Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Water-Borne Cues of a Non-Indigenous Seaweed Mediate Grazer-Deterrent Responses in Native Seaweeds, but Not Vice Versa

  • Hee Young Yun ,

    hyyun77@gmail.com

    Affiliation Section Functional Ecology, Biologische Anstalt Helgoland, Alfred-Wegener-Institute for Polar and Marine Research, Helgoland, Germany

  • Aschwin H. Engelen,

    Affiliation Marine Plant Ecology Research Group, Centre of Marine Sciences, University of Algarve, Faro, Portugal

  • Rui O. Santos,

    Affiliation Marine Plant Ecology Research Group, Centre of Marine Sciences, University of Algarve, Faro, Portugal

  • Markus Molis

    Affiliation Section Functional Ecology, Biologische Anstalt Helgoland, Alfred-Wegener-Institute for Polar and Marine Research, Helgoland, Germany

Abstract

Plants optimise their resistance to herbivores by regulating deterrent responses on demand. Induction of anti-herbivory defences can occur directly in grazed plants or from emission of risk cues to the environment, which modifies interactions of adjacent plants with, for instance, their consumers. This study confirmed the induction of anti-herbivory responses by water-borne risk cues between adjoining con-specific seaweeds and firstly examined whether plant-plant signalling also exists among adjacent hetero-specific seaweeds. Furthermore, differential abilities and geographic variation in plant-plant signalling by a non-indigenous seaweed as well as native seaweeds were assessed. Twelve-day induction experiments using the non-indigenous seaweed Sargassum muticum were conducted in the laboratory in Portugal and Germany with one local con-familiar (Portugal: Cystoseira humilis, Germany: Halidrys siliquosa) and hetero-familiar native species (Portugal: Fucus spiralis, Germany: F. vesiculosus). All seaweeds were grazed by a local isopod species (Portugal: Stenosoma nadejda, Germany: Idotea baltica) and were positioned upstream of con- and hetero-specific seaweeds. Grazing-induced modification in seaweed traits were tested in three-day feeding assays between cue-exposed and cue-free ( = control) pieces of both fresh and reconstituted seaweeds. Both Fucus species reduced their palatability when positioned downstream of isopod-grazed con-specifics. Yet, the palatability of non-indigenous S. muticum remained constant in the presence of upstream grazed con-specifics and native hetero-specifics. In contrast, both con-familiar (but neither hetero-familiar) native species reduced palatability when located downstream of grazed S. muticum. Similar patterns of grazer-deterrent responses to water-borne cues were observed on both European shores, and were almost identical between assays using fresh and reconstituted seaweeds. Hence, seaweeds may use plant-plant signalling to optimise chemical resistance to consumers, though this ability appeared to be species-specific. Furthermore, this study suggests that native species may benefit more than a non-indigenous species from water-borne cue mediated reduction in consumption as only natives responded to signals emitted by hetero-specifics.

Introduction

Herbivory is an important factor in structuring ecological communities that is presumed to have a stronger effect in marine than in terrestrial habitats [1], [2]. Extreme grazing events may completely defoliate plants [3], or denude entire seascapes [4]. However, plants usually persist with variable success in the presence of grazers. Hence, rather than being simple and passive participants in their interaction with hungry consumers, plants can actively deter herbivores [5], [6], [7]. Understanding drivers and processes influencing the outcome of plant-grazer interactions is therefore a pivotal goal in ecology to improve predictions regarding community structure under current and future environmental conditions.

Plants may deploy anti-herbivory defences constitutively, as in the use of grazer repulsive secondary metabolites [3], [8]. According to theory, the efficacy of inducible defences is dependent on the predictability of future risk and the speed at which defensive traits are produced [8], [9]. Since grazing by smaller-sized herbivores like insects or gastropods is usually not lethal for larger vascular plants and seaweeds, expression of grazer-deterrent resistance within ecologically meaningful times is possible [10], [11]. Furthermore, tailoring resistance to actual threats may imply a selective advantage in plants if grazer-deterrent responses incur a metabolic cost [12]. Not surprisingly, inducible anti-herbivory defences are widespread in plants [10] and seaweeds [13] and can indirectly have great effects on herbivore species richness [7], inter-specific competition among herbivores [14], [15], and community structure [16]. Moreover, grazing by small (<2.5 cm) herbivores, known as meso-herbivores [17], has been shown to elicit emission of air- and/or water-borne risk cues in vascular plants and seaweeds [18], [19]. In addition, some seaweed species when damaged release gaseous volatile substances such as dimethylsulphide (DMS) or trimethylamine (TMA) that may function as grazer deterrents in the aqueous phase, as found for Dictyota dichotoma [20]. Other soluble organic compounds from seaweeds, such as alginate derivates, may also mediate species interactions [21]. The use of risk compounds probably evolved initially to optimise within-plant signalling to counter grazing [19]. Furthermore, plants and seaweeds with a complex anatomy and/or limited vascular system should gain a selective advantage from using risk signals, as these may prime undamaged parts allowing more rapid and systemic responses, e.g. to attacking grazers, than information transfer by internal transport systems [22]. The emission of risk cues to the environment would principally allow intra-specific signalling between adjoining con-specifics: it was shown, e.g. that undamaged corn (Zea mays) exposed to caterpillar (Spodoptora exigua) induced emission of volatile signals from adjacent grazed corn accelerated and intensified the expression of anti-herbivory traits, and therefore shortened the period of vulnerability once attacked [23]. Furthermore, optimising plant resistance by using emitted signals may allow attacked plants to save their defence budget [22]. Emitted signals, however, become public information that may be used in plant-plant communication, i.e. signals are supposedly positive for both emitter and receiver, or for eavesdropping, i.e. signals benefit the receiver but have neutral or negative effects on the emitting species [19], smoothing the way for mutualistic [24] and antagonistic indirect interactions [25].

Knowledge of antagonistic indirect effects mediated by risk cues is scarce for trophic interactions among plants and herbivores [19], and as far as we know, examples involving seaweeds are lacking. Hence, commonness of and patterns in plant-plant signalling in general and their existence in seaweeds in particular have yet to be determined [22]. The effective spatial range of centimetres reported for emitted risk compounds between terrestrial plants [25], for instance, suggests that plant-plant signalling should occur among specimens dwelling in close proximity. Consequently, plant-plant communication and eavesdropping should be more likely in species that establish dense mono-specific and mixed stands, respectively, than in species with isolated, remotely growing individuals. The ability to respond to risk cues may additionally be affected by the period of shared history. Ancient sympatry has been reported to result in more appropriate host counter-adaptations than is the case with recent sympatry [26]. Thus, the duration of sympatry should be particularly important in driving interactions between native and non-indigenous species. Knowledge of predator-prey interactions suggests that the ability of native gastropods responding to risk cues of a non-indigenous predatory crab manifested in a range of months to decades [27], [28]. Hence, establishment of the densely growing, non-indigenous brown seaweed Sargassum muticum along European shores since the 1980s may constitute a sufficiently long period of shared history to render plant-plant signalling possible for both native seaweeds and S. muticum. To explore this assumption, bio-assayed induction experiments were conducted in which S. muticum from two European shores and two respective local native seaweed species were exposed to grazing by a local isopod species. These experiments tested whether induced chemical resistance to herbivory was mediated by water-borne cues (i) between con-specific native seaweeds as well as S. muticum (intra-specific signalling), (ii) between S. muticum and native seaweeds (inter-specific signalling), and (iii) whether such responses show geographic variation.

Results

Intra-specific Signalling

Southern site.

Consumption by the isopod Stenosoma nadejda was not significantly different between pieces of S. muticum positioned downstream of grazed and ungrazed con-specifics in both bioassays using fresh and reconstituted S. muticum (fresh t7 = 1.40, p = 0.205; reconstituted t7 = 0.47, p = 0.653, Fig. 1A). Similarly, the palatability of fresh and reconstituted pieces of Cystoseira humilis located downstream of grazed con-specifics was not significantly different from the palatability of pieces located downstream of ungrazed C. humilis (fresh t7 = 1.41, p = 0.203; reconstituted t7 = 0.57, p = 0.589, Fig. 1A). In contrast, pieces of fresh Fucus spiralis located downstream of grazed con-specifics were on average 52% less palatable than pieces positioned downstream of ungrazed con-specifics (t7 = 2.98, p = 0.021, Fig. 1A). This pattern was also apparent in assays using reconstituted pieces of F. spiralis (t7 = 3.46, p = 0.011, Fig. 1A).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Effects of waterborne cues on conspecific seaweeds.

Mean consumption (±SE) of fresh and reconstituted seaweed pieces by the isopod Stenosoma nadejda from the southern site (A) and Idotea baltica from the northern site (B) in two-choice feeding assays. The seaweed species Sargassum muticum (Smut), Cystoseira humilis (Chum), Halidrys siliquosa (Hsil), Fucus spiralis (Fspi) and F. vesiculosus (Fves) used in the assays were positioned downstream of grazed ( = cue-exposed) and ungrazed ( = control) con-specifics in the induction phase. Negative values in consumption of reconstituted food indicate lower losses in mass by consumption than increases in mass by non-consumptive effects, e.g. absorption of water. Almost identical consistency and same volume of reconstituted food will result in same magnitude of non-consumptive effects on wet mass change during assays and allow interpretation of preferences between treated and control pieces. Asterisks show significant differences in consumption between cue-exposed and control feed (n = 8). Note different scaling of ordinate between southern and northern sites.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038804.g001

Northern site.

The isopod Idotea baltica consumed fresh as well as reconstituted pieces of S. muticum located downstream of grazed and ungrazed con-specifics in equivalent amounts (fresh t7 = −2.18, p = 0.066; reconstituted t7 = 0.61, p = 0.563, Fig. 1B). Likewise, I. baltica showed no significant preference for either fresh or reconstituted pieces made of Halidrys siliquosa that were located downstream of ungrazed con-specifics over H. siliquosa pieces located downstream of grazed con-specifics (fresh t7 = 1.96, p = 0.091; reconstituted t7 = 0.30, p = 0.977, Fig. 1B). Fresh pieces made of F. vesiculosus positioned downstream of ungrazed con-specifics were also not significantly more palatable to I. baltica than pieces located downstream of grazed con-specifics (t7 = 1.00, p = 0.349, Fig. 1B). There was, however, a significant preference for ungrazed F. vesiculosus by 47% in corresponding reconstituted food assays (t7 = 3.54, p = 0.009, Fig. 1B).

Inter-specific Signalling

Southern site.

S. nadejda showed no significant preference, either in bioassays with fresh or with reconstituted S. muticum, between S. muticum pieces located in the induction phase downstream of grazed and ungrazed F. spiralis (fresh t7 = 0.06, p = 0.958; reconstituted t7 = 1.77, p = 0.121, Fig. 2A). Likewise, the consumption of S. muticum kept downstream of grazed C. humilis pieces was not significantly different from that of S. muticum pieces located downstream of ungrazed C. humilis (fresh t7 = −1.73, p = 0.127; reconstituted t7 = 0.47, p = 0.656, Fig. 2A). In contrast, fresh and reconstituted pieces of C. humilis were both significantly more palatable in bioassays when positioned downstream of ungrazed S. muticum in the induction phase than when positioned downstream of grazed S. muticum (fresh t7 = 3.40, p = 0.011; reconstituted t7 = 2.99, p = 0.020, Fig. 2A). Similarly, fresh C. humilis pieces that were positioned downstream of ungrazed F. spiralis in the induction phase were consumed in bioassays significantly, i.e. 2 times, more on average than C. humilis pieces located downstream of grazed F. spiralis (t7 = 2.56, p = 0.037, Fig. 2A). This effect was considerably more pronounced in assays using reconstituted C. humilis pieces (t7 = 3.65, p = 0.008, Fig. 2A). Isopods significantly preferred fresh pieces and reconstituted food made of F. spiralis pieces that were positioned downstream of ungrazed C. humilis pieces by 2.4- and 2.5-fold, respectively, on average compared to F. spiralis that was positioned downstream of grazed C. humilis (fresh t7 = 2.73, p = 0.029; reconstituted t7 = 3.14, p = 0.016, Fig. 2A). Yet, isopods showed no preference between pieces of F. spiralis that were located downstream of grazed and ungrazed pieces of S. muticum (fresh t7 = −1.82, p = 0.112; reconstituted t7 = −0.55, p = 0.602, Fig. 2A).

thumbnail
Figure 2. Effects of waterborne cues on heterospecific seaweeds.

Mean consumption (±SE) of fresh and reconstituted seaweed pieces by the isopod Stenosoma nadejda from the southern site (A) and Idotea baltica from the northern site (B) in two-choice feeding assays. The seaweed pieces used in the assays were positioned downstream of grazed ( = cue-exposed) and ungrazed ( = control) hetero-specifics in the induction phase. ‘Up’ and ‘down’ indicate the position of the seaweed species in aquaria during the induction phase. Note different scaling of ordinate between southern and northern sites (n = 8). Interpretation of symbols, abbreviations, and explanation of negative values in assays using reconstituted food as in Fig. 1.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038804.g002

Northern site.

Isopods consumed H. siliquosa pieces that were positioned downstream of ungrazed S. muticum significantly, i.e. 2.5 (fresh pieces) and 1.8 times (reconstituted food), more than those located downstream of grazed S. muticum (fresh t7 = 2.92, p = 0.023; reconstituted t7 = 3.13, p = 0.017, Fig. 2B). Yet, in all other assays isopods showed no significant feeding preferences (all t7<0.91, p>0.157, Fig. 2B).

Discussion

Both Fucus species reduced their palatability when nearby con-specifics were grazed by isopods. In contrast, the palatability of the non-indigenous brown seaweed S. muticum did not change either when located downstream of grazed con-specific or of native hetero-specific brown seaweeds. Furthermore, both con-familiar, but neither hetero-familiar native seaweed species reduced palatability when located downstream of grazed S. muticum. These patterns were observed at both European shores and were almost identical between assays using fresh and reconstituted seaweeds. Although the nature of the water-borne risk cues mediating trophic interactions between grazers and seaweeds is still unknown, several other studies working on seaweed-seaweed interactions assumed water-borne cues to evolve as signals in aquatic environments [18], [29], [30].

Intra-specific Signalling

The higher palatability of reconstituted food made of F. vesiculosus positioned downstream of ungrazed than of I. baltica-grazed con-specifics suggests an induction of chemical rather than structural anti-herbivory defences that was mediated by water-borne cues. This finding corroborates Rohde et al. [31], who additionally confirmed, like others, but in contrast to our study, this ability of F. vesiculosus with assays using fresh specimens [29]. We think that this mismatch between results of fresh and reconstituted assays, which was the only case in the entire study, may not represent an artefact from processing reconstituted food as this should have resulted in a higher frequency of mismatches. A single mismatch out of a total number of 18 assays is, however, likely due to chance alone (18×0.05 = 0.9). Moreover, this study documents for the first time that water-borne cues emitted from nearby grazed con-specifics may induce anti-herbivory defences in F. spiralis. At present, experimental evidence on plant-plant signalling is scarce for both vascular plants [19] and seaweeds. There are, for instance, <10 examples from seaweeds, all including species of the families Fucaceae (Ascophyllum nodosum: [18]; F. vesiculosus and F. spiralis: e.g. both this study) and Dictyotacea [30]. Theory suggests [9] and empirical data [23] support an advantage for species with an ability to use plant-plant signalling since risk cues permit early detection of and priming against future grazer attacks. This may intensify and/or accelerate induction of anti-herbivory defences and hence shorten the period of vulnerability [19], [22].

Grazing by isopods reduced palatability in nearby ungrazed con-specifics in only one of the three seaweed species tested at each site. This suggests that intra-specific signalling by water-borne cues may depend on seaweed species. This species specificity may probably be due to a distance-dependent efficacy of risk cues. Defoliated alder (Alnus glutinosa), for instance, affected resistance expression only in con-specifics growing at a distance of a few metres [32]. This suggests that the function of risk cues may be dose-dependent [19]. Consequently, plant species with an aggregated distribution of specimens should benefit more from risk cue signalling than species with a scattered distribution of isolated specimens. Feeding preferences of isopods from both study sites confirmed this assumed pattern for most species tested in this study. On the one hand, both cue-sensitive species occur at the study sites either in dense stands (F. spiralis) or show a clumped distribution (F. vesiculosus) in which neighbours thrive in immediate proximity. On the other hand, responses to water-borne cues emitted from nearby grazed con-specifics were lacking in S. muticum and C. humilis, despite their occurrence in dense stands at the study sites. At least two explanations for this observed cue-insensitivity seem possible. First, both species probably counter grazing losses by compensatory growth. Growth rates of up to 46 and 80 cm per month in spring, i.e. at times when grazer density increases, have been reported for S. muticum at the southern (unpubli. data AE Engelen) and northern study site [33], respectively. Moreover, growth rates are similar in both seaweed species, along with palatability to the isopod S. nadeja [34]. Second, both species may use constitutive defences, although fast growing plants are predicted to invest less in constitutive defences than slower-growing plants [22]. Moreover, in this study S. muticum was consumed at least as much as undefended seaweeds, i.e. controls of Fucus species. Finally, adjacent specimens of the cue-insensitive species at the north European site, H. siliquosa, are separated by >3 m and persist in areas where S. muticum forms dense stands [33]. The minimum distance between adjacent isolated con-specific individuals was larger than the reported range at which risk cues are functional around emitting individuals, e.g. 60 cm in sagebrush [35]. Future tests on spatial limitations and dose dependency in the efficacy of water-borne cues are needed to clarify distance-dependence of risk cues.

Inter-specific Signalling

The palatability of fresh and reconstituted pieces of C. humilis was lower downstream of grazed than ungrazed S. muticum. This suggests an induction of chemical anti-herbivory defences in a hetero-specific seaweed in response to water-borne cues emitted by S. muticum. However, because the palatability of S. muticum did not change significantly when located downstream of either grazed con-specifics or grazed C. humilis, there was no reciprocal benefit for S. muticum at the southern European site. Similarly, the palatability of H. siliquosa at the northern European site was reduced when located downstream of S. muticum, with no reciprocal beneficial effects. These results suggest specificity of inducible effects, which has been already documented, for instance, in the induction of anti-herbivory defences of directly grazed vascular plants and seaweeds [15], [36].

Responses by hetero-specific native seaweeds to signals emitted by non-indigenous S. muticum represent to our knowledge the first report on inter-specific signalling in seaweeds. Due to the experimental set-up, i.e. uni-directional flow of seawater, downstream located cue-receiving seaweeds were able to perceive signals from upstream located cue-emitting seaweeds but not vice versa. Thus, results suggest that S. muticum was eavesdropped by native seaweeds under experimental conditions, but this may be inapplicable under natural conditions. The eavesdropping aspect is, however, supported by missing reciprocal effects in this study, i.e. no change in S. muticum palatability when it was located downstream of native species. Experimental evidence on eavesdropping also exists for terrestrial plants [37], [38]. Eavesdropping should be especially advantageous for isolated specimens that are surrounded by dense emitter stands associated with generalist consumers. Both isopod species used in this study can be characterised as generalists on the respective shores since they displayed similar consumption rates between S. muticum and native seaweed species, including F. spiralis, C. humilis, and F. vesiculosus [34], [39]. Hence, consumption of S. muticum by isopods should provide a reliable indication of the future risk of grazing by isopods for the adjacently growing native seaweeds used in this study and vice versa. However, this study offers no evidence that S. muticum was able to respond to inter-specific signals of any of the tested native seaweed species. The short period for co-evolution does not seem to explain cue immunity by S. muticum as some native species gained the ability to respond to signals emitted by S. muticum within the same period. It is more likely that S. muticum principally refrains from using water-borne cues to induce defences against its consumers. This explanation is supported by the unchanged palatability of S. muticum in the intra-specific signalling experiment and the possibility that this fast-growing species compensates grazing losses with growth (see above discussion).

At both sites, only non-Fucus native seaweed species responded to signals emitted by isopod-grazed S. muticum. The specificity of inter-specific signalling between S. muticum and neighbouring, native seaweeds corroborates results by another study [40], which tested several forbs growing near sagebrush. The fact that both Fucus species lowered their palatability when positioned downstream of grazed con-specifics clearly indicates that lack of plant-plant signalling between S. muticum and Fucus species was not due to general insusceptibility to water-borne cues of the latter. Differences in cue concentration between the intra-specific and inter-specific signalling experiment seem unlikely to explain this differential ability of native seaweed species to respond to S. muticum signals, as consumption rates of upstream located C. humilis as well as H. siliquosa were not significantly different between both experiments. Several other explanations, however, seem possible. First, patterns of plant-plant signalling may be explained by the spatial distances between seaweeds in the field. At both sites S. muticum and the cue-sensitive native species (C. humilis and H. siliquosa) exist in immediate proximity, while cue-insensitive natives grow at a distance of metres (F. spiralis, southern site) to tens of metres (F. vesiculosus, northern site) apart. Similarly, results from vascular plants indicate that the efficacy of risk cues was negatively correlated with distance between emitter and receiver [32]. Where specimens of one species dwell scattered within dense stands of a second species the former will probably encounter a reliable cue regime, at least of generalist consumers, as these will consume specimens of both seaweeds. This would explain why H. siliquosa in our study responded to signals emitted by S. muticum but not to potential cues from more distantly located conspecifics. Second, different exposure intensity to S. muticum cues might have occurred between native seaweeds. While cue-sensitive species cohabit with S. muticum in permanently submersed habitats, i.e. tide pools and the shallow subtidal, cue-immune species inhabit the intertidal. Consequently and in contrast to cue-sensitive species, both cue-immune species were disconnected from S. muticum signals during low tide. Assuming that cue efficacy is dose-dependent, contact-free periods might provoke invalidity of S. muticum cues for intertidal species as induction models suggest that evolution of inducible responses is dependent on cue reliability [9]. Finally, results from the northern but not from the southern European site allow speculating that relatedness and the ability to plant-plant signalling may correlate positively because water-borne cues only affected seaweed palatability in assays using con-specific and con-familiar species. At present it is unknown whether close relatives are more effective communicators than genetically less similar plants [22]. As the number of seaweed species included in our study was too small to draw resilient conclusions about this relationship, more comparisons are needed to clarify whether the ability of inter-specific signalling is kinship-dependent.

This study is to our knowledge the first documentation of inter-specific signalling in seaweeds. The unidirectional ability of plant-plant signalling between native species and S. muticum, through which only anti-herbivory responses of the former are tailored, provides an example where a non-indigenous species may benefit native species. Increasing, knowledge about plant-plant signalling may elucidate additional and effective drivers that structure communities, will lead to a more comprehensive understanding on, and improved prediction capacities of the ecology of communities for the present and in a changing world.

thumbnail
Table 1. Average (±SD) wet mass of apical pieces of seaweeds used in intra-specific signalling experiments at the southern (Portugal) and northern (Germany) European study sites.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038804.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Average (±SD) wet mass of apical pieces of seaweeds used in inter-specific signalling experiments at the southern (Portugal) and northern (Germany) European study sites.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038804.t002

Materials and Methods

Collection Sites and Organisms

Three seaweed and one herbivore species each were collected at two NE Atlantic sites located >2000 km apart: (i) Praia de Queimado, SW Portugal (37° 49' N, 8° 47' W, Southern Europe) and (ii) Nordwatt, Helgoland, Germany (54° 11' N, 7° 52' E, Northern Europe). At both sites the non-indigenous wireweed (S. muticum) was collected together with one con-familiar (Portugal: C. humilis, Germany: H. siliquosa) and one hetero-familiar (Portugal: F. spiralis, Germany: F. vesiculosus) seaweed species. The southern European site is located on a wave-sheltered shore with semidiurnal tides having a range of 3 m. Average (±SE) seawater temperature at this fully marine site was 16.9 (±0.5)°C during the study period. Dense stands of perennial brown seaweeds, including S. muticum were located inside and along rock pool edges. S. muticum was first observed at the southern European site in 2002 [41], where it co-occurs with C. humilis inside rock pools, while F. spiralis grows outside rock pools on emergent rock. The isopod S. nadejda was collected from rock pools, where this seaweed-associated mesograzer is abundant [42]. The northern European site, Nordwatt, is a semi-exposed intertidal rocky shore on Helgoland, Germany. The shore is characterised by large sandstone terraces and semidiurnal tides having an average range of 2.35 m. Seawater temperature was, on average, 18.30 (±1.99)°C during the experiments. Benthic assemblages were dominated by perennial brown seaweeds, such as Fucus species in the intertidal and Laminaria species in the subtidal. The pseudo-perennial S. muticum was first encountered on Helgoland in 1989 [43]. Around May S. muticum quickly develops dense stands in the sheltered shallow subtidal (<3 m), interspersed by the perennial sea oak (H. siliquosa). In contrast, the perennial bladder wrack (F. vesiculosus) inhabits the mid intertidal, i.e. up to tens of metres apart from subtidal habitats where the two other species dwell. The isopod I. baltica used as the grazer was collected around Helgoland from Fucus spp. and S. muticum.

thumbnail
Figure 3. Schematic experimental set-up (displayed for one replicate).

Induction of anti-herbivore defences in non-damaged seaweeds was tested in response to water-borne cues from nearby grazed con-specifics (A) and hetero-specifics (B). Aquaria (large rectangles) were supplied by a unidirectional flow of seawater (indicated by arrow) and were divided by a net (dashed line) into upstream and downstream compartments. Small and large pentagons designate seaweed pieces serving as donors and recipients of water-borne cues, respectively. Seaweed species are differentiated by the colours of the pentagons. After 4 d of acclimatisation, grazers were added to treatment aquaria (solid line) and absent in control aquaria (dotted line) during the subsequent 12 d induction phase. Numbers and letters indicate different use of downstream treatment and control seaweed pieces, respectively, in fresh ( = 1 or a) and reconstituted ( = 2 or b) feeding assays, and as autogenic controls ( = 3 or c). Circles with solid and stippled lines illustrate feeding arenas with and without grazers ( = autogenic controls), respectively, in which fresh or reconstituted (quadrates) pieces of seaweed were offered. For clarity, feeding arenas were only shown for intra-specific communication experiments.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038804.g003

Experimental Design and Set-up

To assess the generality of induced seaweed responses by water-borne cues, experiments were run with organisms from both the southern and northern European site. At each site two experiments (n = 8) were conducted, each divided into three sequential phases: acclimation (4 d), induction (12 d), and bio-assay (3 d). The first experiment started on 7 April and 30 June 2007 at the southern and northern site, respectively, testing whether water-borne cues from grazed seaweeds induce anti-herbivory responses in con-specifics ( = intra-specific signalling). The second experiment assessed whether water-borne cues from grazed seaweeds induce anti-herbivory defences in hetero-specifics ( = inter-specific signalling). Due to limited laboratory space, the inter-specific signalling experiment was divided into three consecutive sub-experiments (57 d total experimental period), starting on 1 May and 2 July 2007 at the southern and northern site, respectively.

The day the experiments started, up to 400 isopods and 8 specimens of each seaweed species were collected and transported to the laboratory within 1 h. Due to the small size of F. spiralis, 24 to 36 specimens were needed to obtain the full number of seaweed pieces in experiments. In the laboratory macroscopic epibionts were gently removed from all seaweeds with a sponge to minimize confounding effects from e.g. ‘co-consumption’ or ‘protective coating’ [44]. Then all apical pieces (Table 1 & 2) needed for one replicate were cut from one specimen (except F. spiralis, where only 2 pieces were cut from each specimen) and individually marked with coloured threads. The pieces of each specimen (multiple F. spiralis individuals) were allocated to transparent plastic aquaria (Fig. 3, detailed description below), containing 2 L (southern site) or 8 L (northern site) of seawater. Each aquarium was divided with a plastic mesh (1 mm mesh size) into equally sized upstream and downstream compartments. The aquaria were individually supplied with a unidirectional flow of cotton-filtered seawater from the nearby sea at an average flow rate of 120 (southern site) or 300 ml min−1 (northern site). These differences in set-ups were due to technical constrains but did not seem to interfere with results obtained at each site, though we cannot rule out any undetected differences. Generated flow-rates represent, however, intermediate levels of dilution compared to other studies testing the effects of water-borne cues on seaweed palatability [18], [31], [45]. Fluorescent lamps (58 W Osram at southern site, 36 W Philips at northern site) irradiated aquaria in a 12 h:12 h light-dark cycle with, on average, 1210 lux (PeakTech light meter 5025) at the southern and 34.4±2.5 µmol−2 s−1 (LI-COR broadband sensor UWQ 8534) at the northern site. Irradiance in the laboratory simulated ambient PAR levels at 1 m water depth during the time when experiments were conducted.

For intra-specific signalling experiments, 12 pieces were cut from each seaweed specimen (6 F. spiralis specimens) and equally allocated to the upstream and downstream compartments of one control and one treatment aquarium, i.e. 3 pieces in each compartment (Fig. 3A), resulting in a total of 48 aquaria (2 treatments x 3 species x 8 replicates) at each site. In inter-specific signalling experiments, 12 pieces were cut from one specimen of (i) S. muticum, (ii) H. siliquosa or C. humilis, and (iii) F. vesiculosus or 6 F. spiralis individuals in the first, second and third sub-experiment, respectively. These pieces were equally allocated to the upstream compartment of four aquaria ( = donor pieces). In addition, 6 pieces were cut from each con- or hetero-familiar specimen and were equally allocated to the downstream compartment ( = recipient pieces) of 2 aquaria (Fig. 3B). This resulted in a total of 96 aquaria (2 grazing treatments x 2 recipient species x 3 donor species x 8 replicates) at each site. Although inter-specific signalling sub-experiments were conducted at slightly different times, each species performed comparably in the sub-experiments, as indicated by insignificant differences in growth rates of pieces of the same seaweed species between sub-experiments (Student’s t-test: all t7<1.931, p>0.05). Following their allocation to aquaria, the seaweed pieces remained there without grazers during the next 4 days (acclimation phase). This allowed seaweeds to acclimate to laboratory conditions and to reduce putative induced anti-herbivory defences acquired during their unknown grazing history in the field, as shown for F. vesiculosus and/or recover from changes in palatability that might have occurred in response to cutting [46]. Cutting, however, did not alter the palatability of F. vesiculosus [31] and other species [47] to isopods. Moreover, prior to their use in experiments all grazers were kept on a mixed algal diet (excluding seaweed species used in the induction phase) in a separate aerated 50 L container with seawater flow-through. On day 5, i.e. at the beginning of the induction phase, 4 isopods were added to the upstream compartment of a randomly selected half of aquaria of each treatment in both intra-specific and inter-specific signalling experiments. The remaining 8 aquaria of each treatment were kept without grazers ( = controls, Fig. 3). Selected grazer density matched naturally observed levels of isopod density on F. vesiculosus and S. muticum at Helgoland [48], as well as on S. muticum and C. humilis in Portugal (unpubl. data AE Engelen). Survival of isopods was recorded at least twice daily and, when appropriate, dead isopods were replaced by live con-specifics. After 12 days, the induction phase was terminated by removing isopods from the set-up and using seaweed pieces from downstream compartments in two-choice feeding assays. Previous studies demonstrated that 10 to 14 days of I. baltica grazing are needed to lower F. vesiculosus palatability [49], [50]. Extending the period of I. baltica grazing had been shown to cause fluctuations in F. vesiculosus palatability but did not increase the efficacy of anti-herbivory responses (unpubl. data CR Flöthe).

Two types of feeding assays tested whether the palatability of seaweed pieces positioned downstream of grazed con- or hetero-specifics ( = treated) was lower than that of pieces positioned downstream of non-grazed con- or hetero-specifics ( = control). The first type of feeding assay used fresh seaweed pieces, testing for the induction of morphological and/or chemical anti-herbivory defences. Thus, two seaweed pieces were taken from each downstream compartment of one treatment and control aquarium to which pieces originating from the same specimen, i.e. genetically identical pieces, had been allocated at the beginning of the experiments. The remaining third piece from each downstream compartment was stored at −80°C and used later in reconstituted food assays (see below). Subsequently, algal pieces were spun 10 times in a salad spinner, blotted with paper towels for 15 sec, and weighed separately on a balance (Sartorius LE323S, Germany) to the nearest 0.001 g before transferring one treated and one control piece to each of two feeding arenas ( = 200 mL glass Petri dish, experimental unit = EU). The assay started after adding 2 naïve isopods to one feeding arena. No grazers were added to the second feeding arena to assess non-feeding related ( = autogenic) changes in wet mass of fresh algae in the first feeding arena during the assay (Fig. 3). Seawater in feeding arenas was exchanged twice daily to reduce artefacts on grazer consumption, e.g. waste products accumulating in feeding arenas. At the end of 3 d feeding assays, each algal piece was reweighed following the above description. Isopod consumption of treated and control pieces was estimated as: Bstart x (Aend/Astart) – Bend, where Bstart and Bend represent the initial and final wet mass of an assayed piece, respectively, and Astart and Aend represent the initial and final mass of the autogenic control piece, respectively [51]. A significantly higher consumption of control than of treated algal pieces was interpreted as an induction of anti-herbivore defences.

The second type of feeding assays used reconstituted food, testing for the induction of chemical anti-herbivory defences. After the induction phase, seaweed pieces that were stored at −80°C, were freeze-dried, ground to a homogenous fine powder, and 0.2 g of this powder were suspended in 1 ml of distilled water. This algal suspension was mixed with molten agar (0.043 g in 1.2 ml of distilled water) after the agar had cooled to 55°C, poured over a mosquito net (1 mm2 mesh size), and flattened between two glass plates [52]. After solidification, pellets of 15×15 mm2 were cut from algae-agar mixtures and marked with different incision patterns to distinguish between control and treated pellets. The control and treated pellet originating from the same seaweed specimen were transferred to one feeding arena (Fig. 3). Assays started after adding 2 naïve isopods and were terminated 36 h later. Prior to weighing, excessive water was removed from each pellet by blotting pellets with paper towels for 5 sec. Special care was taken to ensure that no pellet material was removed during blotting. Set-up and all other conditions were identical to assays using fresh seaweed pieces. Isopod consumption was calculated as the difference in wet mass of a food pellet between start and end of a feeding assay.

Statistical Analysis

Differences in isopod consumption between treated and control pieces were analysed with paired t-tests after confirming normal distribution of differences between the consumption of treated and control pieces with a Kolmogornov-Smirnov test. Despite the large number of paired t-tests (i.e. 18), sequential Bonferroni adjustments were not carried out because χ2 tests indicated that the number of observed significant paired t-tests in assays with fresh (i.e. 5) or reconstituted food (i.e. 6) was significantly different from what could be expected by chance (i.e. 18×0.05 = 0.9; fresh food: χ2 = 16.94, p<0.0001, reconstituted food: χ2 = 26.47 p<0.0001).

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Andreas Wagner, Michael Janke, João Reis, and Miguel Viegas for their technical support. We appreciate the field assistance provided by Inês Paixão and Abraham Pérez Pastor. We thank anonymous reviewers for improving the manuscript. We are grateful for language check by Gareth Pearson.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: HYY AHE RS MM. Performed the experiments: HYY. Analyzed the data: HYY MM. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: AHE MM. Wrote the paper: HYY AHE MM.

References

  1. 1. Hay ME (1991) Marine-terrestrial contrasts in the ecology of plant chemical defenses against herbivores. Trends Ecol Evol 6: 362–365.
  2. 2. Cyr H, Pace ML (1993) Magnitude and patterns of herbivory in aquatic and terrestrial ecosystems. Nature 361: 148–150.
  3. 3. Wittstock U, Gershenzon J (2002) Constitutive plant toxins and their role in defense against herbivores and pathogens. Curr Opin Plant Biol 5: 300–307.
  4. 4. Steneck RS, Graham MH, Bourque BJ, Corbett D, Erlandson JM, et al. (2002) Kelp forest ecosystems: biodiversity, stability, resilience and future. Environ Conserv 29: 436–459.
  5. 5. Cronin G (2001) Resource allocation in seaweeds and marine invertebrates: chemical defense patterns in relation to defense theories. In: McClintock JB, Baker BJ, editors. pp. 325–353. New York: CRC Press.
  6. 6. Hay ME (1996) Marine chemical ecology: What’s known and what’s next? J Exp Mar Biol Ecol 200: 103–134.
  7. 7. Ohgushi T (2005) Indirect interaction webs: herbivore-induced effects through trait change in plants. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 36: 81–105.
  8. 8. Adler FR, Karban R (1994) Defended fortresses or moving targets? Another model of inducible defences inspired by military metaphors. Am Nat 144: 813–832.
  9. 9. Karban R, Nagasaka K (2004) Are defenses of wild radish populations well matched with variability and predictablity of herbivory? Evol Ecol 18: 283–301.
  10. 10. Karban R, Baldwin IT (1997) Induced Responses to Herbivory. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
  11. 11. Tollrian R, Harvell CD (1999) The ecology and evolution of inducible defenses. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
  12. 12. Karban R, Agrawal AA, Thaler JS, Adler LS (1999) Induced plant responses and information content about risk of herbivory. Trends Ecol Evol 14: 443–447.
  13. 13. Toth GB, Pavia H (2007) Induced herbivore resistance in seaweeds: a meta-analysis. J Ecol 95: 425–434.
  14. 14. Long JD, Hamilton RS, Mitchell JL (2007) Asymmetric competition via induced resistance: specialist herbivores indirectly suppress generalist preference and populations. Ecology 88: 1232–1240.
  15. 15. Yun HY, Rohde S, Linnane K, Wahl M, Molis M (2010) Seaweed-mediated indirect interaction between two species of meso-herbivores. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 408: 47–53.
  16. 16. Van Zandt PA, Agrawal AA (2004) Community-wide impacts of herbivore-induced plant responses in milkweed (Asclepias syriaca). Ecology 85: 2616–2629.
  17. 17. Brawley SH (1992) Mesoherbivores. In: John DM, Hawkins SJ, Price JH, editors. pp. 235–263. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
  18. 18. Toth GB, Pavia H (2000) Water-borne cues induce chemical defense in a marine alga (Ascophyllum nodosum). P Natl Acad Sci USA 97: 14418–14420.
  19. 19. Heil M, Karban R (2010) Explaining evolution of plant communication by airborne signals. Trends Ecol Evol 25: 137–144.
  20. 20. Wiesemeier T, Hay M, Pohnert G (2007) The potential role of wound-activated volatile release in the chemical defence of the brown alga Dictyota dichotoma: blend recognition by marine herbivores. Aquat Sci 69: 403–412.
  21. 21. Malin G, C KF, Carpenter L, Baker A, Broadgate W, et al. (2001) Trace gas production by seaweeds: defense, oxidative stress, signalling and atmospheric significance. J Phycol 37: 32–33.
  22. 22. Karban R (2011) The ecology and evolution of induced resistance against herbivores. Funct Ecol 25: 339–347.
  23. 23. Engelberth J, Alborn HT, E.A S, Tumlinson JH (2004) Airborne signals prime plants aaginst insect herbivore attack. P Natl Acad Sci USA 101: 1781–1785.
  24. 24. Hay ME, Parker JD, Burkepile DE, Caudill CC, Wilson AE, et al. (2004) Mutualisms and aquatic community structure: the enemy of my enemy is my friend. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 35: 175–197.
  25. 25. Karban R, Maron J, Felton GW, Ervin G, Eichenseer H (2003) Herbivore damage to sagebrush induces resistance in wild tobacco: evidence for eavesdropping between plants. Oikos 100: 325–332.
  26. 26. Soler M, Møller AP (1990) Duration of symatry and coevolution between the great spotted cuckoo and its magpie host. Nature 343: 748–750.
  27. 27. Seeley RH (1986) Intense natural selection caused a rapid morphological transition in a living marine snail. P Natl Acad Sci USA 83: 689–6901.
  28. 28. Vermeij GJ (1982) Phenotypic evolution in a poorly dispersing snail after arrival of a predator. Nature 299: 349–350.
  29. 29. Haavisto F, Välikangas T, Jormalainen V (2010) Induced resistance in a brown alga: phlorotannins, genotypic variation and fitness costs for the crustacean herbivore. Oecologia 162: 685–695.
  30. 30. Macaya EC, Rothausler E, Thiel M, Molis M, Wahl M (2005) Induction of defenses and within-alga variation of palatability in two brown algae from the northern-central coast of Chile: Effects of mesograzers and UV radiation. J Exp Mar Biol Ecol 325: 214–227.
  31. 31. Rohde S, Molis M, Wahl M (2004) Regulation of anti-herbivore defence by Fucus vesiculosus in response to various cues. J Ecol 92: 1011–1018.
  32. 32. Dolch R, Tscharntke T (2000) Defoliation of alders (Alnus glutinosa) affects herbivory by leaf beetles on undamaged neighbours. Oecologia 125: 504–511.
  33. 33. Domisch S (2008) [Master thesis]. Oldenburg: Carl von Ossietzky Universität. 109 p.
  34. 34. Engelen AH, Henriques N, Monteiro C, Santos R (2011) Mesograzers prefer mostly native seaweeds over the invasive brown seaweed Sargassum muticum. Hydrobiologia 669: 157–165.
  35. 35. Karban R, Shiojiri K, Huntzinger M, McCall AC (2006) Damage-induced resistance in sagebrush: volatiles are key to intra- and interplant communications. Ecology 87: 922–930.
  36. 36. Agrawal AA (2000) Specificity of induced resistance in wild radish: causes and consequences for two specialist and two generalist caterpillars. Oikos 89: 493–500.
  37. 37. Karban R, Baldwin IT, Baxter KJ, Laue G, Felton GW (2000) Communication between plants: induced resistance in wild tobacco plants following clipping of neighboring sagebrush. Oecologia 125: 66–71.
  38. 38. Glinwood R, Ninkovic V, Pettersson J, Ahmed E (2004) Barley exposed to aerial allelopathy from thistles (Cirsium spp.) becomes less acceptable to aphids. Ecol Entomol 29: 188–195.
  39. 39. Vandendriessche S, De Keersmaecker G, Vincx M, Degraer S (2006) Food and habitat choice in floating seaweed clumps: the obligate opportunistic nature of the associated macrofauna. Mar Biol 149: 1499–1507.
  40. 40. Karban R, Huntzinger M, McCall AC (2004) The specificity of eavesdropping on sagebrush by other plants. Ecology 85: 1846–1852.
  41. 41. Engelen A, Santos R (2009) Which demographic traits determine population growth in the invasive brown seaweed Sargassum muticum? J Ecol 97: 675–684.
  42. 42. Xavier R, Santos AM, Lima FP, Branco M (2009) Invasion or invisibility: using genetic and distributional data to investigate the alien or indigenous status of the Atlantic populations of the peracarid isopod, Stenosoma nadejda (Rezig 1989). Mol Ecol 18: 3283–3290.
  43. 43. Kornmann P, Sahling P (1994) Meeresalgen von Helgoland: Zweite Ergänzung. Helg Mar Res 48: 365–406.
  44. 44. Karez R, Engelbert S, Sommer U (2000) ‘Co-consumption’ and ‘protective coating’: two new proposed effects of epiphytes on their macroalgal hosts in mesograzer-epiphyte-host interactions. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 205: 85–93.
  45. 45. Sotka EE, Taylor RB, Hay ME (2002) Tissue-specific induction of resistance to herbivores in a brown seaweed: the importance of direct grazing versus waterborne signals from grazed neighbors. J Exp Mar Biol Ecol 227: 1–12.
  46. 46. Rohde S, Wahl M (2008) Temporal dynamics of induced resistance in a marine macroalga: time lag of induction and reduction in Fucus vesiculosus. J Exp Mar Biol Ecol 367: 227–229.
  47. 47. Pavia H, Toth GB (2000) Inducible chemical resistance to herbivory in the brown seaweed Ascophyllum nodosum. Ecology 81: 3212–3225.
  48. 48. Buschbaum C, Chapman AS, Saier B (2006) How an introduced seaweed can affect epibiota diversity in different coastal systems. Mar Biol 148: 743–754.
  49. 49. Hemmi A, Honkanen T, Jormalainen V (2004) Inducible resistance to herbivory in Fucus vesiculosus - duration, spreading and variation with nutrient availability. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 273: 109–120.
  50. 50. Rohde S, Wahl M (2008) Antifeeding defense in baltic macroalgae: induction by direct grazing versus waterborne cues. J Phycol 44: 85–90.
  51. 51. Cronin G, Hay ME (1996) Susceptibility to herbivores depends on recent history of both the plant and animal. Ecology 77: 1531–1543.
  52. 52. Hay ME, Kappel QE, Fenical W (1994) Synergisms in plant defenses against herbivores: interactions of chemistry, calcification, and plant quality. Ecology 75: 1714–1726.