Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

The Development of Visual Areas Depends Differently on Visual Experience

  • Wen Qin ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Wen Qin, Yong Liu

    Affiliation Department of Radiology, Tianjin Medical University General Hospital, Tianjin, China

  • Yong Liu ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Wen Qin, Yong Liu

    Affiliation LIAMA Center for Computational Medicine, National Laboratory of Pattern Recognition Institute of Automation, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China

  • Tianzi Jiang ,

    chunshuiyu@yahoo.cn (CY); jiangtz@nlpr.ia.ac.cn (TJ)

    Affiliation LIAMA Center for Computational Medicine, National Laboratory of Pattern Recognition Institute of Automation, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China

  • Chunshui Yu

    chunshuiyu@yahoo.cn (CY); jiangtz@nlpr.ia.ac.cn (TJ)

    Affiliations Department of Radiology, Tianjin Medical University General Hospital, Tianjin, China, Department of Radiology, Xuanwu Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China

Abstract

Visual experience plays an important role in the development of the visual cortex; however, recent functional imaging studies have shown that the functional organization is preserved in several higher-tier visual areas in congenitally blind subjects, indicating that maturation of visual areas depend unequally on visual experience. In this study, we aim to validate this hypothesis using a multimodality MRI approach. We found increased cortical thickness in the congenitally blind was present in the early visual areas and absent in the higher-tier ones, suggesting that the structural development of the visual cortex depends hierarchically on visual experience. In congenitally blind subjects, the decreased resting-state functional connectivity with the primary somatosensory cortex was more prominent in the early visual areas than in the higher-tier ones and were more pronounced in the ventral stream than in the dorsal one, suggesting that the development of functional organization of the visual cortex also depends differently on visual experience. Moreover, congenitally blind subjects showed normal or increased functional connectivity between ipsilateral higher-tier and early visual areas, suggesting an indirect corticocortical pathway through which somatosenroy information can reach the early visual areas. These findings support our hypothesis that the development of visual areas depends differently on visual experience.

Introduction

Visual deprivation is an idea model to investigate how visual cortex is reorganized to ‘perceive’ the outside world. Congenitally blind (CB) subjects who have lost visual experiences since birth have been frequently reported to have altered structural and functional characteristics in the visual cortex, including increased cortical thickness [1][3], brain regional homogeneity [4], metabolism and blood flow [5][8], decreased regional volume [9][11], white matter integrity [12], [13], and altered resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) [14]. Moreover, converging evidence suggests that both the early and higher-tier visual areas in CB subjects are activated during performing a variety of tactile or auditory tasks [15], [16]. These findings indicate that visual experience plays an important role in shaping the structural and functional organization of the visual cortex during the process of development.

However, the notion of the visual cortex as a unimodal system molded only by visual experience has recently been challenged. In sighted subjects, tactile- or auditory-related tasks can activate the dorsal and ventral visual pathway [17][22] and MT+ [23], [24]. Furthermore, increasing evidence has confirmed that CB subjects preserved functional organization in several higher-tier visual areas when performing non-visual sensory task, such as the dorsal stream specialized for spatial processing [25], [26] and action control [27], the ventral stream for reading [28] and object recognition [21], [29], [30], and the MT+ for motion processing [23], [31][34]. It is more interesting to note that both CB and SC subjects using visual-to-auditory (or visual-to–tactile) sensory substitute devices (SSD) also recruited the ventral and dorsal visual pathway by both spatial and object related tasks, such as object recognition, motion, orientation and location detection, navigation, and so on [35][41]. Although the extent and magnitude of the activation of the recruited areas differed between sighted control (SC) and CB individuals [21], [23], the co-activation of the higher-tier visual cortex by non-visual sensory inputs in SC and CB highly indicates that the development of at least parts of their functional organization does not require visual experience [42], [43].

Although the structural and functional reorganization of the visual cortex have been frequently studied in CB, little is known on the differential effects of visual deprivation on the development of the structural and resting-state functional organization of the early and higher visual cortices. Based on the earlier findings, we hypothesize that the development of visual areas depends differently on visual experience. The early visual areas are mainly modulated by visual experience through their dense connections with the visual input system, but they can also respond to non-visual sensory inputs via sparse connections with those modalities [44]. Although the higher-tier visual areas receive visual inputs from the early ones, they also have dense connections with non-visual sensory modalities [45] and can be molded by non-visual sensory stimuli. In order to verify our hypothesis, we introduce a multimodality MRI approach, including cortical thickness and resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) analyses. We predict that the cortical thickness of the early visual areas will be altered in CB subjects, whereas that of the higher-tier areas will be less affected or even normal. We also predict that the resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) of the early visual areas with primary somatosensory cortex (S1) cannot develop normally in CB subjects, but those of the higher-tier ones will develop normally.

If our hypothesis is correct, the preserved rsFC between the higher-tier visual areas and non-visual sensory areas may explain the activation of these higher-tier visual areas in CB subjects by non-visual tasks [25], [26], [46]. However, it cannot explain why the early visual areas are also activated in the CB when performing these tasks. One possible mechanism is that the early visual areas may receive non-visual sensory information via the higher-tier ones that have normally developed rsFC with the non-visual sensory areas. This possibility facilitates the hypothesis that the rsFC between the higher-tier and early visual areas will develop normally in CB subjects.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

A total of 95 right-handed subjects comprising 39 CB (24 males, mean age 24.4±4.8 years) and 56 SC subjects (40 males; mean age 25.1±4.7 years) participated in this experiment. Group comparisons did not reveal any significant differences in either age (t = −0.64, P = 0.52) (by two-sample t-test) or gender (chi-square = 1.02, P = 0.31) (by chi-square test). All CB subjects had lost their sight since birth, and 20 of them had weak light perception. None of the CB subjects had experience on pattern vision, such as contours, shapes or orientation, etc. (Table 1). The protocol was approved by the Medical Research Ethics Committee of Tianjin Medical University General Hospital, and written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to the experiment.

thumbnail
Table 1. Demographic information of congenitally blind subjects.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0053784.t001

MRI data acquisition

MRI data were obtained using a 3.0-Tesla MR scanner (Trio Tim system; Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) with a 12-channel head coil. Structural images were acquired using 3D magnetization-prepared rapid-acquisition gradient echo (MP-RAGE) sequences with the following parameters: repetition time (TR)/echo time (TE)/inversion time (TI)  = 2000/2.6/900 ms, flip angle  = 9°, matrix  = 256×224, field of view (FOV)  = 256 mm ×224 mm, 176 continuous sagittal slices with a 1-mm thickness. The resting-state fMRI data were acquired with a gradient-echo echo-planar imaging (GRE-EPI) sequence. The acquisition parameters included: TR/TE  = 2000/30 ms, flip angle  = 90°, matrix  = 64×64, FOV  = 220 mm ×220 mm, 32 axial slices with a 3-mm slice thickness and a 1-mm gap. During the fMRI scans, all subjects were instructed to keep their eyes closed, to relax, and to move as little as possible.

Cortical thickness computation

Cortical thickness was calculated using Freesurfer V.5.1.0 [47], [48] (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/). All procedures were performed using the automated surface-based pipeline with default parameters of the Freesurfer package, which mainly included segmentation, surface reconstruction, and surface-based spatial registration. First, the 3D T1 structural images were registered with the Talairach atlas [49], and the intensity variation in the white matter was removed by intensity normalization. The skull was stripped using a deformable template model [50]. White matter was then segmented based on intensity and neighbor constraints. Thereafter, the gray/white surface was obtained by tessellation of the gray/white matter boundary and topology correction. The pial surface was generated by nudging the gray/white matter surface along the T1 intensity gradients to reach the gray matter/cerebrospinal fluid boundary. Both surfaces were represented by vertices. The distance between the gray/white matter surface and its corresponding pial surface was defined as the cortical thickness [51]. To compare cortical thickness between groups, the cortical surface of each subject was transformed into an average surface space (fsaverage template, provided in Freesurfer package) using a spherical registration method [52]. Finally, the cortical thickness maps were smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of 10 mm full width at half maximum (FWHM).

Resting-state functional connectivity analysis

The resting-state fMRI data were preprocessed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM8, http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). The preprocessing steps included: (1) the first 10 volumes of blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) time series were removed for signal instability caused by T1 relaxation; (2) slice timing was performed to correct the acquisition time delay between slices within each volume; (3) the motion parameters were estimated, and each volume was realigned to the first volume (as a result, subjects CB019 and CB037 were excluded from further analysis because their translational or rotational parameters exceeded 2 mm or 2 degrees); (4) the remaining data set was normalized into the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) EPI template and resampled into 2×2×2 mm3 voxels; (5) the linear trend and several sources of spurious variances, including the estimated motion parameters, and average BOLD signals in the ventricular and white matter regions were removed from the data through linear regression; (6) a band-pass frequency filter (0.01–0.08 Hz) was applied to reduce low-frequency drift and high-frequency noise; (7) the filtered BOLD images were spatially smoothed by convolution with an isotropic Gaussian kernel (FWHM  = 6 mm).

The S1 was defined using the probabilistic map of Harvard-Oxford Cortical Structural Atlas (implemented in FSL package), voxels with probability higher than 50% were included. The region of interest (ROI) of each visual area was extracted using the Human PALS-12 atlas [53]. Because the PASL-12 atlas is population-averaged and surface-based, the surface ROIs were registrated into volumetric ones in MNI space that contain the voxels between white/gray matter surface and pial surface. We first computed the correlation coefficients between the mean time series of the S1 and that of each voxel of the whole visual cortex in a voxel-wise manner. Furthermore, we also calculated the correlation coefficients between the mean time series of the S1 and that of each visual ROI, and between every pair of visual ROIs. Before statistical analysis, the correlation coefficients were transformed into z-values using the Fisher r-to-z transformation to improve the normality.

Statistical analysis

Group comparison of cortical thickness was performed in a vertex-wise manner using a general linear model (GLM) with age, gender and whole brain mean thickness as the nuisance covariates and within a mask bilaterally including the visual cortex and S1. A FDR method with a threshold of P<0.01 was selected to correct for multiple comparisons in combination with a cluster size threshold of P<0.05 (Monte Carlo simulations). Furthermore, the mean cortical thickness of each visual area of each subject was extracted based on the Human PALS-12 atlas. After regressing out age, gender and whole brain mean thickness effects, the ROI-based group comparisons were carried out using two-sample t-test at P<0.05 (Bonferroni corrected).

Voxel-based rsFC analyses between the S1 and the whole visual cortex were performed using an FDR threshold of P<0.01. First, individual z-map for each group (CB or SC) were entered into a random effect one-sample t-test in a voxel-wise manner to determine the visual areas that showed significant correlations with the seed ROIs. Then, a two-sample t-test was carried out to investigate group differences in the rsFC. To validate the results of the voxel-based rsFC analyses, we further compared the group differences in the rsFC between S1 and each visual area using a two-sample t-test after regressing out age and gender influences (P<0.05, Bonferroni corrected). Finally, we investigated inter-group differences in the rsFC between every two visual areas using a two-sample t-test after regressing out age and gender effects (P<0.05, Bonferroni corrected).

Results

Vertex-based cortical thickness analyses

Compared with SC subjects, CB individuals showed significantly increased cortical thickness in the early visual areas (P<0.01, FDR corrected); however, most of the higher-tier visual areas, especially the dorsal visual stream, did not show any significant changes in cortical thickness (Fig. 1). No significant group differences were found in cortical thickness in S1 even using a loose threshold (P<0.05, uncorrected). To investigate whether weak light perception affected the cortical thickness analysis results, we also compared the cortical thickness between CB subjects with and without light perception. There was no significant group difference even using an uncorrected P<0.05.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Comparison maps of cortical thickness between CB and SC subjects in a vertex-wise manner.

A, significantly increased cortical thickness in CB subjects is observed in the visual areas while absent in primary somatosensory cortex (S1) within a searching mask including the whole visual cortex and S1 (P<0.01, FDR corrected). B, significantly increased cortical thickness in CB subjects is observed in the early visual areas (V1, V2 and VP), but not in higher-tier ones (V3A, V7, MT+ and V8) and in S1. Scale bar represents the log-transformed P value.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0053784.g001

ROI-based cortical thickness analyses

To further elucidate the specific change in cortical thickness of each visual area, we extracted the mean cortical thickness of each visual area in each subject based on the Human PALS-12 atlas (Fig. 2D). In the left hemisphere, CB subjects showed significantly increased cortical thickness relative to SC subjects in V1, V2, V3d and LO (P<0.05, Bonferroni corrected) (Fig. 2A). Similarly, in the right hemisphere, CB subjects had significantly increased cortical thickness in V1, V2 and VP (P<0.05, Bonferroni corrected) (Fig. 2B). The mean increase amplitude in cortical thickness of CB subjects versus SC subjects was 8% in V1, 7% in V2, 6% in VP, 5% in V3d, 4% in V3A, and 3% in the LO (Fig. 2C). The remaining higher-tier visual areas did not show any significant group differences in cortical thickness even using an uncorrected P<0.05.

thumbnail
Figure 2. ROI-based analyses of the cortical thickness between CB and SC subjects.

An asterisk (*) represents significant difference between groups. A and B, significant increases in cortical thickness are found in the left V1, V2, V3d and LO, and in the right V1, V2 and VP (P<0.05, Bonferroni corrected). C, percent changes of cortical thickness in CB subjects relative to SC subjects. The cortical thickness is significantly increased in the early visual areas (V1, V2, VP and V3d), but not in several higher-tier visual areas (V3A, V7, MT+ and V8) in CB subjects. D, visual areas derived from the Human PALS-12 atlas.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0053784.g002

Voxel-based rsFC analyses between S1 and visual areas

The S1 of SC subjects showed positive rsFC with most of the visual areas (Fig. 3A) (P<0.01, FDR corrected). In contrast, the S1 of CB subjects had positive rsFC with only parts of the higher-tier visual areas (P<0.01, FDR corrected) (Fig. 3B). Group comparison showed that CB subjects had significantly decreased rsFC between non-visual sensory ROIs and visual areas relative to SC subjects (Fig. 3C). Moreover, the early visual areas were more pronouncedly affected than the higher-tier ones, and the ventral stream was more pronouncedly affected than the dorsal one.

thumbnail
Figure 3. The rsFC maps between the S1 and the visual areas.

A, the rsFC map of SC subjects. B, the rsFC map of CB subjects. C, the differential maps of rsFC between CB and SC subjects with thresholded at P<0.05 (FDR corrected). The upper and lower panel represents the FC results of the S1 area in the left and right hemisphere, respectively. Compared with SC subjects, CB subjects showed significantly decreased (cool color) rsFC between the S1 and the visual areas, especially the early visual areas (V1 and V2) and the ventral stream (VP and V4v).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0053784.g003

ROI-based rsFC analyses between S1 and visual areas

We further computed the rsFC between S1 and each visual ROI. Compared with those of SC subjects, the S1 of the CB subjects showed significantly decreased rsFC with the early visual areas (V1 and V2) and the ventral stream (VP, V4v and V8), but not with several higher-tier visual areas in the dorsal visual stream (V3d, V3A, V7 and MT+), using a threshold of P<0.05 (Bonferroni corrected) (Fig. 4). The hierarchical trend that the early visual areas are more affected than the higher-tier ones is also present in rsFC pattern in CB.

thumbnail
Figure 4. ROI analyses of the rsFC between the S1 and visual areas.

A and B, the mean rsFC of CB (blue bar) and SC (green bar) subjects between intra-hemisphere and intra-hemisphere, respectively. An asterisk represents significantly differences (P<0.05, Bonferroni corrected). C, the defined S1 and visual ROIs. D, rsFC patterns between S1 and visual areas in the SC. E, rsFC patterns between S1 and visual areas in the CB. F, group differences of rsFC between CB and SC subjects. Orange and light blue edges represent positive and negative effects. Blue, red and green nodes represent S1, affected and non-affected visual areas. Compared with SC subjects, CB subjects showed significantly decreased rsFC of the S1 with the early visual areas and the ventral stream ones, with no difference being observed in several higher-tier visual areas.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0053784.g004

ROI-based rsFC analysis within the visual areas

We investigated the rsFC between each pair of the visual areas and found significantly increased rsFC (P<0.05, Bonferroni corrected) between the early visual areas (V1 and V2) and several higher-tier visual areas (V8 and LO) in the same hemisphere in CB subjects (Fig. 5). We also found decreased rsFC between several inter-hemispheric visual areas (Fig. 5).

thumbnail
Figure 5. The rsFC patterns within the visual areas in CB and SC subjects.

A, the mean rsFC in CB and SC subjects, and group comparisons (P<0.05, Bonferroni corrected), in which hot and cool colors represent positive and negative effects, respectively. Color bar represents the log-transformed P value. B, the rsFC patterns between each pair of the ipsilateral visual areas. C, The rsFC patterns between each pair of inter-hemispheric visual areas. Orange and light blue lines represent increased and decreased rsFC. Both CB and SC subjects show positive rsFC between visual areas. Significantly increased rsFC between the early (V1 and V2) and several ipsilateral higher-tier visual areas (V8 and LO), while decreased rsFC between inter-hemispheric visual areas are shown in CB subjects.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0053784.g005

Discussion

In this study, combined structural and functional analyses, we found CB subjects showed significantly increased cortical thickness in the early visual areas while absent in the higher-tier ones; furthermore, the decreased rsFC with the S1 was more prominent in the early visual areas than in the higher-tier ones and were more pronounced in the ventral visual stream than in the dorsal one, suggesting that the development of the structural and functional organization of the visual cortex depends differently on visual experience. Moreover, CB subjects showed normal or increased rsFC between the higher-tier and early visual areas in the ipsilateral hemisphere, suggesting an indirect corticocortical pathway through which non-visual sensory information can reach the early visual areas.

The structural and functional reorganization in CB

It is important to note that evidence of reorganization in the visual cortex in CB comes from studies with different modalities. How to reconcile findings from these modalities is critical for understanding the neural mechanisms of the altered cortical organization in CB. For example, the contradictory findings of the increased cortical thickness [1], [2] and decreased gray matter volume (GMV) [9] in the early visual cortex in the early blind could be explained by the overly reduced surface area (Jiang et al., 2009). Similarly, the unchanged cortical thickness [1] and decreased GMV [54] in the visual cortex in the late-onset blind could also be driven by reduced surface area (Jiang et al., 2009).

The following factors may affect the morphology of the early visual cortex in CB. Firstly, the synaptic pruning of the visual cortex is dependent on visual experience [55], [56] and is interfered in the CB. Thus the relatively high synaptic density may contribute to the thickened early visual cortex in CB [1], [2]. However, these “redundant” synapses cannot explain the recruitment of early visual cortex in a variety of non-visual tasks in CB [15], [16]. Secondly, early visual deprivation really results in the increases in corticocortical [57], [58], thalamocortical [57], [59] and intracortical connections [60], [61] in the visual areas. These increased connections may contribute to the increased cortical thickness in the CB and may form the structural basis for the visual cortex responsing to non-visual stimuli. Finally, axonal degeneration secondary to the peripheral damage of the anterior visual pathway will result in the atrophy of the gray and white ma motion radiation [9], [13] in CB subjects. The atrophied white matter of the visual area may account for the reduction of the surface area in the CB. Therefore, the neural mechanisms of the thickened visual cortex in the CB are complex and represent a final consequence of multiple factors because increased synaptic density contributes to the increased cortical thickness whereas axonal degeneration lead to the decrease in cortical thickness.

Different functional connectivity (FC) patterns between the early visual cortex and non-visual sensory cortex have been reported during resting-state and task-state in the CB. The early blind subjects exhibited increased FC between the S1 and early visual cortex during the stimulation of the S1 [62] and demonstrated increased effective connectivity from the primary auditory cortex to V1 when performing an auditory discrimination task [63]. During resting-state, however, decreased rsFCs between the V1 and non-visual sensory cortex have been frequently reported [14], [64], [65]. Although there is a close relationship in the FC patterns between resting-state and task-state [66], it does not mean that they share the same FC patterns. For example, significant differences in FC intensity between the fusiform gyrus and calcarine sulcus have been found under different task conditions [67]. The effective connectivity that measures the causal flow of one node to another is substantially different with functional connectivity that represents the simultaneous coupling of two nodes [68]. These factors might explain the different connectivity changes in CB under rest-state and task-state. The decreased rsFC only reflects the decoupling of brain activity between the early visual cortex and S1; however, it cannot simply imply the poor performance in handling tactile information, neither the poor activation in the occipital cortex.

The surprising findings with dramatically decreased resting-state FC between LO and S1 is difficult to explain the increased recruitments of this area in tactile processing in the CB, which had been confirmed by several previous task-evoked studies. In fact, not only the LO, but also the ventral pathway areas such as V4/V8 also encountered the same dilemma. Besides the difference in functional representation between task and rest-state as stated above, another possibility is that the tactile signals might reach the LO C and ventral visual areas via other indirect pathways. For example, increased resting-state FCs between occipital cortex and the prefrontal cortex have been reported by several research groups [14], [69]. As a result, the strengthened frontal-occipital FC might convey attention- (or working memory) modulated non-visual sensory signals to these object-related occipital areas for further manipulation. However, this hypothesis needs to be validated by further studies.

It has been suggested that the intrinsic functional connectivity reflects monosynaptic or polysynaptic connections [70], [71]. Bilateral visual cortices were anatomically connected by fibers of the splenium of corpus callosum [72]. Consequently, our finding of the decreased rsFC between inter-hemispheric visual areas might be as a result of the disrupted white matter integrity of these fibers [3], [12].

The development of the visual cortex depends differently on visual experience

As mentioned in the introduction, the activation of the visual cortex by non-visual sensory stimuli both in SC and CB indicates that the visual cortex is supramodal in nature, and the development of the functional architecture in the higher visual cortex does not require visual experience to some extent (for reviews, see [42], [43]). In the present study, we found relative normally developed cortical thickness and rsFC in the higher visual cortex, especially in the dorsal pathway, which provided another aspect of evidence to support this concept. Furthermore, the present study provides converging evidence suggesting that the development of the visual areas depends differently on visual experience. Specifically, the early visual areas depend more on visual experience than the higher-tier ones, and the ventral stream visual areas depend more on visual experience than the dorsal ones. The early visual areas of sighted persons and animals receive sensory inputs primarily from the visual system and may also receive some direct or indirect inputs from non-visual sensory modalities (for reviews, see [44], [45], [57], [73]). However, the direct connections between early sensory areas of different modalities seem relatively sparse, and their functions remain unclear [44]. In contrast, the higher-tier visual areas have dense connections with non-visual sensory areas and have many feedback connections with the early visual areas [45]. The specific connection patterns of the early and higher-tier visual areas may determine their dependence on sensory experiences. The early visual areas are heavily connected with the visual thalamus but sparsely connected with non-visual sensory areas. As a result, these areas depend heavily on visual experience and less on non-visual inputs. The higher-tier visual areas have more connections with non-visual sensory areas than the early ones, and their development can be shaped by these non-visual sensory experiences. However, future studies are required to determine whether this mechanism explains the differential effects of visual experience on the ventral and dorsal visual streams.

It should be noted that differences in maturation rate and their susceptibility to visual deprivation between different visual areas might be another important factor that affects the structural and functional reorganization after early visual deprivation. For example, the sensitive period for normal maturation and susceptibility to damage of global motion (mediated primarily by the higher visual areas such as the MT+) appears to be much earlier and shorter than the sensitive period for those of visual acuity (mediated primarily by the early visual areas) [74]. The development of the motion and action ability (mediated by the dorsal visual stream) was found to be much earlier than that of the contour and form perceptual ability (mediated by the ventral visual stream) [75][78]. Earlier development of dorsal than ventral stream had also been validated by physiological and anatomical findings [79][82]. The relatively late developed visual areas (early visual areas and ventral stream) are more susceptive to early visual deprivation, which can explain our findings of the prominent structural and functional alterations in these visual areas.

The neural pathway from non-visual sensory areas to the visual cortex

Because both the early and higher-tier visual areas can respond to tactile and auditory stimuli in CB subjects, it is important to understand how non-visual sensory information reaches the visual areas, especially the early ones. A tentative hypothesis is that the visual areas receive non-visual information through rewired thalamocortical connections [57], [59], [83], i.e., the lateral geniculate nucleus receives rewired non-visual projections from the auditory and somatosensory thalamic nuclei and then projects fibers to the visual areas in CB subjects. However, this hypothesis is not supported by findings of atrophy in the lateral geniculate nucleus and in its output projections [10], [12], [13]. An alternative hypothesis is that the visual areas receive non-visual sensory information through corticocortical connections between these sensory modalities [45], [84], which is supported by task-based fMRI studies [85], [86], and a resting-state fMRI study [87] in sighted subjects, an effective connectivity study [63], and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) studies in blind humans [58], [62], [88], [89]. Furthermore, these corticocortical connections can be subdivided into direct long-range connections and indirect connections via intervening multimodal association areas. The former has been found in immature cats and hamsters and mature primates [45], [57], [90], while the latter is found in primates [91] and is confirmed by an effective connectivity study in humans [92]. Our findings of decreased rsFC between the early visual areas and S1 and the normal or even increased rsFC between the higher-tier visual areas and S1 and between the early and higher-tier visual areas in the same hemisphere in CB subjects would support the hypothesis wherein indirect corticocortical connections transmit non-visual sensory information to the early visual areas. Specifically, in the CB, non-visual sensory areas could receive tactile or auditory information and then transmit it to the early visual areas via higher-tier ones. It should be noted that this hypothesis needs further confirmation, although it seems to be supported by two effective connectivity studies [63], [92].

Issues regarding to functional preservation and plasticity in occipital cortex

In sighted individuals, visual information is hierarchically processed from V1 to higher-tier visual areas along the ventral and dorsal pathways; however, this visual flow is interrupted by early visual deprivation. In contrast to the relative preserved functional preferences in the extrastriate areas after visual deprivation, V1 seems to be specially evoked by higher-order cognitive tasks such as memory [93], [94] and language [65], [95][97]. The preserved functional preferences are supported by our findings of the relatively unchanged functional connectivity between the extrastriate areas and S1. Furthermore, the decreased FC between early visual areas and S1 does not support direct tactile signal processing in V1; instead, this finding indicates that V1 might process higher cognitive-demanding functions, as revealed by previous task-driven results. Finally, as indicated by previous findings and present study, the intact indirect corticocortical pathways through which peripheral signals transmit from non-visual sensory areas to the “higher-tier” extrastriate areas and then to V1 is consistent with hypothesis by Buchel [98] that the cortical hierarchy in the occipital cortex might be reversed after early visual deprivation, such that extrastriate areas feed into V1, while V1 becomes a higher-tier area capable of processing multiple cognitive functions.

In summary, based on the cortical thickness and rsFC analyses in CB subjects, we propose that the development of visual areas depends differently on visual experience, which can explain most previous structural and functional findings in the visual areas of both sighted and blind subjects. Based on the rsFC analyses, we also suggest that the indirect corticocortical connection is a possible pathway through which non-visual sensory information could reach the early visual areas.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: CY TJ WQ. Performed the experiments: WQ CY YL. Analyzed the data: WQ YL. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: WQ YL. Wrote the paper: WQ YL CY TJ.

References

  1. 1. Jiang J, Zhu W, Shi F, Liu Y, Li J, et al. (2009) Thick Visual Cortex in the Early Blind. J Neurosci 29: 2205–2211.
  2. 2. Park HJ, Lee JD, Kim EY, Park B, Oh MK, et al. (2009) Morphological alterations in the congenital blind based on the analysis of cortical thickness and surface area. Neuroimage 47: 98–106.
  3. 3. Bridge H, Cowey A, Ragge N, Watkins K (2009) Imaging studies in congenital anophthalmia reveal preservation of brain architecture in ‘visual’ cortex. Brain 132: 3467–3480.
  4. 4. Liu C, Liu Y, Li W, Wang D, Jiang T, et al. (2011) Increased regional homogeneity of blood oxygen level-dependent signals in occipital cortex of early blind individuals. Neuroreport 22: 190–194.
  5. 5. De Volder AG, Bol A, Blin J, Robert A, Arno P, et al. (1997) Brain energy metabolism in early blind subjects: neural activity in the visual cortex. Brain Res 750: 235–244.
  6. 6. Mishina M, Senda M, Kiyosawa M, Ishiwata K, De Volder AG, et al. (2003) Increased regional cerebral blood flow but normal distribution of GABAA receptor in the visual cortex of subjects with early-onset blindness. Neuroimage 19: 125–131.
  7. 7. Veraart C, De Volder AG, Wanet-Defalque MC, Bol A, Michel C, et al. (1990) Glucose utilization in human visual cortex is abnormally elevated in blindness of early onset but decreased in blindness of late onset. Brain Res 510: 115–121.
  8. 8. Uhl F, Franzen P, Podreka I, Steiner M, Deecke L (1993) Increased regional cerebral blood flow in inferior occipital cortex and cerebellum of early blind humans. Neurosci Lett 150: 162–164.
  9. 9. Ptito M, Schneider FC, Paulson OB, Kupers R (2008) Alterations of the visual pathways in congenital blindness. Exp Brain Res 187: 41–49.
  10. 10. Noppeney U, Friston KJ, Ashburner J, Frackowiak R, Price CJ (2005) Early visual deprivation induces structural plasticity in gray and white matter. Curr Biol 15: R488–490.
  11. 11. Pan WJ, Wu G, Li CX, Lin F, Sun J, et al. (2007) Progressive atrophy in the optic pathway and visual cortex of early blind Chinese adults: A voxel-based morphometry magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroimage 37: 212–220.
  12. 12. Shimony JS, Burton H, Epstein AA, McLaren DG, Sun SW, et al. (2006) Diffusion tensor imaging reveals white matter reorganization in early blind humans. Cereb Cortex 16: 1653–1661.
  13. 13. Shu N, Li J, Li K, Yu C, Jiang T (2009) Abnormal diffusion of cerebral white matter in early blindness. Hum Brain Mapp 30: 220–227.
  14. 14. Liu Y, Yu C, Liang M, Li J, Tian L, et al. (2007) Whole brain functional connectivity in the early blind. Brain 130: 2085–2096.
  15. 15. Bavelier D, Neville HJ (2002) Cross-modal plasticity: where and how? Nat Rev Neurosci 3: 443–452.
  16. 16. Fiehler K, Rosler F (2010) Plasticity of multisensory dorsal stream functions: evidence from congenitally blind and sighted adults. Restor Neurol Neurosci 28: 193–205.
  17. 17. Amedi A, Jacobson G, Hendler T, Malach R, Zohary E (2002) Convergence of visual and tactile shape processing in the human lateral occipital complex. Cereb Cortex 12: 1202–1212.
  18. 18. James TW, Humphrey GK, Gati JS, Servos P, Menon RS, et al. (2002) Haptic study of three-dimensional objects activates extrastriate visual areas. Neuropsychologia 40: 1706–1714.
  19. 19. Kim JK, Zatorre RJ (2011) Tactile-auditory shape learning engages the lateral occipital complex. J Neurosci 31: 7848–7856.
  20. 20. Amedi A, Malach R, Hendler T, Peled S, Zohary E (2001) Visuo-haptic object-related activation in the ventral visual pathway. Nat Neurosci 4: 324–330.
  21. 21. Pietrini P, Furey ML, Ricciardi E, Gobbini MI, Wu WH, et al. (2004) Beyond sensory images: Object-based representation in the human ventral pathway. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101: 5658–5663.
  22. 22. Kassuba T, Klinge C, Holig C, Menz MM, Ptito M, et al. (2011) The left fusiform gyrus hosts trisensory representations of manipulable objects. Neuroimage 56: 1566–1577.
  23. 23. Ricciardi E, Vanello N, Sani L, Gentili C, Scilingo EP, et al. (2007) The effect of visual experience on the development of functional architecture in hMT+. Cereb Cortex 17: 2933–2939.
  24. 24. Poirier C, Collignon O, Devolder AG, Renier L, Vanlierde A, et al. (2005) Specific activation of the V5 brain area by auditory motion processing: an fMRI study. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 25: 650–658.
  25. 25. Renier LA, Anurova I, De Volder AG, Carlson S, VanMeter J, et al. (2010) Preserved functional specialization for spatial processing in the middle occipital gyrus of the early blind. Neuron 68: 138–148.
  26. 26. Collignon O, Vandewalle G, Voss P, Albouy G, Charbonneau G, et al. (2011) Functional specialization for auditory-spatial processing in the occipital cortex of congenitally blind humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108: 4435–4440.
  27. 27. Fiehler K, Burke M, Bien S, Roder B, Rosler F (2009) The human dorsal action control system develops in the absence of vision. Cereb Cortex 19: 1–12.
  28. 28. Reich L, Szwed M, Cohen L, Amedi A (2011) A ventral visual stream reading center independent of visual experience. Curr Biol 21: 363–368.
  29. 29. Amedi A, Raz N, Azulay H, Malach R, Zohary E (2010) Cortical activity during tactile exploration of objects in blind and sighted humans. Restor Neurol Neurosci 28: 143–156.
  30. 30. Peltier S, Stilla R, Mariola E, LaConte S, Hu X, et al. (2007) Activity and effective connectivity of parietal and occipital cortical regions during haptic shape perception. Neuropsychologia 45: 476–483.
  31. 31. Wolbers T, Zahorik P, Giudice NA (2011) Decoding the direction of auditory motion in blind humans. Neuroimage 56: 681–687.
  32. 32. Matteau I, Kupers R, Ricciardi E, Pietrini P, Ptito M (2010) Beyond visual, aural and haptic movement perception: hMT+ is activated by electrotactile motion stimulation of the tongue in sighted and in congenitally blind individuals. Brain Res Bull 82: 264–270.
  33. 33. Ptito M, Matteau I, Gjedde A, Kupers R (2009) Recruitment of the middle temporal area by tactile motion in congenital blindness. Neuroreport 20: 543–547.
  34. 34. Poirier C, Collignon O, Scheiber C, Renier L, Vanlierde A, et al. (2006) Auditory motion perception activates visual motion areas in early blind subjects. Neuroimage 31: 279–285.
  35. 35. Striem-Amit E, Dakwar O, Reich L, Amedi A (2012) The large-scale organization of “visual” streams emerges without visual experience. Cereb Cortex 22: 1698–1709.
  36. 36. Amedi A, Stern WM, Camprodon JA, Bermpohl F, Merabet L, et al. (2007) Shape conveyed by visual-to-auditory sensory substitution activates the lateral occipital complex. Nat Neurosci 10: 687–689.
  37. 37. Renier L, De Volder AG (2010) Vision substitution and depth perception: early blind subjects experience visual perspective through their ears. Disabil Rehabil Assist Technol 5: 175–183.
  38. 38. Kupers R, Chebat DR, Madsen KH, Paulson OB, Ptito M (2010) Neural correlates of virtual route recognition in congenital blindness. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107: 12716–12721.
  39. 39. Auvray M, Hanneton S, O'Regan JK (2007) Learning to perceive with a visuo-auditory substitution system: localisation and object recognition with ‘the vOICe’. Perception 36: 416–430.
  40. 40. Proulx MJ, Stoerig P, Ludowig E, Knoll I (2008) Seeing ‘where’ through the ears: effects of learning-by-doing and long-term sensory deprivation on localization based on image-to-sound substitution. PLoS ONE 3: e1840.
  41. 41. Kim JK, Zatorre RJ (2008) Generalized learning of visual-to-auditory substitution in sighted individuals. Brain Res 1242: 263–275.
  42. 42. Kupers R, Pietrini P, Ricciardi E, Ptito M (2011) The nature of consciousness in the visually deprived brain. Front Psychol 2: 19.
  43. 43. Reich L, Maidenbaum S, Amedi A (2012) The brain as a flexible task machine: implications for visual rehabilitation using noninvasive vs. invasive approaches. Curr Opin Neurol 25: 86–95.
  44. 44. Driver J, Noesselt T (2008) Multisensory interplay reveals crossmodal influences on ‘sensory-specific’ brain regions, neural responses, and judgments. Neuron 57: 11–23.
  45. 45. Falchier A, Clavagnier S, Barone P, Kennedy H (2002) Anatomical evidence of multimodal integration in primate striate cortex. J Neurosci 22: 5749–5759.
  46. 46. Mahon BZ, Anzellotti S, Schwarzbach J, Zampini M, Caramazza A (2009) Category-specific organization in the human brain does not require visual experience. Neuron 63: 397–405.
  47. 47. Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI (1999) Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 9: 179–194.
  48. 48. Fischl B, Sereno MI, Dale AM (1999) Cortical surface-based analysis. II: Inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. Neuroimage 9: 195–207.
  49. 49. Talairach J, Tournoux P (1988) Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: Georg Thieme Verlag.
  50. 50. Segonne F, Dale AM, Busa E, Glessner M, Salat D, et al. (2004) A hybrid approach to the skull stripping problem in MRI. Neuroimage 22: 1060–1075.
  51. 51. Fischl B, Dale AM (2000) Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97: 11050–11055.
  52. 52. Fischl B, Sereno MI, Tootell RB, Dale AM (1999) High-resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface. Hum Brain Mapp 8: 272–284.
  53. 53. Van Essen DC (2005) A Population-Average, Landmark- and Surface-based (PALS) atlas of human cerebral cortex. Neuroimage 28: 635–662.
  54. 54. Lepore N, Voss P, Lepore F, Chou YY, Fortin M, et al. (2010) Brain structure changes visualized in early- and late-onset blind subjects. Neuroimage 49: 134–140.
  55. 55. Chechik G, Meilijson I, Ruppin E (1999) Neuronal regulation: A mechanism for synaptic pruning during brain maturation. Neural Comput 11: 2061–2080.
  56. 56. Bourgeois JP, Jastreboff PJ, Rakic P (1989) Synaptogenesis in visual cortex of normal and preterm monkeys: evidence for intrinsic regulation of synaptic overproduction. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 86: 4297–4301.
  57. 57. Karlen SJ, Kahn DM, Krubitzer L (2006) Early blindness results in abnormal corticocortical and thalamocortical connections. Neuroscience 142: 843–858.
  58. 58. Kupers R, Fumal A, de Noordhout AM, Gjedde A, Schoenen J, et al. (2006) Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the visual cortex induces somatotopically organized qualia in blind subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103: 13256–13260.
  59. 59. Rehkamper G, Necker R, Nevo E (1994) Functional anatomy of the thalamus in the blind mole rat Spalax ehrenbergi: an architectonic and electrophysiologically controlled tracing study. J Comp Neurol 347: 570–584.
  60. 60. Callaway EM, Katz LC (1991) Effects of binocular deprivation on the development of clustered horizontal connections in cat striate cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 88: 745–749.
  61. 61. Sur M, Leamey CA (2001) Development and plasticity of cortical areas and networks. Nat Rev Neurosci 2: 251–262.
  62. 62. Wittenberg GF, Werhahn KJ, Wassermann EM, Herscovitch P, Cohen LG (2004) Functional connectivity between somatosensory and visual cortex in early blind humans. Eur J Neurosci 20: 1923–1927.
  63. 63. Klinge C, Eippert F, Roder B, Buchel C (2010) Corticocortical connections mediate primary visual cortex responses to auditory stimulation in the blind. J Neurosci 30: 12798–12805.
  64. 64. Yu C, Liu Y, Li J, Zhou Y, Wang K, et al. (2008) Altered functional connectivity of primary visual cortex in early blindness. Hum Brain Mapp 29: 533–543.
  65. 65. Bedny M, Pascual-Leone A, Dodell-Feder D, Fedorenko E, Saxe R (2011) Language processing in the occipital cortex of congenitally blind adults. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108: 4429–4434.
  66. 66. He BJ, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Epstein A, Shulman GL, et al. (2007) Breakdown of functional connectivity in frontoparietal networks underlies behavioral deficits in spatial neglect. Neuron 53: 905–918.
  67. 67. Norman-Haignere SV, McCarthy G, Chun MM, Turk-Browne NB (2012) Category-selective background connectivity in ventral visual cortex. Cereb Cortex 22: 391–402.
  68. 68. Matsui T, Tamura K, Koyano KW, Takeuchi D, Adachi Y, et al.. (2011) Direct Comparison of Spontaneous Functional Connectivity and Effective Connectivity Measured by Intracortical Microstimulation: An fMRI Study in Macaque Monkeys. Cereb Cortex.
  69. 69. Bedny M, Konkle T, Pelphrey K, Saxe R, Pascual-Leone A (2010) Sensitive period for a multimodal response in human visual motion area MT/MST. Curr Biol 20: 1900–1906.
  70. 70. Greicius MD, Supekar K, Menon V, Dougherty RF (2009) Resting-state functional connectivity reflects structural connectivity in the default mode network. Cereb Cortex 19: 72–78.
  71. 71. Honey CJ, Sporns O, Cammoun L, Gigandet X, Thiran JP, et al. (2009) Predicting human resting-state functional connectivity from structural connectivity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106: 2035–2040.
  72. 72. Conturo TE, Lori NF, Cull TS, Akbudak E, Snyder AZ, et al. (1999) Tracking neuronal fiber pathways in the living human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 96: 10422–10427.
  73. 73. Bizley JK, Nodal FR, Bajo VM, Nelken I, King AJ (2007) Physiological and anatomical evidence for multisensory interactions in auditory cortex. Cereb Cortex 17: 2172–2189.
  74. 74. Lewis TL, Maurer D (2005) Multiple sensitive periods in human visual development: evidence from visually deprived children. Dev Psychobiol 46: 163–183.
  75. 75. Kiorpes L, Price T, Hall-Haro C, Anthony Movshon J (2012) Development of sensitivity to global form and motion in macaque monkeys (Macaca nemestrina). Vision Res 63: 34–42.
  76. 76. Kovacs I, Kozma P, Feher A, Benedek G (1999) Late maturation of visual spatial integration in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 96: 12204–12209.
  77. 77. Kovacs I (2000) Human development of perceptual organization. Vision Res 40: 1301–1310.
  78. 78. Kiorpes L, Bassin SA (2003) Development of contour integration in macaque monkeys. Vis Neurosci 20: 567–575.
  79. 79. Distler C, Bachevalier J, Kennedy C, Mishkin M, Ungerleider LG (1996) Functional development of the corticocortical pathway for motion analysis in the macaque monkey: a 14C-2-deoxyglucose study. Cereb Cortex 6: 184–195.
  80. 80. Bourne JA, Rosa MG (2006) Hierarchical development of the primate visual cortex, as revealed by neurofilament immunoreactivity: early maturation of the middle temporal area (MT). Cereb Cortex 16: 405–414.
  81. 81. Wattam-Bell J, Birtles D, Nystrom P, von Hofsten C, Rosander K, et al. (2010) Reorganization of global form and motion processing during human visual development. Curr Biol 20: 411–415.
  82. 82. Rodman HR, Scalaidhe SP, Gross CG (1993) Response properties of neurons in temporal cortical visual areas of infant monkeys. J Neurophysiol 70: 1115–1136.
  83. 83. Bronchti G, Rado R, Terkel J, Wollberg Z (1991) Retinal projections in the blind mole rat: a WGA-HRP tracing study of a natural degeneration. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 58: 159–170.
  84. 84. Bock AS, Kroenke CD, Taber EN, Olavarria JF (2011) Retinal input influences the size and corticocortical connectivity of visual cortex during postnatal development in the ferret. J Comp Neurol.
  85. 85. Werner S, Noppeney U (2010) Distinct functional contributions of primary sensory and association areas to audiovisual integration in object categorization. J Neurosci 30: 2662–2675.
  86. 86. den Ouden HE, Friston KJ, Daw ND, McIntosh AR, Stephan KE (2009) A dual role for prediction error in associative learning. Cereb Cortex 19: 1175–1185.
  87. 87. Eckert MA, Kamdar NV, Chang CE, Beckmann CF, Greicius MD, et al. (2008) A cross-modal system linking primary auditory and visual cortices: evidence from intrinsic fMRI connectivity analysis. Hum Brain Mapp 29: 848–857.
  88. 88. Collignon O, Lassonde M, Lepore F, Bastien D, Veraart C (2007) Functional cerebral reorganization for auditory spatial processing and auditory substitution of vision in early blind subjects. Cereb Cortex 17: 457–465.
  89. 89. Ptito M, Fumal A, de Noordhout AM, Schoenen J, Gjedde A, et al. (2008) TMS of the occipital cortex induces tactile sensations in the fingers of blind Braille readers. Exp Brain Res 184: 193–200.
  90. 90. Innocenti GM (1995) Exuberant development of connections, and its possible permissive role in cortical evolution. Trends Neurosci 18: 397–402.
  91. 91. Rockland KS, Ojima H (2003) Multisensory convergence in calcarine visual areas in macaque monkey. Int J Psychophysiol 50: 19–26.
  92. 92. Fujii T, Tanabe HC, Kochiyama T, Sadato N (2009) An investigation of cross-modal plasticity of effective connectivity in the blind by dynamic causal modeling of functional MRI data. Neurosci Res 65: 175–186.
  93. 93. Amedi A, Raz N, Pianka P, Malach R, Zohary E (2003) Early ‘visual’ cortex activation correlates with superior verbal memory performance in the blind. Nat Neurosci 6: 758–766.
  94. 94. Raz N, Amedi A, Zohary E (2005) V1 activation in congenitally blind humans is associated with episodic retrieval. Cereb Cortex 15: 1459–1468.
  95. 95. Burton H, Diamond JB, McDermott KB (2003) Dissociating cortical regions activated by semantic and phonological tasks: a FMRI study in blind and sighted people. J Neurophysiol 90: 1965–1982.
  96. 96. Burton H, Snyder AZ, Conturo TE, Akbudak E, Ollinger JM, et al. (2002) Adaptive changes in early and late blind: a fMRI study of Braille reading. J Neurophysiol 87: 589–607.
  97. 97. Burton H, Snyder AZ, Diamond JB, Raichle ME (2002) Adaptive changes in early and late blind: a FMRI study of verb generation to heard nouns. J Neurophysiol 88: 3359–3371.
  98. 98. Buchel C (2003) Cortical hierarchy turned on its head. Nat Neurosci 6: 657–658.