Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Where You Live May Make You Old: The Association between Perceived Poor Neighborhood Quality and Leukocyte Telomere Length

  • Mijung Park ,

    parkm@pitt.edu

    Affiliation Department of Health and Community Systems, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America

  • Josine E. Verhoeven,

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry and EMGO Institute for Health and Care Research, VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

  • Pim Cuijpers,

    Affiliation Department of Clinical Psychology and EMGO Institute for Health and Care Research, VU University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

  • Charles F. Reynolds III,

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America

  • Brenda W. J. H. Penninx

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry and EMGO Institute for Health and Care Research, VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

Abstract

Background

Strong evidence supports that living in disadvantaged neighborhoods has direct unfavorable impact on mental and physical health. However, whether it also has direct impact on cellular health is largely unknown. Thus we examined whether neighborhood quality was associated with leukocyte telomere length, an indicator of cellular aging.

Methods

In May 2014, we extracted and analyzed baseline data from the Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety (NESDA), a large epidemiological study of individuals age between 18–65 years (n=2902). Telomere length was determined using quantitative polymerase chain reaction. Neighborhood quality was assessed using modified measures of perceived neighborhood disorder, fear of crime, and noise. We used multivariable linear regression models to examine association between perceived neighborhood quality and telomere length with comprehensive adjustment for individual and community characteristics related to socioeconomic and demographic status, urbanization level, mental and physical health, and lifestyle.

Results

Compared to individuals who reported good neighborhood quality, the mean telomere length of those who reported moderate neighborhood quality was approximately 69 base pair shorter (β =-69.33, 95% CI: -119.49, -19.17, p= 0.007), and that of those who reported poor neighborhood quality were 174 base pair shorter (β =-173.80, 95% CI: -298.80, -49.01, p=0.006). For illustrative purposes, one could extrapolate these outcomes to 8.7 and 11.9 years in chronological age, respectively.

Conclusion

We have established an association between perceived neighborhood quality and cellular aging over and above a range of individual attributes. Biological aging processes may be impacted by socioeconomic milieu.

Introduction

Disadvantaged living situations are harmful to health and shorten life expectancy. Evidence from numerous studies and meta-analyses confirms that individuals living in poor and underprivileged neighborhoods experience increased morbidity [13], disability [48], and mortality [4]. What is largely unknown, however, is whether living in a disadvantaged neighborhood has health impacts on a cellular level.

Leukocyte telomere is nucleic acid-protein complexes at the ends of DNA. Telomere length (TL hereafter) is a biomarker for cellular aging and an indicator of cumulative biological stress [9]. Shorter TL has been associated with mortality [10] and increased risk for a range of morbidities such as cancer [11], heart disease [12], depressive disorders [13], and anxiety disorders [14]. An emerging body of literature has identified demographic, socioeconomic, lifestyle, clinical and environmental factors associated with TL shortening. While TL is shortened continually as we age, male than female and Caucasian race than other races tend to have shorter TL [1517]. A study showed that widowed, divorced, separated or never married individuals had shorter TL than their married or partnered counterparts [18]. Although inconclusive, existing data suggests that lower educational attainment and lower income level are associated with shortened TL [16]. Unhealthy lifestyles, such as smoking [19, 20], drinking [21] and lower energy expenditure [22, 23] are also associated with TL shortening. Some clinical factors, such as greater Body Mass Index [19] and psychological stress [24] (e.g., early life exposure to trauma [25], and experience of racism [26]), have been linked with shortened TL. A recent study also found that individuals living in areas where less green spaces had shorter TL than their counterparts [27]

Understanding relations between the neighborhood quality and TL is novel and important for several reasons. First, although strong body of literature supports the negative health consequences of disadvantaged living situations, its underlying biological mechanism has not been fully examined. Moreover, the majority of research about health outcomes among individuals living in disadvantaged neighborhood is largely grounded on the psychological stress framework or has had relatively narrow focus specific to stress hormones [4]. Although such research has made a tremendous contribution to our understanding, it has limited power to explain if and how social circumstances intersect with biological processes. Second, existing studies on cellular aging have exclusively concentrated on factors internal to the individual, such as mental and physical disease status or life experience [4]. Thus, identifying factors external to the individual that are associated with telomere lengths may help us to understand the complex interplay of cellular aging and health. Lately, researchers have examined the relationship between cellular aging and social characteristics measured at the level of the individual (e.g., socioeconomic status such as education level and income), yet the results have been inconclusive [28]. Few studies, however, have examined the association between cellular aging and social characteristics measured at the level of neighborhood.

The purpose of this study was to examine if perceived neighborhood quality is associated with TL. Our primary hypothesis was that poor perceived neighborhood quality would be associated with shortened TL. Our secondary hypothesis was that each sub-domain of perceived neighborhood quality (i.e., perceived neighborhood disorders, fear of crime, and noise) would be associated with TL.

Theoretical Framework

Our hypotheses were guided by the neighborhood disorder model [29, 30] and a biological framework [3133]. Fig 1 describes our hypothesized conceptual model of potential pathways linking poor neighborhood quality and shortened TL. Disadvantaged neighborhoods are characterized by high level of neighborhood disorders (e.g. violent crime and vandalism), increased risk for toxic exposure (e.g., noise), and limited access to resources and economic and educational opportunities. Consequently, residents of such neighborhoods are exposed to greater level of stress, and experience poor mental and physical health outcomes [34]. Furthermore, poor neighborhood quality may discourage healthy lifestyle (e.g., physical activity) and increase unhealthy lifestyles (e.g. drinking and smoking)[35]. Moreover, the residents of disadvantaged neighborhood are likely to experience allostatic load (the wear and tear on the body) or chronic activation of the physiological stress response and overexposure to stress hormones, which in turn accelerating telomere shortening [3133]. Additionally, poor environmental conditions in disadvantaged neighborhood, such as toxic exposures and poor nutrition, can also produce epigenetic modifications [36] and accelerate cellular aging. Conversely, individuals with mental and/or physical disorders are more likely to live in disadvantaged neighborhoods due to functional disability borne from such conditions.

Materials and Methods

Study sample

The Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety (NESDA) is an ongoing longitudinal cohort study examining the course and consequences of depressive and anxiety disorders. The NESDA sample consists of 2,981 community-residing persons between 18 and 65 years. Participants were recruited between September 2004 and February 2007. The majority (about 95%) of NESDA sample were northern European ancestry. Individuals who had primary clinical diagnosis of severe persistent mental illness (i.e. psychotic disorders, obsessive-compulsive disorder, bipolar disorder, or severe addition disorder) were excluded from the study. Individuals who were not fluent in Dutch language were excluded due to limited funding for translating instruments and for training bilingual interviewers. Also, several instruments were not validated in many languages. Data were collected in four waves: at baseline, 2-, 4-, and 6-year follow-up. The current analyses included baseline data because information on TL was available only at baseline. Baseline assessments included 4-hour face-to-face interview with trained interviewers using computer-assisted modules. In May 2014, to conduct this study, we extracted and analyzed observations collected at baseline with complete data on neighborhood and telomere length (n = 2901). The design and conduct of NESDA have been extensively described in greater detail elsewhere [37]. All participants signed informed consent. The NESDA study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the following centers: VU University medical center, University Medical Center Groningen and the Leiden University Medical Center.

Measures

Telomere Length (TL).

Leukocyte TL was determined using quantitative polymerase chain reaction (qPCR), adapted from the published original method by Cawthon et al [38] and modified by Lin et al [39]. Telomere sequence copy number in each patient’s sample (T) was compared to a single-copy gene number (S). A single copy gene is a non-telomeric reference sequence that is autosomal (i.e., not a sex chromosome) and non-variable in copy number [40]. In this study, human beta-globin was used as the single-gene copy. The T/S ratio is relative to a reference sample and is proportional to the mean TL.

To estimate T/S ratio, fasting blood was drawn from participants in the morning between 8:30am and 9:30am. DNA samples were stored in a −80°C freezer afterwards. Short specific parts of the template DNA (i.e., telomere sequence and human beta-globin sequence) were amplified using enzyme in cycles. These templates were fluorescently labelled. In every cycle, the number of the template DNAs is doubled, leading to an exponential amplification of target. The amount of the fluorescence released during the amplification is directly proportional to the amount of amplified DNA. We converted T/S ratio into absolute length in base pairs (bp) using the following formulae: bp = 3274-2413x((T/S-0.0545)/1.16). Detailed procedures on collection and storage of blood sample, and extraction and measurement of leukocyte telomere length in NESDA have been fully described elsewhere [13]. TL assaying was conducted at the laboratory of Telome Health Inc. (Menlo Park, CA, USA).

Neighborhood Quality.

We assessed two dimensions of neighborhood quality: general appraisal of neighborhood and perceived neighborhood quality. General appraisal of neighborhood, adapted from the Social Capital Benchmark Survey [41], was assessed with a question “How would you rate your neighborhood?” We reverse coded the possible answers so that higher scale values indicated more positive appraisal of neighborhood. The possible scores ranged from bad (1), moderate (2), fair (3), good (4), and very good (5).

Perceived neighborhood quality was assessed using three questions. The first two questions were adapted from the scale of perceived neighborhood disorders developed by Ross and Mirowsky [30], and the other was the fear of crime question developed by Baker et al [42]. Each question assessed individual perception of noise, fear of crime and vandalism in the neighborhood: “How often do you experience noise from neighbors, traffic or other sources in your neighborhood (noise)?” “How often do you feel unsafe when you walk alone in your neighborhood (fear of crime)?” and “How often do you see vandalism in your neighborhood such as damaged property (vandalism)?” The possible scores for each question ranged from never [1], seldom [2], sometimes [3], regularly [4], and often [5]. We summed and transformed the values so that the perceived neighborhood quality scores ranged between 0 and 12, using the following formula: perceived neighborhood quality = (noise + fear of crime + vandalism)- 3. Higher scale values indicate greater degrees of negative neighborhood quality (Cronbach’s α = 0.65). In addition to the 12-point scale, we also created a categorical variable in order to visualize findings and check for potential non-linear trends based on the perceived neighborhood quality scores: good neighborhood quality (0–4), moderate neighborhood quality (5–8), and poor neighborhood quality (9–12).

Demographic and socioeconomic characteristics.

Demographic and socioeconomic characteristics included age (range between18 and 65), gender, currently married/partnered or not, level of education in years, the duration of living in the current address (in years), and poverty status. Poverty status was dichotomized based on monthly household income relative to household size. NESDA collected monthly household income ranging from less than 600 Euros to greater than 5000 Euros, with 200 Euro increment. Individuals living alone (i.e., single households) with monthly income less than 800 Euros per month was coded as living in poverty; all other household composition with monthly household income less than 1200 Euro was coded as living in poverty. This dichotomization was based on the Consensual Budget Study conducted by the Netherlands Institute for Social Research and the National Institute for Family Finance Information [43].

Community characteristic.

Community characteristic was measured via the zip code based level of urbanization, classified by the Dutch Office for Statistics (CBS) [44]. The level of urbanization is based on the number of persons living per square kilometer: no urbanization [fewer than 500], hardly urbanization [500–1000], moderate urbanization [1000–1500], strong urbanization [1500–2500], and extreme urbanization [more than 2500]).

Clinical characteristics.

Clinical characteristics included Body Mass Index (underweight, normal weight, over weight and obese), and the number of chronic diseases. We also adjusted for psychiatric indicators, given the nature of this sample and the earlier findings in this study in which depressive and anxiety disorders have been linked to shorter telomeres [13]. We used information on the Inventory of Depression Symptomatology (IDS, range 0–80), the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI, range 0–62), and the presence of lifetime major depressive disorder and panic disorder was confirmed using the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 4th Edition (DSM-IV) criteria according to the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI, version 2.1).

Lifestyle characteristics.

Lifestyle characteristics included overall energy expenditure in Metabolic Equivalent Total (MET) hours per week [45] and smoking status (never smoked vs. former smoker vs. current smoker). MET is a resting metabolic rate obtained during quiet sitting and used as a method to indicate and compare the absolute intensity and energy expenditure of different physical activities. [46] It is expressed as the oxygen consumption (VO2) of approximately 3.5 ml/kg/min [45]. The level of energy consumption associate an activity is expressed multiples of resting MET level. For example, light home activities are 2.3 METs, while competitive bicycling is 16 METs [45]. We calculated MET hours per week using following formula: MET hours per week = (MET level* minutes of activity*number of events per week)/60.

Statistical Analysis

We first examined the distributions of variables of interest using descriptive statistics. Second, the associations between these variables and TL were examined using linear regression modeling with adjustment for age and gender. This decision was made because it is well documented that age and sex were strongly associated with TL [16, 17]. We also examined the associations between each variable of interest and perceived neighborhood quality scores using unadjusted linear regression modeling. The outcomes of these models were presented in online supplements 1 and 2. Third, to examine the associations between the perceived neighborhood quality and TL, we built five linear regression models with TL as the dependent variable. To examine the effects of potential confounders, we sequentially added a different set of covariates mentioned above, and examined the changes in the magnitude of the associations between TL and each perceived neighborhood quality variable. The first model included age and gender as covariates. Then, we added socioeconomic status, community characteristics, clinical, and life-style related characteristics sequentially. NESDA over sampled individuals with current or lifetime depression. To examine if past or current diagnoses of depression and anxiety disorder modify the associations between perceived neighborhood quality and TL, we added the following interaction terms: each perceived neighborhood quality variable* IDS, each neighborhood quality variable* BAI, each neighborhood quality variable*lifetime major depression, each neighborhood quality variable*lifetime anxiety disorder. Finally, to examine is each sub-domain of perceived quality was associated with TL, we built three linear regression models with each subdomain of perceived neighborhood quality (i.e., noise; fear of crime; and vandalism) as the dependent variable. All analyses were performed with Stata MP version 12.

Results

The mean TL for the sample was 5472.00 (SD±639.83) base pair (bp). The mean age of sample was 41.90 (SD±13.06). The majority was female (66.4%) and had lifetime diagnosis of depression (64.4%) or anxiety disorder (59.2%)(See Table 1). The mean scores of general appraisal of neighborhood and of perceived neighborhood quality were 3.98 (SD±0.89), and 3.79 (SD±2.32), respectively. The mean scores of noise, fear of crime, and vandalism were 2.78 (SD±1.12), 1.64 (SD±0.84), and 2.37 (SD±1.05), respectively.

After adjusting for age and gender, we found that TL was associated with perceived neighborhood quality and each of its domains. On the other hands, we did not find significant association between TL, general appraisal of neighborhood, the number of years living in the current house and the level of urbanization. Inventory of depressive symptom scores and Beck anxiety scale scores were associated with TL and with perceived neighborhood quality (See S1 and S2 Tables).

Table 2 summarizes the multiple multivariable linear regression models with TL as the dependent variable. There were significant linear associations between the perceived neighborhood quality and TL. In the fully adjusted model, a one-point increase in perceived neighborhood quality was associated with approximately 12 bp shortening of TL (b = -12.14, 95% CI: -22.55, -1.72, p = 0.022). In other words, compared to those reporting good neighborhood quality, those who reported moderate neighborhood quality had approximately 69 bp shorter TL (b = -69.33, 95% CI: -119.49, -19.17, p = 0.007). We did not find any significant interaction term mentioned in the previous section.

thumbnail
Table 2. Associations between Telomere Length and Perceived Neighborhood Quality.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0128460.t002

The difference in mean TL between those reporting good neighborhood quality and poor neighborhood quality was about 174 bp (b = -173.80, 95% CI: -298.80,- 49.01 p = 0.006). The difference in mean TL between those reporting moderate and poor neighborhood quality was not statistically significant (p = 0.111). Fig 2 presents mean TL and 95% CIs for the three groups based on the neighborhood quality scale after adjusting for full set of covariates.

thumbnail
Fig 2. The Association between Telomere Length and Perceived Neighborhood Quality.

After adjusting for a comprehensive set of covariates, those living in greater unfavorable neighborhoods quality had shorter telomere length.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0128460.g002

Discussion

Our primary hypothesis was that poor neighborhood quality would be associated with shortened TL. We found that overall index scores of perceived neighborhood quality were significantly associated with accelerated cellular aging even after adjusting for comprehensive sets of covariates: demographic, socioeconomic, community, clinical, and lifestyle characteristics. We also observed a dose response of poor perceived neighborhood quality to cellular aging; the greater degrees of unfavorable perceived neighborhood quality, the shorter the mean TL was. Based on an estimated mean telomere shortening rate of 14–20 bp per year as found in this and other studies [47], the differences observed indicate 8.7–11.9 years of accelerated cellular aging for individuals reporting poor neighborhood quality as compared to those reporting good neighborhood quality. On the other hand, we did not find significant associations between general appraisal of neighborhood and TL. This may be due to that fact that the majority of sample (n = 2,248, 77.5%) rated their neighborhood very good or good, while only 149 respondents (6.65%) rated their neighborhood moderate or bad. Such high degrees of satisfaction with neighborhood in overall sample may have lowered statistical power to detect meaningful differences in TL between varying degrees of general appraisal of neighborhood.

Our secondary hypothesis was that each sub-domain of perceived neighborhood quality would be independently associated with TL. We found that, while fear of crime and vandalism were significantly associated with TL, noise was not associated with the TL after adjusting for age, gender, and demographic characteristics. This finding seems to suggest that different neighborhood characteristics have differential impacts on cellular aging. In other words, unfavorable emotional milieu, such as living in fear of crime, may have more direct impact on cellular aging than the high levels of perceived noise.

To our knowledge, this is one of three studies examining the associations between perceived neighborhood quality and cellular aging. Although each study used a unique study sample and measurements, these studies collectively establish the association between TL and the neighborhood quality perceived by residents. Theall et al [48] examined salivary TL in a sample of 99 children age between 4 and 14 recruited from urban New Orleans, Louisiana. The authors concluded that children living in disadvantaged neighborhoods (greater level of neighborhood disorder by the mother’s appraisal) had lower salivary TL than those who did not. Needham et al [49] examined the associations between several neighborhood characteristics and leukocyte TL, using multi-ethnic U.S. sample aged between 45 and 84. The authors concluded that individuals who lived in neighborhood with lower aesthetic quality, safety, and social cohesion had shortened TL than those who lived in neighborhoods with a more favorable social environment, after controlling for individual-level socioeconomic status, neighborhood socioeconomic disadvantage, and biomedical and lifestyle risk factors. In all three studies, interestingly, the authors did not find significant associations between TL and objective measures of neighborhood quality: neighborhood-level economic deprivation (Theall et al), neighborhood SES (Neehan et al), and population density (current study). More studies are needed to examine why subjective indicators, but not objective indicators, of neighborhood quality are associated with TL.

A unique contribution of our study is that we extensively examined effects of depression and anxiety in the association between perceived neighborhood quality and TL. Having depression and anxiety may “paint” one’s surroundings more negative tones. Thus, the association between the perceived neighborhood quality and TL found in the literature could have been a reflection of one’s emotional status. However, we found that the association between perceived neighborhood quality and TL remained significant even after adjusting for four measures of depression and anxiety (i.e., depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, lifetime diagnoses of depression and of anxiety disorders). Furthermore, we did not find significantly modifying effects of depression and anxiety in such association. Our finding suggests that the relation between the perceived neighborhood quality and TL is not entirely a reflection of one’s emotional status. Moreover, our finding may be confirmative of the general notion that perceived neighborhood quality is indicative of objective neighborhood quality. Existing literature has found a good correlation between objective and subjective measures of neighborhood quality in general population [5052]. For example, individuals living in high crime areas report high levels of fear of crime [5355]. Our study suggests that these may be also true in the sample including a large number of individuals with depression and anxiety disorders.

There are few limitations in this study. First, due to the cross-sectional nature of this study, we are unable to make causal inferences about the relations between TL and perceived neighborhood quality. Second, although time at current residence was controlled for, we were not able to examine if and how changes in neighborhood quality over time impacted TL. Third, due to the sample characteristics, generalizability of our findings to older adults and to racially diverse populations may be limited. The NESDA baseline sample does not include individuals older than 65. Also, only a small number of participants (n = 146; 5.03%) identified themselves as non-northern European ancestries. Fourth, psychometric property of the perceived neighborhood quality (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.65) is not very strong. This may be due to the fact it comprise only three items. Also, the participants generally rated their neighborhood favorably with relatively small variations. For example, the mean score of fear of crime was 1.64 (SD±0.84). This may have lowered our statistical power.

Despite above limitations, our study is an important addition to growing body of literature on factors associated with cellular aging. In this study we have established that certain neighborhood characteristics matter for cellular aging over and above a range of individual attributes. The findings of this study are indicative of potential pathways that have not been previously examined and offer new directions for measurement and research on cellular aging. Furthermore, biological aging studies that focus purely on individual characteristics may have limited power to explain important correlates and determinants of aging and of health. This study has shown the importance of integrating research on social processes related to health and cellular aging.

Supporting Information

S1. Table. Associations between Variables of Interest and Telomere Length.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0128460.s001

(DOCX)

S2. Table. Associations between Variables of Interest and Perceived Neighborhood Quality.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0128460.s002

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The infrastructure for the NESDA study (www.nesda.nl) is funded through the Geestkracht program of the Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development (Zon-Mw, grant number 10-000-1002) and is supported by VU University Medical Center, University Medical Center Groningen, Leiden University Medical Center and participating mental health care organizations. Drs. Verhoeven, Penninx and telomere length assaying were supported through an NWO-VICI grant (number 91811602). Dr. Reynolds is supported by P30 MH90333. He also reports receiving pharmaceutical support for NIH-sponsored research studies from Bristol-Myers Squibb, Forest, Pfizer, and Lilly; receiving grants from the National Institute of Mental Health, National Institute on Aging, National Center for Minority Health Disparities, National Heart Lung and Blood Institute, Center for Medicare and Medicaid Services, Patient Centered Outcomes Research Institute (PCORI), the Commonwealth of Pennsylvania, the John A Hartford Foundation, National Palliative Care Research Center, Clinical and Translational Science Institute, and the American Foundation for Suicide Prevention. He is serving on the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry editorial review board. He has received an honorarium as a speaker from MedScape/WEB MD. He is the co-inventor (Licensed Intellectual Property) of Psychometric analysis of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) PRO10050447 (PI: Buysse). Dr. Park is supported by National Institute of Health (K01NR015101). Dr. Cuijpers have nothing to declare.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: MP JV BP. Analyzed the data: MP. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: JV BP. Wrote the paper: MP JV PC CFR BP.

References

  1. 1. Mair C, Diez Roux AV, Galea S. Are neighbourhood characteristics associated with depressive symptoms? A review of evidence. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2008;62(11):940–6, 8 p following 6. pmid:18775943.
  2. 2. Latkin CA, Curry AD. Stressful neighborhoods and depression: a prospective study of the impact of neighborhood disorder. Journal of health and social behavior. 2003:34–44. pmid:12751309
  3. 3. Freedman VA, Grafova IB, Rogowski J. Neighborhoods and chronic disease onset in later life. Am J Public Health. 2011;101(1):79–86. pmid:20299643; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2912970.
  4. 4. Balfour JL, Kaplan GA. Neighborhood environment and loss of physical function in older adults: evidence from the Alameda County Study. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2002;155(6):507–15. pmid:11882524
  5. 5. Lynch J, Smith GD, Harper SA, Hillemeier M, Ross N, Kaplan GA, et al. Is income inequality a determinant of population health? Part 1. A systematic review. Milbank Quarterly. 2004;82(1):5–99. pmid:15016244
  6. 6. Feng J, Glass TA, Curriero FC, Stewart WF, Schwartz BS. The built environment and obesity: a systematic review of the epidemiologic evidence. Health & place. 2010;16(2):175–90.
  7. 7. Lovasi GS, Hutson MA, Guerra M, Neckerman KM. Built environments and obesity in disadvantaged populations. Epidemiologic reviews. 2009;31(1):7–20.
  8. 8. Steenland K, Henley J, Calle E, Thun M. Individual-and area-level socioeconomic status variables as predictors of mortality in a cohort of 179,383 persons. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2004;159(11):1047–56. pmid:15155289
  9. 9. Sapolsky RM. Organismal stress and telomeric aging: An unexpected connection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2004;101(50):17323–4. pmid:15579535
  10. 10. Heidinger BJ, Blount JD, Boner W, Griffiths K, Metcalfe NB, Monaghan P. Telomere length in early life predicts lifespan. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2012;109(5):1743–8. pmid:22232671
  11. 11. Willeit P, Willeit J, Kloss-Brandstätter A, Kronenberg F, Kiechl S. Fifteen-year follow-up of association between telomere length and incident cancer and cancer mortality. JAMA: the journal of the American Medical Association. 2011;306(1):42–4.
  12. 12. Fitzpatrick AL, Kronmal RA, Gardner JP, Psaty BM, Jenny NS, Tracy RP, et al. Leukocyte telomere length and cardiovascular disease in the cardiovascular health study. Am J Epidemiol. 2007;165(1):14–21. pmid:17043079.
  13. 13. Verhoeven J, Révész D, Epel E, Lin J, Wolkowitz O, Penninx B. Major depressive disorder and accelerated cellular aging: results from a large psychiatric cohort study. Molecular psychiatry. 2013.
  14. 14. Hoen P, Rosmalen J, Schoevers R, Huzen J, van der Harst P, de Jonge P. Association between anxiety but not depressive disorders and leukocyte telomere length after 2 years of follow-up in a population-based sample. Psychological medicine. 2013;43(04):689–97.
  15. 15. Sanders JL, Newman AB. Telomere length in epidemiology: a biomarker of aging, age-related disease, both, or neither? Epidemiologic reviews. 2013;35(1):112–31.
  16. 16. Starkweather AR, Alhaeeri AA, Montpetit A, Brumelle J, Filler K, Montpetit M, et al. An Integrative Review of Factors Associated with Telomere Length and Implications for Biobehavioral Research. Nursing research. 2014;63(1):36–50. pmid:24335912
  17. 17. Gardner M, Bann D, Wiley L, Cooper R, Hardy R, Nitsch D, et al. Gender and telomere length: systematic review and meta-analysis. Experimental gerontology. 2014;51:15–27. pmid:24365661
  18. 18. Mainous AG, Everett CJ, Diaz VA, Baker R, Mangino M, Codd V, et al. Leukocyte telomere length and marital status among middle-aged adults. Age and ageing. 2010:afq118.
  19. 19. Valdes A, Andrew T, Gardner J, Kimura M, Oelsner E, Cherkas L, et al. Obesity, cigarette smoking, and telomere length in women. The Lancet. 2005;366(9486):662–4. pmid:16112303
  20. 20. Morla M, Busquets X, Pons J, Sauleda J, MacNee W, Agusti A. Telomere shortening in smokers with and without COPD. European Respiratory Journal. 2006;27(3):525–8. pmid:16507852
  21. 21. Pavanello S, Hoxha M, Dioni L, Bertazzi PA, Snenghi R, Nalesso A, et al. Shortened telomeres in individuals with abuse in alcohol consumption. International Journal of Cancer. 2011;129(4):983–92. pmid:21351086
  22. 22. Ludlow AT, Zimmerman JB, Witkowski S, Hearn JW, Hatfield BD, Roth SM. Relationship between physical activity level, telomere length, and telomerase activity. Medicine and science in sports and exercise. 2008;40(10):1764. pmid:18799986
  23. 23. Du M, Prescott J, Kraft P, Han J, Giovannucci E, Hankinson SE, et al. Physical activity, sedentary behavior, and leukocyte telomere length in women. American journal of epidemiology. 2012:kwr330.
  24. 24. Epel ES, Blackburn EH, Lin J, Dhabhar FS, Adler NE, Morrow JD, et al. Accelerated telomere shortening in response to life stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101(49):17312–5. pmid:15574496; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC534658.
  25. 25. Price LH, Kao HT, Burgers DE, Carpenter LL, Tyrka AR. Telomeres and early-life stress: an overview. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;73(1):15–23. pmid:22831981; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3495091.
  26. 26. Chae DH, Nuru-Jeter AM, Adler NE, Brody GH, Lin J, Blackburn EH, et al. Discrimination, racial bias, and telomere length in African-American men. Am J Prev Med. 2014;46(2):103–11. pmid:24439343.
  27. 27. Woo J, Tang N, Suen E, Leung J, Wong M. Green space, psychological restoration, and telomere length. The Lancet. 2009;373(9660):299–300.
  28. 28. Robertson T, Batty GD, Der G, Fenton C, Shiels PG, Benzeval M. Is socioeconomic status associated with biological aging as measured by telomere length? Epidemiologic reviews. 2013;35(1):98–111.
  29. 29. Skogan WG. Disorder and decline: Crime and the spiral of decay in American neighborhoods: Univ of California Press; 1992.
  30. 30. Ross CE, Mirowsky J. Neighborhood disadvantage, disorder, and health. Journal of health and social behavior. 2001:258–76. pmid:11668773
  31. 31. O'Donovan A, Pantell MS, Puterman E, Dhabhar FS, Blackburn EH, Yaffe K, et al. Cumulative inflammatory load is associated with short leukocyte telomere length in the Health, Aging and Body Composition Study. PloS one. 2011;6(5):e19687. pmid:21602933
  32. 32. Epel ES, Lin J, Wilhelm FH, Wolkowitz OM, Cawthon R, Adler NE, et al. Cell aging in relation to stress arousal and cardiovascular disease risk factors. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2006;31(3):277–87. pmid:16298085
  33. 33. Tomiyama AJ, O'Donovan A, Lin J, Puterman E, Lazaro A, Chan J, et al. Does cellular aging relate to patterns of allostasis?: An examination of basal and stress reactive HPA axis activity and telomere length. Physiology & behavior. 2012;106(1):40–5.
  34. 34. Aneshensel CS. Neighborhood as a social context of the stress process. Advances in the Conceptualization of the Stress Process: Springer; 2010. p. 35–52.
  35. 35. Hill TD, Angel RJ. Neighborhood disorder, psychological distress, and heavy drinking. Social Science & Medicine. 2005;61(5):965–75.
  36. 36. Rutter M, Moffitt TE, Caspi A. Gene–environment interplay and psychopathology: multiple varieties but real effects. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47(3‐4):226–61.
  37. 37. Penninx BW, Beekman AT, Smit JH, Zitman FG, Nolen WA, Spinhoven P, et al. The Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety (NESDA): rationale, objectives and methods. International journal of methods in psychiatric research. 2008;17(3):121–40. pmid:18763692
  38. 38. Cawthon RM, Smith KR, O'Brien E, Sivatchenko A, Kerber RA. Association between telomere length in blood and mortality in people aged 60 years or older. The Lancet. 2003;361(9355):393–5. pmid:12573379
  39. 39. Lin J, Epel E, Cheon J, Kroenke C, Sinclair E, Bigos M, et al. Analyses and comparisons of telomerase activity and telomere length in human T and B cells: insights for epidemiology of telomere maintenance. Journal of immunological methods. 2010;352(1):71–80.
  40. 40. Nussey DH, Baird D, Barrett E, Boner W, Fairlie J, Gemmell N, et al. Measuring telomere length and telomere dynamics in evolutionary biology and ecology. Methods in Ecology and Evolution. 2014;5(4):299–310. pmid:25834722
  41. 41. Seminar S, editor. The social capital community benchmark survey2001: Saguaro Seminar, John F. Kennedy School of Government, Harvard University.
  42. 42. Baker MH, Nienstedt BC, Everett RS, McCleary R. Impact of a Crime Wave: Perceptions, Fear, and Confidence in the Police, The. Law & Soc'y Rev. 1982;17:319.
  43. 43. Hoff S, van Gaalen C, Soede A, Luten A, Vrooman C, Lamers S. The minimum agreed upon. 2010.
  44. 44. CBS. Available: http://www.cbs.nl/nl-NL/menu/methoden/begrippen/default.htm?ConceptID=658.
  45. 45. Ainsworth BE, Haskell WL, Herrmann SD, Meckes N, Bassett DR Jr., Tudor-Locke C, et al. 2011 Compendium of Physical Activities: a second update of codes and MET values. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2011;43(8):1575–81. pmid:21681120.
  46. 46. American College of Sports Medicine. Position stand on the recommended quantity and quality of exercise for developing and maintaining cardiorespiratory muscular fitness in healthy adults. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1990;22:265–74. pmid:2355825
  47. 47. Müezzinler A, Zaineddin AK, Brenner H. A systematic review of leukocyte telomere length and age in adults. Ageing research reviews. 2013;12(2):509–19. pmid:23333817
  48. 48. Theall KP, Brett ZH, Shirtcliff EA, Dunn EC, Drury SS. Neighborhood disorder and telomeres: Connecting children's exposure to community level stress and cellular response. Social Science & Medicine. 2013;85:50–8.
  49. 49. Needham BL, Carroll JE, Roux AVD, Fitzpatrick AL, Moore K, Seeman TE. Neighborhood characteristics and leukocyte telomere length: The Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis. Health & place. 2014;28:167–72.
  50. 50. Echeverria MSE, Diez-Roux AV, Link BG. Reliability of self-reported neighborhood characteristics. Journal of Urban Health. 2004;81(4):682–701. pmid:15466849
  51. 51. Sampson RJ, Raudenbush SW. Seeing disorder: Neighborhood stigma and the social construction of “broken windows”. Social psychology quarterly. 2004;67(4):319–42.
  52. 52. Wen M, Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. Objective and perceived neighborhood environment, individual SES and psychosocial factors, and self-rated health: An analysis of older adults in Cook County, Illinois. Social science & medicine. 2006;63(10):2575–90.
  53. 53. Lewis DA, Salem G. Fear of crime: Incivility and the production of a social problem: Transaction Books New Brunswick, NJ; 1986.
  54. 54. Stafford MC, Galle OR. Victimization rates, exposure to risk, and fear of crime. Criminology. 1984;22(2):173–85.
  55. 55. Brunton-Smith I, Sturgis P. Do Neighborhoods Generate Fear of Crime? An Empirical Test Using the British Crime Survey. Criminology. 2011;49(2):331–69.