Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Testing the Effect of Medical Positive Reinforcement Training on Salivary Cortisol Levels in Bonobos and Orangutans

  • Verena Behringer ,

    verena_behringer@eva.mpg.de

    Affiliations Department of Primatology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany, Centre for Research and Conservation, Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium

  • Jeroen M. G. Stevens,

    Affiliation Centre for Research and Conservation, Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium

  • Gottfried Hohmann,

    Affiliation Department of Primatology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany

  • Erich Möstl,

    Affiliation Department of Biomedical Sciences/Biochemistry, University of Veterinary Medicine, Vienna, Austria

  • Dieter Selzer,

    Affiliation Working Group for Wildlife Biology, Justus-Liebig-University Gießen, Gießen, Germany

  • Tobias Deschner

    Affiliation Department of Primatology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany

Abstract

The management of captive animals has been improved by the establishment of positive reinforcement training as a tool to facilitate interactions between caretakers and animals. In great apes, positive reinforcement training has also been used to train individuals to participate in simple medical procedures to monitor physical health. One aim of positive reinforcement training is to establish a relaxed atmosphere for situations that, without training, might be very stressful. This is especially true for simple medical procedures that can require animals to engage in behaviours that are unusual or use unfamiliar medical devices that can be upsetting. Therefore, one cannot exclude the possibility that the training itself is a source of stress. In this study, we explored the effects of medical positive reinforcement training on salivary cortisol in two groups of captive ape species, orangutans and bonobos, which were familiar to this procedure. Furthermore, we successfully biologically validated the salivary cortisol assay, which had already been validated for bonobos, for orangutans. For the biological validation, we found that cortisol levels in orangutan saliva collected during baseline conditions were lower than in samples collected during three periods that were potentially stressful for the animals. However, we did not find significant changes in salivary cortisol during medical positive reinforcement training for either bonobos or orangutans. Therefore, for bonobos and orangutans with previous exposure to medical PRT, the procedure is not stressful. Thus, medical PRT provides a helpful tool for the captive management of the two species.

Introduction

Over the past few decades, the management of captive animals has been improved in a variety of ways. One particularly effective element was the establishment of specific enrichment programs that contributed to the improvement of the well-being of captive individuals [1]. Another one concerns the establishment of positive reinforcement training (PRT) as a tool to facilitate interactions between caretakers and animals during examinations [1]. PRT has long been part of care and management in marine mammals such as dolphins and sea lions [2], [3] and has since then been applied more and more to the management of terrestrial animal species in zoos and laboratories [3].

By definition, PRT relies on the cooperation of the animal. It is a form of operant conditioning in which the animal gets a reward (e.g., food) from the trainer after showing the desired behavior (e.g., [4][6]. That is why PRT is also named reward training [2]. Often a handheld clicker is used as a conditioned or secondary reinforcer connecting the acoustic signal with the positive, or primary, reinforcer (e.g., food) [7]. PRT with a clicker is commonly used in different animal species and has become a common training method in nonhuman primates [5], [8], [9]. This kind of training gives the animal the choice to cooperate or not [4], and the training of medical procedures is a helpful tool in monitoring physical health [10][12].

PRT has become a valuable tool for scientific, veterinary and husbandry procedures and it is recommended by a number of professional guidelines (e.g., [13] or the “Guide of the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals” mentioned by Perlman et al. [14]). It has been carried out in a variety of species including birds [2], big cats [10], and bears [4]. PRT has been particularly successful when applied in the management of captive non-human primates (prosimians [15], New World monkeys [8], [14], [16], Old World monkeys [5], [17], and great apes [18][20] including chimpanzees [21][23], gorillas [24][26], bonobos [27][29], and orangutans [30][32]; for a review see Prescott and Buchanan-Smith [33]).

Common husbandry procedures in great apes include, for example: presentation of parts of the body [1], [18], [27], hand injections or venepuncture [27], [34][36], collection of blood [35][37], collection of urine [1], [24], [36], [38] as well as nail trimming, taking of body temperatures or body weight, and monitoring of heart and respiratory function (reviewed in [12]). In great apes, training has also been used to manage behavior, for example to improve maternal skills in gorillas [25], [39], bonobos [28] and orangutans [40].

PRT techniques, used as a part of nonhuman primate behavioral management programs, can be beneficial for both trainers and animals. For example, PRT can prevent stress during veterinary procedures and/or increase cooperation in research procedures [1], [14], [34]. It is widely believed that the quality of these interactions during PRT positively affects the well-being of the animal ([41], reviewed in [42]) for example by giving the animals more control over their environment (e.g., [4], [5], [34]). Therefore, PRT is considered to be beneficial and to improve the lives of animals through increased welfare and reduced stress [6]. However, for PRT to be successful, a trustful, close and safe relationship between keepers and the animals has to be established [27].

One aim of conducting PRT is to establish a relaxed atmosphere that facilitates animal care that, without the training, might be stressful [34], [35]. At the same time, one cannot exclude the possibility that the training itself is a source of stress because, for example, training interferes with other ongoing activities; it exposes subjects to unusually close proximity with human care givers and – in group-living species – may induce competition for attention of the trainer or for access to the reward [43]. On the other hand, group training (in which animals are not separated for training) may have the advantage that individuals can learn from each other, thereby increasing the effectiveness of medical PRT. Furthermore, separating animals for PRT can be stressful, especially for individuals of gregarious species [3], [44]. In this study, stress was defined as the response of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis to a stressor, resulting in an increase in cortisol in saliva (for a review see [45]).

To investigate if medical PRT could reduce stress hormone levels in comparison to otherwise necessary invasive examinations, previous studies have used blood or serum to measure levels of corticosteroids and found that PRT reduced stress during husbandry procedures like caging [46], venepuncture [47], hand injections [34] or blood sampling [9]. While these studies showed that PRT can help to reduce stress in husbandry procedures, the effects of PRT itself on stress in the animals in the training process has so far received little attention. Moreover, using blood to evaluate stress can be problematic, because the collection of these samples itself could result in cortisol increase [48][50]. Furthermore, blood cannot always be obtained easily even in a captive setting. While long-term stress responses can be measured non-invasively in primates from urine and fecal samples ([51], [52] but see [53] for potential pitfalls), saliva is more accurate to assess short-term stress response, as salivary cortisol levels reflect changes in serum cortisol levels with a delay of only five minutes [54]. Furthermore, saliva can be obtained relatively easily from apes in zoo settings using PRT, and has been used in bonobos [55][57], orangutans [58], gorillas [26] and chimpanzees [59]. PRT in lab-housed baboons did not lead to an increase in salivary cortisol levels [60]. However, the task the animals were trained for in this study was simple and included neither extensive trainer –animal interactions nor direct physical contact. Furthermore, the PRT was conducted with separated animals. Therefore it remains to be tested whether PRT with tasks including medical procedures that necessitate touching the animals, such as the measurement of body temperature from the ear or monitoring of heart and respiratory functions, would be perceived as stressful by the subjects.

In this study we assessed the effects of medical PRT on salivary cortisol in group training sessions in two species of captive apes: orangutan and bonobo. All animals were familiar and well experienced with the medical PRT. Therefore, the study design investigated the extent to which well-trained animals experience medical PRT to be stressful. In bonobos, salivary cortisol response has already been biologically validated and was shown to increase during periods of stress [55], [56]. For orangutans, a study had already measured salivary cortisol [58], but this study showed results of a single individual. Therefore, we first did a biological validation for salivary cortisol by comparing cortisol levels in saliva collected during baseline conditions, with samples collected during three periods that were potentially stressful for the animals [61]; in the next step we used the biologically validated salivary cortisol measurements to assess the influence of medical PRT on stress hormone levels, both in orangutans and bonobos, by comparing cortisol levels in samples collected during medical PRT with baseline levels. Furthermore, in bonobos, we compared training with half of the group with training with the whole group to investigate if the number of bonobos participating simultaneously in the PRT influenced the stress response.

Methods

Ethics Statement

Saliva collection is a non-invasive method. It was carried out in accordance with NIH published standards and the protocol of sample collection was approved by authorities of Frankfurt Zoo, Germany (Dr. Thomas Wilms). Study was approved by committee of Frankfurt Zoo (Dr. Rüdiger Dmoch and Dr. C. R. Schmidt 30th May 2006).

Saliva samples were collected from a group of seven Sumatran orangutans and a group of ten bonobos in Frankfurt Zoo (table 1). Both species lived in social groups at all times; food was offered at least three times a day and consisted mainly of a mixture of fruits and vegetables. Animals had ad libitum access to fresh water. All subjects had access to indoor and outdoor enclosures. All apes had nearly continuous access to the indoor enclosures; access was restricted only during periods of cleaning. The enclosures contained natural substrates or concrete, climbing structures, and many manipulable objects (e.g., fire hose, ropes, wood-wool).

thumbnail
Table 1. Species, name of the individual, sex, age, and number (N) of saliva samples collected pre, during and post medical positive reinforcement training for salivary cortisol measurements.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0108664.t001

Individuals of both groups had already participated previously in medical PRT sessions. The bonobo group was familiar with medical PRT since 1999 and PRT of the orangutans started in 2000. Training for these apes for saliva collection procedure is described in detail in Behringer et al. [55][57]. Apes were trained by PRT to chew on cotton rolls, and to return the chewed cotton for a reward.

Every medical PRT session included body examination, oral inspection, nail trim, measurement of body temperature from the ear, and monitoring of heart and respiratory function for every individual per session. No new tasks were introduced during the session in which saliva samples were collected. Training was carried out by the same keeper using preferred fruits as a primary reinforcement and a hand-held clicker as secondary reinforcement. Each orangutan and bonobo participated in every session, and individuals were trained in a random order. Generally, medical PRT could be conducted at any time of the day. However, samples for this study were taken at the same time of day to minimize the influence of diurnal patterns of salivary cortisol on the results. Since medical PRT in combination with sample collection was always interspersed between routine medical PRT, the anticipation of the training session by the apes was avoided. To have comparable situations for this study design, each training session lasted exactly 20 min. Although participation in the training sessions was voluntary and apes were not forced to approach the medical PRT place or to cooperate with the trainer, all individuals joined all training sessions. Training sessions started with the collection of a saliva sample. For the orangutans, in the first two training sessions, samples were only collected at the beginning and at the end. In the next five sessions, saliva samples were taken at the start (pre), after 10 min. (during) and again after 20 min. (post). The first sample was used as a baseline sample for each individual and served as a control for the samples taken after 10 and 20 min. [54]. After 10 min. of training, every individual had been involved at least once in the training. Although it was not possible to count the time each individual spent in each training session, the trainer ensured that every individual was trained in each procedure. For bonobos, in the first and third sessions, all ten individuals were trained together in one group. In the second and fourth sessions, the group was split up to explore whether group size has an effect on salivary cortisol concentration. The separation of the bonobos into two groups occurred during the morning hours to avoid the possible influence of the group split on salivary cortisol levels. Individuals could freely choose between two enclosures and thereby group composition was decided upon by the animals themselves. In all four sessions, samples were collected before, during and after medical PRT. In all sessions, 92 saliva samples were collected for bonobos.

Orangutan saliva sampling protocol

For the orangutan group, two samples sets were collected. Sample set 1: To investigate the validity of salivary cortisol as a marker for stress response, we compared samples collected during potentially stressful situations (N = 50) with samples collected under baseline conditions (N = 68). Sample set 2: Multiple saliva samples were collected from the same individual during seven different training sessions (for details see table 1 and 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Number (N) of orangutans and saliva samples for each condition.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0108664.t002

1. Baseline values and stress conditions.

For baseline conditions, 68 salivary cortisol samples from the orangutan group were randomly chosen from a large sample set collected between December 2006 and December 2008. On these days, neither stressful events occurred nor was medical PRT performed. Samples were collected either during normal saliva collection at 13:00 (38 samples) or at 16:00 (30 samples). To investigate cortisol secretion under stress conditions in orangutans we chose three potential stress events. During these events, stress indicating behavior patterns such as yawning and self-scratching increased in frequency [61]. A) The capture of a three-year-old female orangutan on 22nd December 2006. The young female was separated from the group at 12:45 and captured for medical reasons at 13:10. After 20 min. she was released back into the group. Samples were collected from all group members including the caught female, first before the stress event at 11:00, then during the event at 13:00 and finally directly after the event at 14:00. B) Orangutans were transferred into a new great ape house on 13th May 2008. Orangutans were trained before to enter a transport cage and were therefore not anesthetized. Transfer into the sleeping boxes of the new ape house started at 8:30 and at 13:00 the new inside orangutan enclosure was opened for the whole group. The 21 saliva samples were collected whenever possible between 8:30 and 17:30. C) The third sample set was collected on the day following the transfer (first day in the new enclosure) at 13:00 and 16:00.

2. Training conditions.

To test the short term effect of medical PRT on salivary cortisol in the orangutan group, we collected saliva samples of all orangutans before and during seven routine training sessions performed with the whole group. All training sessions were carried out between 19th February and 24th April 2008 and started at 14:00±10 min. This time window for medical PRT was chosen for two reasons: First, to avoid interference with feeding times and therefore with food, which may affect measurements of enzymes and hormones in saliva [62] but see also [63]. Second, midday was chosen for sample collection to minimize confounding effects of diurnal variation of salivary cortisol [58], [64].

Bonobo saliva sampling protocol

In a previous study we found that in response to socially stressful events such as transfer to a new facility or introduction of new group members, salivary cortisol of captive bonobos increased compared to baseline cortisol levels [56]. In the present study we collected saliva samples before, during and after medical PRT to compare the effect of the training with baseline before training. Sample material was collected during four routinely performed training sessions conducted between 26th March and 10th April, 2008 around 14:00 (same time and season as described for the orangutans).

Sample preparation and cortisol assay

Samples were frozen immediately after collection at −20°C and shipped to the Department of Biomedical Sciences, University of Veterinary Medicine, Vienna, Austria. The frozen cotton rolls containing saliva were thawed and centrifuged (1500 g, 10 min.). While the use of cotton rolls produced artificial high testosterone values their use is unproblematic for cortisol measurements [59], [65]. Three microliters of the resulting saliva plus 47 µl of assay buffer were used for the cortisol assay. Cortisol was measured with an enzyme immunoassay (EIA) previously described by Palme and Möstl [66]. Validation of the EIA and measurement procedure are described in Behringer et al. [56], [61]. Intra- and inter-assay coefficients of variation of high and low value quality controls were 8.6% and 14.5% (N = 37) and 9.6% and 13.8% (N = 93), respectively. Samples were re-measured if bindings were outside of a 30–70% range (linear range of the assay) or if divergence of concentration duplicates was greater than 10%.

Statistical analysis

We conducted three General Linear Mixed Models (GLMM) [67] using the function ‘lmer’ of the R-package lme4 [68], [69]. The first GLMM was run to investigate the effects of potentially stressful situations on salivary cortisol levels in the orangutan group. The second GLMM was used to determine the influence of medical PRT on salivary cortisol levels in orangutans, and the third GLMM to test the effects of medical PRT on salivary cortisol levels in the bonobo group. In all three models approximate normality and homogeneity of residuals was assessed by visual inspection of residuals plotted against fitted values and a qq-plot. All model assumptions were met. To achieve this, the response variable ‘cortisol’ was log-transformed in all three cases. To check for absence of collinearity, we examined Variance Inflation Factors (VIF [70]) using the function vif of the R-package car [71] applied to a standard linear model excluding random effects. These indicated that collinearity was not an obvious issue (maximum VIF; model 1: 1.13; model 2 (orangutan): 1.05; model 3 (bonobo): 1.19). Also, in all three models, the time of sample collection was z-transformed to a mean of zero and a standard deviation of one to achieve comparable estimates [72].

In the first GLMM we tested the influence of potentially stressful situations on salivary cortisol in orangutans. Four different stress events were included as a fixed effect, categorical predictor (with the levels: baseline conditions, catch, transfer, day after transfer). We also included sex, age, and time of sampling as predictors with fixed effects in the model and animal was included as a random intercept term. Sex and individual were included in the models because stress hormone levels may vary with sex and age [73]. To establish the significance of the fixed effects as a whole, we compared the full model with a null model excluding all fixed effects but retaining the random effects using a likelihood ratio test [74]; R function ‘ANOVA’). In order to achieve reliable P-values for the individual effects we used Markov Chain Monte Carlo sampling (MCMC) [67].

In the second and third model, the influence of medical PRT on the response salivary cortisol levels was investigated for the two data sets of orangutans and bonobos, respectively. Animal and session ID were included as random intercept terms in both models. The time of sample collection and age (z-transformed) were included as fixed effects, categorical predictor (with the levels pre, during and post medical PRT) as well as session number and sex. Furthermore, we also included random slopes of time of sample collection within animal into the model [75]. The random slope of time of sample collection within animal was not significant in orangutans (Chisq = 0, df = 3, P = 1) and bonobos (Chisq = 0, df = 2, P = 1), indicating that the effect of time of sample collection was not significant different across animals. Also we did a full null model comparison for both models, as described above for model 1. Tests of significance were calculated using MCMC as described above. To establish significance for the overall effect of treatment (pre, during, or post medical PRT) we used the functions ‘pvals.fnc’ and ‘aovlmer.fnc’, as provided by the R package languageR [76]. Significance for all tests was set at the 0.05 level.

Results

To investigate the possible influence of the fixed effect in the three different models on salivary cortisol, we compared the full model with a null model in both species. These comparisons revealed significant effects for each species and models (model 1: P<0.001; model 2 (orangutan): P = 0.01983; model 3 (bonobo): P = 0.005).

Orangutans

Comparison of orangutan salivary cortisol levels in stressed and baseline conditions.

We compared cortisol levels in orangutan saliva under baseline conditions (median 2.92 ng/ml, SD. 1.34 ng/ml) and potentially stressful situations (median 6.59 ng/ml, SD. 10.0 ng/ml). When subjects were exposed to potentially stressful events (inducing all three tested stress events), salivary cortisol levels were elevated compared to the baseline condition (overall test of the effect of the factor: PMCMC<0.001, Figure 1). To investigate whether all three events affect salivary cortisol levels, we conducted a post-hoc comparison. For each of the three potentially stressful events, salivary cortisol levels were significantly higher compared to the baseline condition (Table 3), with the effect being most pronounced on the day of the transfer (Table 3). Sex, age, and time of sample collection did not have a significant effect on salivary cortisol levels (Table 3).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Average salivary cortisol concentration in orangutans for baseline levels and for the three potentially stressful events (catch event, day of transfer, and new enclosure).

The boxes illustrate the 25th and 75th percentiles, bars indicate medians, and circles indicate outliers.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0108664.g001

thumbnail
Table 3. Results of the General Linear Mixed Models of the subset obtained from orangutans when exposed to stress and at baseline conditions with salivary cortisol as response variable (sampling time (z-transformed), age and sex were included as fixed effects and animal ID was included as random intercept term). Bold values indicate P<0.05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0108664.t003

Salivary cortisol levels during medical PRT in an orangutan and bonobo group.

During medical PRT, salivary cortisol levels showed no significant changes in orangutans or bonobos (overall test of the effect of the factor orangutans: PMCMC = 0.435; bonobo: PMCMC = 0.869; Table 4). Furthermore, salivary cortisol levels were independent of sex and age in both species (Table 4). However, in both species cortisol levels differed significantly between sessions (Table 4). Salivary cortisol levels during medical PRT with the whole group were not significant different from levels during training with half of the group (Figure 2).

thumbnail
Figure 2. Average salivary cortisol concentration of males and females of a bonobo group during four medical PRT sessions (two training sessions with the whole group and two with half of the group).

The boxes illustrate the 25th and 75th percentiles, bars indicate medians, and circles indicate outliers.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0108664.g002

thumbnail
Table 4. Results of the two General Linear Mixed Models of the two subsets for orangutans and bonobos for medical positive reinforcement training with salivary cortisol as response variable (sex, age, and session number are included as fixed effects.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0108664.t004

Discussion

Salivary cortisol levels remained unaffected during 20 min. of medical PRT in a group of bonobos and orangutans, respectively. This result was similar for all individuals independent of sex and age. Furthermore, in bonobos the number of individuals participating in a medical PRT session did not affect salivary cortisol levels. When captive orangutans were exposed to potentially stressful events, salivary cortisol increased in comparison to baseline concentrations. This is similar to what has been found in orangutans [58], bonobos [56] and chimpanzees [64] and suggests that salivary cortisol levels provides a biologically meaningful marker of physiological stress in hominoids.

In chimpanzees, Bloomsmith and colleagues showed that during PRT individuals varied in their willingness to participate in training sessions depending on their age and sex [77]. In our analyses we controlled for individual ID. Therefore, we can exclude that certain animals had a disproportionately high impact on the overall result.

Spatial proximity during training may affect stress hormone levels in wild and captive animals [78]. This question was explored by comparing multiple samples obtained at different times within each medical PRT session. If serum cortisol levels increased in the apes as a result of PRT, a corresponding increase in saliva samples would be expected in less than 5 min. [79][81]. Therefore, we expected that if there was an effect of medical PRT on salivary cortisol concentration it should be detectable in samples taken after 10 and 20 minutes from the start of the training sessions. However, neither in the samples from orangutans nor in those from bonobos did we find changes in salivary cortisol during the 20 min. medical PRT session. A similar result in salivary cortisol levels during medical PRT was found in baboons [60]. Taken together, these results suggest that the training of these primates to perform behaviours and interactions with humans did not lead to an increase in cortisol levels and, by inference, did not create a stressful situation. However, the absence of a stress response could be also due to the long-term exposure the apes had with the medical PRT. Nevertheless, the results show that in well-trained apes the medical PRT does not induce a stress response in form of an activation of the HPA axis. The effect of human/non-human primate interactions on the well-being of primates was less of a focus in previous studies [22] although the general assumption exists that such interactions have a positive effect on the well-being of the animals [41]. With our study we showed that cortisol excretion in well-trained bonobos and orangutans is not influenced by intensive human/non-human primate interaction during medical PRT. Moreover, by rewarding specific behaviours a closer contact between keepers and animals is established [33], [82]. Taken together, medical PRT in combination with close keeper-animal relationships significantly facilitate the management and handling of primates during complicated management and behaviour problems [42].

Interestingly, the two species differed significantly in salivary cortisol levels across PRT sessions. This result is in contrast to the baboon study by O'Brien et al. [60] which did not find changes in salivary cortisol levels across training sessions. Our findings indicate that the general day-to-day variation in salivary cortisol levels can vary remarkably and therefore it is of paramount importance that baseline samples for comparison for the group and the individual are collected closely in time to the experimental procedure.

In bonobos, we tested the effect of number of animals in a training session. Average salivary cortisol levels were not different between medical PRT with the whole or with only half of the group. Our results suggest that training in a trained group of bonobos does not affect salivary cortisol levels and therefore bonobos were not stressed by this medical PRT. This is important since in general the advantage of having more than one individual for medical PRT is that nonhuman primates are most of the time more relaxed when in a group and furthermore they can learn easier and faster by observing the other individuals [83].

Our results do not indicate that medical PRT in itself improves the welfare of the apes. However, the medical PRT does not increase salivary stress hormones and moreover, it can replace much more invasive procedures, such as anesthesia [1], [14], [34]. By establishing a trustful human-ape relationship (apes showing special parts of the body and tolerance to being touched), medical PRT also allows easier interventions such as wound treatment.

Conclusions

In orangutans, salivary cortisol increased in stressful situations. However, we did not find significant changes in salivary cortisol during medical PRT in either bonobos or in orangutans, which had years of experience with the training. We conclude that medical PRT is not stressful for bonobos and orangutans which are familiar with PRT, and therefore provides a helpful tool in zoo management of the two species. Furthermore, it seems that in gregarious species like bonobos, the number of animals per session does not influence the stress response. Since daily variations in salivary cortisol occur in both species, it is important that control samples are always taken close to the situation of interest.

Acknowledgments

Sincere thanks go to the director, curator, and staff of Frankfurt Zoo. Special thanks go to caregivers of the facility for training apes and collecting saliva samples. The statistical help by Roger Mundry is gratefully acknowledged, and Alexandra Kuchar for assisting with the cortisol assays. We are also grateful to Colleen Stephens for editing earlier drafts of the manuscript.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: VB EM DS. Performed the experiments: VB DS. Analyzed the data: VB EM TD GH. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: EM TD. Wrote the paper: VB JMGS GH EM DS TD.

References

  1. 1. Whittaker M, Laule GE, Perlman JE, Shapiro S, Keeling M (2001) A behavioral management approach to caring for great apes. The apes: challenges for the 21st century. Chicago, IL.: Brookfield Zoo. pp. 131–134.
  2. 2. Heidenreich B (2007) An introduction to positive reinforcement training and its benefits. J Exot Pet Med 16: 19–23
  3. 3. Laule G, Bloomsmith M, Schapiro S (2003) The use of positive reinforcement training techniques to enhance the care, management, and welfare of primates in the laboratory. J Appl Anim Welf Sci 6: 163–173
  4. 4. Bloomsmith MA, Jones ML, Snyder RJ, Singer RA, Gardner WA, et al. (2003) Positive reinforcement training to elicit voluntary movement of two giant pandas throughout their enclosure. Zoo Biol 22: 323–334
  5. 5. Coleman K, Pranger L, Maier A, Lambeth SP, Perlman JE, et al. (2008) Training rhesus macaques for venipuncture using positive reinforcement techniques: a comparison with chimpanzees. J Am Assoc Lab Anim Sci 47: 37.
  6. 6. Laule G (1993) Using training to enhance animal care and welfare. Anim Welf Inf Cent Newsl 4: 8–9.
  7. 7. Pryor K (1975) Lads before the wind: adventures in porpoise training. 1st ed. New York: Harper & Row. 278 p.
  8. 8. Gillis TE, Janes AC, Kaufman MJ (2012) Positive reinforcement training in squirrel monkeys using clicker training. Am J Primatol 74: 712–720
  9. 9. Videan EN, Fritz J, Murphy J, Howell S, Heward CB (2005) Does training chimpanzees to present for injection lead to reduced stress? Lab Primate Newsl 44: 1–2.
  10. 10. Broder JM, MacFadden AJ, Cosens LM, Rosenstein DS, Harrison TM (2008) Use of positive reinforcement conditioning to monitor pregnancy in an unanesthetized snow leopard (Uncia uncia) via transabdominal ultrasound. Zoo Biol 27: 78–85
  11. 11. Brown CS, Loskutoff NM (1998) A training program for noninvasive semen collection in captive western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla). Zoo Biol 17: 143–151
  12. 12. Desmond T, Laule G (1994) Use of positive reinforcement training in the management of species for reproduction. Zoo Biol 13: 471–477
  13. 13. International Primatological Society (1989) IPS International guidelines for the acquisition, care and breeding of nonhuman primates. Primate Rep 25: 3–27.
  14. 14. Perlman JE, Bloomsmith MA, Whittaker MA, McMillan JL, Minier DE, et al. (2012) Implementing positive reinforcement animal training programs at primate laboratories. Appl Anim Behav Sci 137: 114–126
  15. 15. Lukas KE, Marr MJ, Maple TL (1998) Teaching operant conditioning at the zoo. Teach Psychol 25: 112–116
  16. 16. Anzenberger G, Gossweiler H (1993) How to obtain individual urine samples from undisturbed marmoset families. Am J Primatol 31: 223–230
  17. 17. Reinhardt V (2003) Working with rather than against macaques during blood collection. J Appl Anim Welf Sci 6: 189–197
  18. 18. Pomerantz O, Terkel J (2009) Effects of positive reinforcement training techniques on the psychological welfare of zoo-housed chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Am J Primatol 71: 687–695
  19. 19. Seiver D, Walsh P, Weber B, MacPhee M (2001) Operant conditioning of apes to facilitate medical procedures and immobilizations. The apes: challenges for the 21st century. Chicago, IL.: Brookfield Zoo. pp. 137–139.
  20. 20. Yoshida H (1997) On the training of a female bonobo for artificial insemination in the Columbus zoo. Pan Afr News 4: 16–17.
  21. 21. Lambeth SP, Perlman JE, Schapiro SJ (2000) Positive reinforcement training paired with videotape exposure decreases training time investment for a complicated task in female chimpanzees. Twenty-Third Annual Meeting of the American Society of Primatologists. American Journal of Primatology 79–80
  22. 22. Bloomsmith MA, Laule GE, Alford PL, Thurston RH (1994) Using training to moderate chimpanzee aggression during feeding. Zoo Biol 13: 557–566
  23. 23. Schapiro SJ, Perlman JE, Thiele E, Lambeth S (2005) Training nonhuman primates to perform behaviors useful in biomedical research. Lab Anim 34: 37–42
  24. 24. Bond M (1991) How to collect urine from a gorilla. Gorilla Gaz 5: 12–13.
  25. 25. Keiter M, Pichette P (1977) Surrogate Infant prepares a Lowland gorilla Gorilla g. gorilla for motherhood. Int Zoo Yearb 17: 188–189
  26. 26. Kuhar CW, Bettinger TL, Laudenslage ML (2005) Salivary cortisol and behaviour in an all-male group of western lowland gorillas (Gorilla g. gorilla). Anim Welf 14: 187–193.
  27. 27. Bell B, Khan P (2001) Training multi-task medical behaviors in the bonobo (Pan paniscus). The apes: challenges for the 21st century. Chicago, IL.: Brookfield Zoo. pp. 128–130.
  28. 28. Bell B, Clyde VL, Kahn P, Maurer J (2001) Advanced operant conditioning and reproductive applications in the bonobo (Pan paniscus). The apes: challenges for the 21st century. Chicago, IL.: Brookfield Zoo. p. VII Poster & Abstracts.
  29. 29. Clyde VA, Bell B, Wallace RS, Roth L (2001) Cardiac evaluation in nonanesthetized bonobos (Pan paniscus). The apes: challenges for the 21st century. Chicago, IL.: Brookfield Zoo. pp. 125–127.
  30. 30. Moore BA, Suedmeyer K (1997) Blood sampling in 0.2 Bornean orangutans at the Kansas City Zoological Gardens. Anim Keep Forum 24: 537–540.
  31. 31. Raper JR, Bloomsmith MA, Stone AM, Mayo L (2002) Use of positive reinforcement training to decrease stereotypic behaviors in a pair of organutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Twenty-Fifth Annual Meeting of The American Society of Primatologists. American Journal of Primatology Vol. 57: 70–71.
  32. 32. Vandevoort CA, Neville LE, Tollner TL, Field LP (1993) Noninvasive semen collection from an adult orangutan. Zoo Biol 12: 257–265
  33. 33. Prescott M, Buchanan-Smith H (2003) Training nonhuman primates using positive reinforcement techniques. J Appl Anim Welf Sci 6: 157–161
  34. 34. Lambeth SP, Hau J, Perlman JE, Martino M, Schapiro SJ (2006) Positive reinforcement training affects hematologic and serum chemistry values in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Am J Primatol 68: 245–256
  35. 35. Perlman JE, Thiele E, Whittaker MA, Lambeth SP, Schapiro SJ (2004) Training chimpanzees to accept subcutaneous injections using positive reinforcement training techniques. Twenty-seventh Annual Meeting The American Society of Primatologists. National Primate Research Center University of Wisconsin Madison, WI, USA. American Journal of Primatology Vol. 62: 96.
  36. 36. Laule GE, Thurston RH, Alford PL, Bloomsmith MA (1996) Training to reliably obtain blood and urine samples from a diabetic chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Zoo Biol 15: 587–591
  37. 37. Lambeth SP, Hau J, Perlman JE, Martino MA, Bernacky BJ, et al. (2004) Positive reinforcement training affects hematologic and serum chemistry values in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Twenty-seventh Annual Meeting The American Society of Primatologists. National Primate Research Center University of Wisconsin Madison, WI, USA:. American Journal of Primatology Vol. 62: 37–38.
  38. 38. Stone AM, Bloomsmith MA, Laule GE, Alford PL (1994) Documenting positive reinforcement training for chimpanzee urine collection. Seventeenth Annual Meeting of The American Society of Primatologists. University of Washington Seattle, Washington:. American Journal of Primatology Vol. 33: 242.
  39. 39. Joines S (1977) A training programme designed to induce maternal behaviour in a multiparous female Lowland gorilla Gorilla g. gorilla at the San Diego Wild Animal Park. Int Zoo Yearb 17: 185–188
  40. 40. Fontaine R (1979) Training an unrestrained orang-utan mother Pongo pygmaeus. Int Zoo Yearb 19: 168–170
  41. 41. Wolfle TL (1987) Control of stress using non-drug approaches. J Am Vet Med Assoc 191: 1219–1221.
  42. 42. Laule G, Whittaker M (2007) Enhancing nonhuman primate care and welfare through the use of positive reinforcement training. J Appl Anim Welf Sci 10: 31–38
  43. 43. Chelluri GI, Ross SR, Wagner KE (2013) Behavioral correlates and welfare implications of informal interactions between caretakers and zoo-housed chimpanzees and gorillas. Appl Anim Behav Sci 147: 306–315
  44. 44. Reimers M, Schwarzenberger F, Preuschoft S (2007) Rehabilitation of research chimpanzees: Stress and coping after long-term isolation. Horm Behav 51: 428–435
  45. 45. McEwen BS (2007) Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiol Rev 87: 873–904
  46. 46. Clarke AS, Mason WA, Moberg GP (1988) Interspecific contrasts in responses of macaques to transport cage training. Lab Anim Sci 38: 305–309.
  47. 47. Dettmer EL, Phillips KA, Rager DR, Bernstein IS, Fragaszy DM (1996) Behavioral and cortisol responses to repeated capture and venipuncture in Cebus apella. Am J Primatol 38: 357–362
  48. 48. Beerda B, Schilder MBH, Janssena NSCRM, Mol JA (1996) The use of saliva cortisol, urinary cortisol, and catecholamine measurements for a noninvasive assessment of stress responses in dogs. Horm Behav 30: 272–279
  49. 49. Creel S (2001) Social dominance and stress hormones. Trends Ecol Evol 16: 491–497
  50. 50. Wilson MI, Brown GM, Wilson D (1978) Annual and diurnal changes in plasma androgen and cortisol in adult male squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) studied longitudinally. Eur J Endocrinol 87: 424–433
  51. 51. Heistermann M (2010) Non-invasive monitoring of endocrine status in laboratory primates: methods, guidelines and applications. Adv Sci Res 1: 1–9
  52. 52. Anestis SF (2010) Hormones and social behavior in primates. Evol Anthropol Issues News Rev 19: 66–78
  53. 53. Goymann W (2012) On the use of non-invasive hormone research in uncontrolled, natural environments: the problem with sex, diet, metabolic rate and the individual. Methods Ecol Evol 3: 757–765
  54. 54. Kirschbaum C, Hellhammer DH (1994) Salivary cortisol in psychoneuroendocrine research: Recent developments and applications. Psychoneuroendocrinology 19: 313–333
  55. 55. Behringer V, Clauß W, Hachenburger K, Kuchar A, Möstl E, et al. (2009) Effect of giving birth on the cortisol level in a bonobo groups’ (Pan paniscus) saliva. Primates 50: 190–193
  56. 56. Behringer V, Deschner T, Möstl E, Selzer D, Hohmann G (2012) Stress affects salivary alpha-amylase activity in bonobos. Physiol Behav 105: 476–482
  57. 57. Behringer V, Borchers C, Deschner T, Möstl E, Selzer D, et al. (2013) Measurements of salivary alpha amylase and salivary cortisol in hominoid primates reveal within-species consistency and between-species differences. PLoS ONE 8: e60773
  58. 58. Elder CM, Menzel CR (2001) Dissociation of cortisol and behavioral indicators of stress in an orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus) during a computerized task. Primates 42: 345–357
  59. 59. Kutsukake N, Ikeda K, Honma S, Teramoto M, Mori Y, et al. (2009) Validation of salivary cortisol and testosterone assays in chimpanzees by liquid chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry. Am J Primatol 71: 696–706.
  60. 60. O'Brien JK, Heffernan S, Thomson PC, McGreevy PD (2008) Effect of positive reinforcement training on physiological and behavioural stress responses in the hamadryas baboon (Papio hamadryas). Anim Welf 17: 125–138.
  61. 61. Behringer V (2011) Ethophysiolgische Untersuchung zu haltungsbedingten Einflüssen auf das Verhalten und die Stresssituation von Westlichen Flachlandgorillas (Gorilla g. gorilla), Sumatra Orang-Utans (Pongo abelii) und Bonobos (Pan paniscus) unter Zoobedingungen Gießen: Justus-Liebig-Universität.
  62. 62. Bosch JA, Veerman ECI, de Geus EJ, Proctor GB (2011) α-Amylase as a reliable and convenient measure of sympathetic activity: don't start salivating just yet!. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36: 449–453
  63. 63. Gröschl M, Biskupek-Sigwart J, Rauh M, Dörr HG (2000) Measurement of cortisol in saliva using a commercial radioimmunoassay developed for serum. Messung von speichel-cortisol mittels eines kommerziellen serum-radioimmunoassays. LaboratoriumsMedizin 24: 314–318
  64. 64. Heintz MR, Santymire RM, Parr LA, Lonsdorf EV (2011) Validation of a cortisol enzyme immunoassay and characterization of salivary cortisol circadian rhythm in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Am J Primatol 73: 903–908
  65. 65. Dabbs Jr JM (1991) Salivary testosterone measurements: collecting, storing, and mailing saliva samples. Physiol Behav 49: 815–817
  66. 66. Palme R, Möstl E (1997) Measurement of cortisol metabolites in faeces of sheep as a parameter of cortisol concentration in blood. Int J Mamm Biol 62 Suppl. II: 192–197.
  67. 67. Baayen RH (2008) Analyzing Linguistic Data. Cambridge, UK; New York: Cambridge University Press.
  68. 68. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S (2013) lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4.
  69. 69. R Core Team (2013) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R foundation for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria.
  70. 70. Field AP (2009) Discovering statistics using SPSS. 3rd ed. Los Angeles: SAGE Publications. 821 p.
  71. 71. Fox J (2011) An R companion to applied regression. 2nd ed. Thousand Oaks, Calif: SAGE Publications. 449 p.
  72. 72. Aiken LS, Reno RR, West SG (1998) Multiple regression: testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, Calif.: SAGE.
  73. 73. Larsson CA, Gullberg B, Råstam L, Lindblad U (2009) Salivary cortisol differs with age and sex and shows inverse associations with WHR in Swedish women: a cross-sectional study. BMC Endocr Disord 9: 16
  74. 74. Dobson AJ, Barnett AG (2008) An introduction to generalized linear models. 3rd ed. Boca Raton: CRC Press. 307 p.
  75. 75. Schielzeth H, Forstmeier W (2009) Conclusions beyond support: overconfident estimates in mixed models. Behav Ecol 20: 416–420
  76. 76. Baayen RH (2011) Languager: data sets and functions with “analyzing linguistic data: a practical introduction to statistics”. R package version 1.2.
  77. 77. Bloomsmith MA, Stone AM, Laule GE (1998) Positive reinforcement training to enhance the voluntary movement of group-housed chimpanzees within their enclosures. Zoo Biol 17: 333–341
  78. 78. Morgan KN, Tromborg CT (2007) Sources of stress in captivity. Appl Anim Behav Sci 102: 262–302
  79. 79. Read GF, Walker RF, Wilson DW, Griffiths K (1990) Steroid analysis in saliva for the assessment of endocrine function. Ann N Y Acad Sci 595: 260–274
  80. 80. Vining RF, McGinley RA, Maksvytis JJ, Ho KY (1983) Salivary cortisol: a better measure of adrenal cortical function than serum cortisol. Ann Clin Biochem 20 (Pt 6) 329–335.
  81. 81. Kirschbaum C, Hellhammer DH (1989) Salivary cortisol in psychobiological research: an overview. Neuropsychobiology 22: 150–169
  82. 82. Carrasco L, Colell M, Calvo M, Abelló MT, Velasco M, et al. (2009) Benefits of training/playing therapy in a group of captive lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla). Anim Welf 18: 9–19.
  83. 83. Prescott MJ, Buchanan-Smith HM (1999) Intra-and inter-specific social learning of a novel food task in two species of tamarin. Int J Comp Psychol 12: 71–92.