Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

The Roles of Standing Genetic Variation and Evolutionary History in Determining the Evolvability of Anti-Predator Strategies

  • Daniel R. O'Donnell ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Daniel R. O'Donnell, Abhijna Parigi, Jordan A. Fish

    odonn146@msu.edu

    Affiliations Program in Ecology, Evolutionary Biology, and Behavior, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, United States of America, Department of Zoology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, United States of America

  • Abhijna Parigi ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Daniel R. O'Donnell, Abhijna Parigi, Jordan A. Fish

    Affiliations Program in Ecology, Evolutionary Biology, and Behavior, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, United States of America, Department of Zoology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, United States of America

  • Jordan A. Fish ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Daniel R. O'Donnell, Abhijna Parigi, Jordan A. Fish

    Affiliations Department of Zoology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, United States of America, Center for Microbial Ecology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, United States of America

  • Ian Dworkin,

    Affiliations Program in Ecology, Evolutionary Biology, and Behavior, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, United States of America, Department of Zoology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, United States of America, BEACON Center for the Study of Evolution in Action, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, United States of America

  • Aaron P. Wagner

    Affiliation BEACON Center for the Study of Evolution in Action, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, United States of America

Abstract

Standing genetic variation and the historical environment in which that variation arises (evolutionary history) are both potentially significant determinants of a population's capacity for evolutionary response to a changing environment. Using the open-ended digital evolution software Avida, we evaluated the relative importance of these two factors in influencing evolutionary trajectories in the face of sudden environmental change. We examined how historical exposure to predation pressures, different levels of genetic variation, and combinations of the two, affected the evolvability of anti-predator strategies and competitive abilities in the presence or absence of threats from new, invasive predator populations. We show that while standing genetic variation plays some role in determining evolutionary responses, evolutionary history has the greater influence on a population's capacity to evolve anti-predator traits, i.e. traits effective against novel predators. This adaptability likely reflects the relative ease of repurposing existing, relevant genes and traits, and the broader potential value of the generation and maintenance of adaptively flexible traits in evolving populations.

Introduction

The diversity and complexity of any biological system reflects past evolutionary responses to environmental conditions. Underlying those responses are a number of extrinsic and intrinsic factors influencing populations' capacities to evolve responses through the generation or utilization of genetic variation [1], [2]. Because evolutionary responses rely largely on available heritable variation, mechanisms that influence the generation and maintenance of variation [3] can strongly shape the evolutionary trajectories of populations [4]. Among the potential factors involved, standing genetic variation (SGV) and evolutionary history (EH) are likely to be significant determinants of adaptability to novel environments [5]. Accordingly, understanding how these critical factors either promote or constrain population evolutionary potential provides insight into the realized pathways that led to historical evolutionary outcomes, as well as those that will shape future populations.

Standing genetic variation is the presence of alternative forms of a gene (alleles) at a given locus [5] in a population. While an allele may be mildly deleterious or confer no fitness advantage over other forms under one set of environmental conditions [6], that allele may become beneficial if the environment changes. As selection can act only on available variation, SGV provides a potential means for more rapid adaptive evolution ([7][20]; reviewed in [21]) compared with the de novo mutations [5], [22], particularly if the environment changes (e.g. if a new predator or competitor invades the system, or if abiotic conditions change).

In addition to SGV, a population's historical selection environment (i.e. evolutionary history) may play a strong role in determining the speed and the extent to which populations can adapt to directional environmental change [23][25]. In particular, EH will have influenced the genetic variation and genetic architecture of traits in contemporary populations. If changes in the environment alter the strength of selection on a trait, populations with an evolutionary history of adaptation to similar pressures may be mutationally “closer” to the discovery of new [26], or rediscovery of historical beneficial traits [27]. For example, Gillings and Stokes [25] suggest that historical exposure to antibiotics may confer greater adaptability of bacteria when exposed to novel antibiotics.

With the contemporary rise of experimental evolution as a means of testing evolutionary and ecological hypotheses [28], [29], a valuable and untapped opportunity now exists to elucidate the roles SGV and EH have played in determining historical, realized rates of adaptive evolution. Furthermore, while SGV is known to be an important determinant of the speed of evolution [5], [11], it is less clear how levels of SGV and EH, alone or in combination, impact the overall evolutionary potential of populations. Such an understanding could allow population geneticists to evaluate how SGV and EH may contribute to or constrain the future evolvability of populations, particularly in human-modified environments [1], [30], [31].

To evaluate the individual and interactive effects of SGV and EH on evolutionary outcomes, we measured their independent as well as their combined contributions toward evolutionary potential in populations using the digital evolution software Avida [32]. Digital organisms in Avida inhabit a controlled, two-dimensional environment, and experience evolutionary adaptation. The complexity of evolutionary dynamics possible in Avida can be compared to that of bacteria or viruses [33]. In Avida, as in biological systems, three necessary and sufficient conditions for producing evolution via natural selection are met: replication, heritable variation, and differential fitness resulting from that variation [34].

Digital evolution experiments carry several significant advantages for addressing evolutionary questions requiring systematic manipulation and highly controlled environments. Among these, generation times are rapid, the experimenter has full control over initial environmental conditions, and detailed genetic, demographic, and behavioral trait data can be recorded perfectly. Organisms in Avida can also engage in ecological competition and other complex interactions, and the system can allow for co-evolution in predator-prey systems [35][37]. Furthermore, unlike in evolutionary models and evolutionary simulations that impose artificial selection via explicit selection functions, Avida uniquely allows for unrestricted, unsupervised, and non-deterministic evolution via natural selection [32]. With biological organisms, it is difficult to know the exact number of unique genotypes present at a given time, especially when population sizes are large. Hence, manipulating levels of SGV in populations of biological organisms is difficult (but see [19]). Because our question required precise knowledge of the amount of genetic diversity present within each treatment, experimental evolution with biological organisms would have been impractical, given the scale of our study.

Predation is an ecologically important agent of selection [38], [39] as demonstrated by the diverse array of prey defenses that have evolved in nature [40][43]. Accordingly, we used Avida to test whether historical exposure to predation influenced how prey populations responded to pressures from new, invasive predators. We then further examined which factors (SGV, EH, and their interaction) were important in determining the future defensive and competitive abilities of those populations. While both factors played significant roles, we show that evolutionary history has the stronger influence on evolutionary potential, with the strength of SGV's role being contingent on a population's evolutionary history. Irrespective of the level of SGV, populations with predator EH almost always outcompeted populations without predator EH. In predator EH populations, SGV appeared to have no effect on evolvability, whereas, in no predator EH populations, higher SGV increased evolvability.

Materials and Methods

Avida

The Avida digital evolution platform is a tool for conducting evolutionary experiments on populations of self-replicating computer programs, termed “digital organisms” [32]. Digital organisms are composed of a set of instructions constituting their “genome”. Organisms execute genome instructions in order to perform actions such as processing information and interacting with their environment, and for reproduction. Additionally, predefined combinations of instructions allow organisms to consume resources from the simulated environment. Sufficient consumption of resources, to a level defined by the experimenter, is a prerequisite for organisms to copy their genomes and divide (i.e. reproduce). During the copy process, there is an experimenter-defined probability that genomic instructions will be replaced with a different instruction (substitution mutation) randomly selected from the full set of all 60 possible instructions (instruction set files are in the skel directory in the GitHub repository [https://github.com/fishjord/avida_predation_scripts]). Separately, there are also set probabilities for the copy of an instruction to fail (creating a deletion mutation) or for a chance insertion of a new instruction into the copy's genome. In real-time, generation times in Avida are typically only a few seconds, and experimental environments are fully definable.

Environment.

We ran all Avida trials in a 251×251 bounded grid-cell environment. Each cell started with one unit of resource. If a prey organism fed in a given cell (by executing an ‘output’ instruction), the resource in that cell was completely consumed and that cell was replenished with resources after 100 updates (an update is the measurement of time in Avida, reflecting the number of genomic instructions executed across the population).

Timing.

All Avida trials were run for a set number of updates, as indicated in each of the Phase descriptions below. Here, each organism executed 30 instructions per update. Organisms had a maximum lifespan of 15,000 instructions executed (500 updates). Realized average number of generations for the populations in each treatment is given in their respective sections below.

Reproduction.

Organisms, both predator and prey, were required to have consumed 10 resource units (in one “bite”, individuals could consume a maximum of 1 resource unit) and be at least 100 updates old to successfully reproduce. Genome copying and reproduction occurred when the organism executed a single reproduction (‘repro’) instruction. In most cases, biological organisms must consume copious resources (and thus perform a number of other feeding-related behaviors, e.g. moving, searching, handling) in order to execute one reproductive event; thus, execution of the reproduction instruction took one update, compared to 1/30th of an update for other instructions (except for predation handling time, as noted below). When an organism reproduced, its offspring was placed in the cell the parent was facing. This new organism's genome was a copy of the parent's genome, with the exception any substitutions, insertions, and deletions that occurred during the copy process. Substitutions were independent and only one insertion or deletion was allowed per reproduction (at rates specified in the ‘Phase 1’ and ‘Phase 2’ descriptions, below). In addition to the birth of the new organism upon reproduction, the parent organism was effectively reborn, with all internal states (e.g. stored values such as those describing objects seen, genomic marker positions, and execution pointer position) reset. Unlike in legacy Avida experiments, multiple organisms could occupy the same cell (as in [35][37]). Thus, newborn organisms did not replace existing occupants of the cells into which they were born.

Predation.

In Avida, predation occurs via the evolution of an ‘attack-prey’ instruction, the execution of which allows an organism to attack and kill a non-predator in the cell it is facing. Predation in Avida and the base predator Avida configuration used are described in [32]. Here, as in handling time, the attack-prey instruction costs the predator 10 cycles (one-third of an update) if the attack is successful. Predators receive 10% of each prey individual's consumed resources after a successful kill, which is then applied toward the consumption threshold required for reproduction. An organism is classified as a predator in Avida if it attacks a prey organism, not simply by evolving the ‘attack-prey’ instruction. Predators can only consume prey organisms, and cannot consume resources.

In simple (i.e. lacking topographic features like refuges) and confined environments like the one used here, Avida trials containing predators typically require a minimum prey population level below which predator attacks fail. In top-down limited populations like those used here, minimum prey levels prevent population extinctions and serve to standardize prey population sizes and thus intra-specific competitive pressures. In practice, because prey are constantly being born into populations, prey kills are prevented for very short periods of time and failures simply require predators to make multiple, rapid attacks. Here, attacks on prey were always fatal if there were more than 1,000 prey in the environment (i.e., 1,000 was a minimum prey level below which fatal attacks were prevented). Similarly, in trials without predators, prey population sizes were controlled via a preset maximum population cap (1,000 organisms), as described for each Phase below. When a birth caused that limit to be exceeded, a random prey organism was removed from the population.

Phase One: Evolutionary History

Our study was divided into three experimental phases (Figure 1). In the first phase (“Phase 1”), we evolved two sets of 30 base populations. Each of these populations was initiated via the placement of 9 identical prey organisms into a new grid environment (Figure 1, Phase 1). These ancestors had simple genomes that allowed them only to blindly move around the environment, repeatedly attempting to feed and reproduce. In one set of populations, the attack-prey instruction was allowed to appear in the organisms' genomes through random mutations, thus allowing for the potential evolution of predators (Figure 1, Phase 1). Henceforth we refer to these populations as “predator EH” treatments. In the second set of populations, the “attack” instruction was prevented from mutating into organisms' genomes. This second set of “no predator EH” base populations thus evolved in the absence of predators in Phase 1. Each population evolved for two million updates (on average, 20,000 generations). During this phase, reproduction carried a 25% probability of a single, random, genomic instruction substitution (maximum one substitution per genome, conditions identified in [44] as being optimal to facilitate evolution of complexity in Avida), and 5% probabilities (each) of single insertion and deletion mutations.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Design of the experiment to test the effects of SGV and EH on evolution of anti-predator traits.

A) 9 identical prey are injected to initialize two sets of 30 base populations. B) Over the course of 2 million updates of evolution, populations diversified, including the de novo evolution of predators in half of these EH populations (top). C) One fully evolved prey population from each EH treatment (with and without evolving predators) was used to create 30 identical sets of high, intermediate, and no diversity (clone) SGV populations (totaling 2×30×3 = 180 populations), with clones of the best evolved predator from Phase 1 added to each. D) After a further 2 million updates of evolution at a low mutation rate, replicate populations converged on an intermediate level of diversity, but SGV and EH differences result in trait variation among populations. E) To evaluate the effectiveness of evolved anti-predator traits, each of the 180 fully evolved populations from D were introduced, in turn, into separate competition assays with one randomly selected replicate from each of the other EH×SGV combination populations, both in the presence and in the absence of a new novel predator. F) Separately, prey rates of executing “anti-predator” actions (moves, turns, looks) were recorded in the presence and absence of predators, to quantify expressed behavioral responses to predators for all fully evolved populations.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0100163.g001

Phase Two: Standing Genetic Variation

The second phase (Figure 1, Phase 2) of the experiments allowed us to evaluate how the amount of SGV as well as EH influenced the evolutionary potential of prey in the presence of novel predators. For all Phase 2 trials, prey populations (compositions described below) were evolved for 200,000 updates in the presence of identical copies of a single static predator population (i.e. predators in Phase 2 were prevented from evolving further, dying, or reproducing). We used static predators in Phase 2 trials in order to prevent predator-prey coevolution dynamics from causing predation pressure to vary among replicates. Taking in sensory information is a prerequisite for behaviorally responding to and targeting prey, and we used visual input rates as a proxy for predator effectiveness. If organisms evolve to use visual sensors, these sensors provide them with information about their environment, allowing them to “look” at the objects (food, prey or other organisms) in the 45-degree area in front of them. Because predator populations varied across Phase 1 replicates, we selected the Phase 2 predator population from the Phase 1 replicate with highest realized usage of visual sensors (i.e. highest per-predator ‘look-ahead-intercept’ instruction executions) to ensure that an effective predator population was used in Phase 2. Ultimately, the predator population selected and copied for use in all Phase 2 trials had 209 predator organisms.

To create Phase 2 Evolutionary History (EH) treatment source populations, we first selected a single prey population from a random Phase 1 replicate in each EH treatment (predator and no predator EH), excluding the population from which the Phase 2 predator population was drawn. For each of the two selected populations, we excluded any organisms that were classified as predators or had parents that were predators (as determined by an internal state, see [35], [36]), to ensure that Phase 2 prey populations consisted only of prey organisms. An organism must execute an attack instruction to be designated a predator, and some prey organisms may have attack instructions in their genomes, even if they have not yet used them; we therefore replaced any attack instructions in the remaining prey populations with a ‘do nothing’ (nop-X) instruction, and prevented any new copies of the attack instruction from mutating into offspring genomes.

To create Phase 2 SGV treatment populations, we created separate “clone”, “intermediate”, and “high” SGV populations by sampling each of the two EH treatment source populations ("seed populations"; one predator EH population and one no predator EH population: Figure 1, Phase 2). First, for high SGV populations, we simply used duplicates of the selected EH treatment source populations. Because all possible genomes are present in this treatment, evolutionary trajectories of replicates within this treatment were not constrained by the lack of genetic variation. Second, we created each clone SGV population by randomly selecting a single genotype from a seed population and making as many duplicates of that genotype as there were organisms in the seed population. A genotype could be selected for multiple Phase 2 clone replicates. Given that most evolution experiments with microorganisms begin with a single clone, we wanted to compare the evolutionary trajectory of a clonal population to the high SGV populations. Third, we created the intermediate SGV populations by randomly sampling genotypes (with replacement) from a seed population, creating up to 55 identical copies of each sampled genotype. We repeated this process until the new intermediate SGV population was the same size as the seed population; this sampling scheme yielded intermediate SGV prey populations the same size as their high SGV seed populations, but with ∼50% lower diversity (Shannon's diversity index), on average. Evolution experiments with diploid, sexually reproducing organism like Drosophila melanogaster start with large, wild caught natural populations. However, sampling from a natural population cannot capture all possible genotypes. Intermediate SGV populations are analogous to such “sampled” populations that have some, but not all the available SGV. In all, there were six (2 EH×3 SGV) Phase 2 treatments, each with 30 replicates (Figure 1).

For each of the six combinations of SGV (clone, intermediate, high) and EH (predator, no predator EH), we evolved 30 replicate populations for 200,000 updates (500 generations, on average), each in the presence of a copy of the constructed Phase 2 predator population. The mutation rates in Phase 2 were lowered to 0.1% substitution probability, and 0.5% insertion and deletion probability per generation. We found empirically that these mutation rates yielded, on average, 5% divergence (see below) between ancestor and final organisms in Avida after 500 generations (200,000 updates using the Phase 2 settings), matching the expected divergence of a bacterial genome over 500 generations [45]. Other configuration settings were identical to those used in Phase 1.

At the end of Phase 2, we calculated how different the resulting population was from the starting population by using a dynamic programming algorithm [46]. We measured the genetic divergence of each prey organism in each population by aligning the current genome sequence with that of its ancestor from the beginning of Phase 2 (alignment was necessary to account for changes in genome length due to insertion/deletion). Each organism from the seed population was tagged with a unique Lineage ID, which is shared with all progeny of the organism. The Lineage ID of the organisms present in the population at the end of Phase 2 could then be used to identify their ancestor from the beginning of Phase 2. We then calculated the divergence as the percent identity between the two aligned genome sequences.

Phase 3: Competitive Evaluation of prey populations and assessment of trait evolution

For the third and final phase (Figure 1, Phase 3a), we used a set of both “ecological” evaluation simulations to measure the fitness of the final prey populations in the presence and absence of predators (referred to as “PT” for “predator treatment” from now on), and trait assays to evaluate the evolution of anti-predator traits during Phase 2. For all Phase 3 evaluations, substitution, insertion and deletion mutation rates were set to zero. For all types of Phase 3 trials (described below), in order to create an uneven resource landscape (i.e. in the absence of consumption by prey, all cells would contain resources that could be consumed), populations were introduced into their test environments, run for an initial 1,000 updates, and then reintroduced a second time. Once prey were reintroduced, we ran each trial for an additional 10,000 updates, and recorded population sizes for the two competing populations.

As in Phase 2, Phase 3 trials used copies of a single Phase 1 predator population. The Phase 3 predator was intended to exhibit novel strategies relative to the Phase 2 predator population. We selected the Phase 3 predator population by first eliminating the three Phase 1 replicates used for creating Phase 2 predator and prey populations, and then selecting one predator population that exhibited an average level of visual sensor usage (about half that of the predator population selected for Phase 2). The final selected predator population for Phase 3 contained 253 predators.

Phase 3 involved two sets of trials: the first set allowed for evaluating competitiveness, while the second set allowed for evaluating the effects of SGV and EH on expressed levels of evolved anti-predator behavioral traits. In the first set of Phase 3 trials (Figure 1, Phase 3a), we conducted pairwise competitions in which each population from each of the six Phase 2 treatments was paired with a randomly selected population from each of the five other treatments (for a total of 30 pairings ×30 replicates  = 900 competitions). For each pairing, the two populations were injected once into an environment with the Phase 3 predator population (“predators present PT”) and once into an environment without predators (“predators absent PT”). For each of these trials, we enforced a prey population level of 2,000 and recorded the relative abundance of each of the two competing populations every 1,000 updates over the course of the 10,000-update trial.

The second set of Phase 3 trials was to evaluate the degree to which traits likely involved in defense evolved in prey populations during Phase 2 (Figure 1, Phase 3b), thus elucidating proximate causes of the outcomes of ecological competition in Phase 3a. Each of the starting (“pre-Phase 2”) and final (“post-Phase 2”) Phase 2 prey populations was exposed to a copy of the new Phase 3 predator population and reintroduced into a fresh environment. We then recorded the number of moves and turns executed and the usage of visual sensors (“look” instructions), as proxies for anti-predator responses. Each prey population was then introduced a second time in a separate evaluation in a predators absent PT environment. For each of these trials, we kept the number of prey at a constant 1,000 individuals via enforcement of population caps (i.e. a random prey was killed if a new birth would bring the prey population above 1,000) and minimum thresholds (below which predator attacks would fail).

Statistical analysis

Due to the anticorrelation of the relative abundances of lineages in competition, as well as the extreme heterogeneity of variances among competition scenarios (Figure 2), we used a permutational multiple analysis of variance (PerMANOVA) (Table S3) with SGV, EH, and PT as predictors and the relative abundances of lineages in competition as response variables (Table S3) to analyze the effects of SGV, EH, and PT outcomes of ecological competition (Phase 3; Figure 2). The SGV×EH×PT interaction was far from significant (F = 0.60; df = 2; P = 0.56), so we dropped this term from the model and used a reduced model containing only main effects and all first-order interactions (Table S3).

thumbnail
Figure 2. Ecological competition.

Populations from all SGV and EH treatment combinations are randomly paired in competition in the absence (gray boxes, above diagonal) and presence (black boxes, below diagonal) of a novel predator (Phase 3). EH had a far greater effect on competitive outcomes than SGV in both the presence and absence of predators, with predator EH populations competitively dominant in most cases. Y-axis: relative abundance of each lineage in competition. Colors for each lineage indicate SGV and EH treatment history. Black: clone, predator; Gray: clone, no predator; Blue: intermediate, predator; Red: intermediate, no predator; Gold: high, predator; Pink: high, no predator. Dots indicate mean relative abundance for lineages of each treatment after 10,000 updates of competition.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0100163.g002

To generate response variables for models describing change in traits due to Phase 2 evolution, we divided instruction counts by total instructions performed (e.g., number of moves/total lifetime instructions) to derive the proportion of instructions represented by each instruction type. Trait assays were conducted pre- and post-Phase 2. Thus, to determine the effect of evolution specifically during Phase 2, we subtracted pre-Phase 2 instruction proportions from post-Phase 2 proportions, yielding the raw change in instruction proportions.

We used linear mixed effects models (Equation 1; Appendix S1) fit by maximum likelihood to determine the effects of SGV, EH, and Phase 3 predator treatment (PT) on change in total instructions and in instruction proportions due to Phase 2 evolution. SGV, EH, and PT were included as fixed effects. Since both predator treatment (PT) levels (predators present, predators absent) were applied to each population, we fitted replicate as a random intercept, and included a random slope across PT levels to account for non-independence of PT levels. Where appropriate, we also allowed for unequal variances among factor levels using the “varIdent()” function from the “nlme” [47] package (Table S4). Equation 1 describes the change in total prey instructions due to Phase 2 evolution. Models describing change in prey moves, turns, and looks varied in complexity, but were of the same general form (Appendix S1). For each trait, we used a likelihood ratio test for model selection, starting with the full model (Equation 1) and dropping interaction terms that did not improve the model fit (Table S4).

EQUATION 1where β0ij are the random intercepts, β1ij are the randomly varying slopes across PT levels, and ∼EH is the variance function allowing variances to differ between EH levels. We used a method recently developed by [48] to calculate marginal (proportion of variation accounted for by fixed effects alone) and conditional R2 (proportion of variation accounted for by fixed and random effects together). and for each model are given in the Table S5.

To test the effects of SGV and EH on predator attack rates during trait assays, we used a general linear model with SGV and EH included as fixed effects, and the raw difference in attack rates between pre- and post-Phase 2 evolution as the response variable. A single replicate population was excluded from analyses of change in total instructions, as this replicate had an unusually high pre-Phase 2 instruction count, was highly unrepresentative of prey populations in general, and greatly increased the variance of the clone SGV + no predator EH treatment group (Figure S6).

Software and Hardware

We used Avida version 2.14 and the Heads-EX hardware [44] for all experiments. Avida did not originally support immortal, sterile predators as used in Phase 2 and Phase 3, so we added an additional option to Avida that, when set, causes predators to ‘reset’ (akin to being reborn) instead of reproducing or dying of old age. These modifications are available as a patch file against Avida 2.14, along with all analysis scripts, in the GitHub repository (https://github.com/fishjord/avida_predation_scripts).

We conducted all experiments using the Michigan State University's High Performance Computing Cluster (HPCC). The MSU HPCC contains three general purpose computing clusters containing 2944 cores. Individual Avida runs were submitted as compute jobs to the HPCC general processing queue in parallel where possible. The Avida jobs took between 30 minutes and seven days to run, depending on the number of updates (2,000 to 2,000,000 updates).

We performed all statistical analyses and constructed all figures using the R statistical programming language version 3.0.2. The PerMANOVA was conducted using the “adonis()” function in the “vegan” package (last updated by [49]). Linear mixed effects models were constructed using the “lme” function from the “nlme” package (last updated by [47]), and we used the “allEffects” function from the “effects” package (last updated by [50]) to extract marginalized fixed effects. To calculate marginal and conditional , we used the “r.squaredGLMM” function from the “MuMIn” package (last updated by [51]).

Results

Ecological competitions

Evolutionary history, i.e. historical exposure to predation in Phase 1, was the most important selective agent shaping competitive abilities of prey both in the presence and in the absence of a novel predator. Prey that evolved with predator EH were, in general, stronger competitors than those that evolved with no predator EH. In competitions between predator EH and no predator EH treatments, predator EH treatments always had the higher mean final relative abundance (Figure 2; Table S3). Competitive exclusion of a predator EH lineage by a no predator EH lineage after 10,000 updates in competition was extremely rare (but see e.g. Figure 2, panel D3, in which 1 out of 30 predator lineages was excluded). Competitive outcomes were not affected by the presence of a novel predator, regardless of SGV or EH (Figure 2; Table S3). While there was no main effect of SGV on mean final relative abundances (SGV effect: Table S3), higher SGV seems to have conferred some benefit to no predator EH populations (Figure 2; SGV×EH effect: Table S3). Similarly, there appears to be a subtle interaction between PT and EH, wherein in the presence of a predator, the difference in relative abundances between predator EH and no predator EH populations during competition assays increases in favor of predator EH populations.

Evolution of prey traits during Phase 2

Lifetime instruction counts for prey do not reflect higher rates of instruction execution, but rather longer lifespans of prey organisms. Changes in instruction counts varied greatly among treatments (PT×EH×SGV interaction; Figure S2). During Phase 2 evolution, regardless of presence or absence of a novel predator during trait assays, predator EH populations generally evolved to increase total instructions executed per lifetime, except in the case of clone populations in the absence of predators (Figure 3). Conversely, no predator EH populations universally decreased in lifetime instruction counts. In predator EH populations, SGV had a markedly positive effect on the magnitude of the decrease in instruction counts, while SGV had little or no effect on instruction counts in predator EH populations. Prey generally changed more in total instructions executed with predators present than with predators absent (Figure 3).

thumbnail
Figure 3. Change in total prey instructions executed (start of Phase 2 minus end of Phase 2), reflecting change in gestation time (or lifespan) resulting from Phase 2 evolution.

Predator present/absent” refers to the presence or absence of a novel predator during trait assays before and after Phase 2 evolution. Total instructions either increased or decreased very little in predator EH populations, and universally decreased in no predator EH populations. Bars are ±95% CI.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0100163.g003

Prey moves as proportions of total prey instructions universally decreased in predator EH populations as a result of Phase 2 evolution, and largely increased in no predator EH populations (EH effect, Figure 4; Figure S3). Effects of SGV on change in moves were subtle, though slightly less so with predators absent. There was no main effect of PT on change in moves (Figure 4; Figure S3).

thumbnail
Figure 4. Change in (top) prey moves, (middle) prey turns, and (bottom) prey looks as proportions of total prey instructions, resulting from Phase 2 evolution with a novel predator.

Predator present/absent” refers to the presence/absence of the Phase 3 novel predator during the trait assays. Traits in predator EH populations generally evolved to a greater extent than did traits in no predator EH populations, and traits often evolved in different directions between the two EH treatments. Bars are ±95% CI.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0100163.g004

Prey turns as proportions of total prey instructions increased dramatically in predator EH populations, but changed very little in no predator EH populations (Figure 4; Figure S4). Effects of SGV were subtle, but were most pronounced in no predator EH populations: clone and intermediate SGV populations decreased slightly on average in proportion moves, while high SGV populations increased (EH×SGV interaction, Figure S4). PT did not affect change in turns (no PT main effect, Figure 4; Figure S4), nor was there a main effect of SGV (Figure S4).

Proportions of instructions that were looks generally decreased in predator EH populations, but changed little in no predator EH populations; EH effects varied among levels of SGV, with the smallest EH effects in clone SGV populations (EH×SGV interaction, Figure 4; Figure S5). Main effects of SGV and PT were not statistically significant (Figure S5).

Effects of Phase 2 prey evolution on predator attack rates

Phase 2 evolution led to a reduction in predator attacks in nearly all cases, and EH and SGV both affected predator attacks on prey (Figure 5; Table S2). High and intermediate SGV populations experienced qualitatively similar reductions in predator attacks as a result of Phase 2 evolution, and prey evolved with predator EH experienced similar reductions in predator attacks across SGV treatments (Figure 5). Only clone SGV + no predator EH populations were attacked more after Phase 2 evolution.

thumbnail
Figure 5. Change in attacks in behavioral assays pairing the Phase 3 predator with pre- and post-Phase 2 prey populations.

Predator attack frequencies on predator EH populations universally decreased, and generally changed more than did attack frequencies on no predator EH populations. Bars are ±95% CI.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0100163.g005

Discussion

Though the role of SGV in aiding rapid adaptive evolution has been established [5], [7][12], [22], few studies have empirically tested the effect of varying levels of SGV on evolvability. Additionally, how a population's EH affects evolvability, both independently and in combination with SGV is still largely unexplored [26]. For example, Lohbeck et al. 2012 demonstrated that clonal and genetically diverse populations of the marine coccolithophore Emiliana huxleyi adapted similarly to increased levels of CO2. In genetically diverse populations clone sorting occurred, such that only a subset of the clones persisted in each CO2 treatment. However, all E. huxleyi clones were isolated from the same location, so the effect of EH on adaptation to elevated CO2 was not tested in this case.

Here, we were able to separately control for and vary the background levels of SGV and the EH, and subsequently test the roles of each in shaping the evolvability of populations in a predator-prey context. Avida is meant to be analogous to biological systems, and the hypotheses testable in Avida could, in theory, apply to biological systems as well. However, the greatest challenge in conducting evolution experiments is the time required to measure significant evolution. Digital organisms reproduce much more quickly than even bacteria, making large-scale experiments covering millions of generations practical. Evolution experiments on the scale of our study are only practical in digital systems, as the labor and time required to conduct such experiments in biological systems would be prohibitive.

Role of evolutionary history

Evolutionary history (the combination of selective pressures under which populations evolve) likely determines which mutations become fixed in populations [23]. We found that having an evolutionary history with a predator improves a population's ability to evolve defenses in response to novel pressures from novel invading predator populations: in general, predator EH populations experienced greater changes in trait expression compared to no predator EH populations (Figure 4) when evolving defenses against invading (Phase 2) predators. This occurs regardless of the level of SGV, suggesting that the importance of EH eclipses that of evolvability originating from SGV. Furthermore, in pairwise competitions (Figure 2), populations with predator EH demonstrated competitive superiority over no predator EH populations, as in [35]. If a novel predator was present during the competition, the advantage was even more pronounced (also see Table S3), with the vast majority of predator EH populations competitively excluding their no predator EH competitors (Figure 2). Additionally, when introduced into separate ecological trials with the novel predator, populations that evolved with predator EH experienced a greater decline in predator attacks over Phase 2 than populations evolved with no predator EH (Figure 5).

Because populations in both EH treatments were further evolved in the presence of predators (in Phase 2) before they were competed against each other, the ecological advantage enjoyed by predator EH populations in competition with no predator EH populations was due, in part, to their historical exposure to predators (i.e. in Phase 1), even though that initial evolution was in response to an entirely different predator population than that used in Phase 2. It is clear that evolution in the presence of predators yields not only improved antipreditor traits, but also improved ecological efficiency. It is likely that traits such as enhanced moving, turning, and looking improve both predator resistance and foraging success [36].

Large differences in behavioral traits or competitive ability in the presence v. absence of predators (Phase 3) would indicate plasticity. However, while some plasticity is apparent, differences in trait values and competitive outcomes between PT levels are extremely subtle. The observed changes across Phase 2 were thus primarily in constitutively expressed traits, not plastic ones.

Both the mutation rates used for the evolution of Phase 1 predator EH populations and the duration of the evolutionary trials will have enhanced the effect of evolutionary history in each trial (i.e. the degree to which prey populations were adapted to predators). Because we did not vary Phase 1 mutation rates or duration, we cannot comment on how experimentally altering the strength of historical selection could impact the observed trends in trait evolution and competitive outcomes.

Role of standing genetic variation

Many studies have pointed to SGV as a key component of a population's evolutionary potential [6]. When there is greater genetic variation in a population, there is more raw material upon which natural selection can act. Thus, we expected SGV alone to play a significant role in the rapid evolution of anti-predator traits. However, we found marginal effects of historical SGV in determining the final outcomes of ecological competitions (Figure 2). Furthermore, the effects of SGV on the evolution of specific anti-predator traits (moves, turns, looks) were detectable only within EH treatments (Figure 4).

Standing genetic variation (SGV) vs Evolutionary history (EH)

Across the predator EH populations, we found no detectable effects of SGV on anti-predator trait values (Figure 4) or competitive outcomes (Figure 2). We suggest that a lack of an SGV effect on predator EH populations may be due, in part, to the historical predator weeding out unfit prey genotypes. For example, predators reduced prey diversity (Figure S1; Table S1) because they operate as a very strong agent of selection against less fit phenotypes. However, we would expect SGV to have an effect within no predator EH populations (as seen in Figure 4). Given the lack of an evolutionary history with predators, in no predator EH populations, the odds of one sampled genotype (clone SGV) being able to quickly acquire a beneficial mutation are low. On the other hand, if the full suite of all discovered genotypes (high SGV) remains in the population, there is a greater potential for providing precursory material for rapidly generating anti-predator traits. However, if EH leads to a large number of genotypes having the prerequisites for rapid adaptive response, the odds of sampling a beneficial genotype are high, and differences in SGV will not significantly affect evolvability.

As we have shown, in order to adaptively address novel predation pressures, it appears to be easier to modify historically realized and relevant adaptations than to repurpose unrelated genes, or evolve effective traits de novo. Thus, the greater evolvability of the predator EH populations would have arisen out of their ability to adapt anti-predator strategies across predation contexts. More broadly, evolvability may commonly be determined by a population's ability to use strategies across environmental contexts, i.e. their adaptive complexity [52].

Supporting Information

Figure S1.

Shannon's diversity across 200,000 updates of Phase 2 evolution in (A) high, (B) intermediate, and (C) clone SGV treatments. Filled circles represent no predator EH treatments, open circles represent predator EH treatments. Bars are ±95% CI.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0100163.s001

(EPS)

Figure S2.

Change in instructions. Shown are the marginalized fixed effects of a generalized linear mixed effects model of scaled change in total prey instructions across Phase 2 evolution as a function of SGV (A. clone; B. intermediate; C. high), EH, and PT. Bars are ±95% CI. EH had the greatest effect on change in prey instructions, though PT also affected change in instructions, especially in clone SGV populations. Formal model, random effects output, and marginal and conditional R2 can be found in Table S5.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0100163.s002

(EPS)

Figure S3.

Change in prey moves. Shown are the marginalized fixed effects of a generalized linear mixed effects model of scaled change across Phase 2 evolution in the proportion of prey instructions that were moves as a function of A. SGV and PT and B. EH. Bars are ±95% CI. Both EH and SGV affected change in proportion moves, with SGV increasing more dramatically across SGV treatments with predator present. Formal model, random effects output, and marginal and conditional R2 can be found in Table S5.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0100163.s003

(EPS)

Figure S4.

Change in prey turns. Shown are the marginalized fixed effects of a generalized linear mixed effects model of scaled change across Phase 2 evolution in the proportion of prey instructions that were turns as a function of A. PT and B. SGV×EH. Bars are ±95% CI. Predator EH populations increased turns as a results of Phase 2 evolution; change in turns was marginal for no predator EH populations, and SGV affected predator and no predator populations differently. PT had no detectable effect. Formal model, random effects output, and marginal and conditional R2 can be found in Table S5.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0100163.s004

(EPS)

Figure S5.

Change in prey looks. Shown are the marginalized fixed effects of a generalized linear mixed effects model of scaled change across Phase 2 evolution in the proportion of prey instructions that were looks as a function of A. PT and B. SGV×EH. Bars are ±95% CI. Clone and high SGV populations with predator EH increased looks as a result of Phase 2 evolution and change in turns was marginal to negative for no predator EH populations. PT had no detectable effect. Formal model, random effects output, and marginal and conditional R2 can be found in Table S5.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0100163.s005

(EPS)

Figure S6.

Total instructions executed by each replicate prey population before and after Phase 2 evolution. The slope of each line represents the evolutionary change in instructions executed per lifetime (i.e. gestation time). The same replicate populations were assayed in the absence (top) and in the presence (bottom) of the Phase 3 predator. Arrows indicate the replicate population that was excluded from all statistical analyses, as it is not representative of its treatment group (clone SGV, no predator EH).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0100163.s006

(EPS)

Table S1.

Analysis of Variance (calculated using the anova() function in the R 3.0.2 base package) on a linear model* testing the effects of SGV and EH and their interaction on Shannon's diversity among populations at the 200,000th update of Phase 2 evolution.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0100163.s007

(DOCX)

Table S2.

Analysis of Variance (calculated using the anova() function in the R 3.0.2 base package) on a linear model* testing the effects of SGV and EH and their interaction on change in predator attacks as a result of Phase 2 evolution.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0100163.s008

(DOCX)

Table S3.

Permutational multiple analysis of variance (PerMANOVA) output describing effects on SGV, EH, and PT (and all first-order interactions) on outcomes of ecological competitions (see Figure 2). 999 permutations were used.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0100163.s009

(DOCX)

Table S4.

AIC, LRT output, and model selection processes for linear mixed-effects models describing change in A. total prey instructions; proportion moves; C. proportion turns and D. proportion looks. Variance functions are shown with traits modeled (in italics).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0100163.s010

(DOCX)

Table S5.

Random intercepts and slopes (linear contrasts) between levels of PT with parametric bootstrap 95% confidence intervals for models describing change in A. total prey instructions; B. proportion moves; C. proportion turns; and D. proportion looks. Also included are marginal and conditional R2 and residual standard deviations with parametric bootstrap 95% CI.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0100163.s011

(DOCX)

Appendix S1.

S1.1: Genetic diversification during Phase 2 evolution. S1.2: Linear mixed effects models describing evolutionary change in traits.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0100163.s012

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We thank Chris Adami for comments and feedback on an earlier draft of this manuscript, and Max McKinnon for contributions to early stages of this research. Finally, we thank two anonymous reviewers for comments on the earlier versions of this work.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: AP DRO JAF APW. Performed the experiments: JAF APW. Analyzed the data: DRO. Wrote the paper: DRO AP JAF APW ID. Designed all figures and tables, aided in experimental design, contributed significantly to interpretation of results, and conducted significant revision of the manuscript: DRO. Significant revision/editing of manuscript: ID.

References

  1. 1. Frankham R, Lees K, Montgomery ME, England PR, Lowe EH, et al. (1999) Do population size bottlenecks reduce evolutionary potential? Anim Conserv 2: 255–260.
  2. 2. Colegrave N, Collins S (2008) Experimental evolution: experimental evolution and evolvability. Heredity 100: 464–470.
  3. 3. Mitchell-Olds T, Willis JH, Goldstein DB (2007) Which evolutionary processes influence natural genetic variation for phenotypic traits? Nat Rev Genet 8: 845–856.
  4. 4. Futuyma DJ (1979). Evolutionary Biology. 1st ed. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, Massachusetts. ISBN 0-87893-199-6.
  5. 5. Barrett R, Schluter D (2008) Adaptation from standing genetic variation. Trends Ecol Evol 23: 38–44.
  6. 6. Orr HA, Betancourt AJ (2001) Haldane's sieve and adaptation from the standing genetic variation. Genetics 157: 875–884.
  7. 7. Innan H, Kim Y (2004) Pattern of polymorphism after strong artificial selection in a domestication event. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101: 10667–10672.
  8. 8. Colosimo PF, Hosemann KE, Balabhadra S, Villarreal Jr G, Dickson M, et al. (2005) Widespread parallel evolution in sticklebacks by repeated fixation of ectodysplasin alleles. Science 307: 1928–1933.
  9. 9. Prezeworski M, Coop G, Wall JD (2005) The signature of positive selection on standing genetic variation. Evolution 59: 2312–2323.
  10. 10. Pennings PS (2012) Standing genetic variation and the evolution of drug resistance in HIV. PLoS Comput Biol 8: e1002527.
  11. 11. Elmer KR, Meyer A (2011) Adaptation in the age of ecological genomics: insights from parallelism and convergence. Trends Ecol Evol 6: 298–306.
  12. 12. Yoshida T, Jones LE, Ellner SP, Fussmann GF, Hairston NG (2003) Rapid evolution drives ecological dynamics in a predator-prey system. Nature 424: 303–306.
  13. 13. Litchman E, Edwards KF, Klausmeier CA, Thomas MK (2012) Phytoplankton niches, traits and eco-evolutionary responses to global environmental change. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 470: 235–248.
  14. 14. Jerome JP, Bell JA, Plovanich-Jones AE, Barrick JE, Brown CT, et al. (2011) Standing Genetic Variation in Contingency Loci Drives the Rapid Adaptation of Campylobacter jejuni to a Novel Host. PLoS ONE 6: e16399.
  15. 15. Dionne M, Miller KM, Dodson JJ, Bernatchez L (2009) MHC standing genetic variation and pathogen resistance in wild Atlantic salmon. Philos T Roy Soc B 364: 1555–1565.
  16. 16. Dettman JR, Anderson JB, Kohn LM (2008) Divergent adaptation promotes reproductive isolation among experimental populations of the filamentous fungus Neurospora. BMC Evol Biol 8: 35.
  17. 17. Bell G, Collins S (2008) Adaptation, extinction and global change. Evol Appl 1: 3–16.
  18. 18. McGuigan K, Sgrò CM (2009) Evolutionary consequences of cryptic genetic variation. Trends Ecol Evol 24: 305–311.
  19. 19. Lohbeck KT, Riebesell U, Reusch TBH (2012) Adaptive evolution of a key phytoplankton species to ocean acidification. Nat Geosci 5: 346–351.
  20. 20. Hayden EJ, Ferrada E, Wagner A (2011) Cryptic genetic variation promotes rapid evolutionary adaptation in an RNA enzyme. Nature 474: 92–95.
  21. 21. Messer PW, Petrov DA (2013) Population genomics of rapidadaptation by soft selective sweeps. Trends Ecol Evol 28: 659–669.
  22. 22. Anderson CJ (2013) The role of standing genetic variation in adaptation of digital organisms to a new environment. Artif Life 13: 3–10.
  23. 23. Hermisson J (2005) Soft sweeps: molecular population genetics of adaptation from standing genetic variation. Genetics 169 (4) 2335–2352.
  24. 24. Levins R (1963) Theory of fitness in a heterogeneous environment. II. Developmental flexibility and niche selection. Am Nat 97: 75–90.
  25. 25. Gillings MR, Stokes HW (2012) Are humans increasing bacterial evolvability? Trends Ecol Evol 27: 346–352.
  26. 26. Blount ZD, Borland CZ, Lenski RE (2008) Historical contingency and the evolution of a key innovation in an experimental population of Escherichia coli. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105: 7899–7906.
  27. 27. Hedrick PW (1976) Genetic variation in a heterogeneous environment. II. Temporal heterogeneity and directional selection. Genetics 84: 145–157.
  28. 28. Kawecki TJ, Lenski RE, Ebert D, Hollis B, Olivieri I, et al. (2012) Experimental evolution. Trends Ecol Evol 27: 547–560.
  29. 29. Fry JD (2001) Direct and correlated responses to selection for larval ethanol tolerance in Drosophila melanogaster. J Evolution Biol 14: 296–309.
  30. 30. Bonnell ML, Selander RK (1974) Elephant seals: genetic variation and near extinction. Science 184 (4139) 908–909.
  31. 31. Lande R (1988) Genetics and demography in biological conservation. Science 241: 1455–1460.
  32. 32. Ofria C, Bryson DM, Wilke CO (2009) Avida: A Software Platform for Research in Computational Evolutionary Biology. In: Adamatzky A, Komosinski M, editors. Artificial Life Models in Software. London, UK: Springer Verlag, London, UK. Second edition.
  33. 33. Wilke CO, Adami C (2002) The biology of digital organisms. Trends Ecol Evol 17: 528–532.
  34. 34. Lenski RE, Ofria C, Pennock RT, Adami C (2003) The evolutionary origin of complex features. Nature 423: 139–144.
  35. 35. Fortuna MA, Zaman L, Wagner AP, Ofria C (2013) Evolving Digital Ecological Networks. PLoS Comput Biol 9: e1002928.
  36. 36. Wagner AP, Zaman L, Dworkin I, Ofria C (2013) Coevolution, behavioral chases, and the evolution of prey intelligence. arXiv:1310.1369v2.
  37. 37. Lehmann K, Goldman BW, Dworkin I (2013) From cues to signals: evolution of interspecific communication via aposematism and mimicry in a predator-prey system. PLoS One 9: e91783.
  38. 38. Lima SL, Dill LM (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 68: 619–640.
  39. 39. Sih A (1987) Predator and prey lifestyles: an evolutionary and ecological overview. Predation: direct and indirect impacts on aquatic communities (Kerfoot WC, Sih A, eds). Hanover, New Hampshire: University Press of New England; 203–224.
  40. 40. McCollum SA, Van Buskirk J (1996) Costs and benefits of a predator-induced polyphenism in the gray treefrog Hyla chrysoscelis. Evolution 50: 583–593.
  41. 41. Dodson S (1989) Predator-induced reaction norms. BioScience 39: 447–452.
  42. 42. Rundle HD, Nosil P (2005) Ecological speciation. Ecol Lett 8: 336–352.
  43. 43. Schmitz OJ (2008) Effects of predator hunting mode on grassland ecosystem function. Science 319: 952–954.
  44. 44. Bryson DM, Ofria C (2013) Understanding evolution potential in virtual CPU architectures. PLoS ONE 8: e83242.
  45. 45. Drake JW, Charlesworth B, Charlesworth D, Crow JF (1998) Rates of spontaneous mutation. Genetics 148: 1667–1686.
  46. 46. Needleman SB, Wunsch CD (1970) A general method applicable to the search for similarities in the amino acid sequence of two proteins. J Mol Biol 48: 443–453.
  47. 47. Pinheiro J, Bates D, DeRoy S, Sarkar D (R Core Development Team) (2014) Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. Version 3.1–117. Updated 31 March 2014.
  48. 48. Nakagawa S, Schielzeth H (2013) A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods Ecol Evol 4: 133–142.
  49. 49. Oksanen J, Blanchet FG, Kindt R, Legendre P, Minchin P, et al. (2013) vegan: Community ecology package. Version 2.0–10. Updated 12 December 2013.
  50. 50. Fox J, Weisberg S, Friendly M, Hong J, Andersen R, et al. (2013) R package effects: Effects displays for linear, generalized linear, multinomial-logit, proportional-odds logit models and mixed-effects models. Version 2.3–0. Updated 7 November 2013.
  51. 51. Bartón K (2013) R package MuMIn: Multi-modal inference. Version 1.9.13. Updated 29 October 2013.
  52. 52. Potts R (1996) Evolution and climate variability. Science 273: 922–923.