Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Behavioural Disturbances in a Temperate Fish Exposed to Sustained High-CO2 Levels

  • Fredrik Jutfelt ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Fredrik Jutfelt, Karine Bresolin de Souza

    fredrik.jutfelt@bioenv.gu.se

    Affiliation Department of Biological and Environmental Sciences, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden

  • Karine Bresolin de Souza ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Fredrik Jutfelt, Karine Bresolin de Souza

    Affiliation Department of Biological and Environmental Sciences, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden

  • Amandine Vuylsteke,

    Affiliation Department of Biological and Environmental Sciences, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden

  • Joachim Sturve

    Affiliation Department of Biological and Environmental Sciences, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden

Abstract

As atmospheric CO2 levels rise, the CO2 concentration in ocean surface waters increases through a process commonly referred to as ocean acidification. Recently, surprising behavioural modifications has been detected in the early life stages of tropical coral reef fish exposed to ocean acidification-relevant CO2 concentrations, but it has been unclear if this effect could occur in temperate waters. Here we show several severe behavioural disturbances, including effects on boldness, exploratory behaviour, lateralisation, and learning in a temperate fish, the three-spined stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus). The behavioural effects were consistent throughout the exposure period and increased in effect size with exposure time. We observed the effects on adult sticklebacks, a species known to be tolerant to other environmental stressors. Our findings suggest that behavioural abnormalities that stem from CO2 exposure are not restricted to sensitive tropical species or early life stages and may therefore affect fish on a global scale. The severity of disturbances and the possibility of a serious behavioural problem for fish across the globe is cause for concern.

Introduction

Anthropogenic CO2 emissions are increasing the atmospheric CO2 concentration, which drives increasing dissolution of CO2 into the oceans through a process commonly referred to as ocean acidification [1]. Teleost fish have been regarded as highly tolerant to the effects of ocean acidification [2][4]. As fish are active organisms with a high metabolism, their muscle activity produces CO2 at a variable rate. Consequently, they experience internal fluctuations in CO2 concentration. The gill is the major organ for pH regulation where hydrogen ions are excreted and bicarbonate ions absorbed [2]. These mechanisms effectively buffer the blood pH during transient periods of high water CO2 concentration, and thus, fish have been assumed to be tolerant to the relatively modest CO2-challenge of ocean acidification [3], [5]. Maintaining blood pH despite CO2 exposure, however, requires modifications in concentrations of blood ions, including increased HCO3, decreased Cl, and increased Na+ concentrations [5][7].

Recently a series of studies from an Australian group reported unexpected behavioural effects in several coral reef species [8]. The effects include olfactory disturbance and olfaction preference reversal in larval clownfish [9]; impairment of visual risk assessment in juvenile damselfish (Pomacentrus amboinensis) [10]; reduced auditory response [11] and learning [12] in clownfish (Amphiprion percula); reduced olfactory prey detection, reduced feeding, and increased activity in brown dottyback (Pseudochromis fuscus) [13]; and reduced lateralisation and prey detection in a larval Neopomacentrus azysron, another coral reef species [14], [15]. Atlantic cod larvae were found to be behaviourally resistant to very high pCO2 (4200 µatm) [16], indicating that sensitivity to CO2 changes can be highly variable among teleost fish. It has been suggested that fish living at the warm edge of their tolerance range are more sensitive to CO2-induced acidification [17]. As a result, tropical reef fish have been suggested to be among the most sensitive.

Nilsson (2012) suggested a neural mechanism as the cause of some or all of the observed behavioural effects of CO2 exposure. As fish experience increased CO2 with accompanying acidosis, they partially reduce the acidosis via the Cl/HCO3 exchanger in the gill epithelium. This leads to decreased Cl concentration as well as increased HCO3 concentration in the extracellular fluid. The GABAA receptor is a major vertebrate inhibitory receptor [18], and its main mechanism is hyperpolarisation of the post-synaptic neuron by the function as an ion channel permitting the passage of negatively charged Cl and HCO3 ions. Reduced extracellular Cl concentrations reduce the electrochemical gradient for Cl influx, while increased intracellular HCO3 concentrations may lead to efflux of anions and cause depolarisation of post-synaptic neurons and therefore possibly a reversal of normal GABAA receptor function [15]. The ubiquitous nature of this receptor in the vertebrate brain suggests that this mechanism likely affects many aspects of behaviour. To date, however, only one study has tested this hypothesis [15].

The three-spined stickleback is a species with remarkable physiological plasticity and high tolerance to fluctuations in water chemistry and temperature [19]. This species is found in a wide range of habitats, from marine to limnic and Arctic to subtropical waters [19]. The ability to acclimatise to such wide environmental conditions suggests that these fish, in theory, may be tolerant to the increases in pCO2 predicted for the end of the century.

Thus, to test the hypothesis that temperate fish are more tolerant to CO2 exposure than tropical reef fish we exposed sticklebacks to control water (330 µatm CO2) or high pCO2 water (991 µatm CO2) to mimic present-day surface CO2 concentrations and a predicted scenario for CO2 concentrations in the year 2100 (the fossil fuel intensive IPCC A1F1 emission scenario [20]), respectively. A range of behavioural tests were performed after 20 days of exposure and repeated after 40 days of exposure to test for long-term acclimation capacity. The behavioural tests were lateralisation, a measure of the behavioural asymmetry of individuals; novel object test, a widely used method for assessing boldness and curiosity in fish; and an escape chamber test designed for testing exploratory behaviour and boldness.

Materials and Methods

Ethics statement

Rearing, handling and experimental procedures were approved by the ethical committee on animal experiments of Gothenburg, Sweden (ethical permit: Fredrik Jutfelt 100-2010 and 151-2011).

Experimental animals

The study was conducted at Sven Lovén Centre for Marine Sciences, University of Gothenburg, Kristineberg (Sweden) in July and August of 2012. Marine female sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus) were caught using a seine net in Sälvik, Fiskebäckskil bay, Lysekil, Sweden (geographic coordinates: 58°14′50″N11°27′30″O). A total of 100 fishes were distributed into ten 25 L glass aquaria (ten fish each). Five aquaria were randomly assigned “control” and five “CO2”. The aquaria were constantly supplied with flow-through seawater at a rate of two L min−1 from one of four header tanks (200 L). Each header tank had a flow of five L min−1 of flow-through seawater taken from five m depth, and constant aeration. The fish were kept at 14 h∶10 h light∶dark cycle and fed ad lib twice daily with frozen Artemia naupli. Water salinity, oxygen saturation, temperature and pCO2 were measured daily and alkalinity was measured weekly. Oxygen saturation remained above 90% in all measurements. The water temperature was 17.6-°C±1.2 (SD) and salinity averaged 24.2 PSU±3.4 (SD).

Experimental treatments

The pCO2 of all aquaria was measured daily using direct pCO2 measurements with an infra-red CO2 probe (GMT 222, Vaisala, Finland) connected to a submerged gas-permeable silicone membrane and the air inside the membrane was circulated in a closed loop to equilibrate with dissolved pCO2, as described elsewhere [21]. Correct factory calibration of the probe was confirmed on several occasions during the experiment using water thoroughly bubbled with a gas mixture of 1010±10 ppm CO2 in air (AGA, Sweden).

The pCO2 of the two CO2 treatment header tanks was maintained at the target value of 1000 µatm using pH stat Computers (Aqua Medic, Bissendorf, Germany) connected to solenoid valves regulating administration of 100% CO2 gas (AGA, Sweden). The pCO2 of control aquaria was 333 µatm±30 SD and the pCO2 of the CO2 treatment aquaria was 991 µatm (57±SD). The variance in pCO2 between aquaria within each header tank system was below the detection limit (<10 µatm), while the variance between header tank systems within treatment was measurable but not significant (p = 0.29). The seawater carbonate system speciation was calculated using salinity, temperature, pCO2, and alkalinity in CO2calc (Hansen, USGS, USA); constants: [22] and [23]. The water chemistry data is summarized in table 1.

thumbnail
Table 1. Water chemistry for the treatments Control and Elevated CO2.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0065825.t001

Half of the fishes were exposed to increased pCO2 over a period of 43 days while the other half was kept in control water. Aquaria were randomly attributed control or elevated CO2. The fish were allowed to acclimatize to the aquaria for ten days before the CO2 exposure started.

Behaviour experiments

The behavioural experiments were carried out in the same environmental conditions (temperature, salinity, light, pCO2) as during the exposure. 20 fishes from each treatment (randomly netted) were used in each experiment. All tests were done by direct visual observation. Experiments were performed using individual sticklebacks gently introduced into the experimental chambers, always at the same starting point and position. PlastKapTek, Sweden manufactured the acrylic experimental chambers. The behavioural tests were performed twice, on four consecutive days each time, with one behavioural test each day. The tests were performed on days 20–23 and 40–43 after initiation of exposure.

Lateralization.

Fishes were individually introduced into a double T-chamber (dimensions: 50 cm long with a 9 cm wide double T-channel (figure 1), according to Domenici et al. 2012) and gently encouraged by a plastic rod to move forward until a left or right turning choice was made. The procedure was repeated twelve times for each of 20 fish at day twenty, and twenty times for each of 25 fish at day 40. The purpose of increasing the number of turning events and n at day 40 was to reduce variance and increase statistical power. The relative and absolute lateralization indexes were calculated according to Domenici et al. 2012.

thumbnail
Figure 1. View from above of the double T-chamber used for lateralization tests.

A turning choice is manually recorded every time the fish leave the central channel and enter one of the side channels. The use of a double T minimizes handling of the fish as multiple choice events can be performed by only gently encouraging the fish back along the channel.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0065825.g001

Novel object.

Fishes were left to settle individually in a test aquarium for 3 min, followed by the introduction of the novel object (a 5 cm Rubik's cube) to the aquarium. The time each fish spent investigating the object was recorded during 7 minutes. The novel object test is intended to measure fear of novelty, and can thus be used to categorize fish on a bold to shy axis [24].

Escape challenge.

Fish were placed in a white vertical cylindrical chamber (Ø16 cm×9 cm with a Ø5 cm exit hole on the side), with their heads facing away from the chamber's exit. Time spent inside the chamber until escape was recorded. The procedure was performed twice for each fish. We had not seen this experiment described elsewhere and it was designed to assess exploratory behaviour after pilot studies indicated reduced exploratory behaviour.

Statistics

No tank effects were detected (one-way ANOVA, p>0.1), so tanks were pooled within treatment. Statistical differences between treatment groups were tested for using an independent samples t-test with equal variances not assumed in SPSS. Differences are considered statistically significant when p<0.05, and p-values are given in the figures. Data is presented as mean ± SEM unless otherwise noted.

Results

The fish gained weight during the experiment (Figure 2) and the treatment did not significantly affect final weight (p = 0.14). Total mortality over the whole experimental period was high as expected in post-spawning females: 36% in the control group and 28% in the CO2 group, but was not affected by CO2-exposure (p = 0.59).

thumbnail
Figure 2. Weight at experiment start and finish (day 43).

Control fish in blue and CO2-exposed fish in orange. N = 50 for each treatment. Data is shown as means ± SEM.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0065825.g002

The control fish showed a wide spread of turning preferences, shown as relative lateralization index, while the CO2-exposed fish had a reduced distribution (figure 3). This difference is shown as absolute lateralization index (Figure 4) where the average turning preference is reduced by CO2-exposure. At day 20 the difference is a factor of 2 (p = 0.006), while at day 40 the effect size is larger with a factor of 3.6 (p<0.00001).

thumbnail
Figure 3. Relative lateralisation index of fish exposed to control water or CO2-enriched water for 20 and 40 days.

The histograms show the frequency of fish with each side preference from −100 to 100, where −100 indicates that all turns were to the left, 0 indicates that half of the turns were to the left and half were to the right, and 100 indicates that all turns were to the right. N = 20–25. See figure 4 for statistical analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0065825.g003

thumbnail
Figure 4. Absolute lateralisation of fish exposed to control water or CO2-enriched water for 20 and 40 days.

N = 20–25. Data are presented as means ± standard error of the means (SEM), and p-values indicate statistical significance (t-test).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0065825.g004

When presented with a novel object control fish showed curiosity and explored the object. The average time spent investigating the object was five times longer for control fish than CO2-exposed fish (p<0.00001) at day 20 (figure 5). On day 40, neither group showed interest in the Rubik's cube, possibly because the fish had examined the object on the previous test occasion (data not shown).

thumbnail
Figure 5. Novel object test.

Bars show the average time spent investigating a novel object after 20 days of control or CO2 exposure. N = 20 for each treatment. Data are presented as means ± SEM, and the p-value indicates statistical significance (t-test).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0065825.g005

Both control fish and CO2-exposed fish took roughly half a minute to escape the escape chamber at day 20, with no significant effect of treatment (figure 6). At day 40 the CO2-exposed fish took the same amount of time to escape the chamber while the control fish were six times faster (p = 0.007).

thumbnail
Figure 6. Average escape time from a chamber by fish exposed to control water or CO2-enriched water for 20 and 40 days.

N = 20 for each treatment. Data are presented as means ± SEM, and p-values indicate statistical significance (t-test).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0065825.g006

Discussion

We found surprisingly strong effects in all of the behavioural tests performed. This indicates that most, if not all, of the sticklebacks' behavioural repertoire can be affected by CO2.

As expected, the control fish turned to their preferred side 70% of the time on average. In contrast, CO2-exposed fish lacked a preference and turned to each side 50% of the time. A similar reduction in lateralisation was previously described in larvae of a coral reef fish exposed to CO2 [14], [15], indicating that this effect may be widespread both geographically as well as among species and life stages. A behavioural side preference is beneficial in a range of situations, such as multi-tasking, orientation, and escape [25], [26]. Any disturbance in behavioural lateralisation may therefore reduce fitness.

When fish were presented with a novel object in the form of a small Rubik's cube on day 20 of the experiment, control fish examined the object five times longer than CO2-exposed fish. As the novel object test is a well-established method for estimating boldness and curiosity in fish [27], [28], the finding likely indicate that CO2-exposed fish were less bold and/or less curious than control fish, with unknown fitness implications.

The difference in escape time at day 40 was mainly due to reduced escape time by the control group. The experiment was not specifically designed to test learning, yet the improvement by control fish in escape time from day 20 to day 40 is possibly due to memory of the escape technique from the previous trial. The CO2-exposed group showed no improvement at the second test, suggesting decreased learning ability. Learning is vital for acquiring new skills and behaviours. Interaction with predators is an efficient way of learning the identity of the predators, albeit sometimes with immediate costs (i.e., injury). Fish can learn to recognise predators through their own experience, through cues from injured conspecifics, and from social learning [29]. Any impairment in learning ability may therefore affect fish survival through reduced predator avoidance.

Sticklebacks are physiologically plastic with a large potential for acclimation to environmental challenges, such as changes in salinity [19] or temperature [30] and environmental toxins [31]. Acclimation to large variations in salinity and temperature commonly takes days to weeks in temperate fish [32], [33]. Additionally, G. aculeatus have short life spans of an average of 3 years; therefore, a 40-day exposure is considered long-term compared to the life span of sticklebacks and should be long enough to allow maximal acclimation. Because any possible acclimation failed to restore normal behaviours in the exposed fish, evolutionary selection may be required for sticklebacks to develop tolerance to high CO2 levels. The physiological reason behind the lack of acclimatory capacity is unknown; however, GABAA receptors may have be adapted to a narrow range of ambient CO2 levels, whereas at higher ambient pCO2, the receptors are unable to function correctly due to the associated modification of extracellular and intracellular ion concentrations.

The three behavioural tests employed in this study are artificial, and so the relevance of these tests to ecological fitness is largely unknown. In addition, the CO2 treatment had no significant effect on survival or growth over the 40-day exposure period, suggesting that the behavioural effects are not severe enough to affect performance in an aquarium setting with ad lib food supply. However, because performance on all three tests, and tentatively learning, was severely degraded, it is likely that the majority of the fishes behavioural repertoire was affected by CO2 exposure. From a physiological perspective, ubiquitous changes in behaviour are consistent with the proposed neurophysiological mechanism involving altered GABAA function [15], as GABAA receptors are omnipresent in the central nervous systems of vertebrates and any disturbance to the GABA system would affect most behaviours. Thus, complex behaviours, such as prey capture, predator avoidance, and mating rituals, would likely be disturbed in a natural setting with subsequent negative fitness consequences.

Conclusions

The current study provides indications that ocean acidification could adversely affect fish behaviour on a global scale this century. As sticklebacks are highly tolerant to many other environmental factors, it is unlikely that these animals are unusually sensitive to CO2 exposure. Severe behavioural effects have been documented in a few coral reef species (all from the order Perciformes) and here in temperate sticklebacks (order Gasterosteiformes). Thus, CO2 could potentially affect the behaviour of many, if not most, marine teleost species. As acclimation to counter the effects was not detected, despite the long exposure time, the most pressing issue now is the potential of fish to inheritably adapt to high CO2 levels to counter these behavioural disturbances. If the evolution of tolerance to CO2 is slower than the rate of CO2 increase, the ecological consequences of ocean acidification could be severe.

Acknowledgments

We thank Joacim Näslund for expertize on stickleback experimentation and Leon Green for excellent technical assistance.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: KBdS FJ JS AV. Performed the experiments: KBdS FJ AV. Analyzed the data: KBdS FJ. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: FJ. Wrote the paper: FJ KBdS.

References

  1. 1. Doney SC, Ruckelshaus M, Emmett Duffy J, Barry JP, Chan F, et al. (2012) Climate Change Impacts on Marine Ecosystems. Annu Rev Marine Sci 4: 11–37
  2. 2. Melzner F, Gutowska MA, Langenbuch M, Dupont S, Lucassen M, et al. (2009) Physiological basis for high CO2 tolerance in marine ectothermic animals: pre-adaptation through lifestyle and ontogeny? Biogeosciences 6: 2313–2331.
  3. 3. Melzner F, Göbel S, Langenbuch M, Gutowska MA, Pörtner HO, et al. (2009) Swimming performance in Atlantic Cod (Gadus morhua) following long-term (4–12 months) acclimation to elevated seawater P(CO2). Aquat Toxicol 92: 30–37
  4. 4. Ishimatsu A, Hayashi M, Kikkawa T (2008) Fishes in high-CO2, acidified oceans. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 373: 295–302
  5. 5. Brauner CJ, Baker DW (2009) Patterns of Acid-Base Regulation During Exposure to Hypercarbia in Fishes. Glass ML, Wood SC, editors Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg. doi:10.1007/978-3-540-93985-6.
  6. 6. Foss A, Røsnes BA, Øiestad V (2003) Graded environmental hypercapnia in juvenile spotted wolffish (Anarhichas minor Olafsen): effects on growth, food conversion efficiency and nephrocalcinosis. Aquaculture 220: 607–617
  7. 7. Fivelstad S, Haavik H, Løvik G, Olsen AB (1998) Sublethal effects and safe levels of carbon dioxide in seawater for Atlantic salmon postsmolts (Salmo salar L.): ion regulation and growth. Aquaculture 160: 305–316
  8. 8. Briffa M, la Haye de K, Munday PL (2012) High CO2 and marine animal behaviour: Potential mechanisms and ecological consequences. Marine Pollution Bulletin 64: 1519–1528
  9. 9. Munday PL, Dixson DL, Donelson JM, Jones GP, Pratchett MS, et al. (2009) Ocean acidification impairs olfactory discrimination and homing ability of a marine fish. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106: 1848–1852
  10. 10. Ferrari MCO, McCormick MI, Munday PL, Meekan MG, Dixson DL, et al. (2012) Effects of ocean acidification on visual risk assessment in coral reef fishes. Functional Ecology 26: 553–558
  11. 11. Simpson SD, Munday PL, Wittenrich ML, Manassa R, Dixson DL, et al. (2011) Ocean acidification erodes crucial auditory behaviour in a marine fish. Biol Lett 7: 917–920
  12. 12. Ferrari MCO, Manassa RP, Dixson DL, Munday PL, McCormick MI, et al. (2012) Effects of Ocean Acidification on Learning in Coral Reef Fishes. PLoS ONE 7: e31478
  13. 13. Cripps IL, Munday PL, McCormick MI (2011) Ocean acidification affects prey detection by a predatory reef fish. PLoS ONE 6: e22736
  14. 14. Domenici P, Allan B, McCormick MI, Munday PL (2012) Elevated carbon dioxide affects behavioural lateralization in a coral reef fish. Biol Lett 8: 78–81
  15. 15. Nilsson GE, Dixson DL, Domenici P, McCormick MI, Sørensen C, et al. (2012) Near-future carbon dioxide levels alter fish behaviour by interfering with neurotransmitter function. Nature Climate Change 2: 201–204
  16. 16. Maneja RH, Frommel AY, Browman HI, Clemmesen C, Geffen AJ, et al. (2012) The swimming kinematics of larval Atlantic cod, Gadus morhua L., are resilient to elevated seawater pCO2. Mar Biol
  17. 17. Pörtner HO, Farrell AP (2008) Physiology and climate change. Science 322: 690–692.
  18. 18. Rabow LE, Russek SJ, Farb DH (2004) From ion currents to genomic analysis: Recent advances in GABAA receptor research. Synapse 21: 189–274
  19. 19. Östlund-Nilsson S, Mayer I, Huntingford FA (2006) Biology of the three-spined stickleback. Boca Raton: Taylor & Francis.
  20. 20. Solomon S, Qin D, Manning M, Alley RB, Berntsen T, et al.. (2007) Contribution of Working Group I to the Fourth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change. Cambridge, UK and NY, USA: Cambridge University Press.
  21. 21. Munday PL, Pratchett MS, Dixson DL, Donelson JM, Endo GGK, et al. (2012) Elevated CO2 affects the behavior of an ecologically and economically important coral reef fish. Mar Biol
  22. 22. Roy RN, Roy LN, Vogel KM, Porter-Moore C, Pearson T, et al. (1993) The dissociation constants of carbonic acid in seawater at salinities 5 to 45 and temperatures 0 to 45 C. Marine Chemistry 44: 249–267.
  23. 23. Dickson AG (1990) Standard potential of the reaction: , and and the standard acidity constant of the ion HSO4− in synthetic sea water from 273.15 to 318.15 K. The Journal of Chemical Thermodynamics 22: 113–127
  24. 24. Toms CN, Echevarria DJ, Jouandot DJ (2010) A methodological review of personality-related studies in fish: focus on the shy-bold axis of behavior. International Journal of Comparative Psychology 23: 1–25.
  25. 25. Dadda M (2006) Lateralized female topminnows can forage and attend to a harassing male simultaneously. Behavioral Ecology 17: 358–363
  26. 26. Nepomnyashchikh VA, Izvekov EI (2006) Variability of the behavioral laterality in Teleostei (Pisces). J Ichthyol 46: S235–S242
  27. 27. Maximino C, de Brito TM, da Silva Batista AW, Herculano AM, Morato S, et al. (2010) Measuring anxiety in zebrafish: A critical review. Behavioural Brain Research 214: 157–171
  28. 28. Dahlbom SJ, Lagman D, Lundstedt-Enkel K, Sundström LF, Winberg S (2011) Boldness Predicts Social Status in Zebrafish (Danio rerio). PLoS ONE 6: e23565
  29. 29. Sloman KA, Wilson RW, Balshine SS (2006) Behaviour and physiology of fish. San Diego, California: Elsevier Academic Press.
  30. 30. Barrett RDH, Paccard A, Healy TM, Bergek S, Schulte PM, et al. (2011) Rapid evolution of cold tolerance in stickleback. Proc Biol Sci 278: 233–238
  31. 31. Pottinger TG, Carrick TR, Yeomans WE (2002) The three-spined stickleback as an environmental sentinel: effects of stressors on whole-body physiological indices. J Fish Biol 61: 207–229
  32. 32. Beitinger T, Lutterschmidt W (2011) Measures of Thermal Tolerance. Elsevier Inc. 8 pp. doi:10.1016/B978-0-1237-4553-8.00200-8.
  33. 33. Sangiao-Alvarellos S (2005) Time course of osmoregulatory and metabolic changes during osmotic acclimation in Sparus auratus. J Exp Biol 208: 4291–4304