Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Kindness Counts: Prompting Prosocial Behavior in Preadolescents Boosts Peer Acceptance and Well-Being

  • Kristin Layous ,

    klayo001@ucr.edu

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside, California, United States of America

  • S. Katherine Nelson,

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside, California, United States of America

  • Eva Oberle,

    Affiliation Department of Educational and Counseling Psychology and Special Education, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

  • Kimberly A. Schonert-Reichl,

    Affiliation Department of Educational and Counseling Psychology and Special Education, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

  • Sonja Lyubomirsky

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside, California, United States of America

Abstract

At the top of parents’ many wishes is for their children to be happy, to be good, and to be well-liked. Our findings suggest that these goals may not only be compatible but also reciprocal. In a longitudinal experiment conducted in 19 classrooms in Vancouver, 9- to 11-year olds were instructed to perform three acts of kindness (versus visit three places) per week over the course of 4 weeks. Students in both conditions improved in well-being, but students who performed kind acts experienced significantly bigger increases in peer acceptance (or sociometric popularity) than students who visited places. Increasing peer acceptance is a critical goal, as it is related to a variety of important academic and social outcomes, including reduced likelihood of being bullied. Teachers and interventionists can build on this study by introducing intentional prosocial activities into classrooms and recommending that such activities be performed regularly and purposefully.

Introduction

At the top of parents’ many wishes is for their children to be happy, to be good, and to have positive relationships with others [1][2]. Fortunately, research suggests that goals for happiness, prosociality, and popularity may not only be compatible but also reciprocal. Happy people are more likely to engage in prosocial behavior [3][4] and have satisfying friendships [5]. Similarly, students who are well-liked by peers (i.e., sociometrically popular) are also helpful, cooperative, and emotionally well-adjusted [6][8]. Past studies indicate that the link between happiness and prosociality is bidirectional–not only do happy people have the personal resources to do good for others, but prompting people to engage in prosocial behavior also increases well-being [9][12]. Based on this prior research–which is predominantly cross-sectional–we predicted that prompting preadolescents to engage in prosocial behavior will boost not only their happiness but also their popularity.

To our knowledge, this study is the first longitudinal experimental intervention of prosocial behavior in preadolescents (“tweens”), and the first to link a manipulation of a simple helping behavior to increases in sociometric popularity (as assessed by peer reports). To explore whether doing good for others (versus engaging in a simple pleasant activity) over 4 weeks would simultaneously increase happiness and promote positive relationships with peers, we randomly assigned 9- to 11-year-olds either to perform acts of kindness (“kindness”) each week or to keep track of places they visited that week (“whereabouts”).

Although the efficacy of happiness-increasing strategies is better established in adults [13], some interventions have boosted well-being in children and adolescents by encouraging gratitude [14][15]. Prompting youth to engage in kind acts, however, may have benefits beyond personal happiness, as prosocial behavior predicts academic achievement and social acceptance in adolescents [16]. The dearth of work on enhancing happiness and prosociality in youth, coupled with evidence of their many benefits, highlights the desirability of extending research to this age group.

We predicted that committing kind acts (e.g., carrying groceries) and tracking whereabouts (e.g., visiting grandma’s house or the mall) would both be rewarding activities that would increase well-being in preadolescents. Indeed, the whereabouts task was designed to be a mildly pleasant and distracting control activity (for similar mood-boosting benefits of such activities, see [17][18]). For ethical and pragmatic reasons, we wanted to avoid potential harm or waste by not administering the types of “neutral” activities previously used as control tasks (e.g., listing daily hassles or writing essays), which preadolescents may find boring, pointless, or even unpleasant. We also wanted to include a mildly positive comparison group to rule out the possibility that doing kindness increases popularity merely because it feels good. Accordingly, we expected students who practice kind acts–an activity that promotes positive relationships–to experience increases in peer acceptance in addition to increases in well-being. Distinct from other animals, humans as young as 18 months eagerly engage in altruistic acts [19], suggesting that prosociality has a unique evolutionary advantage for human social behavior. Indeed, prosocial behavior has a strong positive association with later peer acceptance [16], and this relationship is likely bidirectional, as children who feel accepted are more likely to do things for others [20], and, in turn, children who do things for others might gain the acceptance of their peers. This latter path has not been studied experimentally. Increasing peer acceptance is a critical goal, as it is related to a variety of important academic [21] and social [22] outcomes, including reduced likelihood of being bullied [23].

Method

Consent forms describing the study were sent home with students and signed by their guardians. If students brought back a signed consent form, they were then given their own consent form to sign during the researchers’ first classroom visit. The student consent form was verbally explained to the students by the researchers and then students provided written consent. Consent from guardians and students were recorded on a class roster. Only if both guardian and student gave consent was the student given baseline measures. The consent procedure and all data collection were approved by the University of British Columbia’s Behavioral Research Ethics Board (H11-00271) and the Vancouver School Board Ethics Committee.

Nineteen classrooms in the Vancouver, BC school district were randomly assigned to one of two conditions in the second half of the school year. Every week over the course of 4 weeks, students (N = 415, Mage = 10.6), nested within classrooms, were instructed either to perform 3 acts of kindness (for anyone they wish) or visit 3 places (anywhere they wish). Throughout the 4-week intervention, students in both conditions reported what they did each week on in-class surveys. Examples of kind acts included “gave my mom a hug when she was stressed by her job,” “gave someone some of my lunch,” and “vacuumed the floor.” Examples of locations visited included “shopping centre,” “baseball diamond,” and “grandma’s house.” All students were told the study was about children’s experiences and emotions.

Before and after the intervention, students reported their life satisfaction (Satisfaction With Life Scale adapted for children; [24]), happiness (Subjective Happiness Scale adapted for children; [25]), and positive affect (child version of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule; [26]). In addition to the self-report measures, students were provided with a roster of their classmates and asked to circle students (fellow participants) who they “would like to be in school activities [i.e., spend time] with” (a measure of peer acceptance). Students were instructed that they could circle as many or as few names as they liked. At posttest, students were presented with a blank list of their classmates, so they made their new nominations from scratch. Because the study was conducted during the latter half of the school year, students in each classroom already knew each other and were relatively unlikely to continue to make new friends spontaneously. Pre-post changes in self-reports and peer nominations were analyzed using multilevel modeling to account for students’ nesting within classrooms. No baseline condition differences were found on any outcome variables. Further details about method and results are available from the first author.

Results

Consistent with previous research, overall, students in both the kindness and whereabouts groups showed significant increases in positive affect (γ00 = 0.15, S.E. = 0.04, t(17) = 3.66, p<.001) and marginally significant increases in life satisfaction (γ00 = 0.09, S.E. = 0.05, t(17) = 1.73, p = .08) and happiness (γ00 = 0.11, S.E. = 0.08, t(17) = 1.50, p = .13). No significant differences were detected between the kindness and whereabouts groups on any of these variables (all ps>.18). Results of t-tests mirrored these analyses, with both groups independently demonstrating increases in positive affect, happiness, and life satisfaction (all ts>1.67, all ps<.10).

All students increased in the raw number of peer nominations they received from classmates (γ00 = 0.68, S.E. = 0.27, t(17) = 2.37, p = .02), but those who performed kind acts (M = +1.57; SD = 1.90) increased significantly more than those who visited places (M = +0.71; SD = 2.17), γ01 = 0.83, S.E. = 0.39, t(17) = 2.10, p = .05, gaining an average of 1.5 friends. The model excluded a nonsignificant term controlling for classroom size (p = .12), which did not affect the significance of the kindness term. The effects of changes in life satisfaction, happiness, and positive affect on peer acceptance were tested in subsequent models and all found to be nonsignificant (all ps>.54). When controlling for changes in well-being, the effect of the kindness condition on peer acceptance remained significant. Hence, changes in well-being did not predict changes in peer acceptance, and the effect of performing acts of kindness on peer acceptance was over and above the effect of changes in well-being.

Discussion

Our study demonstrates that doing good for others benefits the givers, earning them not only improved well-being but also popularity. Considering the importance of happiness [27][28] and peer acceptance in youth [21][22], it is noteworthy that we succeeded in increasing both among preadolescents through a simple prosocial activity. Similar to being happy [29], being well-liked by classmates has ramifications not only for the individual, but also for the community at large. For example, well-liked preadolescents exhibit more inclusive behaviors and less externalizing behaviors (i.e., less bullying) as teens [20]. Thus, encouraging prosocial activities may have ripple effects beyond increasing the happiness and popularity of the doers (cf. [30]). Furthermore, classrooms with an even distribution of popularity (i.e., no favorite children and no marginalized children) show better average mental health than stratified classrooms [8], suggesting that entire classrooms practicing prosocial behavior may reap benefits, as the liking of all classmates soars. Teachers and interventionists can build on our work by introducing intentional prosocial activities into classrooms and recommending that such activities be performed regularly and purposefully.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: KL SKN EO KAS SL. Performed the experiments: KL EO. Analyzed the data: KL SKN. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: KL SKN EO KAS SL. Wrote the paper: KL SKN EO KAS SL.

References

  1. 1. Diener ML, Lucas RE (2004) Adults’ desires for children’s emotions across 48 countries. J Cross Cult Psychol 35: 525–547.
  2. 2. Wang S, Tamis-Lemonda CS (2003) Do child-rearing values in Taiwan and the United States reflect cultural values of collectivism and individualism? J Cross Cult Psychol 34: 629–642.
  3. 3. Aknin LB, Dunn EW, Norton MI (2012) Happiness runs in a circular motion: Evidence for a positive feedback loop between prosocial spending and happiness. J Happiness Stud 11: 347–355.
  4. 4. Krueger RF, Hicks BM, McGue M (2001) Altruism and antisocial behavior: Independent tendencies, unique personality correlates, distinct etiologies. Psychol Sci 12: 397–402.
  5. 5. Lyubomirsky S, Tkach C, DiMatteo MR (2006) What are the differences between happiness and self-esteem? Soc Indic Res 78: 363–404.
  6. 6. Cillessen AHN, Rose AJ (2005) Understanding popularity in the peer system. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 14: 102–105.
  7. 7. Holder MD, Coleman B (2008) The contribution of temperament, popularity, and physical appearance to children’s happiness. J of Happiness Stud 9: 279–302.
  8. 8. Ostberg V (2003) Children in classrooms: Peer status, status distribution and mental well-being. Soc Sci and Med 56: 17–29.
  9. 9. Aknin LB, Hamlin JK, Dunn EW (2012) Giving leads to happiness in young children. PLoS ONE 7: e39211.
  10. 10. Dunn EW, Aknin LB, Norton MI (2008) Spending money on others promotes happiness. Science 319: 1687–1688.
  11. 11. Lyubomirsky S, Sheldon KM, Schkade D (2005) Pursuing happiness: The architecture of sustainable change. Rev Gen Psychol 9: 111–131.
  12. 12. Weinstein N, Ryan RM (2010) When helping helps: Autonomous motivation for prosocial behavior and its influence on well-being for the helper and recipient. J Pers Soc Psychol 98: 222–244.
  13. 13. Sin NL, Lyubomirsky S (2009) Enhancing well-being and alleviating depressive symptoms with positive psychology interventions: A practice-friendly meta-analysis. J Clin Psychol 65: 467–487.
  14. 14. Froh JJ, Kashdan TB, Ozimkowski KM, Miller NM (2009) Who benefits the most from a gratitude intervention in children and adolescents? Examining positive affect as a moderator. J Posit Psychol 4: 408–422.
  15. 15. Froh JJ, Sefick WJ, Emmons RA (2008) Counting blessings in early adolescents: An experimental study of gratitude and subjective well-being. J Sch Psychol 46: 213–233.
  16. 16. Caprara CV, Barabaranelli C, Pastorelli C, Bandura A, Zimbardo PG (2000) Prosocial foundations of children‘s academic achievement. Psychol Sci 11: 302–306.
  17. 17. Rottenberg J, Ray RD, Gross JJ (2007). Emotion elicitation using films. In Coan JA, Allen JJB, eds. Handbook of Emotion Elicitation and Assessment. New York: Oxford University Press. 9–28.
  18. 18. Van Dillen LF, Koole SL (2007) Clearing the mind: A working memory model of distraction from negative mood. Emotion 7: 715–723.
  19. 19. Warneken F, Tomasello M (2006) Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science 311: 1301–1303.
  20. 20. Sandstrom MJ, Cillessen AHN (2006) Likeable versus popular: Distinct implications for adolescent adjustment. Int J Behav Dev 30: 305–314.
  21. 21. Wentzel KR (2005) Peer relationships, motivation, and academic performance at school. In: Elliot A, Dweck C, eds. Handbook of Competence and Motivation. New York: Guilford. 279–296.
  22. 22. Wentzel KR, Baker S, Russell S (2009) Peer relationships and positive adjustment at school. In Gilman R, Huebner ES, Furlong MJ, eds. Handbook of Positive Psychology in Schools. New York: Routledge. 229–243.
  23. 23. de Bruyn EH, van den Boom DC (2005) Interpersonal behavior, peer popularity, and self-esteem in early adolescence. Soc Dev 14: 555–573.
  24. 24. Gaderman AM, Schonert-Reichl KA, Zumbo BD (2010) Investigating validity evidence of the Satisfaction With Life Scale adapted for children. Soc Indic Res 96: 229–247.
  25. 25. Holder MD, Klassen A (2010) Temperament and happiness in children. J Happiness Stud 11: 419–439.
  26. 26. Laurent J, Cantanzaro SJ, Joiner TE Jr, Rudolph KD, Potter KI, et al. (1999) A measure of positive and negative affect for children: Scale development and preliminary validation. Psychol Assess 11: 326–338.
  27. 27. Suldo S, Thalji A, Ferron J (2011) Longitudinal academic outcomes predicted by early adolescents’ subjective well-being, psychopathology, and mental health status yielded from a dual factor model. J Posit Psychol 6: 17–30.
  28. 28. Richards M, Huppert FA (2011) Do positive children become positive adults? Evidence from a longitudinal birth cohort study. J Posit Psychol 6: 75–87.
  29. 29. Lyubomirsky S, King LA, Diener E (2005) The benefits of frequent positive affect: Does happiness lead to success? Psychol Bull 131: 803–855.
  30. 30. Fowler JH, Christakis NA (2010) Cooperative behavior cascades in human social networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107: 5334–5338.