Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Socio-Psychological Factors Driving Adult Vaccination: A Qualitative Study

Correction

20 Mar 2015: The PLOS ONE Staff (2015) Correction: Socio-Psychological Factors Driving Adult Vaccination: A Qualitative Study. PLOS ONE 10(3): e0119893. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119893 View correction

Abstract

Background

While immunization is one of the most effective and successful public health interventions, there are still up to 30,000 deaths in major developed economies each year due to vaccine-preventable diseases, almost all in adults. In the UK, despite comparatively high vaccination rates among ≧65 s (73%) and, to a lesser extent, at-risk ≤65 s (52%) in 2013/2014, over 10,000 excess deaths were reported the previous influenza season. Adult tetanus vaccines are not routinely recommended in the UK, but may be overly administered. Social influences and risk-perceptions of diseases and vaccines are known to affect vaccine uptake. We aimed to explore the socio-psychological factors that drive adult vaccination in the UK, specifically influenza and tetanus, and to evaluate whether these factors are comparable between vaccines.

Methods

20 in-depth, face-to-face interviews were conducted with members of the UK public who represented a range of socio-demographic characteristics associated with vaccination uptake. We employed qualitative interviewing approaches to reach a comprehensive understanding of the factors influencing adult vaccination decisions. Thematic analysis was used to analyze the data.

Results

Participants were classified according to their vaccination status as regular, intermittent and non-vaccinators for influenza, and preventative, injury-led, mixed (both preventative and injury-led) and as non-vaccinators for tetanus. We present our finding around five overarching themes: 1) perceived health and health behaviors; 2) knowledge; 3) vaccination influences; 4) disease appraisal; and 5) vaccination appraisal.

Conclusion

The uptake of influenza and tetanus vaccines was largely driven by participants' risk perception of these diseases. The tetanus vaccine is perceived as safe and sufficiently tested, whereas the changing composition of the influenza vaccine is a cause of uncertainty and distrust. To maximize the public health impact of adult vaccines, policy should be better translated into high vaccination rates through evidence-based implementation approaches.

Introduction

While immunization is one of the most effective and successful public health strategies in reducing or eliminating the health, economic and societal burden of many infectious diseases [1], major developed economies such as the US and Germany still report up to 30,000 deaths each year due to vaccine-preventable diseases, almost all in adults [2], [3]. The extraordinary success of childhood routine immunization programs across the world, which show high immunization coverage levels, has not been matched in adult programs [4][6]. This disparity is of increasing relevance in the context of a rapidly aging population and the attendant societal and economic burden.

Influenza and tetanus-containing boosters (tetanus boosters) are two commonly recommended vaccines for adults. Most countries follow World Health Organization (WHO) influenza vaccination recommendations: an annual vaccine, particularly for people who are at higher risk of developing influenza-related complications [7]. Although the WHO recommends an extra tetanus toxoid-containing dose in adulthood [8], recommendations for adult tetanus boosters vary across countries. For example, the US Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) recommends a 1-time dose of tetanus, diphtheria and pertussis (Tdap), followed by a tetanus and diphtheria (Td) booster every 10 years [9], whereas in France a tetanus, diphtheria and polio booster (Td/IPV) is recommended for under 25 s, a second dose at 45 years old and every 10 years for over 65 s, with one booster being replaced by a Tdap/IPV [10].

In the UK, an influenza vaccine is recommended and available free of charge for people ≧65 years old, ≤65 s with an eligible chronic health condition and pregnant women. Yet, despite comparatively high vaccination rates among ≧65 s (73%) and, to a lesser extent, ≤65 s in a clinical risk group (52%) in 2013/2014 [11], over 10,000 excess deaths were reported in UK the previous influenza season [12]. Although tetanus boosters are not included in ‘The complete routine immunisation schedule 2013/14’, the National Health Service (NHS) recommends a Td/IPV to those who have not or have been partially immunized, or are travelling to a country with limited medical facilities [13]. The number of tetanus cases reported in the UK is low (83 in England and Wales since 2002) [14]. However, research suggests that the success of the tetanus vaccination program may be partly attributed to a mismatch between clinical practice and immunization guidelines, reflected in emergency departments' tendency to over-vaccinate patients who attend them [15]. This misalignment may also be occurring in primary care.

In countries with universal vaccination coverage, where structural barriers to access are limited, social and psychological influences such as perceived risk of diseases and vaccines are important determinants of acceptance and uptake of influenza vaccine [16][20]. Specifically in the UK, few qualitative studies to date have explored influenza vaccination decision-making and most have focused on the elderly [21][23]. Research evaluating factors driving tetanus boosters' uptake from the perspective of the vaccinee is scarce [24], [25].

To better understand social and psychological drivers of adult vaccination, we have set up a large-scale multinational qualitative study, which aims to use interview-based techniques to explore in depth adults' perceptions of vaccination and the factors that drive them to have themselves vaccinated (or not) [26]. The detailed qualitative dataset will subsequently be used to inform the development of practical survey tools that can reliably capture key determinants of vaccination behavior and predict uptake. The study that we report here is part of this larger research program. It explores the social and psychological factors that drive adult vaccination in the UK, specifically influenza and tetanus, and evaluates whether these factors are comparable between vaccines or vaccine-specific.

Methods

This research was approved by the Imperial College Research Ethics Committee. Participants were presented with a research information sheet and briefed on confidentiality/anonymity of their data before they were asked to sign a consent form. The overall research protocol and methods used in our research program are reported in detail elsewhere [26]. We summarize them below and include specific information about the UK data collection and analysis.

Sampling and recruitment

Interviews were carried out in three regions in the UK: London, South East and West Midlands. Although representative samples are not required in qualitative research, we sought to attain relevant perspectives by recruiting participants from areas where the majority of the UK population reside. We used a purposive sampling strategy to select 20 adult participants who were both vaccinated or not vaccinated against influenza and tetanus, and represented a range of socio-demographic characteristics associated with vaccination uptake, particularly age and health status (see Table 1). We excluded pregnant women and healthcare professionals (HCPs), as their vaccination decisions are significantly influenced by those they care for and/or regulated by healthcare authorities and professional bodies, thus some of their motivations and concerns are likely different [27], [28]. To reduce recall bias [29], only those who had been vaccinated in the past 12 months were eligible as vaccinated participants. A screening question was used to exclude participants who were fundamentally opposed to vaccination, as this stance represents only a small minority of the non-vaccinated population and thus could bias the results [30]. Participants were recruited via telephone using random dialing, sourced from telephone directories by Ipsos MORI, an international market research company.

Piloting

The interview schedule was designed through expert consultations and a review of the relevant literature. The schedule was then tested with two researchers from Imperial College and two researchers from Ipsos MORI who were not involved in the present study. The duration and flow of the interview were discussed and the schedule was finessed as a result. The refinements to the schedule were related to wording (e.g. using ‘flu’ instead of ‘influenza’ for simplicity) and reordering and/or deletion of redundant probes. These interviews were not included in the final sample. Piloting was subsequently carried out for the first three interviews, whereby the research team observed each interview conducted by an Ipsos MORI trained interviewer behind a one-way mirror and evaluated its quality in real-time. At the end of the session, minor improvements were made to the interviewers' instructions included in the schedule.

Procedure

Participants were interviewed face-to-face in their homes or at a central interviewing facility for approximately 60 minutes. Each participant received £70 in return for their time. Half of the interviews were conducted by an academic researcher (AW) from Imperial College and half by trained interviewer from Ipsos MORI. Two versions of the interview schedule were used: one for vaccinated and one for non-vaccinated participants (see Table 2).

The interview schedule comprised six sections. Section1 obtained an overview of participants' life and values, to build rapport with the interviewee and to identify important issues to assist with probing throughout the interview. Section 2 elicited participants' general information-seeking behaviors and influences. Section 3 examined participants' views toward health, HCPs and adult vaccines. Section 4 evaluated individual participants' decision making ‘journeys’ to vaccination or non-vaccination. Section 5 examined participants' attitudes toward pediatric influenza and tetanus vaccines. We aimed to understand whether or not people's views about adult vaccines correspond with their views about childhood vaccines. Finally, in section 6 we explored participants' knowledge of the two diseases and vaccines (i.e. influenza and tetanus) to understand to what extent their decision-making is influenced by factual information.

We explored the set of circumstances and emotional factors that drove participants to accept or refuse vaccination, aided by qualitative interviewing approaches aimed at obtaining information which explicit enquiry (i.e. a direct question) may fail to capture – as follows. First, throughout the interview we used an elicitation technique called ‘laddering’, which provides a simple and systematic way of establishing people's core values and beliefs, and the linkages between these and key behaviors, in this case, vaccination [31]. In section 3, general views on adult vaccines were evaluated by asking participants to spontaneously arrange and rank, using a category of their choice, cards depicting five adult vaccines (influenza, tetanus, pneumonia, hepatitis and measles, mumps and rubella (MMR)) into one or more groups. In section 4, we employed the “Journey to vaccination” approach [26], a visual exercise in which the interviewer and the participant jointly draw a timeline that captures salient events that lead the participant to get or not to get vaccinated. These results will be presented elsewhere, as they require different analysis. Briefly, a journey to vaccination for influenza and other for tetanus is drawn for each participant. Differences and commonalities emerging from these data are identified and synthesized, and typical journeys proposed.

Data Analysis

The recorded interviews were professionally transcribed verbatim and checked for accuracy by Ipsos MORI. To ensure reliability of coding and interpretation, all the transcripts were analyzed by the first author and 50% of the transcripts were double-coded independently by the second author [32]. Differences were resolved through discussion and review until consensus was reached. Using thematic analysis, an initial categorizing system was developed based on the study objectives and the topics explored [33], [34]. New themes and sub-themes emerging from the data analysis were identified and included when deemed relevant by the coders. Two identical thematic indexes, one for influenza and one for tetanus, were produced to code the majority of the data. Additionally, a separate matrix was developed to code and analyze the categories and rankings proposed by participants during card exercise in section 3. This coding strategy enabled us to evaluate whether the assessed factors were comparable between vaccines or were vaccine-specific. We also examined whether participants' views varied depending upon their vaccination status. The interpretation of the findings was initially carried out by the first and second author. Contributions from the rest of the authors further shaped the analysis.

Results

Participant characteristics

Twenty members of the UK public were interviewed in May 2013. The sample was equally split by gender. Eight participants were 18–49 yrs, seven were 50–64 yrs and five were 65+. The majority were white British (N = 16) and half were educated to university level. Just under half of the sample were retired or had a chronic health condition. Participants' characteristics are fully described in Table 3.

Distinctive vaccination behavior patterns emerged from the data. Thus, the dichotomous vaccination status (i.e. vaccinated/not vaccinated) initially employed was fine-tuned; participants were classified as regular (vaccinated most or all the time; N = 8), intermittent (had vaccinated only for a period of time; N = 5) and non-vaccinators (never had vaccinated; N = 7) for influenza; and preventative (vaccinated before an injury; N = 4), injury-led (vaccinated after an injury; n = 9), mixed (both preventative and injury-led) (N = 3) and non-vaccinators (never had vaccinated; N = 4) for tetanus. In the sections that follow, we use this classification to report our findings.

Context of vaccine perceptions

Participants widely agreed on the importance of vaccination in general and childhood vaccination in particular. When asked to rank influenza, tetanus, pneumococcal, hepatitis and MMR for adults (cards exercise), participants employed three main categories: disease severity, vaccine importance and vaccination age. Most found it difficult to categorize MMR and generally regarded it as a childhood vaccine, despite interviewers stressing that it also applies to adults.

Severity of the disease emerged as a key classification category. Regular vaccinators classified influenza as the most or one of the most severe diseases, followed by tetanus, pneumonia and hepatitis, although some stressed their knowledge of the latter was limited. Overall influenza non-vaccinators did not perceive this disease to be as severe as hepatitis or pneumonia, and ranked tetanus as being more severe than influenza. Those who had previously vaccinated against influenza chose vaccine importance to categorize vaccines. Perceived vaccine importance was related to both perceived severity and likelihood of the disease it protected against, and, consequently, the importance of “being protected”. The influenza vaccine was ranked among the two most important vaccines, followed by tetanus, pneumonia, hepatitis and MMR. Some participants used vaccination age as a ranking category and associated the influenza and pneumonia vaccines with adults, particularly the elderly. Hepatitis was also linked to adulthood. Tetanus was associated with teenagers and adults and MMR with children.

Drivers and barriers to vaccination: Findings of the thematic analysis

Thematic saturation was reached at 14 interviews (i.e. no new themes appeared in the last 6 interviews that were carried out). Five overarching themes emerged from the analysis: 1) perceived health and health behaviors; 2) knowledge; 3) vaccination influences; 4) disease appraisal; and 5) vaccination appraisal. These are reported in detail below. Illustrative quotes are provided in Tables 4 and 5.

thumbnail
Table 4. Context of vaccine perceptions, perceived health and health behaviors, knowledge and influences: key emerging themes and illustrative quotes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0113503.t004

thumbnail
Table 5. Disease and vaccination appraisal: key emerging themes and illustrative quotes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0113503.t005

1. Perceived health and health behaviors

Exercise, a balanced diet, not smoking and moderate alcohol consumption were perceived by most participants as desirable healthy behaviors, yet vaccination was generally not associated with the ‘healthy living’ paradigm. Those with chronic conditions were generally aware of their health status and the recommended health behaviors for preventing complications, including influenza vaccination. Accordingly, two thirds of participants who reported having a chronic health condition regularly vaccinated against influenza and had had a preventative tetanus booster. However, they were the least likely to engage in other healthy behaviors.

2. Knowledge

Several participants felt they did not know enough about influenza and tetanus, or the associated vaccines. Most mentioned that influenza was a potentially severe illness, yet those who had vaccinated in the past were more likely to stress it is a life-threatening disease.

Participants were widely aware about high-risk groups but were less specific about the influenza symptoms, which were commonly referred to as a “very bad cold”. Most knew the influenza vaccine was offered once a year and a third mentioned (accurately) that the vaccine did not protect against all circulating virus strands.

Tetanus was generally referred as “lockjaw” and perceived to be a very serious disease; yet additional symptoms were seldom identified. Cuts, rust and animal bites were mentioned as main sources of infection. Some believed tetanus does not exist in the UK any more. Just under half mentioned that a tetanus booster was recommended every 10 years and several noted they would get it if they had an injury, but they did not see the need to have it preventatively.

3. Vaccination influences

3.1. Salient previous experiences.

Most participants reported experiencing influenza-like symptoms once or more times over the course of their lives and several stressed, unprompted, that they knew the difference between a cold and influenza. None of the participants had contracted tetanus. Some participants who had intermittently or never vaccinated against influenza recalled a traumatic health-related experience during childhood, including painful vaccination, allergy to (injected) penicillin or frequent tonsillitis (which required penicillin injections).

3.2. Family and peers.

All influenza non-vaccinators, except one young healthy participant, mentioned that a family member, a friend or both had had a bad experience with the vaccine, and some recalled that although in certain seasons they had heard or read media reports about influenza-related deaths, no one they knew had been severely affected. Others, including non-vaccinators, reported that family or friends had recommended or were in favor of the influenza vaccine.

Some participants reported having had a tetanus booster due to their mother's advice or their memories of their mother's warnings about the severity of tetanus during childhood. Others mentioned advice from a family member and work colleagues' jokes about contracting tetanus as influencing their decision to seek medical help after an injury.

3.3. Healthcare professionals and vaccine manufacturers.

Many participants had discussed vaccinations, particularly influenza, with their general practitioner (GP). Several unvaccinated or intermittent vaccinators expressed negative feelings toward healthcare professionals, which revolved around GP's lack of empathy, NHS perverse incentives (e.g. GPs receiving payments for each administered vaccine) and general distrust of the medical profession. In contrast, all regular influenza vaccinators reported that their GPs had recommended and routinely reminded them to get vaccinated during regular consultations, through GP surgery adverts and letters. Some participants mentioned they had either got vaccinated against influenza or were reminded to do so at a pharmacy.

Receiving a tetanus booster preventatively or after an injury was generally triggered by a recommendation from a GP or an emergency department doctor. Some participants, however, reported that HCPs seemed somewhat unsure as to what the course of action was, particularly when the request for a booster was patient-led. Others mentioned they hoped their GPs would know from their electronic medical records when they were due for a tetanus booster.

Two participants, one intermittent and one non-vaccinator, raised specific concerns regarding the pharmaceutical industry's lack of trustworthiness.

3.4. Media.

Some participants mentioned finding out the risks of catching the influenza through the news (accessed through different mediums), whilst others had read about influenza vaccines' risks mainly through user-generated web-based sources such as personal blogs and social media.

4. Disease appraisal

4.1. Perceived susceptibility.

Susceptibility denotes constitutional vulnerability to a particular hazard, rather than the likelihood of exposure to it. All regular and some intermittent influenza vaccinators reported feeling susceptible to the disease. In contrast, although some non-vaccinators said they would consider having the vaccine when they were older and therefore more vulnerable to influenza, most felt that the risks influenza currently posed to their health did not warrant vaccination. Furthermore, some intermittent and non-vaccinators, felt they were able to prevent influenza by “improving the immune system” or “keeping healthy”.

Few participants reported feeling particularly vulnerable to tetanus, and they had had a preventative booster. Having a “good immune system” was raised by only one injury-led vaccinator as a reason why they did not feel the need to have a preventative tetanus booster.

4.2. Perceived severity.

Most participants acknowledged that influenza could be life-threatening for high-risk groups, but only a few were concerned about having a severe bout of the disease. The majority, particularly non-vaccinators, perceived themselves as being able to “cope” with the disease. In contrast, just under half of participants, most of whom had had a preventative booster, stated that they had a tetanus booster for fear of contracting a life-threatening disease.

4.3. Perceived likelihood.

Most regular influenza vaccinators felt they were likely to catch influenza, whereas some intermittent and non-vaccinators felt it was unlikely they would catch it. Participants generally interchanged susceptibility to influenza and likelihood of getting it in their interviews.

Almost half of the sample mentioned they were likely to contract tetanus, commonly due to lifestyle choices (e.g. gardening or travelling abroad), risky work environment (e.g. construction) or injuries. The other half, however, felt the likelihood of contracting tetanus was very low.

5. Vaccination appraisal

5.1. Perceived benefits.

Although many were aware that the influenza vaccine was not 100% effective, those who vaccinated regularly were more likely to acknowledge the benefits of being protected or protecting a vulnerable family member against influenza, albeit partially. Remaining productive at work/home was raised as one of the benefits of influenza vaccination. Most participants, however, were unsupportive of the pediatric influenza vaccine. Many stated that “children's immune system should be built naturally” whereas others questioned the need for an influenza vaccine for children due to perceived low prevalence of the disease among this group.

Being immunized against tetanus was considered important, yet many thought a booster was only needed after injury and not as a preventative measure. In contrast, the majority of participants felt that vaccinating children against tetanus preventatively was necessary, as they were more prone to falls and injuries.

5.2. Perceived costs and practical barriers.

Some intermittent and most non-vaccinators mentioned the influenza vaccine's side-effects as a main concern. The most commonly mentioned adverse effects were influenza-like symptoms and pain in the arm. Some mentioned side-effects could change yearly, depending of the composition of the vaccine, and others believed the vaccine caused influenza or was unsafe. Most regular vaccinators had not experienced memorable side-effects; those who had felt that contracting influenza was worse.

No important concerns regarding the side-effects or safety of tetanus boosters were raised. Several participants noted that the vaccine had been sufficiently tested and that they had not heard any negative things about it.

Some participants, including regular influenza vaccinators, were apprehensive about needles. Feelings of fear generally stemmed from their previous experiences or those of others. Fear of needles, however, waned when confronted with the decision of vaccinating against a disease which was perceived as a serious threat to their health: tetanus in most cases and influenza in the case of those who reported feeling particularly vulnerable to it.

Competing priorities or lack of time was raised by some as the main practical barrier to influenza vaccination. Although affordability was not an issue, two participants who were not eligible to have a free vaccine through the (publicly funded) NHS had difficulties getting it elsewhere. One reported lack of vaccine availability at the local pharmacy and the other was denied the vaccine at a supermarket due to high blood pressure. Just under a third of participants mentioned that keeping up-to-date with tetanus boosters was challenging due to their recommended time interval (10 years).

Discussion

This study investigated the socio-psychological factors influencing adult vaccination uptake in the UK. Our results suggest that the public have no general concept of adult immunization, as they have for childhood immunization. Instead, their beliefs and attitudes are vaccine-specific and in some cases age-specific. Participants classified influenza and tetanus, and to a lesser extent pneumonia, as severe diseases. Consistent with their disease appraisal, participants felt that the influenza, tetanus and pneumococcal vaccines were important. Understandably, few participants had heard of the hepatitis vaccines or knew about hepatitis, as both hepatitis A and B are uncommon in the UK. Participants generally associated influenza and pneumonia vaccines with older age, tetanus with adolescence and MMR with childhood.

The perceived age segmentation and general lack of awareness of an adult immunization schedule may be a reflection of the way adult immunization policy in the UK has been communicated. Although a recent shift toward ‘life course vaccination’ is a move in the right direction, unlike the CDC's annually recommended adult immunization schedule [35], the NHS provides an overall schedule from 2 to 70 years old, which only includes three adult vaccines, all for over 65 s: influenza, pneumococcal and the recently introduced shingles vaccine [36]. The rest of the recommended adult vaccines fall into the “Vaccines for special groups” and “Travel vaccines” categories detailed in additional webpages, which in turn include many sub-categories and at times somewhat ambiguous exceptions. A case in point is the tetanus booster recommendation, which states: “A tetanus vaccination is usually recommended for anyone who: has not been vaccinated before, has not been fully vaccinated, is travelling to a country with limited medical facilities, and whose last dose of the tetanus vaccine was more than 10 years ago[13].

Participants' knowledge about influenza and the influenza vaccine was reasonably accurate and generally acquired through mainstream media, mostly TV news, and HCPs, whereas information about vaccine risks was usually found online and in some cases in social networking sites such as Facebook. These findings resonate with those from previous research evaluating the impact of different media on vaccination behavior [37] and suggest that mass vaccination campaigns are indeed improving knowledge and prompting uptake. They also indicate that confirmation bias [38] may be driving hesitant vaccinators to seek unofficial sources that provide information at odds with scientific evidence and against vaccination – which confirms their own hesitation. Knowledge about tetanus was less specific, but most participants were aware of the severity of the disease.

Key vaccination drivers for influenza were the perceived risk of the disease, commonly assessed in the literature as a combination of perceived disease susceptibility, severity and likelihood [39], and a GP recommendation. In contrast, perceived lack of susceptibility to influenza, perceived vaccine side-effects and partial effectiveness were the main barriers to vaccination. These findings are comparable to those reported in previous studies assessing social and psychological underpinnings of vaccination [40], [41]. Whilst the drivers of tetanus vaccination were analogous to those of influenza vaccination, the barriers were somewhat different and mostly related to lack of awareness, consistent with previous research [24], [25], and variable vaccination practices. There is some indication that preventative vaccination is initiated by the vaccinee, while injury-led vaccination is instigated by HCPs.

We found that social influences played an important role in vaccination behavior, sometimes trumping participants' factual knowledge. Regular influenza vaccinators were more prone to consider the advice of relatives and peers, and have positive attitudes toward healthcare professionals than non-vaccinators. Furthermore, vaccinators reported that receiving regular reminders from their GPs about the influenza vaccine triggered vaccination uptake, which suggests that such reminders were indeed falling on fertile ground. This resonates with a vast body of literature which shows that HCP recommendation and routine reminders significantly influence vaccination uptake [42]. Conversely, participants who showed a lack of trust toward HCPs or had a relative or friend who had reported a negative experience with the vaccine were more likely to refuse vaccination or ignore their GP's recommendation. Preventative tetanus vaccinators also displayed favorable opinions toward doctors. However, some participants also felt HCPs were hesitant when asked for advice on whether or not to receive a booster, which is another indication that the current tetanus vaccination policy in the UK may lead to inconsistent or inappropriate practice [15].

A striking and novel psychological finding of this study is that previous experiences related to injections, particularly during childhood, had both a positive and a negative influence on vaccination uptake. One-third of influenza non-vaccinators reported having had a traumatic experience with vaccines, injections or medication in the past, which they stated had influenced their decision to not vaccinate against influenza. This resonates with previous research which showed that painful neonatal experiences such as circumcision or Heel Stick Capillary Blood sampling in neonates can magnify the experience of pain later in life [43], [44]. Similarly, some tetanus vaccinators recalled that the memory of their mother's warnings about the danger of contracting tetanus in childhood had influenced their decision to have a tetanus booster. These findings suggest that pain (caused by needles) and fear (of contracting a severe disease) during childhood could become both a potent vaccination deterrent or enabler. More research in this area is needed.

Consistent with previous research reporting tetanus over-vaccination in emergency departments [15], participants generally associated tetanus boosters with the treatment of injuries as opposed to the prevention of tetanus, yet, they also felt that “keeping up-to-date” with tetanus vaccination (every 10 years) was important. Our findings suggest that a convoluted tetanus immunization policy may not only underpin the routine administration of tetanus boosters in UK emergency departments but also GP practices. Excess vaccination may be further exacerbated by the recently added ‘Adult Immunisation Programme’ section of the Green Book (the official and most up-to-date source of information on immunization for HCPs in the UK), which features tetanus vaccination for adults prominently, above the influenza and pneumococcal vaccines [45]. Worryingly, our findings show that tetanus boosters may often be administered after an injury to prevent a possible infection. However, current guidelines state that the recommended course of action is administering intravenous human tetanus immunoglobulin, as the tetanus vaccine may not boost immunity in time to provide adequate protection [46].

Attitudes toward childhood vaccines were often discordant with views on the adult versions of the same vaccines. Whilst participants were accepting of children being immunized against tetanus preventatively rather than after an injury, they were largely unsupportive of pediatric influenza vaccination, consistent with parental concerns reported in previous studies [47]. Although some attitudes may be unsubstantiated (e.g. “children's immune system should be built naturally”), others, such as the lack of perceived need for pediatric influenza vaccines, may be explained by the comparatively low child mortality attributed to this disease [48]. The advantages of pediatric influenza vaccines for both the recipient and the wider community should be better communicated.

Our findings have policy implications. The UK has some of the highest seasonal influenza vaccination rates in the developed world, but gaps remain, particularly in patients with chronic conditions. National strategies to sustain and increase high influenza vaccination uptake should be built upon an evidence-based understanding of the attitudes of the public and HCPs to vaccination, and should include longitudinal evaluation of their impact [49]. A national campaign could focus on engendering feelings of susceptibility, tailored to specific at-risk population groups and their characteristics, rather than a generic fear of serious health risk, which may be inaccurate and therefore less effective for younger at risk-individuals. Communication on the risks of influenza and the benefits - and risks - of vaccination should follow risk-communication principles on how best to structure and deliver messages and be more actively diffused into social media channels. A key aspect of such communications should be introducing the concept of life course vaccination in the public's mind.

Consistent with a large body of data, we found that a GP recommendation was an important trigger for vaccination. Reminders have been effective in increasing vaccination rates in primary care practices in the UK and elsewhere, yet their utilization remains insufficient, particularly for younger at-risk individuals [50], [42]. Moreover, our findings suggest that an effective GP-led communication approach could be based on the notion of relative or contextualized risk. For example, to better understand the risks of an untrusted vaccine (e.g. influenza), it may be beneficial to draw comparisons with the risks posed by trusted vaccines (e.g. tetanus). Similarly, communicating the likelihood of experiencing side-effects from a vaccine by using the likelihood (and potential consequences) of a bout of the disease as a comparator, may facilitate evidence-based decision-making. More research on risk-communication interventions aimed at improving adult vaccination rates is warranted. Given the stated importance of negative past experiences with needles, support to help GPs to make all childhood encounters with injections as easy as possible may be a good investment in the success of vaccination programs in the future. A clearer adult tetanus immunization policy should be considered, alongside an effective dissemination plan for HCPs – as current national tetanus immunization guidelines appear not to be followed in practice.

Limitations

This study is not without limitations. Although interviews were conducted in an open and non-judgmental manner and efforts were made to minimize availability and social desirability biases, it is possible that some participants may at times have felt compelled to give what they perceived to be rational or desirable answers. Furthermore, it is likely that recall bias may have influenced some of the participants' recollections about past experiences, particularly those around tetanus boosters. Of similar importance, our sample size, although appropriate for a qualitative study and supported by the theme saturation that was achieved, and the purposive recruitment of participants, may have an effect on the generalizability of our results. Experimental and quantitative study designs should be used to further test our findings.

Conclusions

We found that the uptake of both influenza and tetanus vaccines is largely driven by people's risk perception of these diseases. For influenza, this appears to be mediated by trust in HCPs and the perceived risks of the vaccine, insofar the latter do not outweigh the perceived risks of influenza. The tetanus vaccine is largely viewed as sufficiently tested and safe, whereas the changing composition of the influenza vaccine is a cause of uncertainty and distrust. As we await an effective universal influenza vaccine, the advantages of newer vaccines, such as quadrivalent influenza vaccines that provide broader coverage, should be emphasized. To maximize the public health impact of current adult vaccines, policy should be better translated into high vaccination rates through evidence-based implementation approaches which draw upon a wealth of evidence from fields such as psychology and communication sciences.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the participants of this study for their willingness to contribute to our research. We are also very grateful to Jonathan Nicholls, Helen Cox, Kate Duxbury, Gareth Turley, Reena Sangar and Rebeccah Szyndler (Ipsos MORI), as well as the local research partners, for their valuable advice and management support throughout this study. The authors would also like to thank Marion Dolbeau (Sanofi Pasteur) for her contribution to the design and execution of this research and Professor Peter Openshaw (Imperial College) for his invaluable advice.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: AW BR AT NS. Performed the experiments: AW. Analyzed the data: AW AP. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: AW BR AT MM NS. Wrote the paper: AW AP BR AT MM CV NS.

References

  1. 1. CDC (2014) Benefits from Immunization During the Vaccines for Children Program Era — United States, 1994–2013. MMWR 63:352–355.
  2. 2. National Vaccine Advisory Committee (2012) A pathway to leadership for adult immunization: recommendations of the National Vaccine Advisory Committee: approved by the National Vaccine Advisory Committee on June 14, 2011. Public Health Rep 127:11–42.
  3. 3. Zucs P, Buchholz U, Haas W, Uphoff H (2005) Influenza associated excess mortality in Germany, 1985–2001. Emerg Themes Epidemiol 2:1–9.
  4. 4. WHO (2014) Immunization surveillance, assessment and monitoring. Available: http://www.who.int/immunization/monitoring_surveillance/en/. Accessed 2014 Feb 28.
  5. 5. CDC (2012) Adult vaccination coverage —United States. MMWR 2012 61:66–72.
  6. 6. ECDC (2014) Implementation of the Council Recommendation on seasonal influenza vaccination (2009/1019/EU). Available: http://www.ecdc.europa.eu/en/publications/Publications/Implementation-seasonal-influenza-vaccination-Council-Recommendation-Jan-2014.pdf. Accessed 2014 Feb 28.
  7. 7. WHO (2014) Influenza (seasonal) - Fact sheet N°211. Available: http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs211/en/. Accessed 2014 Feb 28.
  8. 8. WHO (2006) Tetanus vaccine. Wkly Epidemiol Rec 81:197–208.
  9. 9. CDC (2012) Recommended Adult Immunization Schedule — United States, 2012. MMWR 61:1–7.
  10. 10. Institut de Veille Sanitaire (2013) 2013 vaccination schedule and recommendations from the “Haut Conseil de la santé publique” in France. BEH 14–15:131–157.
  11. 11. PHE (2014) Seasonal flu vaccine uptake 2013/14 - Data on GP registered patients. Available: https://www.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/319558/SeasonalFluData_GPPatients_Jan2014_acc.pdf. Accessed 2014 Feb 28.
  12. 12. PHE (2013) Surveillance of influenza and other respiratory viruses, including novel respiratory viruses, in the United Kingdom: Winter 2012/13. Available: http://www.hpa.org.uk/webc/HPAwebFile/HPAweb_C/1317139321787. Accessed 2014 Feb 28.
  13. 13. NHS (2014) Tetanus - Vaccination. Available: http://www.nhs.uk/Conditions/Tetanus/Pages/Prevention.aspx. Accessed 2014 Feb 28.
  14. 14. PHE (2014) Tetanus Cases by Age Group and Year of Onset (All Sources*): England and Wales. Available: http://www.hpa.org.uk/Topics/InfectiousDiseases/InfectionsAZ/Tetanus/EpidemiologicalData/tet01TetanusCasesAge/. Accessed 2014 Feb 28.
  15. 15. Savage EJ, Nash S, McGuinness A, Crowcroft NS (2007) Audit of tetanus prevention knowledge and practices in accident and emergency departments in England. Emerg Med J 24:417–421.
  16. 16. Honkanen PO, Keistinen T, Kivela SL (1996) Factors associated with influenza vaccination coverage among the elderly: Role of health care personnel. Public Health 110:163–168.
  17. 17. Telford R, Rogers A (2003) What influences elderly peoples' decisions about whether to accept the influenza vaccination? A qualitative study. Health Educ Res 18:743–753.
  18. 18. Mangtani P, Breeze E, Stirling S, Hanciles S, Kovats S, et al. (2006) Cross-sectional survey of older peoples' views related to influenza vaccine uptake. BMC Public Health 6:249.
  19. 19. Keenan H, Campbell J, Evans PH (2007) Influenza vaccination in patients with asthma: Why is the uptake so low? Brit J Gen Prac 57:359–363.
  20. 20. Bekker HL, Gough D, Williams M (2003) Attendance choices about the influenza immunization programme: Evidence for targeting patients' beliefs. Psychol Health Med 8:279–288.
  21. 21. Evans MR, Prout H, Prior L, Tapper-Jones LM, Butler CC (2007) A qualitative study of lay beliefs about influenza immunisation in older people. Brit J Gen Pract 57:352.
  22. 22. Kwong EW-y, Pang SM-c, Choi P-p, Wong TK-s (2010) Influenza vaccine preference and uptake among older people in nine countries. J Adv Nurs 66:2297–2308.
  23. 23. Telford R, Rogers A (2003) What influences elderly peoples' decisions about whether to accept the influenza vaccination? A qualitative study. Health Educ Res 18:743–753.
  24. 24. Böhmer M, Walter D, Krause G, Müters S, Gößwald A, et al. (2011) Determinants of tetanus and seasonal influenza vaccine uptake in adults living in Germany. Human Vaccines 7:1317–1325.
  25. 25. Johnson DR, Nichol KL, Lipczynski K (2008) Barriers to adult immunization. Am Journal Med 121:S28–S35.
  26. 26. Wheelock A, Miraldo M, Parand A, Vincent C, Sevdalis N (2014) Journey to vaccination: a protocol for a multinational qualitative study. BMJ Open 4.
  27. 27. Riphagen Dalhuisen J (2012) Predictors of seasonal influenza vaccination among healthcare workers in hospitals: a descriptive meta-analysis. Occup Environ Med 69:230.
  28. 28. Meharry PM, Colson ER, Grizas AP, Stiller R, Vázquez M (2013) Reasons why women accept or reject the trivalent inactivated influenza vaccine (TIV) during pregnancy. Matern Child Health J 17:156–164.
  29. 29. Coughlin, SS (1990) Recall bias in epidemiologic studies. J Clin Epidemiol 43: 87-91.
  30. 30. Leask J, Kinnersley P, Jackson C, Cheater F, Bedford H, et al. (2012) Communicating with parents about vaccination: a framework for health professionals. BMC Pediatr 12:154.
  31. 31. Reynolds TJ, Olson JC (2001) Understanding consumer decision making: the means-end approach to marketing and advertising strategy. New Jersey: Psychology Press.
  32. 32. Pope C, Ziebland S, Mays N (2000) Analysing qualitative data. BMJ 320:114–116.
  33. 33. Silverman D (2001) Interpreting qualitative data: methods for analyzing talk, text and interaction. London: Sage.
  34. 34. Flick U (2009) An introduction to qualitative research. London: Sage.
  35. 35. Advisory Committee on Immunization Practices (2013) Recommended Adult Immunization Schedule: United States, 2013. Ann Intern Med 158:191–199.
  36. 36. NHS (2014) Vaccination schedule. Available: http://www.nhs.uk/Conditions/vaccinations/Pages/vaccination-schedule-age-checklist.aspx. Accessed 2014 Feb 28.
  37. 37. Maurer J, Uscher-Pines L, Harris KM (2010) Perceived seriousness of seasonal and A(H1N1) influenzas, attitudes toward vaccination, and vaccine uptake among U.S. adults: Does the source of information matter? Prev Med 51:185–187.
  38. 38. Nickerson RS (1998) Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises. Rev Gen Psychol 2:175.
  39. 39. Brewer NT, Chapman GB, Gibbons FX, Gerrard M, McCaul KD, et al. (2007) Meta-analysis of the relationship between risk perception and health behavior: the example of vaccination. Health Psychol 26:136.
  40. 40. Wheelock A, Thomson A, Sevdalis N (2013) Social and psychological factors underlying adult vaccination behavior: lessons from seasonal influenza vaccination in the US and the UK. Expert Rev Vaccines 1–9.
  41. 41. Nagata JM, Hernández-Ramos I, Kurup AS, Albrecht D, Vivas-Torrealba C, et al. (2013) Social determinants of health and seasonal influenza vaccination in adults> = 65 years: a systematic review of qualitative and quantitative data. BMC Public Health 13:388.
  42. 42. Thomas RE, Russell M, Lorenzetti D (2010) Interventions to increase influenza vaccination rates of those 60 years and older in the community. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 8:CD005188.
  43. 43. Chamberlain DB (1989) Babies remember pain. J Prenat Perinat Psychol Health 3:297–310.
  44. 44. Taddio A, Katz J, Ilersich AL, Koren G (1997) Effect of neonatal circumcision on pain response during subsequent routine vaccination. Lancet 349:599–603.
  45. 45. PHE (2011) The UK immunisation programme: The Green Book, chapter 11. Available: http://webarchive.nationalarchives.gov.uk/20130123170526/https://www.wp.dh.gov.uk/immunisation/files/2012/07/chapter-11.pdf. Accessed 2014 Feb 28.
  46. 46. PHE (2013) Tetanus: The Green Book, chapter 30. Available: https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/tetanus-the-green-book-chapter-30. Accessed 2014 Feb 28.
  47. 47. Ramet J, Weil-Olivier C, Sedlak W (2007) Influenza vaccination: The paediatric perspective. Vaccine 25:780–787.
  48. 48. CDC (2014) Children, the Flu, and the Flu Vaccine. Available: http://www.cdc.gov/flu/protect/children.htm. Accessed 2014 Feb 28.
  49. 49. Cairns G, MacDonald L, Angus K, Walker L, Cairns-Haylor T, et al.. (2012) Systematic literature review of the evidence for effective national immunisation schedule promotional communications. Stockholm: ECDC.
  50. 50. Dexter LJ, Teare MD, Dexter M, Siriwardena AN, Read RC (2012) Strategies to increase influenza vaccination rates: outcomes of a nationwide cross-sectional survey of UK general practice. BMJ Open 2.