Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Self-Relevance Appraisal Influences Facial Reactions to Emotional Body Expressions

  • Julie Grèzes ,

    julie.grezes@ens.fr

    Affiliation Laboratoire de Neurosciences Cognitives (LNC) - INSERM U960 & IEC - Ecole Normale Supérieure (ENS), 75005 Paris, France

  • Léonor Philip,

    Affiliation Laboratoire de Neurosciences Cognitives (LNC) - INSERM U960 & IEC - Ecole Normale Supérieure (ENS), 75005 Paris, France

  • Michèle Chadwick,

    Affiliation Laboratoire de Neurosciences Cognitives (LNC) - INSERM U960 & IEC - Ecole Normale Supérieure (ENS), 75005 Paris, France

  • Guillaume Dezecache,

    Affiliation Laboratoire de Neurosciences Cognitives (LNC) - INSERM U960 & IEC - Ecole Normale Supérieure (ENS), 75005 Paris, France

  • Robert Soussignan,

    Affiliation Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation (CSGA) UMR 6265 CNRS - 1324 INRA, Université de Bourgogne, 21000 Dijon, France

  • Laurence Conty

    Affiliations Laboratoire de Neurosciences Cognitives (LNC) - INSERM U960 & IEC - Ecole Normale Supérieure (ENS), 75005 Paris, France, Laboratoire de Psychopathologie and Neuropsychologie (LPN, EA2027), Université Paris 8, Saint-Denis 93526 cedex, France

Abstract

People display facial reactions when exposed to others' emotional expressions, but exactly what mechanism mediates these facial reactions remains a debated issue. In this study, we manipulated two critical perceptual features that contribute to determining the significance of others' emotional expressions: the direction of attention (toward or away from the observer) and the intensity of the emotional display. Electromyographic activity over the corrugator muscle was recorded while participants observed videos of neutral to angry body expressions. Self-directed bodies induced greater corrugator activity than other-directed bodies; additionally corrugator activity was only influenced by the intensity of anger expresssed by self-directed bodies. These data support the hypothesis that rapid facial reactions are the outcome of self-relevant emotional processing.

Introduction

Emotional expressions are critical to the coordination of social interactions by providing information about the emitter's emotional states and behavioral intentions and by evoking reactions in the observer [1][4]. The research agrees that when exposed to emotional expressions, people display rapid facial reactions (RFRs) detectable by electromyography (EMG) [5][9]. While viewing static or dynamic happy faces elicits increased zygomaticus major activity (pulling the corners of the mouth back and upwards into a smile), angry faces evoke increased corrugator supercilii activity (pulling the brows together) [10][20]. Nevertheless, exactly what mechanism mediates these facial reactions remains a debated issue [8], [21][23].

One major theoretical framework proposes that these facial reactions reflect the readout of emotional processing [6], [24], [25]. Within this framework, the appraisal perspective postulates that a multimodal organization of response patterns (which includes facial expressions and physiological reactions) is established according to appraisal configurations (novelty, coping potential, relevance, etc.) that are emotion-specific [1], [26], [27]. The emotional readout framework implies that people would be disposed to react with emotion-specific response patterns to biologically relevant stimuli such as expressions of anger [6]; and also that a given facial expression can elicit a different emotion and thus a divergent reaction in the observer, such as, for instance, a posture of submission in response to a threatening expression. This partly explains why facial reactions are less automatic than first thought [28], and why their production varies substantially as a function of the social context, the perceived emotion [29], [30], and the relationship between the expresser and the observer [31].

The present experiment manipulated the self-relevance of stimuli to further verify the contribution of affective processes to RFRs. Recent work converges toward the view that the ability to initiate adapted behaviors in response to others' emotional signals mainly depends on the capacity to correctly evaluate the functional significance of the emitted signal for the self [32]. Several factors can therefore influence how self-relevant a given emotional signal is, thereby determining how an observer will evaluate and respond to it. Direction of gaze and body posture are among the most socially relevant cues through which we gain information regarding the source of an individual's emotional reaction and the target of their impending actions. Such cues are particularly significant for anger because of their prime importance in regulating social interactions in both human [33] and non-human [34] primates. Facial expressions of anger have been shown to be more accurately and quickly recognized, and judged to be more intense, when coupled with direct gaze [35][40]. Additionally, Hess et al. [31] revealed an increase in the EMG activity of the orbicularis occuli in response to funny films of increasing intensity in the presence of friends but not of strangers; strongly suggesting that both self-relevance appraisal and the intensity of eliciting stimuli are important determinants of emotional facial reactions.

Here we elaborated upon the above-mentioned results by varying two independent critical cues in face to face interactions: body orientation, proven to be important in determining to whom social attention is directed (toward or away from the observer), and the intensity of the emotional display (different levels of angry body expressions). We presented dynamic bodily expressions of anger of increasing intensity, directed toward or away from the observer. First, whether previous findings could be generalized to angry body expressions remains to be established, but if affective processes participate in facial reactions, RFRs should be elicited for other forms of emotional communication signals than facial expressions, such as bodily expressions. Second, the observer's facial EMG responses to emotional expressions as a function of face direction has only been explored in two studies [17], [41]. Besides presenting conflicting results, these studies were limited in that subjects were explicitely instructed to determine the presence or absence of eye contact. Thus, by potentially influencing the importance attributed to gaze direction, they might have biased facial EMG activity. Yet, if the relevance of other's emotional expressions impact the oberver's affective processing, being the target of an expression of anger is expected to implicitely trigger more activity in the corrugator supercilii, as compared to being a simple observer of that expression. Moreover, the level of muscle activity is expected to fluctuate with the intensity of the displayed expression.

Methods

Ethics

The present study obtained ethics approval from the local research ethics committees (CPP Ile de France III and Institut Mutualiste Montsouris) at all institutions where participants were recruited and human experimentation was conducted.

Stimuli

Eight professional actors (four males) were hired and instructed to begin at neutral and to increase their expression of anger in seven to nine 3 s increments according to the experimenters signal in front of a camera until deemed satisfactory. Performances were filmed with two cameras: one was facing the actor; the second at a 45°angle relative to the first creating the impression that the expression was aimed toward the observer (oriented-to-self condition) or toward another (oriented-to-other condition).

Videos were edited using Windows Movie Maker and several 2 sec (25 frames per second) fragments were selected to obtain two extracts for each condition from neutral to extreme anger with two different viewpoints. Clips of actors seen from the side were flipped to obtain equal numbers of left and right videos and faces were blurred using the Adobe After-effect software, to preclude extraction of any emotional cues conveyed by them and restricting information to the body.

Selection of the final material was based on the results of a behavioral pilot study. A total of 312 edited video clips including all the original steps from neutral to anger for each actor were presented on a PC screen. Participants (n = 23) were instructed to evaluate the intensity of the actor's bodily expression on a continuous scale from neutral to high anger. Two-tailed paired t-tests were used to compare increments and the results permitted the selection of the most consistently convincing performances of each actor's range, corresponding to 4 significantly different steps in the degree of expressed anger (p<0.05). We retained 96 videos corresponding to 8 actors, 4 levels of anger (neutral; mild; moderate; intense anger) and 2 points of view (oriented to self and other, both right and left viewpoints). A 2×4 factorial design was built, with Target of Attention (Self or Other) and Levels of emotion (neutral (1); mild (2), moderate (3) and intense anger (4)) as factors (see Fig. 1).

thumbnail
Figure 1. 2*4 factorial design.

Short movies of neutral (1), mild (2), moderate (3) and intense anger (4) oriented-to-Self and oriented-to-Other were presented.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055885.g001

Validation of the stimuli

Two behavioral experiments were conducted on the selected 96 stimuli.

Identification of Anger.

This study assessed the ability to identify anger from dynamic body expressions. Participants (n = 20) were requested to decide (forced-choice) for each video whether the expression of the actor was “neutral”, “angry” or “other”. The order of the stimuli was fully randomized, as well as the order of the response words on the response screen. Categorization rates were percent transformed and submitted to a repeated measures ANOVA with within-subject factors of Target of Attention (Self or Other), Levels of Emotion (1, 2, 3, 4) and Choice (Anger, Neutral, Other). Greenhouse-Geisser epsilons (ε) and p values after correction were reported where appropriate. Post-hoc comparisons (two-tailed t-tests) were performed for the analysis of simple main effects when significant interactions were obtained.

The ANOVA revealed a main effect of Choice, F(2,38) = 36.57; p<.001, but no main effect of Target (F(1,19) = 1.30; p = .26), nor a main effect of Levels of Emotion, F(3,57) = 2.42; p = .075; ε = 0.67; pcorr = .101. Of interest, only the interaction between Levels of Emotion and Choice, F(6,114) = 143.06; p<.001; ε = 0.55; pcorr<.001 reached significance. For both Self- and Other-directed expressions, level 1 was correctly categorized as “Neutral” (as compared to “Anger” and “Other”, all ps<.001), and levels 3 and 4 as “Anger” (as compared to “Neutral” and “Other”, all ps<.001). The response accuracy for these conditions was above 75% and differed from chance level (33%) at p<0.001 (See Fig. 2). This was not the case for the mild levels of anger where accuracy did not significantly differ from chance level (Other2 = 36%, p = .497; Self2 = 39%, p = .195). These mild levels were ambiguous as participants responded “Neutral”, “Angry” or “Other” equally for both Self- and Other-directed expressions (all ps>.169; See Fig. 2 and Table S1).

thumbnail
Figure 2. Results from the categorization task.

Mean percentage for each choice (Anger, Neutral or Other) of the categorization task plotted as a function of the Levels of Emotion (1, 2, 3, 4).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055885.g002

Subjective Feelings.

The second experiment assessed the intensity of the participants (n = 20)’ feelings when confronted with angry body expressions. Participants were requested to evaluate the intensity of Felt Confusion, Surprise, Sadness, Threat and Irritation on 5 graduated scales from 0 to 9. The five scales appeared on the screen following each video, and their order was randomized between subjects. The order of the stimuli was fully randomized. Ratings were submitted to a repeated-measures ANOVA with within-subject factors of Feelings (Confusion, Surprise, Sadness, Threat and Irritation), Target of Attention (Self or Other) and Levels of Emotion (1, 2, 3, 4). Greenhouse-Geisser epsilons (ε) and p values after correction were reported where appropriate. Post-hoc comparisons (two-tailed t-tests) were performed for the analysis of simple main effects when significant interactions were obtained.

The ANOVA indicated a main effect of Feelings, F(4,76) = 16.09; p<.001, ε = 0.82; pcorr<.001, and a main effect of Levels of Emotion, F(3,57) = 48.59; p<.001; ε = 0.38; pcorr<.001, but no main effect of Target, F(1,19) = 2.64; p = .12. There was a significant interaction between Feelings * Levels of Emotion, F(12,228) = 19.57; p<.001; ε = 0.37; pcorr<.001. The intensity of the Feelings increased with the Levels of Emotion (Level1<Level2<Level3<Level4 - all t(19)>36.22; all ps<.001), except for Sadness (Level1 = Level2 = Level3<Level4)(See Table S2 and Fig. 3). Of interest here, there was a significant interaction between Feelings * Target, F(4,76) = 6.25; p<.001; ε = 0.68; pcorr = .001. Self- as compared to Other-directed expressions were perceived as more Threatening (t(19) = 2.67; p = .015) and more Irritating (t(19) = 2.54; p = .02). There was no difference for the other Feelings (ps>.23).

thumbnail
Figure 3. Intensity of felt emotions.

The intensity of Felt Emotions (Threatened, Irritated, Surprised, Confused and Sad) with standard-errors are plotted as a function of the Target of Attention (S for Self, O for Other), and the Levels of Emotion (1, 2, 3, 4). The grey asterisks on the right signal feelings that significantly increased with Levels of Emotion. Blacks asterisks on panels signals feelings that significantly increased for Self as compared to Other-directed body.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055885.g003

We then conducted a repeated-measures ANOVA with within-subject factors of Feelings (Threat and Irritation), Target of Attention (Self or Other) and Levels of Emotion (1, 2, 3, 4). This ANOVA revealed a main effect of Feelings, F(1,19) = 14.63, p = .001: participants felt more threatened than irritated when confronted with body expressions of anger (Mean (SEM) Threat = 3.24(.19); Irritation = 2.42(.23)(See Figure 3). It also revealed a main effect of Target, F(1,19) = 7.84; p = .011; a main effect of Levels of Emotion, F(3,57) = 65.51; p<.001; ε = 0.42; pcorr<.001, a significant interaction between Feelings * Levels of Emotion, F(3,57) = 14.86; p<.001; ε = 0.55; pcorr<.001, a significant interaction between Feelings * Target, F(3,57) = 19.87; p<.001 but no triple interaction between Feelings * Levels of Emotion * Target, F(3,57) = .082; p = .97. Importantly here, while participants rated their feeling of both Threat and Irritation higher when exposed to Self- as compared to Other-directed expressions and increased their rating of intensity of feeling as a function of the increased intensity of the stimuli, these effects were more marked for feelings of Threat (see table S2). Together, these results strongly suggest that the perception of Self-directed angry body expressions mainly prompted a feeling of Threat in the observer, as compared to other feelings (See Fig. 3).

Facial EMG experiment

Participants.

Forty-four participants (21 women) participated in the physiological experiment. All had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, were right-handed, naive to the aim of the experiment and presented no neurological or psychiatric history. All provided written informed consent according to institutional guidelines of the local research ethics committee and were paid for their participation. Due to a bad signal-to-noise ratio in physiological signals, four subjects (2 men) were excluded from final analysis leaving 40 participants (mean age = 24±0.4 years).

Experiment.

Participants had to fix a white cross centered on a 19-inch black LCD screen for a random duration of 800 to 1200 ms followed by a silent 2000 ms video showing an actor in one of the eight experimental conditions. Each video was followed by an inter-stimulus interval of 1000 ms. Additionally, 15 oddball stimuli (upside-down video-clips; see below) and 38 null events (black screen of 2 sec) were included pseudo-randomly within the stimulus sequence. The order of the stimuli was fully randomized. Subjects were instructed to press a button each time the upside-down video-clip appeared to ensure they paid attention to all the stimuli throughout the session. The participants performed at 100% of accuracy (at mean 648±22 ms) in this oddball task.

Data acquisition and analysis.

Using the acquisition system ADInstruments (ML870/Powerlab 8/30), EMG activity was continuously recorded using Sensormedics 4 mm shielded Ag/AgCl miniature electrodes (Biopac Systems, Inc). Fixation cross and stimuli onset were automatically signaled on the channels of the LabChart Pro software by a PCMCIA Parallel Card (Quatech SPP-100). Before attaching the electrodes, the target sites of the left face were cleaned with alcohol and gently rubbed to reduce inter-electrode impedance. Two pairs of electrodes filled with electrolyte gel were placed on the target sites and secured using adhesive collars and sticky tape. Following the guidelines proposed by Fridlund & Cacioppo [42], the two electrodes of a pair were placed at a distance of approximately 1.5 cm over 2 muscle regions associated with different emotional expressions. Activity over the left corrugator supercilii muscle, which lowers brows, was used as a marker of negative emotional expression [6]. Activity over the left zygomaticus major muscle, which pulls lip corners up and indexes pleasure/happiness, was used as a control recording site to verify that participants responded selectively to anger expressions. The ground electrode was placed on the upper right forehead. The signal was amplified, band-pass filtered online between 10–500 Hz, and then integrated. EMG trials containing artifacts were manually rejected. No more than 15% of the trials were dropped per muscles. Integral values were subsampled offline at 10 Hz and log transformed to reduce the impact of extreme values [9], [23]. Values were then standardized within participants and within muscle to allow comparisons. Temporal profiles of facial EMG during the first 1000 ms following stimulus onset were investigated by calculating mean amplitudes during 10 time intervals of 100 ms. Pre-stimulus values (computed over 200 ms before the stimuli onset) were then subtracted from post-stimulus activity to measure the activity level caused by viewing each stimulus (i.e., to calculate the change from baseline). EMG activity was thus defined as the change from the baseline occurring between 0 and 1000 ms after stimuli onset [10], [23]. Finally, mean levels of corrugator and zygomaticus activity were computed separately for each experimental condition.

Physiological data were first submitted, separately for each muscle, to repeated measures ANOVA using Target of Attention (Self or Other), Levels of Emotion (1, 2, 3, 4) and Time Windows (10) as within-subject factors. Second, when the Time Windows factor interacted with another factor of interest, we performed post-hoc t-tests to determine the time windows for which the effect occurred and submitted the mean activity of these windows to a new ANOVA using Target of Attention (Self or Other) and Levels of Emotion (1, 2, 3, 4) as within-subject factors. Greenhouse-Geisser epsilons (ε) and p values after correction were reported when appropriate. Post-hoc comparisons (two-tailed t-tests) were also performed for the analysis of simple main effects when significant interactions were obtained.

Results

Corrugator activit

The ANOVA indicated significant effects of Target of Attention, F(1,39) = 11.05; p = .002, Levels of Emotion, F(3,117) = 2.71; p = .048, and Time Windows F(9,351) = 45.55; p<.001; ε = 0.20; pcorr<.001 (See Table S3, and Fig. 4). The interaction between Target of Attention and Levels of Emotion, F(3,117) = 5.39; p = .002; ε = 0.77; pcorr = .004, was significant after correction, whereas the other interactions didn't reach significance after correction: Time Windows×Target of Attention, F(9,351) = 2.63; p = 0.006; ε = 0.26; pcorr = .068, and Time Windows×Levels of Emotion, F(27,1053) = 1.66; p = .019; ε = 0.23; pcorr = .127. Yet, the triple interaction between Time windows, Target of Attention and Levels of Emotion reached significance after correction, F(27,1053) = 1.67; p<.001; ε = 0.23; pcorr = .035. We then submitted the data for each time window to a second ANOVA with within-subject factors of Target of Attention (Self or Other) and Levels of Emotion (1, 2, 3, 4). This analysis revealed that the interaction between Target of Attention and Levels of Emotion was significant between 300 and 700 ms Time windows, all F(3,117)>4.4; all pcorr<.01.

thumbnail
Figure 4. Time course of the mean EMG activity.

A) Over the corrugator supercilii region as a function of the Target of Attention (S for Self (green), O for Other (blue)) and the Levels of Emotion (1,2,3,4). Activity reflects average activation during each 100-ms time interval. B) Over the zygomaticus region as a function of the Target of Attention (S for Self, O for Other) and the Levels of Emotion (1,2,3,4).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055885.g004

We thus computed the mean activity between 300 and 700 ms and submitted these data to a second ANOVA with within-subject factors of Target of Attention (Self or Other) and Levels of Emotion (1, 2, 3, 4)(See Table S4, and Fig. 5). This second ANOVA revealed a main effect of Target of Attention. Self-directed body induced greater corrugator activity than Other-directed bodies, F(1,39) = 13.02; p<.001. An interaction between Target of Attention and Levels of Emotion was also observed, F(3,117) = 6.31; p<.001; ε = 0.75; pcorr = .002, revealing that the effect of Target of Attention increased with the Levels of Emotion: the effect of Target of Attention was not significant at level 1 (i.e. Neutral stimuli-t(39) = −.605; p = .548); failed to reach significance at level 2, t(39) = 1.855; p = .071; appeared significant at level 3, t(39) = 2.338; p = .025, and reached high significance at level 4 of emotion, t(39) = 5.826; p<.001. Interestingly, for Self-directed bodies, level 1 was significantly different from level 2 (t(39) = −2.687; p = .011); level 2 and level 3 were not significantly different, t(39) = −.134; p = .897; but level 3 appeared significantly different from level 4, t(39) = −2.342; p = .024. By contrast, the different levels of emotion did not significantly differ in the Other-directed condition, all ps>.434. Finally, post-hoc analyses revealed that activity between 300–700 ms significantly differs from 0 in all experimental conditions (all t(39)>4.7;all p<.001) suggesting that all conditions triggered RFRs (see Fig. 5).

thumbnail
Figure 5. Mean activity over the corrugator supercilii region between 300 and 700 ms.

The mean (SEM) activity is represented as a function of A) the Target of Attention (Self (green), Other (blue)) and the Levels of Emotion (1,2,3,4) and, B) only for Self-oriented conditions for the 4 Levels of Anger. *p<0.05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055885.g005

Zygomatic activity

Using zygomatic activity as a control recording site, the ANOVA with Target of Attention (Self or Other), Levels of Emotion (1,2,3,4) and Time Windows of analyses (10), as within-subject factors, did not reveal any main effect nor significant interaction, all F<1.45 (Table S5, Fig. 4).

Discussion

Previous EMG experiments have consistently demonstrated that people tend to produce facial reactions when looking at other's facial expressions of emotion. Here, we found that participants displayed early facial reactions to body expressions of anger, as revealed by an increase of corrugator activity occurring 300 to 700 ms after stimulus onset. RFRs were stronger when anger signals were directed toward them as compared to averted gaze, and the higher the intensity of displayed anger, the stronger their facial reactions. We propose early RFRs to body expressions of anger might be related to an emotional appraisal process [38].

Our data reveal the same influence of the direction of attention in the RFRs to body expressions, as has been shown for faces [35], [38]. Using virtual avatars and manipulating face orientation, Schrammel et al. [17,17] demonstrated significantly higher corrugator activity for angry faces with direct gaze as compared to angry faces with averted gaze. More recently, we further provided facial EMG evidence of the critical role of attention on interpersonal facial matching by manipulating gaze direction rather than face orientation [43]. Here, even in the absence of gaze information, self-directed body expressions of anger triggered higher corrugator reactivity as compared to other-directed bodies. Our data converge with the appraisal perspective which proposes that the evaluation of emotional stimuli depends on the degree of self-relevance of the event. Within such a framework, it is proposed that anger should be rated as more intense when coupled with direct gaze as it signals a direct threat for the observer [38], [40], [44]. Indeed, this was confirmed by our behavioural pre-tests revealing that the perception of self-directed angry body expressions specifically increased the subjective feelings of being threatened.

Crucially, we have demonstrated for the first time that the intensity of bodily expressions of anger displayed by a congener enhanced RFRs only when directed toward the self. The absence of such an increase for averted bodies dismisses the possibility that these findings are strictly related to the amount of movement involved in body expressions. Together with the findings of Hess et al. [31] of increased EMG reactivity to funny films of increasing intensity in the presence of friends only, our results imply that it is the interaction between these factors that influences how self-relevant an emitted signal is and determines the levels of RFRs (here: direction of the emitters' attention and the intensity of their expression), rather than each factor individually. Moreover, our results strongly suggest that the higher the potential for interaction with another (positive in Hess et al., negative here), the higher the facial reactions in the observer.

Recently, using EEG under fMRI, we revealed that the degree of potential social interaction with another relies on the binding of self-relevant social cues 200 ms after stimulus onset in motor-related areas [45]. The present early RFRs, beginning at 300 ms after stimulus onset, may thus reflect the emotional motor response to being threatened. Activity in the corrugator supercilii muscle is largely accepted as a reflection of negative emotional reactions to negative-valenced stimuli, such as spiders and snakes [6], unpleasant scenes [46] or to negative facial expressions [23], [30], [47], and has also been demonstrated in response to static body expressions of fear [48], [49]. The present activity in the corrugator supercilii muscle triggered in response to body expressions of anger may thus relate to the observer's negative emotional reaction. As anger displays are appraised as power and dominance signals, which have been shown to trigger divergent rather than convergent responses [50], one can speculate that these RFRs convey a divergent fear response [23], [30].

RFRs over the corrugator muscle occur in response to body expressions in the absence of facial information, and regardless of body orientation and of emotional content. Although it is acknowledged that RFRs may result from multiple processes [18], [23], the presence of early RFRs in absence of facial expressions cannot be explained by strict motor mimicry as the body expressions here did not provide the cues necessary for facial motor matching. A strict motor mimicry process is indeed not sufficient to explain why RFRs are displayed to non-facial and non-social emotional pictures [6], emotional body expressions [48], [49] and auditory stimuli [51][53], nor why they are occasionally incongruent with the attended signals [23]. Moreover, our results are clearly at odds with the predictions that can be derived from a motor mimicry perspective, i.e. that participants should either display congruent RFRs to others' angry faces, irrespective of the direction of attention of the emitter [28] or display less mimicry when directed at the observer as anger conveys non-ambiguous signals of non-affiliative intentions [29], [37]. Yet, the present early RFRs elicited in all experimental conditions, including neutral bodies (Level 1), also rule out the possibility that they reflect emotional processes only and suggest that RFRs could partly result from a mere orienting response to the apparition of the stimuli and/or the observer's cognitive effort [54] to decode an emotional expression in the absence of facial information and/or the appraisal of goal-obstructiveness [54], [55]. Also, as the present findings were revealed using body expressions of anger only, we cannot simply rule out that motor-mimicry processes would occur under other experimental circumstances, nor specify how motor, emotional and appraisal processes might interact. Further experiments are thus needed to determine whether the present results can be generalized to a wider range of emotions as well as whether (and to what extent) both motor and affective processes operate when facial information is available [18].

To conclude, we not only demonstrate that the corrugator supercilii muscle can be triggered in response to angry expressions but extend these findings to dynamic bodies. The present findings corroborate the emotional readout framework and further suggest that rapid facial reactions reflect the appraisal of the context and its self-relevance which varies as a function of the emitter's direction of attention and the intensity of his/her anger.

Supporting Information

Table S2.

Mean (SEM) intensity ratings of feelings

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055885.s002

(DOC)

Table S3.

Mean (SEM) data from the Corrugator activity submitted to a repeated measures ANOVA using Target of Attention (Self or Other), Level of Emotion (1, 2, 3, 4) and Time Windows (10) as within-subject factors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055885.s003

(DOC)

Table S4.

Mean activity (SEM) between 300 and 700 ms for the Corrugator muscle region submitted to a repeated measures ANOVA with within-subject factors of Target of Attention (Self or Other) and Level of Emotion (1, 2, 3, 4).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055885.s004

(DOC)

Table S5.

Mean (SEM) data from the zygomatic activity submitted to a repeated measures ANOVA using Target of Attention (Self or Other), Level of Emotion (1, 2, 3, 4) and Time Windows (10) as within-subject factors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055885.s005

(DOC)

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Sylvie Berthoz (INSERM U669 & IMM) for administrative supports and to the anonymous referees for their constructive comments.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: JG LC RS LP. Performed the experiments: LP MC GD RS LC. Analyzed the data: LP MC GD LC JG. Wrote the paper: JG LC.

References

  1. 1. Frijda NH (1986) The emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  2. 2. Keltner D, Haidt J (2001) Social functions of emotions. In: Mayne T, Bonanno GA, editors. Emotions: Current issues and future directions. New-York: Guilford Press. pp. 192–213.
  3. 3. Fischer AH, Manstead ASR (2008) Social functions of emotions. In: Lewis M, Haviland-Jones JM, Barrett L, editors. Handbook of emotions. New-York: Guilford Press. pp. 456–470.
  4. 4. Frijda NH (2010) Impulsive action and motivation. Biol Psychol 84: 570–579 .
  5. 5. Bush LK, McHugo GJ, Lanzetta JT (1986) The effects of sex and prior attitude on emotional reactions to expressive displays of political leaders. Psychophysiology 23: 427.
  6. 6. Dimberg U, Thunberg M (1998) Rapid facial reactions to emotional facial expressions. Scand J Psychol 39: 39–45 9619131.
  7. 7. Dimberg U, Thunberg M, Elmehed K (2000) Unconscious facial reactions to emotional facial expressions. Psychol Sci 11: 86–89.
  8. 8. Hess U, Blairy S (2001) Facial mimicry and emotional contagion to dynamic emotional facial expressions and their influence on decoding accuracy. Int J Psychophysiol 40: 129–141.
  9. 9. McIntosh GJ (2006) Spontaneous facial mimicry, liking, and emotional contagion. Polish Psychological Bulletin 37: 31–42.
  10. 10. Dimberg U (1982) Facial reactions to facial expressions. Psychophysiology 19: 643–647.
  11. 11. Wild B, Erb M, Bartels M (2001) Are emotions contagious? Evoked emotions while viewing emotionally expressive faces: quality, quantity, time course and gender differences. Psychiatry Res 102: 109–124.
  12. 12. Sonnby-Borgstrom M (2002) Automatic mimicry reactions as related to differences in emotional empathy. Scand J Psychol 43: 433–43.
  13. 13. de Wied M, van Boxtel A, Zaalberg R, Goudena PP, Matthys W (2006) Facial EMG responses to dynamic emotional facial expressions in boys with disruptive behavior disorders. J Psychiatr Res 40: 112–121 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2005.08.003.
  14. 14. Weyers P, Mehlberger A, Hefele C, Pauli P (2006) Electromyographic responses to static and dynamic avatar emotional facial expressions. Psychophysiology 43: 450–453 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00451.x.
  15. 15. Schilbach L, Eickhoff SB, Rotarska-Jagiela A, Fink GR, Vogeley K (2008) Minds at rest? Social cognition as the default mode of cognizing and its putative relationship to the “default system” of the brain. Conscious Cogn 17: 457–467 10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.013.
  16. 16. Sato W, Fujimura T, Suzuki N (2008) Enhanced facial EMG activity in response to dynamic facial expressions. Int J Psychophysiol 70: 70–74 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.06.001.
  17. 17. Schrammel F, Pannasch S, Graupner ST, Mojzisch A, Velichkovsky BM (2009) Virtual friend or threat? The effects of facial expression and gaze interaction on psychophysiological responses and emotional experience. Psychophysiology 46: 922–931 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00831.x.
  18. 18. Moody EJ, McIntosh DN (2011) Mimicry of Dynamic Emotional and Motor-Only Stimuli. Social Psychological and Personality Science 2: 679–686.
  19. 19. Rymarczyk K, Biele C, Grabowska A, Majczynski H (2011) EMG activity in response to static and dynamic facial expressions. Int J Psychophysiol 79: 330–333 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.11.001.
  20. 20. Dimberg U, Andreasson P, Thunberg M (2011) Emotional Empathy and Facial Reactions to Facial Expressions. Journal of Psychophysiology 25: 26–31.
  21. 21. Hess U, Philippot P, Blairy S (1998) Facial Reactions to Emotional Facial Expressions: Affect or Cognition? Cogn Emot 12: 509–531.
  22. 22. Moody EJ, McIntosh DN (2006) Mimicry and autism: bases and consequences of rapid, automatic matching behavior. In: Rogers S, Williams J, editors. Imitation and the social mind: Autism and typical develoment. New-York: Guilford Press. pp. 71–95.
  23. 23. Moody EJ, McIntosh DN, Mann LJ, Weisser KR (2007) More than mere mimicry? The influence of emotion on rapid facial reactions to faces. Emotion 7: 447–457 2007-06782-020.
  24. 24. Buck R (1994) Social and emotional functions in facial expression and communication: The readout hypothesis. Biol Psychol 38: 95–115.
  25. 25. Cacioppo JT, Petty RP, Losch ME, Kim HS (1986) Electromyographic activity over facial muscle regions can differentiate the valence and intensity of affective reactions. J Pers Soc Psychol 50: 268.
  26. 26. Ellsworth PC, Scherer KR (2003) Appraisal processes in emotion. In: Davidson RJ, Goldsmith H, Scherer KR, editors. Handbook of Affective Sciences. New-York: Oxford University Press. pp. 572–595.
  27. 27. Roseman IJ, Smith CA (2001) Appraisal Theory. In: Schere KR, Schorr A, Johnstone T, editors. Appraisal Processes in Emotion: Theory, Methods, Research. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 3–19.
  28. 28. Chartrand TL, Bargh JA (1999) The chameleon effect: The perception–behavior link and social interaction. J Pers Soc Psychol 76: 893–910.
  29. 29. Bourgeois P, Hess U (2008) The impact of social context on mimicry. Biol Psychol 77: 343–352 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.11.008.
  30. 30. van der Schalk J, Fischer A, Doosje B, Wigboldus D, Hawk S, et al. (2011) Convergent and divergent responses to emotional displays of ingroup and outgroup. Emotion 2: 298.
  31. 31. Hess WR, Akert K (1995) The intensity of facial expression is determined by underlying affective state and social situation. J Pers Soc Psychol 69: 280–288.
  32. 32. Loveland K (2001) Toward an ecological theory of autism. In: Burack CK, Charman T, Yirmiya N, Zelazo PR, editors. The Development of Autism: Perspectives from Theory and Research. New Jersey: Erlbaum Press. pp. 17–37.
  33. 33. Argyle M (1988) Bodily Communication (2nd edition). New York: Methuen.
  34. 34. Emery NJ, Amaral DG (2000) The role of the amygdala in primate social cognition. In: Lane RD, Nadel L, editors. Cognitive neuroscience of emotion. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 156–191.
  35. 35. Adams RB Jr, Gordon HL, Baird AA, Ambady N, Kleck RE (2003) Effects of gaze on amygdala sensitivity to anger and fear faces. Science 300: 1536.
  36. 36. Adams RB Jr, Kleck RE (2005) Effects of direct and averted gaze on the perception of facially communicated emotion. Emotion 5: 3–11.
  37. 37. Hess U, Adams R, Kleck R (2007) Looking at You or Looking Elsewhere: The Influence of Head Orientation on the Signal Value of Emotional Facial Expressions. Motivation & Emotion 31: 137–144 Article.
  38. 38. Sander D, Grandjean D, Kaiser S, Wehrle T, Scherer KR (2007) Interaction effects of perceived gaze direction and dynamic facial expression: Evidence for appraisal theories of emotion. Eur J Cognit Psychol 19: 470–480.
  39. 39. Bindemann M, Burton MA, Langton SRH (2008) How do eye gaze and facial expression interact? Visual Cognition 16: 733.
  40. 40. Cristinzio C, N'Diaye K, Seeck M, Vuilleumier P, Sander D (2010) Integration of gaze direction and facial expression in patients with unilateral amygdala damage. Brain 133: 248–261.
  41. 41. Mojzisch A, Schilbach L, Helmert JR, Pannasch S, Velichkovsky BM, Vogeley K (2006) The effects of self-involvement on attention, arousal, and facial expression during social interaction with virtual others: A psychophysiological study. Social Neuroscience 1: 184–195 10.1080/17470910600985621.
  42. 42. Fridlund AJ, Cacioppo JT (1986) Guidelines for Human Electromyographic Research. Psychophysiology 23: 567–589.
  43. 43. Soussignan R, Chadwick M, Philip L, Conty L, Dezecache G, Grèzes J (2012) Self-relevance appraisal of gaze direction and dynamic facial expressions: Effects on facial electromyographic and autonomic reactions. Emotion 10.1037/a0029892.
  44. 44. N'Diaye K, Sander D, Vuilleumier P (2009) Self-relevance processing in the human amygdala: gaze direction, facial expression, and emotion intensity. Emotion 9: 798–806.
  45. 45. Conty L, Dezecache G, Hugueville L, Grèzes J (2012) Early Binding of Gaze, Gesture, and Emotion: Neural Time Course and Correlates. The Journal of Neuroscience 32: 4531–4539.
  46. 46. Bradley MM, Lang PJ (2007) Emotion and motivation. In: Cacioppo JT, Tassinary JG, Bernston G, editors. Handbook of Psychophysiology. New-York: Cambridge University press. pp. 581–607.
  47. 47. Balconi M, Bortolotti A, Gonzaga L (2011) Emotional face recognition, EMG response, and medial prefrontal activity in empathic behaviour. Neurosci Res 71: 251–259 10.1016/j.neures.2011.07.1833.
  48. 48. Magnee MJCM, Stekelenburg JJ, Kemner C, de Gelder B (2007) Similar facial electromyographic responses to faces, voices, and body expressions. Neuroreport 18: 369–372.
  49. 49. Tamietto M, Castelli L, Vighetti S, Perozzo P, Geminiani G, et al. (2009) Unseen facial and bodily expressions trigger fast emotional reactions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106: 17661–17666.
  50. 50. Tiedens LZ, Fragale AR (2003) Power moves: Complementarity in submissive and dominant nonverbal behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol 84: 558–568.
  51. 51. Bradley MM, Lang PJ (2000) Affective reactions to acoustic stimuli. Psychophysiology 37: 204–215.
  52. 52. Hietanen JK, Surakka V, Linnankoski I (1998) Facial electromyographic responses to vocal affect expressions. Psychophysiology 35: 530–536.
  53. 53. de Gelder B, Vroomen J, Pourtois G, Weiskrantz L (1999) Non-conscious recognition of affect in the absence of striate cortex. Neuroreport 10: 3759–3763 10716205.
  54. 54. Smith CA (1989) Dimensions of appraisal and physiological response in emotion. J Pers Soc Psychol 56: 339–353.
  55. 55. Aue T, Scherer KR (2011) Effects of intrinsic pleasantness and goal conduciveness appraisals on somatovisceral responding: Somewhat similar, but not identical. Biol Psychol 86: 65–73.