Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Incidental and Intentional Learning of Verbal Episodic Material Differentially Modifies Functional Brain Networks

  • Marie-Therese Kuhnert,

    Affiliations Department of Epileptology, University of Bonn, Bonn, Germany, Helmholtz Institute for Radiation and Nuclear Physics, University of Bonn, Bonn, Germany, Interdisciplinary Center for Complex Systems, University of Bonn, Bonn, Germany

  • Stephan Bialonski,

    Affiliation Max Planck Institute for the Physics of Complex Systems, Dresden, Germany

  • Nina Noennig,

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, University of Magdeburg, Magdeburg, Germany

  • Heinke Mai,

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, University of Magdeburg, Magdeburg, Germany

  • Hermann Hinrichs,

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, University of Magdeburg, Magdeburg, Germany

  • Christoph Helmstaedter,

    Affiliation Department of Epileptology, University of Bonn, Bonn, Germany

  • Klaus Lehnertz

    klaus.lehnertz@ukb.uni-bonn.de

    Affiliations Department of Epileptology, University of Bonn, Bonn, Germany, Helmholtz Institute for Radiation and Nuclear Physics, University of Bonn, Bonn, Germany, Interdisciplinary Center for Complex Systems, University of Bonn, Bonn, Germany

Abstract

Learning- and memory-related processes are thought to result from dynamic interactions in large-scale brain networks that include lateral and mesial structures of the temporal lobes. We investigate the impact of incidental and intentional learning of verbal episodic material on functional brain networks that we derive from scalp-EEG recorded continuously from 33 subjects during a neuropsychological test schedule. Analyzing the networks' global statistical properties we observe that intentional but not incidental learning leads to a significantly increased clustering coefficient, and the average shortest path length remains unaffected. Moreover, network modifications correlate with subsequent recall performance: the more pronounced the modifications of the clustering coefficient, the higher the recall performance. Our findings provide novel insights into the relationship between topological aspects of functional brain networks and higher cognitive functions.

Introduction

During the last years evidence has accumulated suggesting that an improved characterization of time-variant interactions between different regions within the complex network brain can be achieved with graph-theoretical approaches (see [1][6] for an overview). Within this framework a network (or graph) is considered as a set of nodes (or vertices) and a set of links (or edges) connecting the nodes. Functional brain networks can be derived from direct or indirect measurements of neural activity (e.g., electroencephalogram (EEG), magnetoencephalogram (MEG), or functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data). The sampled brain regions are usually considered as network nodes, and network links represent interactions between pairs of brain regions that can be assessed by evaluating interdependencies (see [7][11] for an overview) between their neural activities. The resulting connection schemes can then be characterized by network metrics [12] such as the average shortest path length or the clustering coefficient. The average shortest path length is the mean number of steps along the shortest paths for all possible pairs of nodes. The clustering coefficient is the mean of the local clustering coefficients of all nodes and quantifies the tendency of nodes to form local clusters. The local clustering coefficient of a node is the fraction of triangles among all connected triples with the node as their center. Large values of both and are characteristic for an ordered, lattice-like structure; low values of and are typically observed for random networks. When analyzing functional brain networks, the clustering coefficient is often interpreted as a measure of the local efficiency of information transfer, and/or of the robustness of the network to deletion of individual nodes. Similarly, the average shortest path length is often interpreted as a measure of the global efficiency of a network to transfer information between nodes [2], [5].

Higher cognitive functions are thought to result from dynamic interactions of distributed brain areas operating in large-scale networks [13][18]. Using graph-theoretical approaches, previous studies provided evidence that cognitive tasks–such as motor learning or (n-back) working memory–specifically modify functional brain networks derived from recordings of neural activities (EEG, MEG, fMRI) acquired during the tasks [19][35] and that these modifications appear to be associated with task performance [19], [24], [29], [35]. Other studies reported on associations between properties of functional brain networks recorded during a resting state condition and cognitive performance metrics assessed independently during neuropsychological evaluation [36][39].

Most of the aforementioned studies investigated cognitive functions that mainly involve prefrontal areas [40] and primary association cortices but only rarely temporolateral neocortex as well as important structures of the mediotemporal lobe, namely hippocampus and surrounding perirhinal cortex and parahippocampal gyri. The lateral and mesial temporal lobe structures are known to be involved in learning- and memory-related processes [41][49], and their functionality has frequently been investigated with fMRI [50][53], intracranial electrophysiological techniques [54][59], or via non-invasively recorded event-related electric potentials or magnetic fields [60].

Here we investigate whether incidental and intentional learning of verbal episodic material differentially modifies functional brain networks and whether modifications are related to subsequent recall performance. To this end, we assess–in a time-resolved manner–global statistical characteristics ( and ) of functional brain networks that we derive from ongoing multichannel EEG data recorded non-invasively from healthy subjects and from epilepsy patients during a neuropsychological test schedule. Chronic epilepsy and its treatment is known to impair cognitive processes and to induce functional reorganization and behavioral compensation [61][63]. Investigating both healthy subjects and epilepsy patients thus provides a spectrum of recall performances that is required to study possible relationships between learning-induced network modifications and memory.

Materials and Methods

Ethics statement

The study was approved by the local Ethical Committees, and all subjects gave written informed consent.

Subjects

Thirteen patients (age mean of years; 8 females; mean duration of epilepsy years) with pharmacoresistant epilepsies of suspected temporal or extratemporal neocortical origin as well as twenty healthy controls (age mean of years; 10 females) were included in the study. All patients had been submitted for pre-surgical evaluation at the University of Bonn Epilepsy Program [64]. Seven patients were diagnosed as having a unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy (mesial seizure-onset zone in four patients and lateral seizure-onset zone in three patients). In five patients the seizure-onset zone was located in neocortical structures (central, parietal, parieto-central, parieto-occipital), and one patient was diagnosed as having multiple seizure-onset zones. Surgery was performed in eleven patients (selective amygdalohippocampectomy; extended lesionectomy) and led to complete seizure control in six patients.

Neuropsychological test schedule and EEG recordings

Before participating in the neuropsychological test schedule, subjects were informed about its general outline. They will be asked to learn a set of words (learning task) and later on, to remember the learned words (retrieval task). They will also be asked to perform an extra control task during which they just have to listen to words, however they were not informed about a subsequent retrieval task. The test schedule thus consisted of two blocks of tasks which involved incidental (block 1) or intentional (block 2) learning and retrieval of verbal material (cf. Figure 1):

thumbnail
Figure 1. Neuropsychological test schedule.

The schedule consists of two blocks of tasks which involve incidental (block 1) or intentional (block 2) learning and retrieval of verbal material. Each block is preceded by a rest phase of 5 minutes duration. Learning tasks ( and , 3 minutes duration) are followed by a figural fluency task (FF, 1 minutes duration) and a rest phase of 3 minutes duration. Retrieval tasks ( and , 3 minutes duration) consist of a free recall of words listened to/memorized during the respective learning task.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080273.g001

  • Block 1: during a learning task () subjects were orally presented 15 highly frequent German nouns (cf. [65]) five times in randomized order. During the retrieval task () subjects were asked to freely recall words they listened to during .
  • Block 2: during a learning task () subjects were required to learn 15 highly frequent German nouns (different from those presented during) that were orally presented five times in randomized order. During the retrieval task () subjects were asked to freely recall words memorized during .

Learning and retrieval tasks lasted 3 minutes each and were separated by a figural fluency task (to minimize recency effects, to discourage verbal rehearsal, and to avoid interference of any verbal information with the learned verbal material) of 1 minute duration followed by a rest phase of 3 minutes duration. Blocks were separated by a rest phase (with eyes open) of 5 minutes duration, and a baseline recording () of 5 minutes was performed with eyes open preceding the experiment. In order to minimize possible order effects, we balanced the sequence of the blocks over subjects.

The number of correctly retrieved words ( and ; excluding perseverations) during the retrieval tasks served as measure of verbal memory (recall performance). Control subjects recalled 9.1±3.5 (range: 3–14) words () during , and 11.5±2.8 (range: 6–15) words () during . Epilepsy patients recalled 5.6±3.6 (range: 0–11) words () during , and 7.7±3.5 (range: 3–13) words () during . The correlation between and (Pearson's correlation coefficient; control subjects: ; epilepsy patients: ) as well as the difference between the distributions of performance measures (t-tests; both groups: ) proved significant. Control subjects performed significantly better than epilepsy patients (t-tests; both tasks: ), and both groups showed a significantly increased recall performance during (t-tests; both groups: ).

During the whole examination procedure EEG data were acquired continuously at a sampling rate of 254.31 Hz (16 bit A/D conversion) within a bandwidth of 0–50 Hz from 29 electrodes, and right mastoid served as physical reference. Locations and nomenclature of these electrodes are standardized by the American Electroencephalographic Society [66]. In addition, we recorded right horizontal and vertical electrooculogram. In order to minimize the influence of technical (e.g., amplifier resets) and physiologic artifacts (e.g., eye movements/blinks or head movements) we applied a wavelet-based correction scheme as described in detail elsewhere [67].

Patients were investigated, on average, seven days prior to pre-surgical evaluation with blood levels of the anticonvulsant medication within the therapeutic range.

Construction of functional networks

To construct functional networks from EEG data we associated network nodes with EEG electrodes () and inferred network links by estimating interdependencies between EEG time series from pairs of brain regions, regardless of their anatomical connectivity. For this purpose, and motivated by an increasing evidence for (phase) synchronization between distant brain areas to play an important role in learning- and memory-related processes [49], [68], [69], we employed the concept of time-variant phase coherence [70] which is an established method for studying time-variant changes in phase synchronization. The mean phase coherence [71] (1)is the temporal average of the differences of the instantaneous phases of time series from nodes and , and denotes the number of data points. By definition, is confined to the interval [0,1] where indicates fully phase-synchronized systems. We used the analytic signal approach [72], [73] with which instantaneous phases are obtained from the Hilbert transform of a time series. An important property of the analytic signal approach is that the the instantaneous frequency–in case of two or more superimposed oscillatory components–relates to the predominant frequency in the Fourier spectrum [74], [75] which may be subject to fluctuations in the time series. In such a case the instantaneous frequency varies rhythmically around the predominant frequency resulting in spurious estimates of the instantaneous phase. However, such effects can be reduced by taking the temporal average (cf. Eq. 1) or by applying adaptive approximation methods proposed recently [76]. From an electrophysiological point of view, we consider it more reasonable to look adaptively (via the Hilbert transform) at synchronization between predominant rhythms in the EEG than to look at synchronization in some a priori fixed frequency bands (e.g., via wavelet) for which there is no power in the time series (cf. Refs. [75], [77], [78]).

For further analyses and to allow an interval-based estimation of the mean phase coherence, we split the EEG time series into consecutive time intervals of 16.1 s duration each (corresponding to data points) and estimated, for each time interval, the elements of the phase synchronization matrix R. From this matrix we constructed binary networks using a thresholding approach, and set the non-diagonal elements of the adjacency matrix A to if the corresponding entry of R exceeded a threshold , and to otherwise (). The choice of a threshold is not trivial, and there is currently no commonly accepted method to infer links from estimators of signal interdependence. However, since the number of nodes and the link density can affect network characteristics [79][85] precautions must be taken to minimize such influences when comparing between different networks. In order to determine we requested that the networks are sparse () and that they do not disintegrate into unconnected network components as this leads to infinite values for the average shortest path length. Connectedness is guaranteed for mean degrees , at least for random networks [86], and with the fixed number of nodes we obtained a mean degree , such that holds.

Computation of statistical network characteristics

Here we considered the average shortest path length and the clustering coefficient as global statistical characteristics of a functional brain network. We used an algorithm proposed in Ref. [87] to determine the shortest paths between all pairs of nodes from the adjacency matrix A. The length of the shortest path between two nodes and is the minimum number of links to traverse in order to get from to (or vice versa). With this definition we derived the average shortest path length of the network by taking the average of all () with . The clustering coefficient for node can be defined as (see, e.g., [1]): (2)where denotes the degree, i.e., the number of in- or out-going connections of node . We averaged over all nodes to obtain the clustering coefficient of a network.

In the following, we consider the baseline recording () and the learning tasks ( and ) only. We omitted the retrieval tasks ( and ) and the figural fluency task in order to avoid the influence of movement-related artifacts. With the interval-based estimation of interdependencies we obtained for each subject 11 intervals for and for and 18 intervals for . Averaging over intervals resulted in mean network characteristics which we denote as and (the suffix denotes task).

Results

Task-related modifications of functional brain networks

In Figure 2 we show, for an exemplary epilepsy patient and an exemplary control subject, temporal evolutions of the network characteristics during different neuropsychological tasks. Independent of tasks, both characteristics exhibited considerable fluctuations over time, and the patient's fluctuations were more pronounced. In both subjects, the learning tasks ( and ) did not appear to modify the average shortest path length of the functional brain networks, as we could not observe clear-cut differences in between any tasks. The same holds true for the clustering coefficient of the patient's functional brain network. For the control subject however, the intentional learning task () led to a significant increase of the clustering coefficient (when compared to both and ; t-tests; ). Both network characteristics attained slightly higher values in the control subject during all tasks. When comparing the characteristics' distributions from the patient group and the control group however, we could not observe any significant differences between the groups despite their significantly different recall performances. For the binary networks considered here, this observation is in line with our previous results [67]. In the following, we present our findings obtained from merged groups.

thumbnail
Figure 2. Exemplary temporal evolutions of global statistical network characteristics during different neuropsychological tasks.

Time courses of average shortest path length (left) and clustering coefficient (right) from an epilepsy patient (red lines) and a control subject (black lines). Baseline recording (); incidental learning task (); intentional learning task (). Lines are for eye-guidance only.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080273.g002

In order to investigate whether the aforementioned influences of neuropsychological tasks on network characteristics extend beyond exemplary data, we estimated–for all subjects and each task–statistical properties of the distributions of and and evaluated possible differences between tasks (ANOVA with Greenhouse-Geisser and Huynh-Feldt corrections for departure from sphericity; ). The distributions differed between tasks for the clustering coefficient but not for the average shortest path length. From the box-plots shown in Figure 3 it can be deduced that both learning tasks did not significantly modify the average shortest path length of the functional networks. The intentional learning task , however, led to a small but significant increase of the networks' clustering coefficient ( and ; t-tests; ).

thumbnail
Figure 3. Comparison of the distributions of global statistical network characteristics from 33 subjects for different neuropsychological tasks.

Box-plots of the average shortest path length (left) and the clustering coefficient (right) for the baseline recording () and for the incidental () and the intentional learning task (). Bottom and top of a box are the first and third quartiles, and the band inside a box is the median. The ends of the whiskers represent the minimum and maximum of the data.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080273.g003

Relationship between network modifications and recall performance

We investigated whether network modifications induced during the learning tasks ( and ) are related to the number of words ( and ) subjects recalled during the subsequent retrieval tasks ( and ). Given the high interindividual variability (cf. Figure 3) we focused on the relative deviation of the average shortest path length and of the clustering coefficient during the learning tasks from the respective values during the baseline recording , i.e. and , where denotes and , respectively.

For the whole group of subjects, both characteristics deviated up to 20% from the baseline (cf. Figure 4). On average, however, deviations of the average shortest path length were close to zero (0.3% for ; 0.2% for ; no significant difference), while those of the clustering coefficient amounted to 2.9% for , being significantly higher than the deviation for (0.2%; t-test; ). Interestingly, only for the intentional learning task–for which subjects were explicitly instructed to learn the presented material–could we observe a significant positive correlation (Pearson's correlation coefficient ; cf. Figure 5) between the relative clustering coefficient and the number of subsequently retrieved words. Due to the aforementioned significant correlation between the recall performances, was also positively correlated to the number of words acquired during (Pearson's correlation coefficient ). Of note, there were no significant correlations between the recall performances and the characteristics and of the networks from the baseline recording .

thumbnail
Figure 4. Comparison of the distributions of task-induced modifications of global statistical network characteristics.

Box-plots (see Figure 3 for details) of the relative deviation of the average shortest path length (left) and of the clustering coefficient (right) during the learning tasks ( and ) from the respective values during the baseline recording .

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080273.g004

thumbnail
Figure 5. Relationships between task-induced modifications of global statistical network characteristics and recall performances.

Scatterplots of relative deviations of the average shortest path length (top) and of the clustering coefficient (bottom) during incidental (left) and intentional learning (right) from the respective values during the baseline recording and subsequent recall performances and . A significant correlation could only be observed between the relative clustering coefficient and the number of recalled words during (linear regression is represented with a solid black line).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080273.g005

Conclusions

We investigated whether functional brain networks are differentially modified during incidental and intentional learning of verbal episodic material and whether modifications are related to subsequent recall performance. We assessed the two global statistical characteristics–clustering coefficient and average shortest path length–of functional brain networks that we derived from ongoing multichannel EEG data recorded from 33 subjects during a neuropsychological test schedule. Despite the fact that learning and memory-related processes involve structures of the lateral and mesial temporal lobes, whose dynamics may not be directly accessible with non-invasive EEG recordings, we observed differential learning-related modifications of the networks' global statistical properties. While there were no detectable modifications of the average shortest path length, networks attained a significantly higher clustering coefficient during intentional learning as compared to incidental learning and to the resting state. Interestingly, modifications during intentional learning even allowed us to predict the subsequent recall performance: the more pronounced the modifications of the clustering coefficient the higher the recall performance. An elevated clustering coefficient might be indicative of an increased occurrence of small but tightly connected groups of network nodes. Identification of these groups (or communities) using appropriate analysis techniques [88], [89] may aid in further exploration of the biological relevance of communities [29] and may provide novel insights into the relationship between the topology of functional brain networks and cognitive functions.

Our findings are in line with some previous reports on task-related modifications of global properties of functional brain networks and their relationship to task performance (e.g. [19], [24], [29], [35]) and support the view that higher cognitive functions–such as learning- and memory-related processes–result from dynamic interactions of distributed brain areas operating in large-scale networks. The differences seen for intentional and for incidental learning, however, indicate that task-induced topological changes of functional brain networks may not only reflect the cognitive process per se but also attentional effects [90][92]. Exploring the interactions between attention-related networks and those important for learning and memory remains a challenging task.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: KL HH CH. Performed the experiments: HM NN HH. Analyzed the data: MTK KL. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: SB. Wrote the paper: MTK KL CH SB.

References

  1. 1. Boccaletti S, Latora V, Moreno Y, Chavez M, Hwang DU (2006) Complex networks: Structure and dynamics. Phys Rep 424: 175–308.
  2. 2. Reijneveld JC, Ponten SC, Berendse HW, Stam CJ (2007) The application of graph theoretical analysis to complex networks in the brain. Clin Neurophysiol 118: 2317–2331.
  3. 3. Ioannides AA (2007) Dynamic functional connectivity. Curr Opin Neurobiol 17: 161–170.
  4. 4. Arenas A, Díaz-Guilera A, Kurths J, Moreno Y, Zhou C (2008) Synchronization in complex networks. Phys Rep 469: 93–153.
  5. 5. Bullmore E, Sporns O (2009) Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat Rev Neurosci 10: 186–198.
  6. 6. Bullmore E, Sporns O (2013) The economy of brain network organization. Nat Rev Neurosci 13: 336–349.
  7. 7. Pikovsky AS, Rosenblum MG, Kurths J (2001) Synchronization: A universal concept in nonlinear sciences. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  8. 8. Pereda E, Quian Quiroga R, Bhattacharya J (2005) Nonlinear multivariate analysis of neurophysiological signals. Prog Neurobiol 77: 1–37.
  9. 9. Hlaváčková-Schindler K, Paluš M, Vejmelka M, Bhattacharya J (2007) Causality detection based on information-theoretic approaches in time series analysis. Phys Rep 441: 1–46.
  10. 10. Lehnertz K, Bialonski S, Horstmann MT, Krug D, Rothkegel A, et al. (2009) Synchronization phenomena in human epileptic brain networks. J Neurosci Methods 183: 42–48.
  11. 11. Lehnertz K, Krug D, Staniek M, Glusenkamp D, Elger CE (2011) Preictal directed interactions in epileptic brain networks. In: Osorio I, Zaveri H, Frei M, Arthurs S, In:editors, Epilepsy: The Intersection of Neurosciences, Biology, Mathematics, Engineering and Physics, Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. pp. 265–272.
  12. 12. da F Costa L, Rodrigues FA, Travieso G, Boas PRV (2007) Characterization of complex networks: A survey of measurements. Adv Phys 56: 167–242.
  13. 13. Squire LR, Zola-Morgan S (1991) The medial temporal lobe memory system. Science 253: 1380–1386.
  14. 14. Varela FJ, Lachaux JP, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J (2001) The brain web: Phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nat Rev Neurosci 2: 229–239.
  15. 15. Robertson LC (2003) Binding, spatial attention and perceptual awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci 4: 93–102.
  16. 16. Schnitzler A, Gross J (2005) Normal and pathological oscillatory communication in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 6: 285–296.
  17. 17. Bressler SL, Menon V (2010) Large-scale brain networks in cognition: emerging methods and principles. Trends Cogn Sci 14: 277–290.
  18. 18. Dehaene S, Changeux JP (2011) Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron 70: 200–227.
  19. 19. Weiss S, Rappelsberger P (2000) Long-range EEG synchronization during word encoding correlates with successful memory performance. Cogn Brain Res 9: 299–312.
  20. 20. Bassett DS, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Achard S, Duke T, Bullmore E (2006) Adaptive reconfiguration of fractal small-world human brain functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103: 19518–19523.
  21. 21. Micheloyannis S, Pachou E, Stam CJ, Breakspear M, Bitsios P, et al. (2006) Small-world networks and disturbed functional connectivity in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 87: 60–66.
  22. 22. Astolfi L, Fallani FDV, Cincotti F, Mattia D, Bianchi L, et al. (2008) Neural basis for brain responses to TV commercials: A high-resolution EEG study. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng 16: 522–531.
  23. 23. Valencia M, Martinerie J, Dupont S, Chavez M (2008) Dynamic small-world behavior in functional brain networks unveiled by an event-related networks approach. Phys Rev E 77: 050905 (R)..
  24. 24. Bassett DS, Bullmore ET, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Apud JA, Weinberger DR, et al. (2009) Cognitive fitness of cost-efficient brain functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106: 11747–11752.
  25. 25. Dimitriadis SI, Laskaris NA, Tsirka V, Vourkas M, Micheloyannis S, et al. (2010) Tracking brain dynamics via time-dependent network analysis. J Neurosci Methods 193: 145–155.
  26. 26. Palva S, Monto S, Palva JM (2010) Graph properties of synchronized cortical networks during visual working memory maintenance. NeuroImage 49: 3257–3268.
  27. 27. Palva JM, Monto S, Kulashekhar S, Palva S (2010) Neuronal synchrony reveals working memory networks and predicts individual memory capacity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107: 7580–7585.
  28. 28. Wang H, Douw L, Hernández JM, Reijneveld JC, Stam CJ, et al. (2010) Effect of tumor resection on the characteristics of functional brain networks. Phys Rev E 82: 021924.
  29. 29. Bassett DS (2010) Clinical applications of complex network analysis. In: Sporns O, editor, Short Course III - Analysis and Function of Large-Scale Brain Networks, Washington, DC, USA: Society for Neuroscience. pp. 55–63.
  30. 30. Kitzbichler MG, Henson RNA, Smith ML, Nathan PJ, Bullmore ET (2011) Cognitive effort drives workspace configuration of human brain functional networks. J Neurosci 31: 8259–8270.
  31. 31. Moussa MN, Vechlekar CD, Burdette JH, Steen MR, Hugenschmidt CE, et al. (2011) Changes in cognitive state alter human functional brain networks. Front Hum Neurosci 5: 83.
  32. 32. Schinkel S, Zamora-López G, Dimigen O, Sommer W, Kurths J (2011) Functional network analysis reveals differences in the semantic priming task. J Neurosci Methods 197: 333–339.
  33. 33. Tsirka V, Simos PG, Vakis A, Kanatsouli K, Vourkas M, et al. (2011) Mild traumatic brain injury: Graph-model characterization of brain networks for episodic memory. Int J Psychophysiol 79: 89–96.
  34. 34. Vlooswijk MCG, Vaessen MJ, Jansen JFA, de Krom MCFTM, Majoie HJM, et al. (2011) Loss of network efficiency associated with cognitive decline in chronic epilepsy. Neurology 77: 938–944.
  35. 35. Giessing C, Thiel CM, Alexander-Bloch AF, Patel AX, Bullmore ET (2013) Human brain functional network changes associated with enhanced and impaired attentional task performance. J Neurosci 33: 5903–5914.
  36. 36. Stam CJ, Jones BF, Nolte G, Breakspear M, Scheltens P (2007) Small-world networks and functional connectivity in Alzheimer's disease. Cereb Cortex 17: 92–99.
  37. 37. van den Heuvel MP, Stam CJ, Kahn RS, Hulshoff Pol HE (2009) Efficiency of functional brain networks and intellectual performance. J Neurosci 29: 7619–7624.
  38. 38. Douw L, Schoonheim MM, Landi D, van der Meer ML, Geurts JJG, et al. (2011) Cognition is related to resting-state small-world network topology: An magnetoencephalographic study. Neuroscience 175: 169–177.
  39. 39. Langer N, Pedroni A, Gianotti LRR, Hanggi J, Knoch D, et al. (2012) Functional brain network efficiency predicts intelligence. Hum Brain Mapp 33: 1393–1406.
  40. 40. Buckner RL, Wheeler ME (2001) The cognitive neuroscience of remembering. Nat Rev Neurosci 2: 624–634.
  41. 41. Alvarez P, Squire LR (1994) Memory consolidation and the medial temporal-lobe – A simple network model. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 91: 7041–7045.
  42. 42. Helmstaedter C, Grunwald T, Lehnertz K, Gleissner U, Elger CE (1997) Differential involvement of left temporolateral and temporomesial structures in verbal declarative learning and memory: Evidence from temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain Cogn 35: 110–131.
  43. 43. Eichenbaum H (2000) A cortical-hippocampal system for declarative memory. Nat Rev Neurosci 1: 41–50.
  44. 44. Brown MW, Aggleton JP (2001) Recognition memory: What are the roles of the perirhinal cortex and hippocampus? Nat Rev Neurosci 2: 51–61.
  45. 45. Eichenbaum H, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C (2007) The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annu Rev Neurosci 30: 123–152.
  46. 46. Squire LR, Wixted JT, Clark RE (2007) Recognition memory and the medial temporal lobe: A new perspective. Nat Rev Neurosci 8: 872–883.
  47. 47. Henke K (2010) A model for memory systems based on processing modes rather than consciousness. Nat Rev Neurosci 11: 523–532.
  48. 48. Norman KA (2010) How hippocampus and cortex contribute to recognition memory: Revisiting the complementary learning systems model. Hippocampus 20: 1217–1227.
  49. 49. Fell J, Axmacher N (2011) The role of phase synchronization in memory processes. Nat Rev Neurosci 12: 105–118.
  50. 50. Cabeza R, Nyberg L (2000) Imaging cognition II: An empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. J Cogn Neurosci 12: 1–47.
  51. 51. Brewer JB, Moghekar A (2002) Imaging the medial temporal lobe: exploring new dimensions. Trends Cogn Sci 6: 217–223.
  52. 52. Stark CEL, Okado Y (2003) Making memories without trying: Medial temporal lobe activity associated with incidental memory formation during recognition. J Neurosci 23: 6748–6753.
  53. 53. Carr VA, Rissman J, Wagner AD (2010) Imaging the human medial temporal lobe with high-resolution fMRI. Neuron 65: 298–308.
  54. 54. Elger CE, Grunwald T, Lehnertz K, Kutas M, Helmstaedter C, et al. (1997) Human temporal lobe potentials in verbal learning and memory processes. Neuropsychologia 35: 657–667.
  55. 55. Fernández G, Effern A, Grunwald T, Pezer N, Lehnertz K, et al. (1999) Real-time tracking of memory formation in the human rhinal cortex and hippocampus. Science 285: 1582–1585.
  56. 56. Fell J, Klaver P, Lehnertz K, Grunwald T, Schaller C, et al. (2001) Human memory formation is accompanied by rhinal-hippocampal coupling and decoupling. Nat Neurosci 4: 1259–1264.
  57. 57. Engel AK, Moll CKE, Fried I, Ojemann GA (2005) Invasive recordings from the human brain: clinical insights and beyond. Nat Rev Neurosci 6: 35–47.
  58. 58. Mormann F, Osterhage H, Andrzejak RG, Weber B, Fernandez G, et al. (2008) Independent delta/theta rhythms in the human hippocampus and entorhinal cortex. Front Hum Neurosci 2: 3.
  59. 59. Lachaux JP, Axmacher N, Mormann F, Halgren E, Crone NE (2012) High-frequency neural activity and human cognition: Past, present and possible future of intracranial EEG research. Prog Neurobiol 98: 279–301.
  60. 60. Rugg MD, Curran T (2007) Event-related potentials and recognition memory. Trends Cogn Sci 11: 251–257.
  61. 61. Elger CE, Helmstaedter C, Kurthen M (2004) Chronic epilepsy and cognition. Lancet Neurol 3: 663–672.
  62. 62. Helmstaedter C, Elger CE (2009) Chronic temporal lobe epilepsy: a neurodevelopmental or progressively dementing disease? Brain 132: 2822–2830.
  63. 63. Bell B, Lin JJ, Seidenberg M, Hermann B (2011) The neurobiology of cognitive disorders in temporal lobe epilepsy. Nat Rev Neurol 7: 154–164.
  64. 64. Kral T, Clusmann H, Urbach J, Schramm J, Elger CE, et al. (2002) Preoperative evaluation for epilepsy surgery (Bonn Algorithm). Zentralbl Neurochir 63: 106–110.
  65. 65. Helmstaedter C, Durwen HF (1990) VLMT: Verbaler Lern- und Merkfähigkeitstest. Ein praktikables und differenziertes Instrumentarium zur Prüfung der verbalen Gedächtnisleistungen. Schweizer Archiv für Neurologie und Psychiatrie 141: 21–30.
  66. 66. Sharbrough F, Chatrian GE, Lesser RP, Lüders H, Nuwer M, et al. (1991) American Electroencephalographic Society guidelines for standard electrode position nomenclature. J Clin Neurophysiol 8: 200–202.
  67. 67. Horstmann MT, Bialonski S, Noennig N, Mai H, Prusseit J, et al. (2010) State dependent properties of epileptic brain networks: Comparative graph-theoretical analyses of simultaneously recorded EEG and MEG. Clin Neurophysiol 121: 172–185.
  68. 68. Weiss S, Mueller HM (2003) The contribution of EEG coherence to the investigation of language. Brain Lang 85: 325–343.
  69. 69. Buzsáki G, Draguhn A (2004) Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science 304: 1926–1929.
  70. 70. Schack B, Rappelsberger P, Weiss S, Moller E (1999) Adaptive phase estimation and its application in EEG analysis of word processing. J Neurosci Methods 93: 49–59.
  71. 71. Mormann F, Lehnertz K, David P, Elger CE (2000) Mean phase coherence as a measure for phase synchronization and its application to the EEG of epilepsy patients. Physica D 144: 358–369.
  72. 72. Gabor D (1946) Theory of communication. J I Electr Eng III 93: 429–441.
  73. 73. Panter P (1965) Modulation, Noise, and Spectral Analysis. New York: McGraw-Hill.
  74. 74. Boashash B (1992) Time frequency signal analysis: methods and applications. Longman Cheshire, Melbourne.
  75. 75. Frei MG, Zaveri HP, Arthurs S, Bergey GK, Jouny C, et al. (2010) Controversies in epilepsy: Debates held during the Fourth International Workshop on Seizure Prediction. Epilepsy Behav 19: 4–16.
  76. 76. Wacker M, Witte H (2011) Adaptive phase extraction: Incorporating the Gabor transform in the matching pursuit algorithm. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 58: 2844–2851.
  77. 77. Bruns A (2004) Fourier-, Hilbert- and wavelet-based signal analysis: Are they really different approaches? J Neurosci Methods 137: 321–332.
  78. 78. Osterhage H, Mormann F, Staniek M, Lehnertz K (2007) Measuring synchronization in the epileptic brain: A comparison of different approaches. Int J Bifurcation Chaos Appl Sci Eng 17: 3539–3544.
  79. 79. Anderson BS, Butts C, Carley K (1999) The interaction of size and density with graph–level indices. Soc Networks 21: 239–267.
  80. 80. Bialonski S, Horstmann MT, Lehnertz K (2010) From brain to earth and climate systems: Small-world interaction networks or not? Chaos 20: 013134.
  81. 81. van Wijk BCM, Stam CJ, Daffertshofer A (2010) Comparing brain networks of different size and connectivity density using graph theory. PLoS ONE 5: e13701.
  82. 82. Ansmann G, Lehnertz K (2011) Constrained randomization of weighted networks. Phys Rev E 84: 026103.
  83. 83. Bialonski S, Wendler M, Lehnertz K (2011) Unraveling spurious properties of interaction networks with tailored random networks. PLoS ONE 6: e22826.
  84. 84. Ansmann G, Lehnertz K (2012) Surrogate-assisted analysis of weighted functional brain networks. J Neurosci Methods 208: 165–172.
  85. 85. Bialonski S, Lehnertz K (2013) Assortative mixing in functional brain networks during epileptic seizures. Chaos 23: 033139.
  86. 86. Albert R, Barabási AL (2002) Statistical mechanics of complex networks. Rev Mod Phys 74: 47–97.
  87. 87. Dijkstra EW (1959) A note on two problems in connexion with graphs. Numer Math 1: 269–271.
  88. 88. Fortunato S (2010) Community detection in graphs. Phys Rep 486: 75–174.
  89. 89. Newman MEJ (2012) Communities, modules and large-scale structure in networks. Nat Phys 8: 25–31.
  90. 90. Posner MI, Petersen SE (1990) The attention system of the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 13: 25–42.
  91. 91. Chun MM, Turk-Browne NB (2007) Interactions between attention and memory. Curr Opin Neurobiol 17: 177–184.
  92. 92. Muzzio IA, Kentros C, Kandel E (2009) What is remembered? Role of attention on the encoding and retrieval of hippocampal representations. J Physiol 587: 2837–2854.