy-legged vampire bat (Diphylla ecaudata), have not been well studied in a natural context. However, all three species (Figure 1) are known to participate in social grooming and cooperative food sharing through regurgitation [13], [40], [45].

thumbnail
Figure 1. Spectrograms of calls from a common, white-winged, and hairy-legged vampire bat.

Shown are a contact call (a), echolocation pulse (b), and portion of distress call (c) from a common vampire bat (Desmodus rotundus); double-note contact call (d), echolocation pulse (e), and portion of distress call (f) from a white-winged vampire bat (Diaemus youngi); contact call (g), echolocation pulse (h) and portion of a distress call (i) from a hairy-legged vampire bat (Diphylla ecaudata). Distress calls are often produced by captured bats.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038791.g001

Adult Diaemus exchange individually distinct contact calls when isolated, and can use contact calls to vocally discriminate individuals [9], [13] and track their spatial locations [9]. Desmodus produce calls during both agonistic and affiliative interactions, and pups produce isolation calls that show variation between individuals [46][50]. However, no study has yet demonstrated individual signatures in adult Desmodus calls. To our knowledge, there are also no previous reports on social call variation in Diphylla.

Here we show that adults of all three species of vampire bat produce contact calls when physically, but not acoustically, isolated from conspecifics. We inspected contact call variation within and between the three vampire bat species using adult individuals recorded under the same conditions of social isolation. Our goals were to (1) describe contact call structure for each species, (2) test if contact calls contain information about individual identity, sex, colony, or population membership, and (3) determine if the vocal individuality of contact calls are encoded by similar or different call features.

Methods

Ethics Statement

We carried out our study in strict accordance with the recommendations in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health and National Research Council [51]. Captive bats were cared for by Organization for Bat Conservation (Cranbrook Institute of Science, Bloomfield Hills, Michigan), Rosamund Gifford Zoo (Syracuse, New York), or Talking Talons (Exhibitor's Permit no. 85-C-0021 issued by USDA Animal and Plant Health Inspection Service, Animal Care Division). Capture and temporary captivity of wild-caught vampire bats in Trinidad was approved by the University of Maryland Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (Protocol R-10-63) and The Wildlife Section, Forestry Division, Republic of Trinidad and Tobago (Special Game License Permit). All fieldwork in Mexico was conducted under SEMARNAT DGVS permit # FAUT-0001.

Recording procedure

All bats were caught in hand nets and placed individually in a small mesh cage for 1–24 hours within 10–30 cm of a CM 16 ultrasound condenser microphone (frequency range 10–200 kHz, Avisoft Bioacoustics, Berlin, Germany) and within hearing range of conspecifics. We digitized sounds with 16-bit resolution at a sampling rate of 250 kHz through an Avisoft Ultrasoundgate 116 or 416 on to a PC using Avisoft Recorder USG software.

Acoustic analysis

We use the term “note” to describe a continuous element on a spectrogram (equivalent to “syllable” elsewhere [14], [18]). We restricted our analysis to the most common note type: a simple downward frequency-modulated sweep (e.g. Figure 1a). We analysed 2881 initial notes from five Desmodus from a captive colony at the Organization for Bat Conservation, 12 wild-caught Desmodus (caught in Trinidad, West Indies), 17 Diaemus youngi from a captive colony (New Mexico), one lone captive Diaemus (Syracuse), and three wild-caught Diphylla ecaudata (near Puebla, Mexico). We used these notes to obtain descriptive acoustic characteristics for each species and to calculate information capacity. For discriminant function analyses, we eliminated Desmodus and Diaemus individuals from which we did not record at least 45 notes, resulting in 2797 notes from four captive female Desmodus, four wild-caught female Desmodus, 18 captive Diaemus, and three wild-caught Diphylla (Table 1). We only analyzed notes that could be unambiguously assigned to individuals, except for notes from the three Diphylla, which we could not unambiguously assign to individual, and therefore pooled (Table 1).

thumbnail
Table 1. Vampire bats used for species-typical descriptions and information estimates.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038791.t001

In our initial analyses, we included only the first note in a call, and excluded notes that occurred within 30 ms of a previous note. However, to analyze the effect of the double-note call structure commonly produced by Diaemus, we took measurements from both notes in a subsequent analysis. Only unclipped notes of adequate signal to noise ratio (10–99% amplitude) were included. We inspected spectrograms and oscillograms for each selected note in the program Batsound Pro (Pettersson Elektronik, Uppsala, Sweden). We hand-labeled the start and end of each note and used a custom-designed Matlab program [13] to automate 36 measurements between the start and end marks (Table 2), using 0.5 ms Blackman windows and 512-point FFTs (488 Hz resolution) with 50% overlap.

thumbnail
Table 2. Acoustic variables used for discriminant function analyses.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038791.t002

General Statistical analysis

To assess the signature information capacity within contact calls, we estimated information in bits per signal, Hs, obtained from an analyses of variance model using principal components extracted from call data [5]. Additionally, we calculated the probability of correct classification using conventional and permuted discriminate function analysis (DFA and pDFA) to train and test models. We favored this approach because it provides an empirical result (the rate at which new notes can be correctly classified) that is independent of model assumptions.

Information calculation

Following Beecher [5] and Arnold & Wilkinson [18], we extracted principal components (PCs) with varimax rotation, then used restricted maximum likelihood to obtain the variance component estimate (VCE) of random factors (species, colony, individual) for each retained PC. We ran a parallel analysis [52] to determine how many PCs to extract from our data, and saved PC scores using the Bartlett method in IBM SPSS Statistics 19 (Chicago, IL, USA). To estimate the percentage of variance contributed by the random factors of species, colony, and individual, we weighted the VCE for each factor by the percentage variance explained by its corresponding PC.

We calculated the total signature information capacity of calls from the captive Desmodus, wild Desmodus, and captive Diaemus separately. We used the VCEs for colony and individual differences (SB2) and within-individual differences (ST2) to calculate the total variance (ST2), then summed the information in each PC, Hi = log2(ST/Sw), to calculate total signature information in the call, Hs, and the repeatability of each PC, SB2/(SB2+Sw2) [5], [18].

Conventional and permuted discriminant function analysis

We tested our ability to assign notes to individuals, colony, sex, or species using permuted discriminant function analyses (pDFA), a randomization approach that calculates confidence intervals for the observed classification rates with nested, non-independent data [53]. The pDFA performs two randomizations. First, it randomly selects training notes from each subject (e.g. individual bat) to derive linear discriminant functions, choosing the number of training notes such that an equal sample of notes is taken from each subject. The remaining unselected notes are then used for testing the discriminant functions (i.e. cross-validation). This DFA procedure is then repeated 100 times with different random selections from each subject to calculate a mean correct classification rate for the dataset [52].

In the second randomization step, 1000 randomized datasets are created where notes from the same subject (the control factor) remain together while a higher-level group label (the test factor), such as species, population, colony, or sex, is randomly shuffled and assigned to subjects. A DFA is performed with each of the 1000 shuffled datasets creating a distribution of correct assignment rates expected from random chance. The proportion of randomized datasets with a correct classification rate at least as large as the original dataset is a one-tailed p-value [53]. In summary, this resampling procedure provides a p-value for determining the significance of the observed correct classification rate of notes to the test factor (e.g. colony), while controlling for a single nested factor (e.g. individual).

Individual bats used in our analyses are shown in Table 1. To assess species-level differences, we conducted a pDFA to test our assignment of notes to the correct species controlling for individual (Test 1) or colony (Test 2). We tested assignment of notes of Diaemus to correct sex while controlling for individual (Test 3), and tested assignment of notes of female Desmodus to the correct population (captive and wild) controlling for individual (Test 4). Using conventional DFAs, we also tested for assignment of notes to correct individual within the captive Desmodus (Test 5) and wild Desmodus (Test 6). To test whether individual variation was merely due to differences between recording sessions, we also conducted a pDFA using five individuals (three captive and two wild Desmodus) for which we had an adequate sample of notes recorded from two different sessions 4–11 days apart (Test 7). Finally, we compared individual variation in captive Diaemus using tests with both single notes (Test 8) and double-notes (Test 9). We did not inspect individual variation in Diphylla, because we could not unequivocally match calls to individuals.

Conventional DFAs for individual signatures were conducted in SPSS using the “leave-one-out” procedure for cross-validation; Wilks' lambda was used to test the null hypothesis that notes from different individuals have equal mean discriminant scores, and a x2 approximation was used to obtain a p-value. Permuted DFAs [53] were conducted using a script written by Roger Mundry and implemented in R (The R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria). We calculated 9 p-values from our DFA and pDFA tests, so we controlled our experimentwise error rate using a sequential Bonferroni correction [54]. Unless otherwise noted, correct classification rates are given for “test” notes (i.e. cross-validation) rather than for “training” notes used to build the model.

Results

Comparison of contact calls by species (Tests 1–2)

When isolated within audible range of conspecifics, adult females in all three species of vampire bats produced tonal calls similar but not identical to the contact calls previously recorded in Diaemus youngi [9], [13]. Female Desmodus rotundus produced mostly single-note calls; only 19% of notes were produced within 30 ms of a previous note (n = 2255). Diphylla ecaudata were similar; our three individuals produced only 13% of notes within 30 ms of a previous note (n = 608). In contrast, Diaemus youngi produced mostly double-note calls [13], [47][48], with a mean interval of 21.5 ms between notes (95% confidence interval, 21.0–22.0, n = 867). Out of 1456 Diaemus calls, 9% were single notes, 75% were double notes, and 16% were three or more notes. We observed antiphonal exchanges between adult Diaemus but not between adults of the other two species.

Note structure varied by species (Figure 1, Table 3). Using only the first notes from calls, we could assign 94% of test notes to the correct species when controlling for individual (Test 1, pDFA, n = 2023, p = 0.001) or 93% of test notes when controlling for colony (Test 2, pDFA, n = 2689, p = 0.001). For training notes used to construct the discriminant functions, the corresponding correct classification rates were 96% and 97% respectively (n = 1073, 407). Overall, Desmodus produced the shortest and steepest calls, typically single notes that were the highest in frequency, with first and second harmonics carrying more energy than the fundamental. Diaemus produced the longest and least steep calls, typically double notes that were intermediate in frequency, with the fundamental carrying more energy than harmonics. Diphylla produced the lowest frequency calls of intermediate duration and slope, with the fundamental carrying less energy than the harmonics.

thumbnail
Table 3. Mean±standard error for 12 acoustic variables by species.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038791.t003

Sex and population-level variation in note structure (Tests 3–4)

While controlling for individual variation, we found no significant effect of sex on the note structure of the captive Diaemus (Test 3, n = 375, p = 0.7). We demonstrated population-level variation in Desmodus notes by correctly classifying 87% of training notes (n = 333) and 83% of test notes to the correct wild or captive population while controlling for individual variation (Test 4, n = 675, p = 0.024).

Individual signatures (Tests 5–9)

Desmodus contact calls could be assigned to individuals with greater than chance accuracy (Table 4). Furthermore, wild individuals from different colonies produced calls that were more individually distinct than bats from the same captive colony; the mean classification rate was significantly higher in the wild-caught bats from different colonies than the bats from a single captive colony (t = 2.987, df = 6, p = 0.024, Table 4, Figure 2). The overall correct classification rate was 66% for four bats from a single captive colony (Test 5, Wilks' lambda = 0.27, x2 = 948.0, df = 126, N = 756, p<0.001, Table 4) and 91% for four wild-caught bats from different colonies (Test 6, Wilks' lambda = 0.01, x2 = 955.6, df = 153, N = 221, p<0.001, Table 4). The ability to assign notes to correct individuals remained after controlling for recording session (Test 7, pDFA, n = 682, p = 0.002).

thumbnail
Figure 2.

Vocal individuality shown by the overlap in discriminate scores of calls from four common vampire bats from either a single captive colony (a) or four different wild colonies (b). Plot shows the discriminant scores for the first two canonical discriminant functions constructed separately for each population. The four bats in each plot are denoted by different symbols.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038791.g002

thumbnail
Table 4. Percentage of correctly assigning notes to four individuals for captive and wild common vampires Desmodus rotundus and captive white-winged vampires Diaemus youngi.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038791.t004

In Diaemus contact calls, the first notes were individually variable (Test 8, Wilks' lambda = 0.02, x2 = 4967.1, df = 612, N = 1293, p<0.001). For first notes, the overall correct classification rate was 57% (chance rate = 6%, range = 11–87%). However, when we included measurements from both notes, the overall correct classification rate increased to 72% (Test 9, range = 48–90%, Wilks' lambda = 0.001, x2 = 6034.3, df = 867, N = 866, p<0.001).

Information analysis and variation within individuals

Contact calls were also highly variable within individuals. Six extracted principal components accounted for 74% of the total variance, and of this variation, 40.9% was accounted for by species (16.4%), colony (8.5%) and individual (16.0%) differences. The low stereotypy and high variability of notes within calls of individuals were reflected by relatively low estimates of signature information (Diaemus = 3.21, captive Desmodus = 3.26, wild Desmodus = 3.88 bits) and low estimates of repeatability across PCs (Diaemus: mean = 0.15, range = 0.004–0.444; captive Desmodus mean = 0.14, range = 0.014–0.26; wild Desmodus: mean = 0.34, range = 0.15–0.57). For the subfamily Desmodontinae as a whole, contact calls contained 4.4 bits with a mean repeatability of 0.31 (range = 0.09–0.70). Despite the within-individual variation, we were still able to assign notes to correct species, populations, and individuals with high accuracy.

Acoustic parameters encoding individual variation

A summary of the canonical structure matrix for the DFA on individual variation in Desmodus notes shows that the acoustic parameters defining the first discriminant function differed between the captive and wild populations (Table 5). In notes from the population of wild individuals from different colonies, all of the five highest loading parameters were measures of frequency. By contrast, in notes from the captive colony, the highest loading parameter was the relative intensity of the third harmonic.

thumbnail
Table 5. Highest variable loadings for discriminant functions assigning notes to individual common vampire bats.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038791.t005

A summary of the canonical structure matrix for the DFA on individual variation in Diaemus notes (Table 6) show that discriminate function 1 was largely defined by the starting frequencies, discriminant function 2 was highly correlated with duration, discriminant function 3 was highly correlated with timing and frequency of the frequency of most energy in the fundamental, and discriminant function 4 was largely defined by the ending frequencies. These four discriminant functions explained 72.7% of the total variance.

thumbnail
Table 6. Highest variable loadings for discriminant functions assigning notes to individual white-winged vampire bats.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038791.t006

Discussion

Variation in contact calls

Contact calls of vampire bats (Phyllostomidae: Desmodontinae) were highly variable between individual bats, even when considering only the first notes of downward sweeping tonal calls. Our acoustic analyses also show that the variation of contact calls produced by a single isolated bat was quite high with 59% of the variation explained by our extracted principal components occurring within individuals. Species and individual differences made roughly equivalent contributions to total variation in contact calls, while population and group factors contributed less variation. In summary, all three vampire bats produced contact calls that were not completely stereotyped within individuals, but demonstrated individual signatures when we tested for them using permuted and conventional discriminant function analyses.

The estimated signature information capacity of the adult vampire bat contact calls in our study was greater than the estimated information in the pup isolation calls of the following bat species (listed in increasing information content): Nycticeinops schlieffenii (non-colonial), Cleotis percivali (colonial), Scotophilus borbonicus (colonies of up to 100 bats), and Rhinolophus simulator (colonies of several hundred). Adult vampire bat contact calls contained less signature information than isolation calls of Hipposideros caffer (colonies of thousands), Chaerephon pumila (colonies of 20–500 bats), Nycticeius humeralis (colonies up to 1000 bats), and Tadarida brasiliensis (colonies over a million) [12], and also less than the adult contact calls of Antrozous pallidus (colony size at study site of up to 100 bats) [18]. A. pallidus produced contact calls with greater stereotypy within an individual across days [18], whereas Desmodus calls from a single bat in a single context were highly variable.

Individual signatures in the common vampire bat Desmodus rotundus

Both the captive and wild Desmodus in our study conveyed individual identity in their contact calls, and several lines of evidence suggest that common vampires can vocally discriminate individuals. First, previous studies [9], [13] demonstrated that the closest extant taxa, Diaemus youngi, which possess similar individual signatures in their contact calls, can vocally discriminate individuals. This was revealed by habituation-discrimination playback experiments [13], and by the experimental result that shuffling the relative positions of caged bats to new locations leads to more calling and a higher response rate compared to shuffling bats back to the same locations [9]. Anecdotal observations also suggest that highly associated individual Desmodus are attracted to each other's contact calls. Captive individuals released back into cages were immediately attended to by roostmates known to have high levels of relatedness or past association (GGC, unpublished data). In another case, a captured wild bat was released and flew out of a building, but when a second bat caught at the same location began calling, the first bat re-entered the building and hung on the ceiling nearby producing social calls. Finally, Desmodus are able to recognize the breathing sounds of individual humans [55], which highlights their ability to use subtle and complex acoustic cues for vocal recognition.

Double-note contact calls of the white-winged vampire bat Diaemus youngi

Diaemus youngi are the most vocal of the vampire bats. They produce more calls, their calls have more notes, and their notes are longer and likely more individually distinct than the other two bat species. To record contact calls from Desmodus and Diphylla, we left bats in physical isolation for up to 24 hours, but Diaemus often produced more than 50 calls in less than two hours. In many bats, including Desmodus, mothers and pups exchange social calls. However, we only observed adult antiphonal exchanges in Diaemus, where calls are exchanged with a latency of about 1/3rd of a second [13].

Contrary to our expectations, the single Diaemus individual from a different population produced notes that were not more distinct. In fact, the correct classification rate for this individual (11%) was an outlier for poor accuracy of classification (n = 18, chi-square outlier test: X2 = 5.0364, p = 0.025) when considering only the first note of a call. However, when considering both notes of a call, this discrepancy disappeared, suggesting that the second note in this individual contained much of the signature information. This observation again highlights the fact that double-note call structures allow for substantial increases in potential information content.

The acoustic parameters defining vocal individuality differed between Desmodus (Table 5) and Diaemus (Table 6). Individuality in Desmodus notes was largely explained by minimum frequencies, slopes, and, in the captive bats, harmonic structure. Whereas in the captive Diaemus calls, discriminant function 1 was defined largely by the frequency values of the frequency of the first half of the note. Discriminant function 2 was most highly correlated with duration. Discriminant function 3 was largely correlated with the frequency of most energy and its timing. Discriminant function 4 was defined largely by the last half of the note.

Population and colony variation in common vampire bats Desmodus rotundus

After controlling for individual as a factor, we could correctly assign notes to the wild-caught Trinidad population or the captive population with greater than chance accuracy. Interestingly, we could more easily assign notes to wild-caught bats from different colonies than to the bats from a single captive colony (Table 4, Figure 2). Calls from wild-caught vampire bats encoded more signature information (3.9 bits wild versus 3.3 bits captive). In other words, vampire bats in the captive colony produced calls with less vocal individuality compared to wild bats from different colonies. These acoustic differences might result from a number of possible factors, including long-term captivity, geographic variation, or vocal convergence in colonies [56]. However, the greater call similarity in the captive colony is not likely to be explained by genetic relatedness. We estimated the pairwise relatedness of individuals from both populations using 13 microsatellite loci [57][58], and found that the four individuals used in this study appear genetically unrelated to each other as are the four wild Desmodus caught at different sites.

Vocal learning in bats has been demonstrated when pup calls converge with mothers [30][32] or adult males [35], and when adult Phyllostomus hastatus learn group-specific calls [33]. If the captive Desmodus vocally converged in call structure, we might expect differences in the acoustic parameters encoding individuality. We predicted that vocal learning would be more likely to change sound production at the larynx (which primarily determines duration, frequency, and frequency modulation of the fundamental) than at the vocal tract (which determines the relative intensity of harmonics), because the vocal folds in the larynx are relatively plastic while the vocal tract above the vocal cords is stable by comparison [59]. Put differently, we predicted that vocal learning in the captive population would have led to converge among the laryngeal-based parameters such as frequency rather than harmonic structure, which is shaped by vocal tract filtering. Consistent with this expectation, the vocal individuality of the wild bats from different groups was based relatively more on differences in the frequencies and modulation of the fundamental, whereas vocal individuality in the captive colony relied relatively more on the distribution of energy across harmonics (Table 5). Further study is needed to determine if vampire bats are capable of vocal learning.

Function of contact calls in adult vampire bats

Pup isolation calls have a clear function, but there is much we do not understand about the function of adult contact calls in vampires or other bat species. It remains unclear why adult vampire bats produce contact calls when isolated. Future work should address whether highly affiliated individuals are more likely to respond to contact calls than other conspecifics and if calls produced in other social contexts (e.g. allogrooming, food sharing) have the same or different structure. Finally, we do not yet know if the large variation produced by a single isolated individual bat corresponds to random variation or unidentified social factors such as the caller's motivational state, perceived proximity to conspecifics [60], or stress [61][64].

Acknowledgments

We thank Thanh Tran and Anna Jiang for help with call analysis. We thank Rob Mies, Jessie Fabian, Daniel Abram, Talking Talons Youth Leadership, Rosamund Gifford Zoo, and Hansraj Abraham for access to animals, facilities, and enthusiastic support. We thank Paul Faure and Brock Fenton for providing recording equipment, and Roger Mundry for providing R scripts for pDFAs. Helpful comments on earlier drafts of this paper were provided by Michelle Nowak and Daniela Rambaldini. Gerald Wilkinson provided helpful discussion.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: GGC. Performed the experiments: GGC RL AM. Analyzed the data: GGC BDA. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: GGC BDA RAM. Wrote the paper: GGC.

References

  1. 1. Caldwell MC, Caldwell DK (1965) Individualized whistle contours in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncates). Nature 207: 434–443.
  2. 2. Cortopassi KA, Bradbury JW (2006) Contact call diversity in wild orange–fronted parakeet pairs, Aratinga canicularis. Anim Behav 71: 1141–1154.
  3. 3. Weiss DJ, Garibaldi BT, Hauser MD (2001) The production and perception of long calls by cotton–top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus): acoustic analyses and playback experiments. J Comp Psychol 115: 258–271.
  4. 4. Bradbury JW, Vehrencamp SL (2011) Principles of Animal Communication, 2nd Edition. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates. 697 p.
  5. 5. Beecher MD (1989) Signaling systems for individual recognition: an information theory approach. Anim Behav 38: 248–261.
  6. 6. Schnitzler H–U, Moss CF, Denzinger A (2003) From spatial orientation to food acquisition in echolocating bats. Trends Ecol Evolut 18: 386–394.
  7. 7. Moss CF, Bohn K, Gilkenson H, Surlykke A (2006) Active listening for spatial orientation in a complex auditory scene. PLoS Biol 4: e79. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0040079.
  8. 8. Barclay RMR (1982) Interindividual use of echolocation calls: eavesdropping by bats. Behav Evol Sociobiol 10: 271–275.
  9. 9. Carter GG, Fenton MB, Faure PA (2009) White–winged vampire bats (Diaemus youngi) exchange contact calls. Can J Zool 87: 604–608.
  10. 10. Dechmann DKN, Heucke SL, Giuggioli L, Safi K, Voigt CC, et al. (2009) Experimental evidence for group hunting via eavesdropping in echolocating bats. Proc R Soc B 276: 2721–2728.
  11. 11. Jones G, Siemers B (2011) The communicative potential of bat echolocation pulses. J Comp Physiol A 197: 447–457.
  12. 12. Wilkinson GS (2003) Social and vocal complexity in bats. In: de Waal FBM, Tyack PL, editors. pp. 322–341. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
  13. 13. Carter GG, Skowronski MD, Faure PA, Fenton B (2008) Antiphonal calling allows individual discrimination in white–winged vampire bats. Anim Behav 76: 1343–1355.
  14. 14. Bohn KM, Schmidt–French B, Ma ST, Pollak GD (2008) Syllable acoustics, temporal patterns, and call composition vary with behavioral context in Mexican free–tailed bats. J Acoust Soc Am 124: 1838–1848.
  15. 15. Bohn KM, Schmidt–French B, Schwartz C, Smotherman M, Pollak GD (2009) Versatility and stereotypy of free–tailed bat songs. PLoS ONE 4: e6746. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0006746.
  16. 16. Schöner CR, Schoner MG, Kerth G (2010) Similar is not the same: social calls of conspecifics are more effective in attracting wild bats to day roosts than those of other bat species. Behav Evol Sociobiol 64: 2053–2063.
  17. 17. Chaverri G, Gillam EH, Vonhof MJ (2010) Social calls used by a leaf–roosting bat to signal location. Biol Lett 6: 441–444.
  18. 18. Arnold BD, Wilkinson GS (2011) Individual specific contact calls of pallid bats (Antrozous pallidus) attract conspecifics at roosting sites. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 65: 1581–1593.
  19. 19. Furmankiewicz J, Ruczynski I, Radoslaw U, Jones G (2011) Social calls provide tree–dwelling bats with information about location of conspecifics at roosts. Ethology 117: 480–489.
  20. 20. Bohn KM, Boughman JW, Wilkinson GS, Moss CF (2004) Auditory sensitivity and frequency selectivity in greater spear–nosed bats suggest specializations for acoustic communication. J Comp Physiol A 190: 185–192.
  21. 21. Bohn KM, Moss CF, Wilkinson GS (2006) Correlated evolution between hearing sensitivity and social calls in bats. Biol Lett 2: 561–564.
  22. 22. Betke M, Hirsh DE, Makris NC, McCracken GF, Procopio M, et al. (2008) Thermal imaging reveals significantly smaller Brazilian free–tailed bat colonies than previously estimated. J Mammal 89: 18–24.
  23. 23. McCracken GF (1984) Communal nursing in Mexican free–tailed bat maternity colonies. Science 233: 1090–1091.
  24. 24. Hoffmann FF, Hejduk J, Caspers B, Siemers BM (2007) In the mating system of the bat Saccopteryx bilineata, bioacoustics constraints impede male eavesdropping on female echolocation calls for their surveillance. Can J Zool 85: 863–872.
  25. 25. Dechmann DKN, Kranstauber B, Gibbs D, Wikelski M (2010) Group hunting– A reason for sociality in Molossid bats? PLoS ONE 5: e9012 doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0009012.
  26. 26. Boughman JW, Wilkinson GS (1998) Greater spear–nosed bats discriminate group mates by vocalizations. Anim Behav 55: 1717–1732.
  27. 27. Gillam EH, Chaverri G (2011) Strong individual signatures and weaker group signatures in contact calls of Spix's disc–winged bat, Thyroptera tricolor. Anim Behav 83: 269–276.
  28. 28. Scherrer JA, Wilkinson GS (1993) Evening bat isolation calls provide evidence for heritable signatures. Anim Behav 46: 847–860.
  29. 29. Knörnschild M, von Helversen O, Mayer F (2007) Twin siblings sound alike: isolation call variation in the noctule bat, Nyctalus noctula. Anim Behav 74: 1055–1063.
  30. 30. Esser KH, Schmidt U (1989) Mother–infant communication in the lesser spear–nosed bat Phyllostomus discolor (Chiroptera, Phyllostomidae): evidence for acoustic learning. Ethology 82: 156–168.
  31. 31. Jones G, Ransome RD (1993) Echolocation calls of bats are influenced by maternal effects and change over a lifetime. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 252: 125–128.
  32. 32. Esser KH (1994) Audio–vocal learning in a non–human mammal: the lesser spear–nosed bat Phyllostomus discolor. Neuroreport 5: 1718–1720.
  33. 33. Boughman JW (1998) Vocal learning by greater spear–nosed bats. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 265: 227–233.
  34. 34. Chen SF, Jones G, Rossiter SJ (2009) Determinants of echolocation call frequency variation in the Formosan lesser horseshoe bat (Rhinolophus monoceros). Proc Biol Sci 276: 3901–3909.
  35. 35. Knörnschild M, Nagy M, Metz M, Mayer F, von Helversen O (2010) Complex vocal imitation during ontogeny in a bat. Biol Lett 6: 156–159.
  36. 36. Bohn KM, Wilkinson GS, Moss CF (2007) Discrimination of infant isolation calls by female greater spear–nosed bats, Phyllostomus hastatus. Anim Behav 73: 423–432.
  37. 37. Wilkinson GS, Boughman JW (1998) Social calls coordinate foraging in greater spear–nosed bats. Anim Behav 55: 337–350.
  38. 38. Suthers RA (1965) Acoustic orientation by fish–catching bats. J Exp Zool 158: 319–334.
  39. 39. Wilkinson GS (1985) The social organization of the common vampire bat Desmodus rotundus I: pattern and cause of association. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 17: 111–122.
  40. 40. Wilkinson GS (1984) Reciprocal food sharing in the vampire bat. Nature 308: 181–184.
  41. 41. Wilkinson GS (1986) Social grooming in the common vampire bat, Desmodus rotundus. Anim Behav 34: 1880–1889.
  42. 42. Kerth G (2008) Causes and consequences of sociality in bats. Bioscience 58: 737–746.
  43. 43. Tschapka M, Wilkinson GS (1999) Free–ranging vampire bats (Desmodus rotundus, Phyllostomidae) survive 15 years in the wild. Z Saugetierkd 64: 239–240.
  44. 44. Weigl R (2005) Longevity of Mammals in Captivity; From the Living Collections of the World. Stuttgart: Kleine Senckenberg–Reihe. 214 p.
  45. 45. Elizalde Arellano C, López–Vidal JC, Arroyo–Cabráles J, Medellín RA, Laundré JW (2007) Food sharing behavior in the hary–legged vampire bat Diphylla ecaudata. Acta Chirop 9: 314–319.
  46. 46. Schmidt U (1972) Social calls of juvenile vampire bats Desmodus rotundus and their mothers. Bonn Zool Beitr 23: 310–316.
  47. 47. Gould E, Woolf NK, Turner C (1973) Double note communication calls in bats: occurrence in 3 families. J Mammal 54: 998–1001.
  48. 48. Gould E (1997) Echolocation and communication. In: Baker RJ, Jones JKJ, Carter DC, editors. pp. 247–279. Lubbock: Texas Tech University Press.
  49. 49. Sailler H, Schmidt U (1978) Social calls of the common vampire bat Desmodus rotundus during aggressive behavior at the feeding place in the laboratory. Z Saugetierkd 43: 249–261.
  50. 50. Schmidt U, Manske U (1973) Die jugendentwicklung der vampirfledermause(Desmodus rotundus). Z Saugetierkd 38: 14–33.
  51. 51. Institute of Laboratory Animal Research, Committee for the Update of the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, National Research Council (2011) Guide for the care and use of laboratory animals. 8th edition. Washington, DC: National Academies Press. 220 p.
  52. 52. O'Connor BP (2000) SPSS and SAS programs for determining the number of components using parallel analysis and Velicer's MAP test. Behav Res Meth Ins C 32: 396–402.
  53. 53. Mundry R, Sommer C (2007) Discriminant function analysis with non independent data: consequences and an alternative. Anim Behav 7: 965–997.
  54. 54. Holm S (1979) A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scand J Stat 6: 65–70.
  55. 55. Groger U, Wiegrebe (2006) Classification of human breathing sounds by the common vampire bat, Desmodus rotundus. BMC Biol 4: doi. pp. 10.1186/1741–3334/29/2/006.
  56. 56. Janik VM, Slater PJ (2000) The different roles of social learning in vocal communication. Anim Behav 60: 1–11.
  57. 57. Piaggio AJ, Johnston JJ, Perkins SL (2008) Development of polymorphic microsatellite loci for the common vampire bat, Desmodus rotundus (Chiroptera: Phylostomidae). Mol Ecol Notes 8: 440–442.
  58. 58. Dechmann D, Garbely E, Kerth G, Garner T (2002) Highly polymorphic microsatellites for the study of the round–eared bat, Tonatia silvicola (d'Orbigny). Conserv Genet 3: 455–458.
  59. 59. Taylor A, Reby D (2010) The contribution of source–filter theory to mammal vocal communication research. J Zool 280: 221–236.
  60. 60. Sugiura H (2007) Effects of proximity and behavioral context on acoustic variation in the coo calls of Japanese macaques. Am J Primatol 69: 1412–1424.
  61. 61. Manser MB, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2002) Suricate alarm calls signal predator class and urgency. Trends Cogn Sci 6: 55–57.
  62. 62. Rendall D (2003) Acoustic correlates of caller identity and affect intensity in the vowel–like grunt vocalizations of baboons. J Acous Soc Ami 113: 3390–3402.
  63. 63. Nicastro N, Owren MJ (2003) Classification of domestic cat (Felis catus) vocalizations by naïve and experienced human listeners. J Comp Psych 117: 44–52.
  64. 64. Pongrácz P, Molnár C, Miklósi Á, Csányi V (2005) Human listeners are able to classify dog (Canis familiaris) barks recorded in different situations. J Comp Psych 119: 136–44.