Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Qualitative and Quantitative Analyses of the Echolocation Strategies of Bats on the Basis of Mathematical Modelling and Laboratory Experiments

Abstract

Prey pursuit by an echolocating bat was studied theoretically and experimentally. First, a mathematical model was proposed to describe the flight dynamics of a bat and a single prey. In this model, the flight angle of the bat was affected by angles related to the flight path of the single moving prey, that is, the angle from the bat to the prey and the flight angle of the prey. Numerical simulation showed that the success rate of prey capture was high, when the bat mainly used the angle to the prey to minimize the distance to the prey, and also used the flight angle of the prey to minimize the difference in flight directions of itself and the prey. Second, parameters in the model were estimated according to experimental data obtained from video recordings taken while a Japanese horseshoe bat (Rhinolphus derrumequinum nippon) pursued a moving moth (Goniocraspidum pryeri) in a flight chamber. One of the estimated parameter values, which represents the ratio in the use of the angles, was consistent with the optimal value of the numerical simulation. This agreement between the numerical simulation and parameter estimation suggests that a bat chooses an effective flight path for successful prey capture by using the angles. Finally, the mathematical model was extended to include a bat and prey. Parameter estimation of the extended model based on laboratory experiments revealed the existence of bat’s dynamical attention towards prey, that is, simultaneous pursuit of prey and selective pursuit of respective prey. Thus, our mathematical model contributes not only to quantitative analysis of effective foraging, but also to qualitative evaluation of a bat’s dynamical flight strategy during multiple prey pursuit.

Introduction

Animals have various sensory systems to localize targets such as prey and conspecifics. In general, sensory systems, i.e., visual, olfactory, and auditory organs, passively detect information originating from such targets. For example, zebra finches have visual organs that allow them to detect ultraviolet wavelengths [1]; male silkmoths sense olfactory information that is unique to the sex pheromones of conspecific females [2], [3]; barn owls precisely estimate time differences in the arrival of sounds generated by prey during darkness [4]; and male concave-eared torrent frogs detect ultrasound to acoustically interact with conspecific males [5]. Moreover, animals pursue moving targets with high accuracy, using such unique sensory systems. For instance, male houseflies use visual information on conspecifics to chase other males [6], while dragonflies steer to minimize the movement of the image of prey on their retina, and directly fly towards the point of interception [7], [8].

Only a few species of animals, e.g., whales and bats, capture prey by active sensing, namely, by emitting ultrasound pulses as sensing signals and detecting the echoes reflected from the prey [9][11]. The echolocating behavior of bats has been studied on the basis of field research and laboratory experiments. Field research has shown that bats exhibit high performance during prey pursuit in natural habitats [12][14]; the bats dynamically change not only the acoustical characteristics of the ultrasound pulses, but also their flight paths to approach prey. Laboratory experiments have demonstrated further details of active sensing by bats, e.g., patterns of gaze angles under controlled flight tasks [15][18]. These behavioral experiments suggest that the bats exhibit a unique flight strategy during prey pursuit. To theoretically evaluate the efficiency of the flight strategy during prey pursuit, mathematical modelling as dynamical systems can be helpful. A number of theoretical studies has used dynamical models to evaluate the behavior of moving animals, such as bird flocks and fish schools [19], [20]. However, the echolocating behavior of bats has not been sufficiently investigated using dynamical models.

In the present study, we performed numerical simulations to theoretically calculate the success rate of prey capture by an echolocating bat. We then estimated the parameters of the mathematical model, based on experiments using Japanese horseshoe bats (Rhinolphus derrumequinum nippon) and moths (Goniocraspidum pryeri) in a flight chamber.

Methods

Mathematical Modelling of Prey Pursuit by an Echolocating Bat

Experimental studies in a flight chamber previously revealed that the positions of bats and moths changed much greater in the horizontal plane of the chamber than those did in the vertical plane [18], [21]; changes in the horizontal plane exceeded m, whereas those in the vertical plane were less than m. Therefore, in the present study, we focus on changes in the positions of a bat and a single prey in the horizontal plane, and model their flight dynamics as follows:(1)(2)where (, ) and (, ) represent the positions of the bat and prey in the horizontal plane, respectively; and are the flight angles of the bat and prey; and the parameters and are the flight velocities of the bat and prey. For simplicity, the bat and prey are assumed to fly with constant velocities and . In addition, is defined as the angle from the bat to the prey, as shown in Figure 1A.

thumbnail
Figure 1. A mathematical model of a bat pursuing a single prey.

(A) Definitions of the variables and parameters used in the model. (B) Echolocation range used in the numerical simulation. and represent the maximum distance and angle at which a bat can detect echoes. (C) The success rate of prey capture, , numerically calculated in , and , and an example of a prey path at (inset). (D) The success rate of prey capture, , in , and , and an example of a prey path at (inset). corresponds to the linear motion of prey, and corresponds to the random motion of the prey. has a high value almost everywhere within . In addition, has a higher value, i.e., almost , around . These numerical simulations were performed under the assumption of m/s, m/s, rad, m, and m, which were estimated from experimental data.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068635.g001

An echolocating bat can detect differences in the time and sound pressure level of arrivals of echoes reflected from moving prey, by using its ears. Whereas these differences include information about the angle from the bat to the prey, the time difference between pulse emission and echo arrival includes information about the distance from the bat to the prey. Therefore, the bat can localize the prey by a single pulse in theory. In addition, the bat successively emits ultrasound pulses, allowing it to roughly estimate the flight angle of the prey. Hence, it is assumed that the bat can use to minimize the distance from itself to the prey, and also to minimize the difference in flight directions of itself and the prey. Then, the prey is considered to fly with linear or random motion, to numerically simulate its various flight paths. Consequently, the dynamics of and are modeled as follows:(3)(4)where

(5)(6)

The parameters and describe how rapidly the bat changes , depending on the angular differences of and , respectively. represents white noise, satisfying and at time and . The parameter describes the intensity of the noise. As shown in Figure 1C and D, corresponds to the linear motion of the prey, and corresponds to the random motion of the prey. Equations 5 and 6 define the relationships of and with and ; varies from to and gives the ratio of and , while is a positive weighting factor common to and . These parameter values, i.e., , , and , can be determined by fitting the model to experimental data on flight paths of a bat and a prey.

Let us consider simple cases for and , to explain why a sinusoidal function is used in Equation 3. The first case (Case 1) is (, ) (, ), and the second case (Case 2) is (, ) (, ). In Case 1, the second term on the right side of Equation 3 is equal to , and does not affect the dynamics of . Then, the sign of depends on : namely, is negative (or positive), when (or ). This means that, when the prey is located to the right (or left) of the bat’s flight direction, the bat changes clockwise (or counter-clockwise) and approaches the prey. In Case 2, the first term on the right side of Equation 3 is equal to . Then, the sign of depends on : namely, is negative (or positive), when (or ). This means that, when the flight direction of the prey is to the right (or left) of the bat’s flight direction, the bat changes clockwise (or counter-clockwise) and flies in the same direction as the prey. These properties of a sinusoidal function are useful for modeling our assumption that an echolocating bat uses and also , to determine its own flight angle of , during prey pursuit.

Results

Numerical Simulation of the Success Rate of Prey Capture

The success rate of prey capture by a bat is calculated as follows:

  • Initial Conditions. The parameters and represent the maximum distance and angle for the bat to detect echoes from the prey (Figure 1B). A bat is located at the origin in the -dimensional space, and then starts to fly towards the right, i.e., , , and . A single prey is located on an edge of bat’s echolocation range, i.e., , with , and starts to fly towards a random direction of between and .
  • Conditions of Prey Capture. The parameter describes the distance within which the bat can capture the prey. If the prey moving with linear () or random () motion escapes from the echolocation range constrained by and , the case is considered a fail. If the prey remains within the echolocation range and is approached by the bat within , the case is considered a success.

The parameters , , and were estimated by using experimental data previously obtained from video and sound recordings in a flight chamber [18], [21]. In the experiments, a moth (Goniocraspidum pryeri) was tethered to the ceiling of the chamber (length m; width m; height m) by using a piece of string; a bat (Rhinolphus derrumequinum nippon) then approached the fluttering moth [18], [21]. The flights of the bat and moth were recorded by high-speed cameras (MotionPro X3, Integrated Design Tools, Inc., Florida, USA) capturing frames per second, as well as a ch horizontal microphone array system with a sampling frequency of kHz. Based on the video recordings obtained from sessions of successful prey capture by bats, the flight paths of the bats and moths were reconstructed in the -dimensional space of the chamber. The average flight velocities of the bats and moths in the horizontal plane of the chamber were m/s and m/s, and therefore and in Equations 1 and 2 were assumed to be m/s and m/s, respectively. In addition, the horizontal angle at which the maximum sound pressure level of the pulses decayed by was rad from the pulse direction of the bats [18], so that was assumed to be rad. Regarding , experiments using several species of prey such as midges and caddisflies revealed the maximum echolocation distance by kHz ultrasound as about m [22]. In our experiments, the dominant frequency emitted by the bats was kHz [18], [21], and the wing span of the moths (mm) was longer than that of the midges and caddisflies used in [22]. Consequently, the sound pressure level of the echoes reflected from the prey in our experiments were likely larger than those reflected from the midges and caddisflies. Therefore, the bats (Rhinolphus derrumequinum nippon) could locate the moths (Goniocraspidum pryeri) far from m in our experiments, and then was assumed as the shortest echolocation range in the numerical simulation. Furthermore, the mean body length of the bats was m [18], so that each bat could capture a moth within m of itself, i.e., in the numerical simulation.

Under the assumption of m/s, m/s, rad, m, and m, the success rate of prey capture was numerically calculated as , where and represent the numbers of successful and failed prey capture for each parameter set of , and . The initial conditions of and were varied in and at the interval of rad.

Figure 1C and D shows the results of the numerical simulation: (C) in , and (corresponding to the linear motion of the prey); and (D) in , and (corresponding to the random motion of the prey). The numerical simulation for was performed using the Euler-Maruyama method [23] with a time step of . It is shown that takes a high value almost everywhere within . Equations 5 and 6 with give a positive , satisfying ( is equal to only in the case of ), where represents the effect of on as shown in Equation 3. Hence, high within means that, if the bat uses mainly but also to determine , the bat can successfully capture its prey.

Furthermore, takes a higher value, i.e., almost , around (Figure 1C and D). Equations 5 and 6 with give and , where represents the effect of on as shown in Equation 3. Therefore, the higher value of around means that, if a bat uses and approximately in the ratio of to , the bat can capture its prey more successfully. Thus, the flight angle of the prey, , is also important for the more successful capture of a single prey by a bat.

In summary, by performing the numerical simulation with the present mathematical model, we have demonstrated that is a suitable parameter value for an echolocating bat to capture a single prey in a flight chamber.

Parameter Estimation: The Pursuit of a Single Moth by a Bat

Based on the flight paths of the bats (Rhinolphus derrumequinum nippon) and the moths (Goniocraspidum pryeri) in the -dimensional space of the flight chamber previously examined by laboratory experiments [18], [21], the values of , , and in Equation 3 were estimated as follows:(7)(8)(9)where (, ) and (, ) represent the flight paths of a bat and a moth in the horizontal plane of the chamber. The parameter represents a time step of the video recordings at frames per second [18], [21], i.e., s. The value of in Equation 3 was estimated as follows:

(10)Using the time series data for , , , and obtained from the laboratory experiments, the parameters and in Equations 5 and 6 were calculated. First, Equation 3 was transformed to with and . Second, the parameters and in Equation 3 were estimated at each time , by applying the least-squares method to the neighboring sets of and with . Finally, the parameters and were then calculated using Equations 5 and 6.

Figure 2 represents the flight paths of a bat and a moth in the horizontal plane of the chamber, and the time series data for during different flight sessions. In each session, different bat and moth individuals were used; s corresponds to the time when the bat captured the moth. As shown in Figure 2, mainly exists between and . Figure 3 shows a normalized histogram of with the bin size of obtained from flight sessions of successful prey capture by bats in a previous study [18], [21]. To empirically verify the result of the numerical simulation that is a suitable range for prey capture, the bins for were used in this histogram. There is an obvious peak in the bin of .

thumbnail
Figure 2. Parameter estimation of in Equations 5 and 6 from different flight paths (A and B) of a bat and a moth.

These paths were experimentally obtained from video recordings in a flight chamber, using different bat and moth individuals. The top panels represent the flight paths of a bat and a moth in the horizontal plane of the chamber, where the paths of the bat and moth are described by black and pink lines, respectively. The bottom panels represent the time series data for estimated by using the least-squares method. Dotted lines in the bottom panels represent and . mainly exists within and .

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068635.g002

thumbnail
Figure 3. Normalized histogram of estimated from experimental data.

This histogram was calculated with the bin size of , using the time series data for obtained from flight sessions of successful prey capture by bats. Blue bars represent the standard deviations in each bin. The histogram of has an obvious peak in the bin of .

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068635.g003

Thus, the distribution of estimated from the experimental data is consistent with the optimal value derived by the numerical simulation shown in Figure 1C and D.

Parameter Estimation: The Pursuit of Two Moths by a Bat

We examined the echolocation behavior of a bat towards moths, based on the previous experimental data [24]. In the experiments, moths were simultaneously provided in the flight chamber, and a bat captured one of these moths. Video and sound recordings were made, following the same procedures described above for a single bat and a single moth. flight sessions with successful prey capture were obtained using bats, and the flight paths of a bat and moths in the horizontal plane of the chamber were calculated as (, ), (, ), and (, ) by analyzing the video recordings. Here, (, ) represents the flight path of the moth captured by a bat, and (, ) represents the flight path of the other moth.

The mathematical model for Equation 3 was extended to include pursuit behavior by a bat towards moths, as follows:(11)where

(12)(13)

(, ) is the angle from the bat to the th moth, and is the flight angle of the th moth. The parameters and (or and ) represent the way in which is affected by the flight path of the th moth.

Using the flight paths of each animal examined in the previous laboratory experiments [24], the parameters and in Equations 12 and 13 were estimated. First, the values of , , , , , and were calculated according to Equations 710. Second, Equation 11 was transformed to with , , , and . Third, the parameters and (, ) in Equation 11 were estimated at each time , by applying the least-squares method to the neighboring sets of , , , and with . Finally, the parameters and were then calculated using Equations 12 and 13.

Figure 4 shows the flight paths of each animal in the horizontal plane, and the time series data for (, ). It can be seen that switches between states of and (the middle and bottom panels of Figure 4). In the sessions shown in Figure 4, the same bat individual and different moth individuals were used.

thumbnail
Figure 4. Parameter estimation of (, ) in Equations 12 and 13, from different flight paths of a bat and moths.

These paths were experimentally obtained from video recordings in the flight chamber, using the same bat individual and different moth individuals. The top panels represent the flight paths of a bat and moths in the horizontal plane of the chamber, and the middle and bottom panels represent the time series data for and . Note that the bat captured the first moth but not the second moth, during each session. In the top panels, the flight paths of the bat, the first moth, and the second moth are given by black, pink, and light blue lines, respectively. In the middle and bottom panels, dotted lines describe and (, ). and mainly exist around and .

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068635.g004

The normalized histogram of (, ) with the bin size of was taken from flight sessions of successful prey capture by bats. As shown in Figure 5A and B, each histogram has obvious peaks in the bins of and ; moreover, the peak in the bin of is higher than that in the bin of . Here, means that a bat approached the th moth, and means that a bat flew away from the th moth: namely, whereas corresponds to a suitable value for a bat to capture a single prey in the numerical simulation, corresponds to the value for a bat not to capture the prey (Figure 1C and D). The origins of the peaks are explained by the existence of moths; if the moths are positioned in different directions from the bat, the bat has to choose one of them and fly away from the other. Figure 5C shows the normalized histogram of in the - plane. There are peaks around (, ) (, ), (, ), and (, ). The peak around (, ) corresponds to simultaneous pursuit of both moths, the peak around (, ) corresponds to selective pursuit of the first moth, and the peak around (, ) corresponds to selective pursuit of the second moth. Thus, the histogram of in the - plane obtained from experimental data interprets the different types of spatial awareness shown by an echolocating bat towards prey.

thumbnail
Figure 5. Normalized histograms of (i =  1, 2) obtained from experimental data, i.e., video recordings of 5 flight sessions of successful prey capture by bats.

(A) and (B) Normalized histograms of (, ) with standard deviations (blue bars) with the bin size of . Each histogram has obvious peaks in the bins of and , and the peak in is higher than that in . (C) Normalized histogram of (, ) in the - plane. There are peaks around (, ) (, ), (, ), and (, ) corresponding to different pursuit strategies of the bat towards moths. The first peak represents simultaneous pursuit of both moths, the second peak represents selective pursuit of the first moth, and the third peak represents selective pursuit of the second moth.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068635.g005

Discussion

Validity of the Present Mathematical Model

In the present mathematical model, Equation 3 describes the effects of and on . Numerical simulation using our model of Equations 3 and 4 has theoretically demonstrated that the effect of is also important for successful capture of a single prey by a bat, corresponding to relatively high within , and much higher around (Figure 1C and D). Note that, in the numerical simulation, the value of was restricted in , because the value of estimated from the experimental data was mainly distributed in that region. Moreover, the parameters and in Equations 5 and 6 were estimated on the basis of experimental data for the flight paths which were recorded by high-speed cameras in the flight chamber. The normalized histogram of obtained from the experimental data had an obvious peak in the bin of (Figure 3). This agreement between the numerical simulation and parameter estimation suggests that the bat chooses an effective flight path for successful prey capture by using and , because Equation 6 with represents nonzero , except for the case of . By contrast, estimated from the experimental data sometimes takes a different value from (the bottom panels of Figure 2). This is inconsistent with the results of our numerical simulation shown in Figure 1C and D. Further studies are required to investigate origin of this inconsistency, by considering other aspects of bat’s and prey’s behavior. For example, some species of insects detect the ultrasound emitted by bats, and thereby avoid being eaten [25], [26]. In such an interactive situation, the dynamics of prey’s escape behavior is important in determining how the bat precisely pursues the prey.

For a bat and moths, the parameters and (, ) in Equations 12 and 13 were also estimated by using the video recordings obtained in the flight chamber. As shown in Figure 5C, the histogram of (, ) in the - plane had obvious peaks around (, ) (, ), (, ) and (, ). This result suggests that the bat’s strategy of pursuit towards prey can be understood on the basis of (, ), because the peaks indicate the different types of spatial awareness shown by an echolocating bat; (, ) (, ) corresponds to simultaneous pursuit of both moths, (, ) (, ) corresponds to selective pursuit of the first moth, and (, ) (, ) corresponds to selective pursuit of the second moth. From theoretical point of view, important future works include numerical simulation of the success rate of prey capture for the case of one bat and two prey by changing the parameter values of and (, ) in Equations 12 and 13, which can be compared with the results of the parameter estimation shown in Figure 5.

In summary, the present mathematical model quantitatively describes a bat’s echolocation strategy, as well as qualitatively elucidates the dynamics of bat’s attention to multiple prey. Our study is the first to evaluate a bat’s flight path during multiple prey pursuit, by using a mathematical model.

Behavioral Meanings of Estimated from Experimental Data

To examine how the parameters and in the mathematical model explain the behavioral aspects of bat’s pursuit towards prey, the estimated values of and were compared with experimental data for the emission angles of ultrasound pulses.

First, the sound pressure levels at various angles from the bat were estimated by using the sound recordings obtained from the microphone array system [18], and the angle of the maximum sound pressure level was defined as the emission angle of the ultrasound pulses, . Second, (, ) was calculated, by using estimated from the video recordings. Here, means that the bat emitted pulses towards the first moth (i.e., the moth captured by the bat), and means that the bat emitted pulses towards the second moth (i.e., the moth not captured by the bat). In other words, we can estimate towards which moth the bat emitted ultrasound pulses, on the basis of the value of obtained from the sound and video recordings. In addition, was calculated from (, ). As shown in Equations 11, 12 and 13, (, ) represents the magnitude of the effect from the th moth on . Therefore, it is expected that, the sign of is positive (or negative), when the effect of the first moth (or the second moth) on is dominant.

Figure 6 represents the time series data for (, ) and for , which were obtained from the flight session shown in Figure 4A. The dynamics of (, ) was qualitatively explained by the dynamics of : namely, when was closer to zero, had a large positive value, corresponding to an increase in the bat’s attention towards the first moth. For instance, when was close to zero and became larger, had a large positive value (see the arrows around s in Figure 6A and B). This suggests that, if the bat was targeting its pulses towards the first moth rather than the second moth, its attention towards the first moth was increasing. Moreover, when became closer to zero, had a large positive value (see the arrows around s in Figure 6A and B). This suggests that, if the bat was more precisely targeting its pulses towards the first moth, its attention towards the first moth was increasing. These agreements between the dynamics of and indicate that the dynamics of the emission angle of the ultrasound pulses can be understood by using and in our present mathematical model.

thumbnail
Figure 6. Comparison of (, ) with .

Time series data for (A) and for (B) were obtained from the flight session shown in Figure 4A. In Figure 6A, and are described by red and green lines, respectively. The dynamics of (, ) is qualitatively explained by the dynamics of , as emphasized by arrows: when was closer to zero, had a large positive value, corresponding to an increase in the bat’s attention towards the first moth. For instance, when was close to zero and became larger, had a large positive value (see the arrows around s in Figure 6A and B); when became closer to zero, had a large positive value (see the arrows around s in Figure 6A and B). Note that was calculated by using experimental data obtained from sound and video recordings, while was estimated by fitting the mathematical model of Equation 11 to experimental data obtained from video recordings.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068635.g006

Thus, the present mathematical model contributes to qualitative evaluation of interaction between a bat’s dynamical flight and echolocation strategies.

Possible Applications of the Mathematical Model

The present mathematical model may be extended to evaluate the natural foraging behavior of bats. To achieve effective pursuit in natural habitats, bats must sense the echoes reflected from multiple prey, and choose a suitable flight path to capture the most prey in the least amount of time. Moreover, several bat individuals simultaneously gather for foraging, and therefore the interactions between bats are important. Future works as an extension of the present model include experimental and theoretical evaluation of the foraging behavior of bats in the field, as well as modelling acoustic interactions between bats. Such studies will facilitate an understanding of the bat strategy for choosing a suitable flight path in a more complex environment, consisting of many bat and prey individuals.

Bats actively use auditory information to pursue prey, and can interact each other via ultrasound. Regarding control and robotics, the present model of echolocating bats will be applicable to autonomous distributed control of multiple agents. In particular, cooperative control of echolocating agents will be useful for achieving human tasks that are difficult to perform in the dark. The mathematical model of echolocating bats can be extended to such a control method for artificial agents in the engineering field, by mimicking the unique sensory systems of bats.

Acknowledgments

We thank Y. Kinoshita, Y. Yamada, N. Fujiwara, K. S. Aihara, R. Kobayashi and K. Aihara, for their valuable comments on the mathematical modelling and data analysis.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: IA EF SH. Performed the experiments: IA EF. Analyzed the data: IA EF. Wrote the paper: IA SH.

References

  1. 1. Bennett A, Cuthill I, Partridge J, Maier E (1996) Ultraviolet vision and mate choice in zebra finches. Nature 380: 433–435.
  2. 2. Kanzaki R, Soo K, Seki Y, Wada S (2003) Projections to higher olfactory centers from subdivisions of the antennal lobe macroglomerular complex of the male silkmoth. Chemical Senses 28: 113–130.
  3. 3. Koontz MA, Schneider D (1987) Sexual dimorphism in neuronal projections from the antennae of silk moths (Bombyx mori, Antheraea polyphemus) and the gypsy moth (Lymantria dispar). Cell and Tissue Research 249: 39–50.
  4. 4. Carr C, Konishi M (1990) A circuit for detection of interaural time differences in the brain stem of the barn owl. Journal of Neuroscience 10: 3227–3246.
  5. 5. Feng AS, Narins PM, Xu CH, Lin WY, Yu ZL, et al. (2006) Ultrasonic communication in frogs. Nature 440: 333–336.
  6. 6. Land M, Collett T (1974) Chasing behaviour of houseflies (Fannia canicularis). Journal of Comparative Physiology A 89: 331–357.
  7. 7. Olberg R, Worthington A, Venator K (2000) Prey pursuit and interception in dragonflies. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 186: 155–162.
  8. 8. Olberg RM, Seaman RC, Coats MI, Henry AF (2007) Eye movement and target fixation during dragonfly prey-interception flights. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 193: 685–693.
  9. 9. Johnson M, Madsen PT, Zimmer WMX, de Soto NA, Tyack PL (2004) Beaked whales echolocate on prey. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 271: 383–386.
  10. 10. Griffin DR (1958) Listening in the dark: The acoustic orientation of bats and men. Oxford, UK: Yale University Press.
  11. 11. Simmons JA, Fenton MB, O’Farrell MJ (1979) Echolocation and pursuit of prey by bats. Science 203: 16–21.
  12. 12. Surlykke A, Moss CF (2000) Echolocation behavior of big brown bats, Eptesicus fuscus, in the field and laboratory. Journal of Acoustic Society of America 108: 2419–2429.
  13. 13. Simmons JA (2005) Big brown bats and june beetles: Multiple pursuit strategies in a seasonal acoustic predator-prey system. Acoustics Research Letters Online 6: 238–242.
  14. 14. Fujioka E, Mantani S, Hiryu S, Riquimaroux H, Watanabe Y (2011) Echolocation and flight strategy of Japanese house bats during natural foraging, revealed by a microphone array system. Journal of Acoustic Society of America 129: 1081–1088.
  15. 15. Ghose K, Moss CF (2003) The sonar beam pattern of a flying bat as it tracks tethered insects. Journal of Acoustic Society of America 114: 1120–1131.
  16. 16. Ghose K, Moss CF (2006) Steering by hearing: a bat’s acoustic gaze is linked to its flight motor output by a delayed, adaptive linear law. Journal of Neuroscience 26: 1704–1710.
  17. 17. Ghose K, Horiuchi TK, Krishnaprasad PS, Moss CF (2006) Echolocating bats use a nearly timeoptimal strategy to intercept prey. PLOS Biology 4(5): e108.
  18. 18. Matsuta N, Hiryu S, Fujioka E, Yamada Y, Riquimaroux H, et al. (2013) Adaptive beam-width control of echolocation sounds by CF-FM bats, Rhinolophus ferrumequinum nippon, during preycapture flight. Journal of Experimental Biology 216: 1210–1280.
  19. 19. Vicsek T, Zafeiris A (2012) Collective motion. Physics Reports 517: 71–140.
  20. 20. Romanczuk P, Couzin ID, Schimansky-Geier L (2009) Collective motion due to individual escape and pursuit response. Physical Review Letters 102: 010602.
  21. 21. Mantani S, Hiryu S, Fujioka E, Matsuta N, Riquimaroux H, et al. (2012) Echolocation behavior of the Japanese horseshoe bat in pursuit of fluttering prey. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 198: 741–751.
  22. 22. Thomas JA, Moss CF, Vater M(2002) Echolocation in bats and dolphins. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
  23. 23. Kloeden PE, Platen E (1992) Numerical solution of stochastic differential equations. New York: Springer Verlag.
  24. 24. Kinoshita Y, Ogata D, Aihara I, Watanabe Y, Riquimaroux H, et al. (2013) Prey pursuit strategy of Japanese horseshoe bats, Rhinolophus ferrumequinum nippon, during target selection task. Proceedings of 21st International Congress on Acoustics 19: 010006.
  25. 25. Rydell J, Skals N, Surlykke A, Svensson M (1997) Hearing and bat defence in geometrid winter moths. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 264: 83–88.
  26. 26. Miller LA, Surlykke A (2001) How some insects detect and avoid being eaten by bats: Tactics and countertactics of prey and predator. BioScience 51: 570–581.