Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Maternal Pre-Pregnancy BMI and Intelligence Quotient (IQ) in 5-Year-Old Children: A Cohort Based Study

  • Mette Bliddal ,

    mette.bliddal@rsyd.dk

    Affiliation Department of Gynaecology and Obstetrics, Odense University Hospital, and Institute of Clinical Research, Research Unit of Gynaecology and Obstetrics, University of Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark

  • Jørn Olsen,

    Affiliation Department of Public Health, Section for Epidemiology, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark

  • Henrik Støvring,

    Affiliation Department of Public Health, Section for Biostatistics, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark

  • Hanne-Lise F. Eriksen,

    Affiliation Center of Functionally Integrative Neuroscience, Institute for Clinical Medicine, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark

  • Ulrik S. Kesmodel,

    Affiliation Department of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, Aarhus University Hospital, Aarhus, Denmark

  • Thorkild I. A. Sørensen,

    Affiliations Novo Nordisk Foundation Center for Basic Metabolic Research, University of Copenhagen, Denmark, Institute of Preventive Medicine, Bispebjerg and Frederiksberg Hospitals – Part of Copenhagen University Hospital, Copenhagen, Denmark

  • Ellen A. Nøhr

    Affiliation Department of Public Health, Section for Epidemiology, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark

Abstract

Background

An association between maternal pre-pregnancy BMI and childhood intelligence quotient (IQ) has repeatedly been found but it is unknown if this association is causal or due to confounding caused by genetic or social factors.

Methods

We used a cohort of 1,783 mothers and their 5-year-old children sampled from the Danish National Birth Cohort. The children participated between 2003 and 2008 in a neuropsychological assessment of cognitive ability including IQ tests taken by both the mother and the child. Linear regression analyses were used to estimate the associations between parental BMI and child IQ adjusted for a comprehensive set of potential confounders. Child IQ was assessed with the Wechsler Primary and Preschool Scales of Intelligence – Revised (WPPSI-R).

Results

The crude association between maternal BMI and child IQ showed that BMI was adversely associated with child IQ with a reduction in IQ of −0.40 point for each one unit increase in BMI. This association was attenuated after adjustment for social factors and maternal IQ to a value of −0.27 (−0.50 to −0.03). After mutual adjustment for the father's BMI and all other factors except maternal IQ, the association between paternal BMI and child IQ yielded a regression coefficient of −0.26 (−0.59 to 0.07), which was comparable to that seen for maternal BMI (−0.20 (−0.44 to 0.04)).

Conclusion

Although maternal pre-pregnancy BMI was inversely associated with the IQ of her child, the similar association with paternal BMI suggests that it is not a specific pregnancy related adiposity effect.

Introduction

Maternal pre-pregnancy obesity is associated with numerous neonatal adverse outcomes such as increased risk of fetal death, macrosomia, and birth complications [1], [2]. Additionally, maternal obesity and excessive weight gain in pregnancy predict negative long-term physical health outcomes in the child such as obesity [3] and elevated blood pressure [4]. Fetal programming by factors induced by maternal obesity has been suggested [3].

If maternal obesity affects the fetal brain development through modifications of the intrauterine environment is not known but some studies have shown associations between maternal pre-pregnancy obesity and ADHD [5], inattention [6], neurodevelopmental disorders [7], and intellectual disability in children [8].

High BMI has been associated with a low IQ in adults and children [9][13] and childhood IQ, as a measure of general cognitive ability, has also been inversely associated with maternal pre-pregnancy BMI in some studies but not all [3], [14][16]. A recent study, investigating predictors for childhood IQ, found maternal BMI to be negatively associated with child IQ – apparently independent of maternal IQ - and hence maternal BMI was a predictor of child IQ [17].

If fat tissue is causally related to cognitive skills, causation may operate via the hormonal activities of fat tissue, by release of environmental fat-soluble toxins, or by an unbalanced diet leading to obesity. Some findings however, indicate that the association between maternal pregnancy BMI and child cognitive outcomes is due to confounding by genetic factors or post-natal social confounding [18]. As stated by Brion [19], it is important, given the global epidemic of obesity, to disentangle the biological effects of intrauterine exposure to maternal obesity on child cognitive development from spurious associations due to confounding. The degree of confounding can be assessed by a comparison of the associations between the maternal exposure and the child outcome with the association between paternal exposure and child IQ. If exposure to intrauterine excessive fat tissue is causally related to child IQ, one would expect the maternal association to be larger than the paternal association [19], [20]. Comparison of the maternal and paternal BMI association on child cognitive development as an estimate test of causality has only been done a few times [18], [21]. We add to this research more detailed data and estimate the association between maternal pre-pregnancy BMI, paternal BMI, and child IQ at the age of five taking into account potential confounding factors including maternal IQ.

Our hypothesis was that an association between maternal BMI and child IQ is not driven by a specific pregnancy programming effect related to fat tissue but rather by genetic and family confounders. We therefore expected to find not only an association for maternal BMI and child IQ but also for paternal BMI. If the association is only seen for maternal BMI, our hypothesis is not corroborated.

Subjects and Methods

Sample

This study was based on data from the ‘Lifestyle During Pregnancy Study (LDPS)’ nested within The Danish National Birth Cohort (DNBC). The DNBC is a cohort of approximately 92,000 pregnant women recruited from 1996 to 2002 at their first antenatal visit [22]. LDPS is a follow-up study focusing on prenatal lifestyle factors (primarily maternal alcohol exposure) and later neurodevelopment of the child [23]. A total of 3,478 women were invited to participate in LDPS between September 2003 and June 2008, when their children reached 60–64 months of age. The sampling was stratified on alcohol exposure with an oversampling of high exposures [23]. Of those invited, 1,783 children (51%) participated in an extensive three-hour assessment of cognitive ability and IQ tests were included for both the mother and the child. No substantial differences were seen between invited participants and non-participants in the LDPS [24]. The tests were carried out by 10 trained psychologists following standardised test procedures at test sites located in four cities in Denmark [23]. IQ of the child was assessed with the widely used Wechsler Primary and Preschool Scales of Intelligence – Revised (WPPSI-R) [25] assessing the IQ of children of 2–7 years of age.

Exclusion criteria were mother's or child's inability to speak Danish, impaired hearing or vision of the child to the extent that the test session could not be performed. Twins and children with congenital diseases prone to cause mental impairment (e.g. trisomy 21) were also excluded (n = 14).

Exposure variables

The main exposure was maternal pre-pregnancy BMI. On the basis of self-reported information on weight and height from the first pregnancy interview in the DNBC, which took place at approximately 16th weeks of gestation (inter-quartile range 13–19 weeks), maternal pre-pregnancy BMI (in kg/m2) was calculated. We had information on paternal BMI, provided by the mother, for 78% of the fathers (n = 1,376) from a postnatal interview 18 months after birth.

Outcome variable

The WPPSI-R, used for estimating child IQ, comprises five verbal subtests and five performance subtests from which Verbal IQ (VIQ), Performance IQ (PIQ), and Full-Scale IQ's (FSIQ) are derived. The short form used in the present study included three verbal (arithmetic, information and vocabulary) and three performance subtests (block design, geometric design and object assembly). When at least two verbal and two performance subtests were available, the IQ's were prorated using standard procedures. At the time of the study, no Danish WPPSI-R norms were available, so Swedish norms were used to derive scores and IQs [26].

Covariates

The measurement of maternal IQ was based on the mean of two verbal subtests (information and vocabulary) from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) [27] and the Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices [28]. Raw scores of each test were standardised based on the results from the full sample and weighted equally in a combined score which was re-standardised to an IQ scale with a mean of 100 and a SD of 15.

A self-completed questionnaire was administered to the mothers with questions on maternal marital status, maternal and parental educational level (years in school plus years of post-school theoretical education), postnatal parental smoking in the home (yes/no), and duration of exclusive breastfeeding (months). The exact age of the child at the time of testing was categorised into 5 years and 1 month, 5 years and 2 months, 5 years and 3 months and 5 years and 4 months.

Birth weight (grams) and gestational age (days) were obtained from the Danish Medical Birth Registry. Maternal age as well as the sex and age of the child were obtained from The Danish Civil Registration System.

The following covariates were obtained from the prenatal telephone interview in the DNBC and coded as follows: parity (0, 1, 2+), maternal prenatal smoking (yes/no), and average alcohol intake per week during pregnancy (0, 1–4, 5–8, 9+).

Statistical analyses

Both maternal and paternal BMI distributions were approximately symmetric. Furthermore, all statistical analyses were weighted by sampling probabilities with robust variance estimation due to the selection method and oversampling of women with intake of alcohol during pregnancy [29]. This allows valid estimation of confidence intervals, even when the outcome variable is not normally distributed. Since a linear association between both maternal and paternal BMI and child IQ was compatible with the data (p = 0.46 and p = 0.30, respectively, based on a test for absence of a quadratic term in BMI) all analyses were done using linear regression on continuous BMI data. However we also generated restricted cubic splines (6 knots), allowing for a non-linear association to describe the associations between parental BMI and child IQ in more detail. The restricted cubic splines (Figure 1) displayed an inverse association between maternal BMI and child IQ in all the maternal analyses, however with a peak indicating highest IQ scores in children of women with a pre-pregnancy BMI of approximately 21. A similar inverse association was observed in restricted cubic splines of paternal BMI and child IQ with a peak at a paternal BMI at 24 units, but the deviation from a linear association was small.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Splines for parental BMI and child IQ at the age of 5.

Splines indicating estimates of child IQ according to maternal and paternal BMI. A. Maternal crude estimates. B. Paternal crude estimates. C. Maternal, mutually adjusted. D. Paternal, mutually adjusted. E. Maternal, adjusted for all covariates except maternal IQ. F. Paternal, adjusted for all covariates except maternal IQ. G. Maternal, adjusted for all covariates including maternal IQ. H. Paternal, adjusted for all covariates including maternal IQ.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094498.g001

In adjusted analyses, we included the other parent's BMI, gestational age, birth weight, sex of child, maternal age at birth, parity, marital status, maternal and paternal educational level, maternal prenatal smoking, postnatal smoking at home, average alcohol intake in pregnancy, breastfeeding and age of child when tested. Finally, maternal IQ was added to the model. We examined the correlation between maternal and paternal BMI and found a Pearson's correlations coefficient of 0.13.

We did the same analyses for maternal BMI and paternal BMI on verbal IQ and performance IQ to see if the results would differ on sub scales of IQ.

Supplementary analyses were stratified by sex of the child after excluding underweight women (n = 65).

Statistical estimates were two-sided with 95% confidence intervals. All statistical analyses were conducted in STATA 11 (StataCorp LP, College Station, TX, USA).

Ethical approval

This study was approved by the Danish Data Protection Agency and access to the data was granted by the DNBC Steering Committee. According to Danish law, ethical approval is not required for registry-based studies.

Results

Table 1 presents sample characteristics according to parental BMI. Of the 1,783 mothers included in the study, 7% were categorized as obese, 18% as overweight, 69% as normal weight, and 4% as underweight. The corresponding numbers in the smaller sample of fathers with data were 7%, 39%, 54% and 0.5%.

thumbnail
Table 1. Crude associations between parental BMI and selected covariates in the ‘Lifestyle during Pregnancy study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094498.t001

We found an inverse trend between both maternal and paternal BMI and maternal IQ and educational status. Maternal BMI was associated with higher birth weight and longer gestational age, which was not seen for paternal BMI. For all other covariates, no strong association with parental BMI was found.

We observed an inverse association between maternal pre-pregnancy BMI and child IQ with a decrease of 0.40 IQ point (−0.64 to −0.17) for every unit increase in BMI (Table 2). Adjusting for maternal IQ attenuated this association to −0.30 IQ points (−0.51 to −0.08).

The crude association between paternal BMI and child IQ was −0.23 IQ points (−0.56 to 0.10) (Table 3). Mutual adjustment for both parents BMI led, for paternal BMI, to numerically smaller estimates and when all other factors, except maternal IQ, were added to the model, the associations showed a regression coefficient of −0.26 IQ points (−0.59 to 0.07) for paternal BMI and child IQ and −0.20 (−0.44 to 0.04) for maternal BMI.

thumbnail
Table 3. Child IQ (age 5) according to maternal and paternal BMI.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094498.t003

We did the same analyses on sub levels of IQ – verbal IQ and performance IQ in the children – and the results remained approximately the same (Supplementary tables S1S4). Supplementary analyses showed similar results for boys and girls. Excluding underweight women (n = 65) from the analyses did not change the results much (not shown).

Discussion

As predicted, we found an inverse association between maternal pre-pregnancy BMI and child IQ which persisted in an attenuated form after adjustment for confounders. When comparing associations for both maternal and paternal BMI, effect estimates were similar suggesting that the association between maternal excessive fat tissue and child IQ is not related to programming induced by maternal fat tissue during pregnancy. The association is most likely confounded by genetic and pre- and post-natal family factors, which could explain why this association is only seen in some studies depending on the association between BMI and IQ in the population.

Only few studies have explored the association between maternal obesity and child cognitive development [5][7], [18], [21], [30][33] or, more specifically, child IQ [14], [15]. In a Finnish study [15] an association between maternal BMI and child IQ was only found in the most recent cohort. Brion et al [18], who examined parental BMI and child verbal- and nonverbal skills and behavioural problems in two cohorts, reported inconsistent evidence across cohorts and associations that were not robust to adjustments. Most of the studies support an inverse association between maternal obesity and various cognitive child outcomes [5][7], [14], [21], [30][33] but more studies have been requested [16] e.g. by comparing the estimates of maternal and paternal influence [19], [20]. A recent study found an association between maternal BMI and child IQ, suggesting maternal BMI to be a minor predictor for child IQ at the age of 5 [17].

Previous studies all adjusted for selected social factors, but only two of them compared maternal and paternal associations [18], [21]. One of these studies found an association between cognitive functions of the child and maternal pre-pregnancy BMI, but not paternal BMI [21] concluding that the maternal association was likely to be causal although the maternal and paternal associations had overlapping confidence intervals [19]. The other study did not find an association between paternal overweight and any child cognitive performance or behaviour tests [18].

Lifestyle and psychosocial conditions during and after pregnancy may confound the relation between maternal BMI and child IQ. Family income, parental education and breastfeeding tend to be strong indicators for cognitive abilities in childhood [34]. Inconsistency is found for birth weight, duration of birth, maternal age at child's birth, and maternal smoking and alcohol intake during pregnancy [16], [24], [29], [35]. Lack of intellectual stimulation and suboptimal home conditions may also affect mental development of the child. Incorporation of maternal-paternal comparisons in our analyses adds information to the adjustments for family factors.

Inclusion of maternal IQ data is a key strength in our study. If the association between maternal BMI and child IQ is solely due to maternal genetic confounding, we expected the correlation to be much weaker after adjusting for maternal IQ, which we did not see, but we observed a modest attenuation of the association indicating that the genetic marker of maternal IQ plays a role. The fact that the sub-cohort was originally designed to investigate maternal alcohol intake in pregnancy with an oversampling in the cohort of high alcohol intake [23] might reduce the power of the study but not its validity.

The data on maternal pre-pregnancy BMI relied on self-reported information at the first telephone interview in the DNBC at the 16th week of gestation. This self-reported anthropometric information has been validated relative to weights observed in the antenatal care at approximately 9th week of gestation for at sub-cohort from the DNBC of 5,033 women. Throughout the entire BMI scale, the BMI was under-estimated with a mean of 0.66 kg, with a slightly increased under-reporting with increasing BMI. Yet, the BMI categories derived from the two BMI estimates agreed in 91.4% of the cases [36]. The information on paternal weight was based on reported information from the mothers 18 months after delivery and is the best data available. Accuracy of informant reports on estimates of the height and weight of family members has been investigated concluding that this information is highly predictive of the actually weight and height [37].

Although maternal pre-pregnancy BMI is inversely associated with the IQ of her child, our findings suggest that this association is not mediated by excessive maternal fat tissue present during pregnancy. There may be many good reasons for reducing overweight before entering procreation but fear of a negative effect on the child's intellectual capacity need not be one of them.

Supporting Information

Table S1.

Verbal child IQ according to maternal BMI.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094498.s001

(DOCX)

Table S2.

Verbal child IQ according to maternal and paternal BMI.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094498.s002

(DOCX)

Table S3.

Performance child IQ according to maternal BMI.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094498.s003

(DOCX)

Table S4.

Performance child IQ according to maternal and paternal BMI.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094498.s004

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all of the participants for their time and effort.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: EAN JO MB. Analyzed the data: MB. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: HS HLFE USK JO TIAS. Wrote the paper: MB JO EAN. Leading role in establishing the DNBC: JO TIAS. Took part in establishing the sub-cohort LDPS: USK. Collected and documented the data in the LDPS: HLFE. Wrote the first draft of the manuscript: MB. Participated in the interpretation of results and in writing the paper: MB JO HS H-LFE USK TIAS EAN.

References

  1. 1. Ovesen P, Rasmussen S, Kesmodel U (2011) Effect of prepregnancy maternal overweight and obesity on pregnancy outcome. Obstet Gynecol 118: 305–312.
  2. 2. Nohr EA, Vaeth M, Baker JL, Sorensen TI, Olsen J, et al. (2008) Combined associations of prepregnancy body mass index and gestational weight gain with the outcome of pregnancy. Am J Clin Nutr 87: 1750–1759.
  3. 3. Poston L (2012) Maternal obesity, gestational weight gain and diet as determinants of offspring long term health. Best Pract Res Clin Endocrinol Metab 26: 627–639.
  4. 4. Oken E Maternal, child obesity: the causal link (2009) Obstet Gynecol Clin North Am. 36: 361–77.
  5. 5. Rodriguez A, Miettunen J, Henriksen TB, Olsen J, Obel C, et al. (2008) Maternal adiposity prior to pregnancy is associated with ADHD symptoms in offspring: evidence from three prospective pregnancy cohorts. Int J Obes (Lond) 32: 550–7.
  6. 6. Rodriguez A (2010) Maternal pre-pregnancy obesity and risk for inattention and negative emotionality in children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 51: 134–43.
  7. 7. Krakowiak P, Walker CK, Bremer AA, Baker AS, Ozonoff S, et al. (2012) Maternal metabolic conditions and risk for autism and other neurodevelopmental disorders. Pediatrics 129: 1121–8.
  8. 8. Mann JR, McDermott SW, Hardin J, Pan C, Zhang Z (2013) Pre-pregnancy body mass index, weight change during pregnancy, and risk of intellectual disability in children. BJOG 120: 309–319.
  9. 9. Yu ZB, Han SP, Cao XG, Guo XR (2010) Intelligence in relation to obesity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes Rev. 11: 656–70.
  10. 10. Sorensen TI, Sonne-Holm S, Christensen U (1983) Cognitive deficiency in obesity independent of social origin. Lancet (14) 1105–6.
  11. 11. Sorensen TI, Sonne-Holm S, Christensen U, Kreiner S (1982) Reduced intellectual performance in extreme overweight. Hum Biol 54: 765–775.
  12. 12. Sorensen TI, Sonne-Holm S (1985) Intelligence test performance in obesity in relation to educational attainment and parental social class. J Biosoc Sci 17: 379–387.
  13. 13. Teasdale TW, Sorensen TI, Stunkard AJ (1992) Intelligence and educational level in relation to body mass index of adult males. Hum Biol 64: 99–106.
  14. 14. Neggers YH, Goldenberg RL, Ramey SL, Cliver SP (2003) Maternal prepregnancy body mass index and psychomotor development in children. Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand 82: 235–40.
  15. 15. Heikura U, Taanila A, Hartikainen AL, Olsen P, Linna SL, et al. (2008) Variations in prenatal sociodemographic factors associated with intellectual disability: a study of the 20-year interval between two birth cohorts in northern Finland. Am J Epidemiol 15: 169–177.
  16. 16. Van Lieshout RJ, Taylor VH, Boyle MH (2011) Pre-pregnancy and pregnancy obesity and neurodevelopmental outcomes in offspring: a systematic review. Obes Rev 12: 548–59.
  17. 17. Eriksen HL, Kesmodel US, Underbjerg M, Kilburn TR, Bertrand J, et al. (2013) Predictors of intelligence at the age of 5: family, pregnancy and birth characteristics, postnatal influences, and postnatal growth. PLoS One 8: e79200.
  18. 18. Brion MJ, Zeegers M, Jaddoe V, Verhulst F, Tiemeier H, et al. (2011) Intrauterine effects of maternal prepregnancy overweight on child cognition and behavior in 2 cohorts. Pediatrics 127: e202–11.
  19. 19. Brion MJ (2013) Commentary: Can maternal-paternal comparisons contribute to our understanding of maternal pre-pregnancy obesity and its association with offspring cognitive outcomes? Int J Epidemiol 42: 518–519.
  20. 20. Smith GD (2008) Assessing intrauterine influences on offspring health outcomes: can epidemiological studies yield robust findings? Basic Clin Pharmacol Toxicol 102: 245–256.
  21. 21. Casas M, Chatzi L, Carsin AE, Amiano P, Guxens M, et al. (2013) Maternal pre-pregnancy overweight and obesity, and child neuropsychological development: two Southern European birth cohort studies. Int J Epidemiol 42: 506–517.
  22. 22. Olsen J, Melbye M, Olsen SF, Sorensen TI, Aaby P, et al. (2001) The Danish National Birth Cohort—its background, structure and aim. Scand J Public Health 29: 300–307.
  23. 23. Kesmodel US, Underbjerg M, Kilburn TR, Bakketeig L, Mortensen EL, et al. (2010) Lifestyle during pregnancy: neurodevelopmental effects at 5 years of age. The design and implementation of a prospective follow-up study. Scand J Public Health 38: 208–219.
  24. 24. Falgreen Eriksen HL, Mortensen EL, Kilburn T, Underbjerg M, Bertrand J, et al. (2012) The effects of low to moderate prenatal alcohol exposure in early pregnancy on IQ in 5-year-old children. BJOG 119: 1191–1200.
  25. 25. Wechsler D. (1990) Manual for the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Revised. Kent GB: The Psychological Corporation. 230 p.
  26. 26. Wechsler D. (1999) Manual for the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence - Revised. Swedish edition. Stockholm, Sweden: Psykologiförlaget AB. 264 p.
  27. 27. Wechsler D. (1955) Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. New York, USA: The Psychological Corporation. 110 p.
  28. 28. Raven JC. (1958) Standard Progressive Matrices. Oxford, GB: Oxford Psychologists Press. 60 p.
  29. 29. Kesmodel U, Bertrand J, Stovring H, Skarpness B, Denny C, et al. (2012) The effect of different alcohol drinking patterns in early to mid pregnancy on the child's intelligence, attention, and executive function. BJOG 119: 1180–1190.
  30. 30. Hinkle SN, Schieve LA, Stein AD, Swan DW, Ramakrishnan U, et al. (2012) Associations between maternal prepregnancy body mass index and child neurodevelopment at 2 years of age. Int J Obes (Lond) 36: 1312–1319.
  31. 31. Tanda R, Salsberry PJ, Reagan PB, Fang MZ (2013) The impact of prepregnancy obesity on children's cognitive test scores. Matern Child Health J 17: 222–229.
  32. 32. Basatemur E, Gardiner J, Williams C, Melhuish E, Barnes J, et al. (2013) Maternal prepregnancy BMI and child cognition: a longitudinal cohort study. Pediatrics 131: 56–63.
  33. 33. Hinkle SN, Sharma AJ, Kim SY, Schieve LA (2013) Maternal prepregnancy weight status and associations with children's development and disabilities at kindergarten. Int J Obes (Lond) 37: 1344–1351.
  34. 34. Lawlor DA, Najman JM, Batty GD, O'Callaghan MJ, Williams GM, et al. (2006) Early life predictors of childhood intelligence: findings from the Mater-University study of pregnancy and its outcomes. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 20: 148–62.
  35. 35. Alati R, Macleod J, Hickman M, Sayal K, MAY M, et al. (2008) Intrauterine exposure to alcohol and tobacco use and childhood IQ: findings from a parental-offspring comparison within the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Pediatr Res 64: 659–666.
  36. 36. Nøhr EA. (2005) Obesity in Pregnancy. Epidemiological studies based on the Danish National Birth Cohort. Doctoral dissertation. Aarhus, Denmark: University of Aarhus.
  37. 37. Reed DR, Price RA (1998) Estimates of the heights and weights of family members: accuracy of informant reports. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 22: 827–835.