Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Captive Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) Spontaneously Using Water Flow to Manipulate Objects

Abstract

Several terrestrial animals and delphinids manipulate objects in a tactile manner, using parts of their bodies, such as their mouths or hands. In this paper, we report that bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) manipulate objects not by direct bodily contact, but by spontaneous water flow. Three of four dolphins at Suma Aqualife Park performed object manipulation with food. The typical sequence of object manipulation consisted of a three step procedure. First, the dolphins released the object from the sides of their mouths while assuming a head-down posture near the floor. They then manipulated the object around their mouths and caught it. Finally, they ceased to engage in their head-down posture and started to swim. When the dolphins moved the object, they used the water current in the pool or moved their head. These results showed that dolphins manipulate objects using movements that do not directly involve contact between a body part and the object. In the event the dolphins dropped the object on the floor, they lifted it by making water flow in one of three methods: opening and closing their mouths repeatedly, moving their heads lengthwise, or making circular head motions. This result suggests that bottlenose dolphins spontaneously change their environment to manipulate objects. The reason why aquatic animals like dolphins do object manipulation by changing their environment but terrestrial animals do not may be that the viscosity of the aquatic environment is much higher than it is in terrestrial environments. This is the first report thus far of any non-human mammal engaging in object manipulation using several methods to change their environment.

Introduction

Object manipulation in animals is defined as any physical touching with the object except for incidental touching [1]. Various types of object manipulation in animals have been reported such as rubbing, dropping, pulling, throwing, and stacking [1], [2]. The ability to manipulate objects is believed to have provided a base for tool use in animals [3].

Various object manipulations using the mouth or hands are present in terrestrial animals, especially in primates. For example, chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) walk backwards while pulling many dry leaves along the ground with both hands [4]. Bonobos (Pan paniscus) rub a substrate or their body with an object in their hands, or hold it in one hand and put it in their mouths [5]. Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) repeatedly pick up stones and drop them, or carry them by hand from one place to another [6]. Domestic cats (Felis silvestris catus) hit objects with their front paws, or hold and lightly bite them in their mouths [7]. Neotropic cormorants (Phalacrocorax brasilianus) hold sticks or fish in their bills and plunge them into the water, or toss fish into the air using quick, upward head movements [8].

Delphinids also show object manipulation, both in the wild and in captivity. They use body parts, such as their mouths or fins, and several detailed studies have addressed these behaviors. For example, bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) use their rostrums to toss food [9], their heads to balance kelp, and their tails to flip food to their dorsal fins [10]. Rough-toothed dolphins (Steno bredanensis) hold a piece of plastic in their mouth, drop it, and receive it with their pectoral fins [11]. Dusky dolphins (Lagenorhynchus obscurus) carry kelp using their rostrums, melons, flippers, and dorsal fins [12].

In most previous studies of terrestrial animals and delphinids, animals manipulate the object using their body parts. In contrast, we here report on captive bottlenose dolphins spontaneously manipulating objects using water flow made by their movements. Object manipulation including tool use has received attention from a viewpoint of cognition, especially understanding of physical phenomena in animals [2], [13]. Use of water flow without distinct adaptive value (like feeding) reported here may relate an understanding of the physical phenomena.

Materials and Method

Ethics statement

This study complied with the “WAZA Ethical Guidelines for the Conduct of Research on Animals by Zoo and Aquariums.” Research permission for this study was granted by Kobe City Suma Aqualife Park, Japan. This study was observational and did not involve the handling of animals. Our observation did not affect the dolphins' welfare.

Subjects and facility

Four captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in Kobe City Suma Aqualife Park in Hyogo, Japan, were the subjects of our study. They comprised one juvenile male, “Smile”, and three adult females, “Coo”, “F1”, and “Mammy” (Table 1). Smile was born in Suma Aqualife Park, but none of the three females was his mother. All dolphins lived in the performance pool (20 m major axis, 13 m minor axis, 3.5 m deep).

thumbnail
Table 1. Sex, age, and year that dolphins were born or year of arrival.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0107796.t001

Data collection

Behavioral data were collected for nine days in total in November 2005. Visual observations were made between 9:00 and 17:00 from the underwater window (1 m×1.5 m, 0.95 m from the water surface). All dolphins had six 30-min feeding times per day, with the exception of seven times on Sunday. We did not observe them during feeding time.

From preliminary observations, we found the typical sequence of object manipulations with food. First, dolphins approached the floor and assumed a head-down posture, and then released the food from their mouths. They then moved the food around their mouths and caught it again, or they caught it without moving it. Dolphins sometimes dropped their food on the floor, failing to catch it, and then picked it up again. Finally, dolphins ceased assuming the head-down posture, and began to swim.

Because dolphins repeatedly released their food and moved it around their mouths, we defined the period of object manipulation as between when dolphins assumed a head-down posture and when they began to swim once more. To investigate the pattern of object manipulation, we recorded the object types (fish or squid) used in object manipulation, places where dolphins first released the object from their mouths, and places where they moved the object after releasing it. To investigate how dolphins manipulated the object, we recorded the method they moved it and how they picked up the object once it had been dropped.

Statistical analysis

To investigate whether the pattern and manner of object manipulation differed between individuals, we used Fisher's exact test with Bonferroni correction (0.05/3 = 0.02) for two analyses as follows: (1) the ratio of times the object was placed at each location was compared between individuals, and (2) a comparison of the ratio of times dolphins used each different method to pick up the object. To test whether object types affected the pattern and manner of object manipulation, we conducted the following analyses using Fisher's exact test for each individual: (a) the ratio of bouts using each object type (fish and squid), (b) the ratio with which dolphins placed objects at each location, and (c) the ratios of each method used to pick up each object type. R 2.14.0 was used for statistical analysis.

Results

The number and duration of object manipulation

There were 407 bouts of object manipulation used for behavior analyses (245 for Coo, 132 for Mammy, 30 for Smile). F1 did not perform any object manipulation with food throughout the study period. The average duration of a bout of manipulation was 41.0 s (SD = 14.6) for Coo, 44.5 s (SD = 20.2) for Mammy and 28.1 s (SD  = 13.6) for Smile.

Food type using object manipulation

Coo and Mammy manipulated both fish and squid, but Smile manipulated only fish. The number of times that Coo manipulated the squid was significantly greater than the number of times she manipulated the fish (Fish, 22 times; Squid, 223 times; Fisher's exact test, d.f. = 1, P<0.001), whereas the number of times that Mammy manipulated the fish was significantly greater than the number of times she manipulated the squid (Fish, 90 times; Squid, 42 times; Fisher's exact test, d.f. = 1, P<0.001).

Object release location and placement area

Dolphins were able to release the object from a position that could be classified both as the front and as the side of their rostrums. All dolphins performing object manipulation always released the object from the sides of their mouths. The three places where dolphins placed the food after releasing it from the sides of their mouths were observed as follows: (a) dolphins placed the object on the sides of their mouths (they did not move the object), (b) dolphins moved and placed the object above their upper jaws, and (c) dolphins moved and placed the object below their lower jaws. The ratio at which the object was placed at each location did not differ between individuals (Fisher's exact test with Bonferroni correction, d.f. = 2, Coo vs. Smile: P = 0.20, Coo vs. Mammy: P = 0.29, Smile vs. Mammy: P = 0.48, Fig. 1). For Coo and Mammy, who used both fish and squid for object manipulation, object types did not affect the ratio with which they placed the object at each location (Fisher's exact test, d.f. = 2, Coo: P = 0.25, Mammy: P = 0.15).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Object placement at each location.

The ratio of object placement at each location for the total observed bouts for each individual. The legend indicates the location at which the object was placed. Placement to the side of the mouth indicates no object manipulation and placement below the lower jaw and above the upper jaw indicate object manipulation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0107796.g001

Object manipulation after release

We observed two methods that the dolphins moved the object from the side of their mouths to above the upper jaw or below the lower jaw, including: (a) dolphins did not show physical movement, but the object was slowly moved by the water current in the pool, and (b) dolphins moved their head, then the object quickly moved around the dolphin's mouth. We observed that all dolphins used both methods, but accurate frequency measurements were not recorded.

Object retrieval after is drop

Dolphins showed two methods of picking up the object that they had dropped on the floor: (a) dolphins directly bit the object on the floor, and (b) dolphins moved their bodies near to the object, then lifted the object from the floor. When the dolphins did not move their bodies, the object remained still on the floor (Video S1), which indicates that dolphins' movements lifted the object using water flow. All dolphins employed both methods. The ratio of use of each pick up method was not significantly different between Coo and Mammy or between Mammy and Smile (Fisher's exact test with Bonferroni correction, d.f. = 1, Coo vs. Mammy: P = 0.02, Mammy vs. Smile: P = 0.21, Fig. 2A). However, the ratio of water flow use for Coo was higher than it was for Smile (Fisher's exact test with Bonferroni correction, d.f. = 1, P = 0.001, Fig. 2A). For Coo and Mammy, who used two object types, the ratio of each method's use did not differ between object types (Fisher's exact test, d.f. = 1, Coo: P = 0.10, Mammy: P = 0.48).

thumbnail
Figure 2. Object pick-up method.

(A) The ratio of biting or making the water flow when picking up the object in relation to the total number of times it was dropped. (B) The ratio of each method of creating water flow out of the total number of times the water flow was created for each individual. The legend indicate the method used to make the water flow.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0107796.g002

Three different method of making water flow were observed: (a) dolphins opened and closed their mouths repeatedly towards the object, (b) dolphins moved their heads lengthwise towards the object, and (c) dolphins made circular motions with their heads towards the object. Coo and Mammy exhibited all three methods, but Smile did not perform circular head movements. There were no differences in the ratio of each method's use between Coo and Mammy or between Mammy and Smile (Fisher's exact test with Bonferroni correction, d.f. = 2, Coo vs. Mammy: P = 0.14, Mammy vs. Smile: P = 0.33, Fig. 2B), although a significant difference was noted between Coo and Smile (Fisher's exact test with Bonferroni correction, d.f. = 2, P = 0.002, Fig. 2B). For individuals who used two object types, object type did not affect the method use ratio (Fisher's exact test, d.f. = 1, Coo: P = 0.33, Mammy: P = 0.06).

Discussion

We describe our observations of bottlenose dolphins spontaneously changing their environment and employing water flow to manipulate objects. Manipulating objects using the mouth is common in terrestrial animals and delphinids (e.g., primates [1], New Caledonian crows; Corvus moneduloides [14], and delphinids [15]). However, object manipulation using physical power (such as gravity, buoyancy, or flow) has been reported in few studies. Common ravens (Corvus corax) have been found to release objects in the air, allowing gravity to lead them to fall, and catching them before they touch the ground [16]. Chimpanzees (Pongo abelii) have been found to use buoyancy in object manipulation [1]. They floating objects on water and poking them with their fingers. Bottlenose dolphins have been showed to use flow during manipulating an object. They released a feather into a water jet and retrieved it after water flow had pushed the feather away [17].

Our observations, on the other hand, demonstrate that dolphins actively and spontaneously change their environment, making water flow to engage in object manipulation. Few studies have shown spontaneous aquatic animal-instigated environment change for object manipulation. Some species, such as south american fresh water stingrays (Potamotrygon castexi) [18], arabesque greenlings (Pleurogrammus azonus) [19], and killer whales (Orcinus orca) [20] have demonstrated spontaneous animal-instigated environmental change. In all of these studies, however, the animals used just one stereotyped method of creating water flow to manipulate their environment. In contrast, our dolphins made water flow using several flexible methods. The reason that only aquatic animals show spontaneous change of the environment may be that the viscosity of the aquatic environment is much higher than that of the terrestrial environment. The power transmitted by the animal's movement is lager when the viscosity is higher than when it is lower. Therefore, aquatic animals may change their environments more easily than terrestrial animals.

Cetaceans use water to manipulate objects as well, especially during feeding. For example, humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) use bubbles during feeding, which is called bubble-net feeding [21]. This feeding method differs across sea areas and individuals [21], [22]. In this case, they control living prey's behavior by encircling the prey with a bubble net, which is automatically created by breathing out. In contrast, our study on dolphins showed two manipulation behaviors to move inanimate objects. First, a water flow was created by an active dolphin's movement and then this water flow moved the object. Here, the reported behavior that inanimate objects are moved by water flow differed from bubble-net feeding that behavior of living prey is controlled.

Another form of using water to manipulate objects during foraging is suction feeding. Odontocetes draw their prey by increasing the volume of their oral cavity [23]. Suction force is higher in odontocetes that can shorten and widen their rostrum and jaws [24]; thus, suction feeding ability is dependent on morphological features. Suction feeding in odontocetes was suggested to evolve as the way that pray are grasped by long jaw and are transported the oral cavity by suction, then the way that prey are caught by suction [25]. In terms of using water current, suction feeding behavior is similar to the water flow object manipulation made by the dolphins' movements. However, the difference is that suction feeding evolve as way to eat prey [25]. In contrast, object manipulation of our study did not feeding behavior and our results suggest that dolphins have learned to pick up the object using water flow (see below). This is the first report of any mammal, except humans, manipulating objects using several methods of changing their environment spontaneously.

The three dolphins performed similar types of object manipulations with their food. The sequence of object manipulation was the same for all dolphins. Dolphins showed a head-down posture during object manipulation. They always released the object from the side of the mouth and manipulated it around the mouth. The methods of manipulation were similar for all individuals: indeed, all dolphins used two techniques when moving the object, and two of the three techniques to make the water flow when picking up the object. The ratio of placements at each location out of the total number did not differ between individuals. There were no significant differences in the ratio of using water flow to pick up the object and the ratio of methods used to make water flow between individuals, except for one pair. The object type did not affect the locations where the dolphins placed the object after releasing it, the method of picking up the object up, or the method of making the water flow to pick up the object.

Matsusaka et al. [26] suggested that social learning played an important role in the spread of behavior within the group, and bottlenose dolphins were previously suggested to have some capacity for social learning [27]. In this study group, the result that object manipulation methods are similar in three dolphins may be possible that some dolphins initiated object manipulation with food and this behavior spread to other dolphins. One dolphin that did not manipulate the object, F1, may be an example of this, but further research regarding learning is needed to investigate the behavior of individual dolphins that do not manipulate.

Our study had several limitations. First, since we observed only one group, we could not demonstrate whether bottlenose dolphins generally produce water flow to manipulate objects, and the types of situations that elicited such behavior. To do so, further research should be conducted on additional dolphin groups, including a wild population, and examine the circumstances surrounding water flow use. Second, we did not observe the process of acquiring the object manipulation technique using water flow. Therefore, we cannot ascertain whether dolphins had understood that their movements made the water flow, or had learned to associate their behavior (i.e., moving the head) with the result (i.e., moving the object). Further study is therefore needed to assess whether bottlenose dolphins understand the physical properties of object manipulation.

Supporting Information

Video S1.

Object manipulation by bottlenose dolphins in Suma Aqualife Park. We recorded video from 12–13 April, 2014. Bottlenose dolphins manipulated the object around their mouth. The object was afloat when dolphins moved near, and this was not due to the general water current in the pool.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0107796.s002

(MOV)

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the member of Suma Aqualife Park, especially Masaharu Hiwada and Naoki Kamezaki for their help in data collection. We would like to thank Shigenori Kondo, Yoshinari Ishida, Ikuko Kanda, and Mai Sakai for their helpful comments on this study. Masao Amano and Yoshihisa Mori supervised the first author (CY). We would like to thank the editor and anonymous reviewer for the constructive suggestions regarding our manuscript.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: CY TM. Performed the experiments: CY. Analyzed the data: CY TM. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: MT KF TM. Wrote the paper: CY MT.

References

  1. 1. Torigoe T (1985) Comparison of object manipulation among 74 species of non-human primates. Primates 26: 182–194.
  2. 2. Hayashi M (2007) Stacking of blocks by chimpanzees: developmental processes and physical understanding. Anim Cogn 10: 89–103.
  3. 3. Hayashi M (2007) A new notation system of object manipulation in the nesting-cup task for chimpanzees and humans. Cortex 43: 308–318.
  4. 4. Nishida T, Wallauer W (2003) Leaf-pile pulling: an unusual play pattern in wild chimpanzees. Am J Primatol 60: 167–173.
  5. 5. Takeshita H, Walraven V (1996) A comparative study of the variety and complexity of object manipulation in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus). Primates 37: 423–441.
  6. 6. Nahallage CAD, Huffman MA (2007) Age-specific functions of stone handling, a solitary-object play behavior, in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Am J Primatol 69: 267–281.
  7. 7. Hall SL, Bradshaw JWS (1998) The influence of hunger on object play by adult domestic cats. Appl Anim Behav Sci 58: 143–150.
  8. 8. Sazima I (2008) Playful birds: cormorants and herons play with objects and practice their skills. Biota Neotrop 8: 259–264.
  9. 9. Bel'kovich VM, Lvanova EE, Kozarovitasky LB, Novikova EV, Kharitonov SP (1991) Dolphin play behavior in the open sea. In: Pryor K, Norris KS, editors. Dolphin societies: Discoveries and puzzles. California: University of California Press. pp. 67–77.
  10. 10. Würsig B, Würsig M (1979) Behavior and ecology of the bottlenose dolphin, Tursiops truncatus, in the south atlantic. Fish Bull 77: 399–412.
  11. 11. Kuczaj SA, Yeater DB (2007) Observations of rough-toothed dolphins (Steno bredanensis) off the coast of Utila, Honduras. J Mar Biol Assoc UK 87: 141–148.
  12. 12. Würsig B (2009) Playful behavior. In: Perrin W, Wursing B, Thewissen JGM, editors. Encyclopedia of marine mammals, second edition. Amsterdam: Academic Press. pp. 885–888.
  13. 13. Mendes N, Hanus D, Call J (2007) Raising the level: orangutans use water as a tool. Biol Lett 3: 453–455.
  14. 14. Chappell J, Kacelnik A (2002) Tool selectivity in a non-primate, the New Caledonian crow (Corvus moneduloides). Anim Cogn 5: 71–78.
  15. 15. Paulos RD, Trone M, Kuzaj SA (2010) Play in wild and captive cetaceans. Int J Comp Psychol 23: 701–722.
  16. 16. Heinrich B, Smolker R (1998) Play in common ravens (Corvus corax). In: Bekoff M, Byers JA, editors. Animal play: evolutionary, comparative, and ecological perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 27–44.
  17. 17. Tavolga MC (1966) Behavior of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus): Social interactions in a captive colony. In: Norris KS, editor. Whales, dolphins, and porpoises. Berkeley Los Angeles: University of California Press. pp. 718–730.
  18. 18. Kuba MJ, Byrne RA, Burghardt GM (2010) A new method for studying problem solving and tool use in stingrays (Potamotrygon castexi). Anim Cogn 13: 507–513.
  19. 19. Kitagawa T, Nakagawa T, Kimura R, Niino H, Kimura S (2011) Vortex flow produced by schooling behavior of arabesque greenling Pleurogrammus azonus. Fish Sci 77: 217–222.
  20. 20. Pitman RL, Durban JW (2012) Cooperative hunting behavior, prey selectivity and prey handling by pack ice killer whales (Orcinus orca), type B, in Antarctic Peninsula waters. Mar Mammal Sci 28: 16–36.
  21. 21. Clapham P (2000) The humpback whale: seasonal feeding and breeding in baleen whale. In: Mann J, Connor RC, Tyack PL, Whitehead H, editors. Cetacean societies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. pp. 173–196.
  22. 22. Friedlaender A, Weinrich M, Bocconcelli A, Cholewiak D, Thompson M, et al. (2011) Underwater components of humpback whale bubble-net feeding behaviour. Behaviour 148: 575–602.
  23. 23. Marshall CD (2009) Feeding morphology. In: Perrin W, Wursing B, Thewissen J, editors. Encyclopedia of marine mammals, second edition. Amsterdam: Academic Press. pp. 406–414.
  24. 24. Werth AJ (2006) Odontocete suction feeding: experimental analysis of water flow and fead shape. J Morphol 1428: 1415–1428.
  25. 25. Werth AJ (2006) Mandibular and dental variation and the evolution of suction feeding in odontoceti. J Mammal 87: 579–588.
  26. 26. Matsusaka T, Nishie H, Shimada M, Kutsukake N, Zamma K, et al. (2006) Tool-use for drinking water by immature chimpanzees of Mahale: prevalence of an unessential behavior. Primates 47: 113–122.
  27. 27. Krützen M, Mann J, Heithaus MR, Connor RC, Bejder L, et al. (2005) Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins. PNAS 102: 8939–8943.