Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Abnormal Default-Mode Network Homogeneity in First-Episode, Drug-Naive Major Depressive Disorder

  • Wenbin Guo ,

    guowenbin76@163.com

    Affiliation Mental Health Center, the First Affiliated Hospital, Guangxi Medical University, Nanning, Guangxi, China

  • Feng Liu,

    Affiliation Key Laboratory for NeuroInformation of Ministry of Education, School of Life Science and Technology, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, Chengdu, Sichuan, China

  • Jian Zhang,

    Affiliation Mental Health Center, the First Affiliated Hospital, Guangxi Medical University, Nanning, Guangxi, China

  • Zhikun Zhang,

    Affiliation Mental Health Center, the First Affiliated Hospital, Guangxi Medical University, Nanning, Guangxi, China

  • Liuyu Yu,

    Affiliation Mental Health Center, the First Affiliated Hospital, Guangxi Medical University, Nanning, Guangxi, China

  • Jianrong Liu,

    Affiliation Mental Health Center, the First Affiliated Hospital, Guangxi Medical University, Nanning, Guangxi, China

  • Huafu Chen,

    Affiliation Key Laboratory for NeuroInformation of Ministry of Education, School of Life Science and Technology, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, Chengdu, Sichuan, China

  • Changqing Xiao

    Affiliation Mental Health Center, the First Affiliated Hospital, Guangxi Medical University, Nanning, Guangxi, China

Abstract

Background

Default mode network (DMN) is one of the most commonly recognized resting-state networks in major depressive disorder (MDD). However, the homogeneity of this network in MDD is poorly understood. As such, this study was conducted to determine whether or not an abnormal network homogeneity (NH) of DMN is observed in patients with first-episode and drug-naive MDD.

Methods

Twenty-four first-episode drug-naive patients with MDD and twenty-four healthy control subjects participated in the study. NH and independent component analysis (ICA) methods were used to analyze data.

Results

Depressed patients exhibited a significantly increased NH in the left dorsal medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) and decreased NH in the right inferior temporal gyrus (ITG) compared with the healthy control subjects. Receiver operating characteristic curves (ROC) were analyzed and results revealed that the NH values of MPFC and ITG could be applied as candidate markers with relatively high sensitivity and specificity to distinguish patients from healthy control subjects. No correlation was observed between the NH values of the two regions and clinical variables.

Conclusions

Our findings suggested that an abnormal DMN homogeneity could be observed in MDD, which highlight the importance of the DMN in the pathophysiology of MDD.

Introduction

As a common psychiatric disorder, major depressive disorder (MDD) is characterized by persistent and pervasive feelings of sadness, guilt, and worthlessness [1]. Despite the rapid progress in the development of antidepressants, approximately 60% of depressed patients suffer at least one recurrence [2] and the pathophysiology of MDD remains equivocal.

Default-mode network (DMN) is involved in self-referential activities [3], such as mental time travel, perspective taking, and mind theory [4], [5]. Evidence has shown that DMN plays a key role in neural activities mediating MDD [6]. DMN comprises a specific set of brain regions, including the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), posterior cingulate cortex/precuneus (PCC/PCu), medial parietal cortex, lateral parietal cortex, and inferior parietal cortex; these regions routinely show decreased activities during task-related cognitive processes [7]. Independent component analysis (ICA) and region-of-interest (ROI) seed-based correlation approaches are the most commonly employed approaches to assess resting-state brain networks. ICA is a model-free approach used to estimate largely overlapping spatial processes. ROI seed-based correlation method is utilized to determine the temporal coherence between the time series of a particular ROI and the time series of all other voxels in the brain by performing correlation or regression analysis [8]. Further studies have also focused on other analytical methods, such as whole brain partial correlation methodology in MDD [9].

Abnormalities in the connectivity of DMN have been associated with the pathophysiology of MDD; however, inconsistent results have been obtained. Increased functional connectivity (FC) in DMN has been found in MDD. For example, increased subgenual cingulate and thalamic FC with DMN are observed in depressed patients by ICA [10]. Sheline et al. [11], Zhou et al. [12], and Hamilton et al. [13] also reported increased FC in the DMN of depressed patients. Two spatially independent subnetworks of DMN, namely, the anterior and posterior subnetworks, have been further identified in MDD; both subnetworks exhibit an increased FC in the pre-treatment of MDD [6]. Hamilton et al. [13] further reported that the dominance level of DMN is related to the extent of maladaptive depressive rumination and adaptive reflective rumination.

Studies have also observed the decreased FC in DMN in MDD. Decreased FC between PCC/PCu and the bilateral caudate has been detected in depressed individuals [14]. Other studies have also revealed the depression-related reduced FC in DMN [15], [16]. In our previous study, voxel-mirrored homotopic connectivity (VMHC) method has been used and revealed a decreased interhemispheric homotopic connectivity in MPFC and PCC/PCu in MDD, but DMN has not been primarily investigated in this study [17]. Furthermore, a dissociation pattern of regional activity has been observed in the DMN in first-episode, drug-naive MDD at rest [18]. Interestingly, increased and decreased FC in DMN have been noted in late-life depressed patients [19] and in first-episode, drug-naive adults with MDD [20]. These findings indicated that further studies on the integrity of DMN in MDD should be conducted.

Previous studies revealed different findings regarding abnormal DMN activities. Such differences can be attributed to the heterogeneity of studies in terms of sample size, illness duration, depression severity, treatment effect, analytical methods, and study designs, such as resting-state versus task-related. These DMN alterations in MDD have also been revealed by connectivity methods, such as ICA or ROI seed-based correlation method. Although these two methods provide important information from data analysis, both methods exhibit various drawbacks. For ICA, the most efficient method that can be used to compare components among subjects and/or between groups is unclear. For the ROI seed-based correlation method, it seems to be arbitrary to place the ROI seed within a network. Therefore, new methods used to analyze functional neuroimaging data should be developed.

In the present study, a novel method termed network homogeneity (NH) was used [21] to conduct an unbiased survey of the DMN by estimating the homogeneity of the whole network. NH is a voxel-wise measure used to evaluate the correlation of a voxel with all other voxels in a particular network. In this case, homogeneity is defined as the mean correlation of any specific time series of a voxel with the time series of every other voxel in the network. This method also provides a potential alternative approach to assess the homogeneity of specific large-scale networks. In contrast to ROI seed-based correlation method and ICA used for resting-state FC, the NH approach can be used to conduct a hypothesis-driven interrogation of long-range networks related to clinical applications; in this approach, the advantages of the two previous methods are combined [21]. In clinical applications, NH method can be applied to analyze data related to attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [21] and schizophrenia [22]. On the basis of our previous findings [17], [18] and the functional associations of DMN, we hypothesized that abnormal DMN homogeneity is detected in the MDD group but not in the control group. We also hypothesized that abnormal DMN homogeneity is correlated with the clinical variables in patients with MDD.

Methods and Materials

Subjects

All of the participants were informed regarding the procedures and signed a written informed consent. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the First Affiliated Hospital, Guangxi Medical University, China.

Twenty-five first-episode drug-naive patients were consecutively recruited from the Mental Health Center, the First Affiliated Hospital, Guangxi Medical University, China. Twenty-five right-handed healthy control subjects were also recruited from the community. Depression attack was diagnosed according to the Structured Clinical Interview of the DSM-IV (SCID) [1]. The severity of depression was evaluated using a 17-item Hamilton rating scale for depression (HRSD) [23]. Executive function was assessed by Wisconsin card sorting test (WCST, 48 cards) [24]. The inclusion criteria of the patients were described as follows: 1) first-episode and drug-naïve conditions; 2) current episode of depression with HRSD total score ≥18; and 3) illness duration ≤1 year. The following exclusion criteria for both groups were considered: 1) other Axis I psychiatric disorders, such as schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, bipolar disorders, anxiety disorders, or severe Axis II personality disorders or mental retardation, assessed with SCID; and 2) a history of organic brain disorders, neurological disorders, cardiovascular diseases, or other serious physical illnesses revealed by personal history or laboratory analysis. None of the control subjects had a family history of major psychiatric or neurological illness in their first-degree relatives.

Image Acquisition

Imaging was conducted using a Siemens 3 T scanner. The participants were instructed to keep their eyes closed, remain motionless, and stay awake. An echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence with the following parameters was used: repetition time/echo time (TR/TE) = 2000/30 ms; 30 slices, 64×64 matrix; flip angle, 90°; FOV, 24 cm; slice thickness, 4 mm; gap, 0.4 mm; and 250 volumes (500 s).

Data Pre-processing

Data were pre-processed in Matlab (Mathworks) by using data processing assistant for resting-state fMRI (DPARSF) [25]. After corrections for slice timing and head motion were conducted, the subjects included in the analysis should have a maximum displacement ≤2 mm in x, y, or z direction, and should have an angular rotation <2° on each axis. The scans were normalized to the standard SPM8 echo-planar imaging template. Two steps were involved in normalization: linear registration and non-linear registration [26], [27]. In brief, registration was initially performed by matching the whole brain with a template to determine the optimum 12-parameter affine transformation (including three translations, three rotations, three shears, and three zooms). Afterward, registration proceeded by matching the brains together with an appropriate weighting of the template voxels. A Bayesian framework was applied to maximize a posteriori probability of being correct. For instance, it maximizes the product of the likelihood function (derived from the residual squared difference) and the prior function (based on the probability of obtaining a particular set of zooms and shears). Non-linear deformations were estimated by linearly combining the three dimensional discrete cosine transform (DCT) basis functions. These parameters represented the coefficients of the deformations in three orthogonal directions. Matching was conducted with the following procedures. The bending energies of the deformation fields and the residual squared difference between images and templates were simultaneously minimized. After spatial normalization was conducted, the images were re-sampled to obtain the dimensions of 3 mm×3 mm×3 mm; temporal bandpass was then filtered (0.01 Hz to 0.08 Hz) and linearly detrended. Several sources of spurious covariates with temporal derivatives were removed from the images by linear regression; six head motion parameters obtained by rigid body correction, a signal from a ventricular ROI, and a signal from a region centered in the white matter [28] were also removed. However, the removal of the global signal in the pre-processed resting-state FC data is debatable [29], [30], [31]. Therefore, the global signal was included in the current study.

DMN Identification

As suggested by two previous studies [18], [32], group ICA method was used to select DMN as a mask from healthy control subjects. Three main steps, including data reduction, independent component (IC) separation, and back reconstruction, were performed using toolbox GIFT (http://mialab.mrn.org/software/#gica). DMN was chosen according to the templates provided by GIFT, which was applied in NH analyses.

NH Analyses

NH analyses were conducted using Matlab (Mathworks). For each participant, voxel-wise NH was calculated using the equation described in a previous study [21]. The average NH of each voxel in the DMN mask was obtained, and the averaged NH maps were smoothened with a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm full-width at half-maximum.

Statistical Analyses

Demographic and clinical data were compared using two-sample t-test and Chi-square test when appropriate. After assessing the normality of the NH data (p>0.5) by software REST [33], we performed NH analyses with two-sample t-tests by using voxel-wise cross-subject statistics in DMN. Significant level was set at the corrected p<0.05 for multiple comparisons according to Gaussian random field (GRF) theory (min z >1.96, cluster significance: p<0.05). Considering that resting-state FC can be affected by micromovements from volume to volume [34], we calculated the framewise displacement (FD) values of each subject. The mean FD was used as a covariate in the group comparison of NH.

Brain regions with abnormal NH were identified as ROIs. Mean z values in these ROIs were extracted for further receiver operating characteristic curve (ROC) analysis and correlation analysis. In addition, the normality of the mean z values in these ROIs was assessed. The obtained p values were 0.764 and 0.984 for the left dorsal MPFC and the right inferior temporal gyrus (ITG), respectively.

Results

Subjects

One patient and one control subject were excluded from further analyses because of excessive head motion during image acquisition. The two groups did not differ significantly in terms of age (t-tests t = 0.89, df = 46, p = 0.38), sex ratio (Chi-square test x2 = 0.09, df = 1, p = 0.77), and years of education (t-tests t = –1.60, df = 46, p = 0.12; Table 1).

thumbnail
Table 1. Demographic and clinical features of the participants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0091102.t001

DMN Maps Determined by ICA

Using a group ICA approach, we selected DMN from the control subjects. DMN was used as a mask in the following NH analyses and comprised brain areas, including bilateral MPFC, ventral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), PCC/PCu, lateral temporal cortex, medial parietal lobe, lateral parietal lobe, inferior parietal lobe, and the cerebellar Crus I and Crus II (Figure 1).

thumbnail
Figure 1. The DMN mask determined by ICA from the control group and statistical maps showing NH differences between subject groups.

Red and blue denote higher and lower NH, respectively. Colored bars indicate the T value from two-sample t-tests. DMN = default mode network; ICA = independent component analysis; NH = network homogeneity.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0091102.g001

NH: Group Differences in the DMN

Using two-sample t-tests by voxel-wise cross-subject comparisons, we observed a significant group difference in NH values in DMN between the patients and the control subjects. The NH in the left dorsal MPFC was higher in the patient group than in the control group. By comparison, the NH in the right ITG was lower in the patient group than in the control group (Figure 1 and Table 2).

ROC Analyses in Patients and Control Subjects

Two regions (the left dorsal MPFC and the right ITG) with abnormal NH were observed in the patient group. This result indicated that the NH values of these two regions may be utilized to separate the two groups. In Figure 2 and Table 3, the areas under the curves of the two regions were high. This result was further analyzed. The results showed that the NH values of these two regions could be used as candidate markers to distinguish patients from control subjects with relatively high sensitivity and specificity.

thumbnail
Figure 2. Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves using the mean NH in the left dorsal MPFC and the right ITG to separate the patients from healthy controls.

NH = network homogeneity; MPFC = medial prefrontal cortex; ITG = inferior temporal gyrus.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0091102.g002

thumbnail
Table 3. ROC analysis for differentiating patients from healthy controls.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0091102.t003

Correlations between NH and Clinical Variables

No correlation was found between the NH values of the left dorsal MPFC and the right ITG and the HRSD or WCST scores of the patient group. No correlation was also observed between NH and age, education level, or illness duration.

Discussion

Using the NH approach, we provided an unbiased survey of the DMN in first-episode, drug-naive MDD at rest. Our principal finding was that the patients exhibited a higher NH in the left dorsal MPFC and a lower NH in the right ITG than the control subjects. Further ROC analyses suggested that the abnormal NH in the left dorsal MPFC and the right ITG could be used as candidate markers to distinguish patients with MDD from healthy control subjects.

Increased self-focus is reported as the core psychopathology of a resting state activity in MDD, which is characterized by the increased self-awareness and decreased focus on environmental phenomena, including ongoing events, persons, and objects [35]. Enhanced self-focus is attributed to negative emotions to oneself and increased cognitive processing of oneself with subsequent ruminations [36]. The dorsal MPFC is regarded as a key area in DMN and dorsal cognitive system. Therefore, we speculated that the increased NH of the left dorsal MPFC may affect the function of this region and prevent the coordination between the dorsal cognitive system and the ventral emotional system. Predominantly negative emotions and increased cognitive deficits have been observed in depressed patients because of this failure [36]. The present results are similar to those of a previous study [11], which showed an increased FC in the bilateral dorsal MPFC in the MDD group. In the same study, the dorsal MPFC connectivity is positively correlated with HRSD scores, suggesting that the functional abnormality in the dorsal MPFC corresponds to emotional dysregulation [11]. MPFC is strongly associated with affective-limbic regions (such as the amygdala and the hippocampus) and executive control as well as emotional processing regions (such as the orbital frontal cortex and the ACC) [37]. Therefore, MPFC may participate in emotional processing and self-referential processing via these links [38]. Any dysfunction in this region, such as the increased NH in the present study, is likely associated with the failure in the modulation of emotional behaviors and self-referential processes investigated with task-related and resting-state functional neuroimaging techniques [39], [40]. In other studies, MPFC exhibits high diagnostic and prognostic accuracy to separate patients with treatment-resistant depression from individuals with treatment-sensitive depression [41]. Consistent with this finding, the NH values of the left dorsal MPFC in the present study could be utilized as a candidate marker with relatively high sensitivity and specificity to differentiate depressed patients from healthy control subjects.

The ITG is one of the most consistently detected regions in the pathophysiology of MDD [42] because this region participates in emotional processing and social cognition [43]. Decreased activation and connectivity in the ITG have been observed in MDD at rest or in response to sad stimuli [42], [44]. We also observed an increased regional homogeneity (ReHo) in the right ITG in first-episode, treatment-sensitive MDD from a 1.5 T scanner [45]. We recruited similar patients and identified the decreased NH values in the right ITG in MDD in the present study. Although the results from these two studies could not be compared directly because different scanners and analyses were applied, the results from both studies indicated a functional abnormality in the ITG. Moreover, the ITG is a key node in a widespread network of frontal, temporal, parietal, occipital, and sub-cortical structures, which can be observed to provide a diagnostic and prognostic prediction to treatment responses [46]. Similar to the results of the previous study [46], the present ROC results suggested that the NH values of the right ITG could be used as a candidate marker to distinguish patients from healthy control subjects.

The present results exhibited a dissociation pattern of NH in the anterior and posterior parts of DMN, in which an increased NH was observed in the left dorsal MPFC and a decreased NH was found in the right ITG in MDD. This pattern is supported by a recent ICA study [20], which reported a dissociation between the anterior and posterior FC in the resting-state DMN in first-episode, drug-naive patients with MDD. This dissociation pattern is also observed in our previous study, in which an increased regional activity is observed in the left dorsal MPFC and a decreased regional activity is found in the left parahippocampal gyrus (PHG) by using a fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations (fALFF) method [18].

Considering that clinical characteristics, such as illness duration and depression severity, are correlated with the abnormal FC of the DMN [9], [17], we predicted that abnormal DMN homogeneity was correlated with the clinical variables of patients with MDD. However, our study revealed that abnormal DMN homogeneity was not correlated with the clinical variables, and this result was unexpected. In our previous study, a significantly positive correlation was observed between VMHC in MPFC and persistent error response of WCST (WCST-Pre) in the same sample [17]. These inconsistent findings can be attributed to the different analytical methods used in the studies. In the previous study, VMHC was used to quantify the resting-state FC between each voxel in one hemisphere and its corresponding voxel in the opposite hemisphere. In the present study, NH is a voxel-wise measure that can be performed to evaluate the correlation of a voxel with all other voxels in the DMN. Different analytical methods may have also contributed to the different clusters of MPFC observed in the two studies, particularly the left dorsal MPFC in the present study and the bilateral ventral MPFC in the previous study.

Several limitations should be noted in this study. First, a relatively small sample size was used in the study. Second, the DMN mask was derived from healthy control subjects by using ICA; thus, bias in the analyses may be introduced. Third, the present study focused on DMN, and this approach may elucidate the pathophysiological contribution of DMN; however, relevant findings from other brain regions may be neglected. Finally, age, sex ratio, and education level were not used as covariates to remove the possible confounding effects as conducted in other studies [9], [47]. This procedure may yield biased results, but no statistically significant differences in these parameters were observed between patients and control subjects. Moreover, imaging studies have not applied these parameters as covariates [20], [48]. Hence, the decision to consider the confounding effects of age, sex ratio, and education level slightly affects the final results.

Despite these limitations, the present findings showed the abnormal NH of DMN in MDD. These abnormalities could be applied as candidate biomarkers to distinguish patients from healthy control subjects. Thus, the results highlighted the importance of DMN in the pathophysiology of MDD.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: WG HC. Performed the experiments: JL LY CX JZ. Analyzed the data: WG FL ZZ. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: WG FL. Wrote the paper: WG FL.

References

  1. 1. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW (1997) Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID). Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press.
  2. 2. Smith GS, Kramer E, Hermann C, Ma Y, Dhawan V, et al. (2009) Serotonin modulation of cerebral glucose metabolism in depressed older adults. Biol Psychiatry 66: 259–266.
  3. 3. Lemogne C, Mayberg H, Bergouignan L, Volle E, Delaveau P, et al. (2010) Self-referential processing and the prefrontal cortex over the course of depression: a pilot study. J Affect Disord 124: 196–201.
  4. 4. Buckner RL, Carroll DC (2007) Self-projection and the brain. Trends Cogn Sci 11: 49–57.
  5. 5. Molnar-Szakacs I, Arzy S (2009) Searching for an integrated self-representation. Commun Integr Biol 2: 365–367.
  6. 6. Li B, Liu L, Friston KJ, Shen H, Wang L, et al. (2013) A Treatment-Resistant Default Mode Subnetwork in Major Depression. Biol Psychiatry 74: 48–54.
  7. 7. Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, et al. (2001) A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98: 676–682.
  8. 8. Biswal B, Yetkin FZ, Haughton VM, Hyde JS (1995) Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn Reson Med 34: 537–541.
  9. 9. Zhang J, Wang J, Wu Q, Kuang W, Huang X, et al. (2011) Disrupted brain connectivity networks in drug-naive, first-episode major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 70: 334–342.
  10. 10. Greicius MD, Flores BH, Menon V, Glover GH, Solvason HB, et al. (2007) Resting-state functional connectivity in major depression: abnormally increased contributions from subgenual cingulate cortex and thalamus. Biol Psychiatry 62: 429–437.
  11. 11. Sheline YI, Price JL, Yan Z, Mintun MA (2010) Resting-state functional MRI in depression unmasks increased connectivity between networks via the dorsal nexus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107: 11020–11025.
  12. 12. Zhou Y, Yu C, Zheng H, Liu Y, Song M, et al. (2010) Increased neural resources recruitment in the intrinsic organization in major depression. J Affect Disord 121: 220–230.
  13. 13. Hamilton JP, Furman DJ, Chang C, Thomason ME, Dennis E, et al. (2011) Default-mode and task-positive network activity in major depressive disorder: implications for adaptive and maladaptive rumination. Biol Psychiatry 70: 327–333.
  14. 14. Bluhm R, Williamson P, Lanius R, Theberge J, Densmore M, et al. (2009) Resting state default-mode network connectivity in early depression using a seed region-of-interest analysis: decreased connectivity with caudate nucleus. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 63: 754–761.
  15. 15. Anand A, Li Y, Wang Y, Wu J, Gao S, et al. (2005) Activity and connectivity of brain mood regulating circuit in depression: a functional magnetic resonance study. Biol Psychiatry 57: 1079–1088.
  16. 16. Veer IM, Beckmann CF, van Tol MJ, Ferrarini L, Milles J, et al. (2010) Whole brain resting-state analysis reveals decreased functional connectivity in major depression. Front Syst Neurosci 4: 41.
  17. 17. Guo W, Liu F, Dai Y, Jiang M, Zhang J, et al. (2013) Decreased interhemispheric resting-state functional connectivity in first-episode, drug-naive major depressive disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 41: 24–29.
  18. 18. Guo W, Liu F, Zhang J, Zhang Z, Yu L, et al. (2013) Dissociation of regional activity in the default mode network in first-episode, drug-naive major depressive disorder at rest. Journal of affective disorders 151: 1097–1101.
  19. 19. Wu M, Andreescu C, Butters MA, Tamburo R, Reynolds CF 3rd, et al. (2011) Default-mode network connectivity and white matter burden in late-life depression. Psychiatry Res 194: 39–46.
  20. 20. Zhu X, Wang X, Xiao J, Liao J, Zhong M, et al. (2011) Evidence of a dissociation pattern in resting-state default mode network connectivity in first-episode, treatment-naive major depression patients. Biol Psychiatry 71: 611–617.
  21. 21. Uddin LQ, Kelly AM, Biswal BB, Margulies DS, Shehzad Z, et al. (2008) Network homogeneity reveals decreased integrity of default-mode network in ADHD. J Neurosci Methods 169: 249–254.
  22. 22. Guo W, Yao D, Jiang J, Su Q, Zhang Z, et al. (2014) Abnormal default-mode network homogeneity in first-episode, drug-naive schizophrenia at rest. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 49: 16–20.
  23. 23. Hamilton M (1967) Development of a rating scale for primary depressive illness. Br J Soc Clin Psychol 6: 278–296.
  24. 24. Greve KW, Stickle TR, Love JM, Bianchini KJ, Stanford MS (2005) Latent structure of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: a confirmatory factor analytic study. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 20: 355–364.
  25. 25. Yan C, Zang Y (2010) DPARSF: A MATLAB toolbox for “pipeline” data analysis of resting-state fMRI. Front Syst Neurosci 4: 13.
  26. 26. Ashburner J, Neelin P, Collins DL, Evans A, Friston K (1997) Incorporating prior knowledge into image registration. Neuroimage 6: 344–352.
  27. 27. Ashburner J, Friston KJ (1999) Nonlinear spatial normalization using basis functions. Hum Brain Mapp 7: 254–266.
  28. 28. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, et al. (2005) The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 102: 9673.
  29. 29. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, et al. (2005) The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102: 9673–9678.
  30. 30. Murphy K, Birn RM, Handwerker DA, Jones TB, Bandettini PA (2009) The impact of global signal regression on resting state correlations: are anti-correlated networks introduced? Neuroimage 44: 893–905.
  31. 31. Saad ZS, Gotts SJ, Murphy K, Chen G, Jo HJ, et al. (2012) Trouble at rest: how correlation patterns and group differences become distorted after global signal regression. Brain Connect 2: 25–32.
  32. 32. Liu CH, Ma X, Li F, Wang YJ, Tie CL, et al. (2012) Regional homogeneity within the default mode network in bipolar depression: a resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging study. PLoS One 7: e48181.
  33. 33. Song XW, Dong ZY, Long XY, Li SF, Zuo XN, et al. (2011) REST: a toolkit for resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging data processing. PLoS One 6: e25031.
  34. 34. Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE (2012) Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage 59: 2142–2154.
  35. 35. Northoff G (2007) Psychopathology and pathophysiology of the self in depression - neuropsychiatric hypothesis. J Affect Disord 104: 1–14.
  36. 36. Hasler G, Northoff G (2011) Discovering imaging endophenotypes for major depression. Mol Psychiatry 16: 604–619.
  37. 37. Alalade E, Denny K, Potter G, Steffens D, Wang L (2011) Altered cerebellar-cerebral functional connectivity in geriatric depression. PLoS One 6: e20035.
  38. 38. Phillips ML, Drevets WC, Rauch SL, Lane R (2003) Neurobiology of emotion perception II: Implications for major psychiatric disorders. Biol Psychiatry 54: 515–528.
  39. 39. Drevets WC, Price JL, Furey ML (2008) Brain structural and functional abnormalities in mood disorders: implications for neurocircuitry models of depression. Brain Struct Funct 213: 93–118.
  40. 40. Sheline YI, Barch DM, Price JL, Rundle MM, Vaishnavi SN, et al. (2009) The default mode network and self-referential processes in depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106: 1942–1947.
  41. 41. Liu F, Guo W, Yu D, Gao Q, Gao K, et al. (2012) Classification of Different Therapeutic Responses of Major Depressive Disorder with Multivariate Pattern Analysis Method Based on Structural MR Scans. PLoS One 7: e40968.
  42. 42. Fitzgerald PB, Laird AR, Maller J, Daskalakis ZJ (2008) A meta-analytic study of changes in brain activation in depression. Hum Brain Mapp 29: 683–695.
  43. 43. Gallagher HL, Frith CD (2003) Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’. Trends Cogn Sci 7: 77–83.
  44. 44. Cullen KR, Gee DG, Klimes-Dougan B, Gabbay V, Hulvershorn L, et al. (2009) A preliminary study of functional connectivity in comorbid adolescent depression. Neurosci Lett 460: 227–231.
  45. 45. Guo WB, Liu F, Xue ZM, Yu Y, Ma CQ, et al. (2011) Abnormal neural activities in first-episode, treatment-naive, short-illness-duration, and treatment-response patients with major depressive disorder: A resting-state fMRI study. J Affect Disord 135: 326–331.
  46. 46. Gong Q, Wu Q, Scarpazza C, Lui S, Jia Z, et al. (2011) Prognostic prediction of therapeutic response in depression using high-field MR imaging. Neuroimage 55: 1497–1503.
  47. 47. Chen JD, Liu F, Xun GL, Chen HF, Hu MR, et al. (2012) Early and late onset, first-episode, treatment-naive depression: same clinical symptoms, different regional neural activities. Journal of affective disorders 143: 56–63.
  48. 48. Liu F, Guo W, Liu L, Long Z, Ma C, et al. (2013) Abnormal amplitude low-frequency oscillations in medication-naive, first-episode patients with major depressive disorder: A resting-state fMRI study. J Affect Disord 146: 401–406.