Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Psychophysiological Responses to Group Exercise Training Sessions: Does Exercise Intensity Matter?

  • Matteo Vandoni,

    Affiliation Department of Public Health, Experimental Medicine & Forensic Science, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy

  • Erwan Codrons,

    Affiliations Department of Public Health, Experimental Medicine & Forensic Science, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy, Department of Brain and Behavioral Sciences, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy

  • Luca Marin,

    Affiliation Department of Public Health, Experimental Medicine & Forensic Science, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy

  • Luca Correale,

    Affiliation Department of Public Health, Experimental Medicine & Forensic Science, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy

  • Marcelo Bigliassi,

    Affiliation Department of Physical Education, State University of Parana, Londrina, Brazil

  • Cosme Franklim Buzzachera

    cosme.buzzachera@unopar.br

    Affiliation Department of Physical Education, North University of Parana, Londrina, Brazil

Abstract

Group exercise training programs were introduced as a strategy for improving health and fitness and potentially reducing dropout rates. This study examined the psychophysiological responses to group exercise training sessions. Twenty-seven adults completed two group exercise training sessions of moderate and vigorous exercise intensities in a random and counterbalanced order. The %HRR and the exertional and arousal responses to vigorous session were higher than those during the moderate session (p<0.05). Consequently, the affective responses to vigorous session were less pleasant than those during moderate session (p<0.05). These results suggest that the psychophysiological responses to group exercise training sessions are intensity-dependent. From an adherence perspective, interventionists are encouraged to emphasize group exercise training sessions at a moderate intensity to maximize affective responses and to minimize exertional responses, which in turn may positively affect future exercise behavior.

Introduction

Though it is well known that regular exercise promotes health and fitness benefits [1], unfortunately, most adults do not meet the minimum recommended levels of exercise participation [2]. Epidemiological data indicate that more than 60% of the worldwide population does not engage in either moderate physical activity for ≥ 30 min at least five times a week or vigorous physical activity for ≥ 20 min at least three times a week [2]. Dropout is considered one of the major contributing factors to the low rates of exercise participation. Evidence shows that approximately 50% of individuals who initiate an exercise program drop out within the first few months of participation [3]. Thus, many people withdraw from exercise programs before physiological gains occur. The identification of potential factors that contribute to non-adherence has been one of the greatest challenges in exercise research over the last few years.

The prescription of vigorous-intensity activities has been recognized as a contributing factor to non-adherence to exercise programs. The current recommendations developed by the American College of Sports Medicine confirmed that adherence is lower in higher-intensity exercise programs. Data from both epidemiological and intervention studies have also shown that sedentary individuals are more likely to adhere to low-intensity activities than to high-intensity activities [3,4,5]. These studies support the notion that exercise intensity influences the adherence to exercise programs. However, none of these investigations provide any information about potential mediators of the relationship between exercise intensity and adherence.

There is speculation that the relationship between exercise intensity and adherence to an exercise program might be mediated by the amount of pleasure that an individual experiences during exercise [6,7,8]. A positive affective response experienced during exercise may lead to greater enjoyment of the activity, creation of a positive memory of the exercise experience, and possibly increased motivation for engagement in future physical activity [3,8]. This hypothesis has been presented using the so-called hedonic theory of motivation [9], which argues that if people derive pleasure from exercise participation, then they would presumably seek to repeat this activity. Conversely, if people derive displeasure from exercise participation, the chances of them repeating this activity would be reduced. In fact, a recent study conducted by Williams et al. [8] reported that affective responses to a moderately intense stimulus were predictive of exercise participation up to 6 and 12 months later. However, the relationship between affective responses and later exercise participation becomes nonsignificant when controlling for perceived exertion, thus suggesting a crucial role for both affective and exertional responses in the adherence to exercise programs.

Group exercise training programs have been introduced in many fitness centers around the world. These programs involve exercising the major muscle groups using weights and/or bodyweight, a standard sequence of music selections, and choreography that is followed by the practitioners. One of the main purposes of these sessions is to improve the health and fitness of the practitioners. Studies utilizing this type of activity have shown that, on average, individuals are predisposed to exercise at intensities that lie within the range recommended to provide health and fitness benefits by the American College of Sports Medicine [10,11]. However, none of these studies provided any information about the affective and exertional responses to group exercise training programs, which may play a key role in determining the adherence to such an activity [8]. Another unsolved issue is whether the psychophysiological responses to this type of activity vary across the range of exercise intensities. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to examine the psychophysiological responses to group exercise training sessions performed at moderate and vigorous intensities. Based upon previous data and theories, we hypothesized that a moderate-intensity exercise trial would result in a more positive affective valence and a less strenuous perceived exertion compared with a vigorous-intensity exercise trial.

Material and Methods

Study Design

To examine our hypothesis, a group of active, young men and women completed four trials, scheduled on different days, with at least 48–72 h between trials. On Day 1, participants underwent a medical screening, anthropometric measurement, and familiarization with the experimental procedures. On Day 2, they performed a maximal graded exercise test. Finally, on Days 3–4, participants performed two group exercise training sessions at moderate and vigorous intensities, in a random and counterbalanced order. This study design enables for the direct comparison of the effects of exercise intensity and gender (independent variables) on psychophysiological responses to group exercise training sessions (dependent variables: %HRR, perceived exertion, affective valence, and perceived activation).

Participants

Twenty-seven individuals (17 men and 10 women) between 18 and 28 years of age volunteered to participate in this study, which was performed in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975 and approved by the Institutional Ethics Committee. Each participant gave their written informed consent after the purpose, experimental procedures, possible risks, and benefits of the study were explained to them. The participants received no monetary compensation but were given the results of their maximal exercise test and an individualized exercise prescription upon completion of the study. Based on a power analysis considering the correlation among measures [12], the a priori sample size was calculated using a software package, GPower, [13]. This power analysis was conducted as follows: 2 (exercise intensity trials) × 5 (time), within subject factorial design with repeated measures, with effect size estimates derived from previous work [14]. For this study design, 6 participants were required for each of the cells to result in a statistical power of 0.99, with a large effect size, f2 = 1.00, [15], a moderate correlation (r = 0.50), and an overall level of significance of p = 0.05. In sum, the sample size of 27 participants was large enough to detect the expected differences between exercise intensity trials.

Participants were recruited from exercise and health courses at the University. However, to be eligible for participation, individuals were required to have engaged in > 30 min of moderate physical activity per day on most days of the week for the previous six months [1]. All participants were in good health; were not taking any medications known to affect cardiovascular, respiratory, muscular, metabolic or cognitive functions; had a stable body mass (<2.5-kg net change over the previous 3 months); and were nonsmokers. The participants’ descriptive data are shown in Table 1.

Procedures

Experimental Trials.

All participants completed two experimental trials, which were scheduled on different days and with at least 48–72 h between trials. Specifically, the participants performed two sessions of group exercise training, involving selected bodyweight exercises targeting the major muscle groups and performed according to music and choreography sequences. All sessions were structured as follows: 15 min of standard warm up with no music, 60 min of functional exercises with synchronous music, and 15 min of cool down with no music. As noted by Simpson and Karageorghis [16], the use of synchronous music entails the conscious performance of repetitive movements in time with the rhythmical elements of music, such as the beat or tempo. In the present study, the trials were performed using synchronous medium-tempo (120 beats · min-1) or fast-tempo (145 beats · min-1) music, and participants were instructed to synchronize their body movements according to the music tempo and the trainer’s choreography. Therefore, the trials used in this study were described as moderate- and vigorous-intensity exercise trials, respectively. As proposed by Karageorghis et al. [17], the aforementioned synchronous tempo music may adequately lead to moderate-intensity (120 beats · min-1) and vigorous-intensity (145 beats · min-1) exercises. The music was played through wall-mounted speakers at a safe and effective standardized intensity [17] in each of the trials. To avoid any influence of different music on the psychophysiological responses to exercise, standardized music was used in the trials [17]. All participants were instructed to individually report their psychological responses to exercise to minimize any possible effects of group dynamics or social interactions. All participants performed the two experimental trials in a random and counterbalanced order. They were instructed to refrain from exercise, avoid alcoholic and caffeinated products in the 24 h before the trials, and present themselves at the laboratory in a 2-h post-absorptive state. All experimental trials were conducted in the morning (between 8:00 am and 12:00 pm) under similar environmental conditions.

HR was continuously recorded before and throughout the trials using a short-range radio telemetry HR monitor (Polar Team System, Polar Electro, Kempele, Finland). The HR responses were then averaged for the final 30-sec intervals of every 15-min period of each trial and 1 min before and 1 min after exercise. For each participant, heart rate reserve (HRR) was calculated by subtracting the HRRest value from the respective maximal value (i.e., HRmax). Accordingly, for each 15-min interval of each of the trials, as well as 1 min before and 1 min after exercise, the increment above resting for each value was divided by the calculated reserve and multiplied by 100 to derive %HRR [18].

Maximal Graded Exercise Test.

After 5 min of seated rest, participants performed a maximal graded exercise test on a motorized treadmill. The standard Bruce protocol was used to determine maximal oxygen consumption (VO2max). Each participant was verbally encouraged to continue to exercise until the point of volitional exhaustion. To achieve a VO2max, participants were required to meet two of the following criteria: (a) a plateau in VO2 (changes of < 150 mL · min-1 in the last three consecutive 15 sec averages), (b) a respiratory exchange ratio of ≥ 1.15, and (c) a heart rate (HR) within 10 beats · min-1 of the age-predicted maximum. Therefore, VO2max was defined as the highest value of VO2 attained after reaching the aforementioned criteria. HRmax was defined as the highest HR value recorded over any continuous 15-sec interval during the test, whereas HRRest was considered the average HR value recorded over the last 2 min of seated rest [18]. Ventilatory threshold (VT) was determined by two independent experienced investigators in a blind fashion using the ventilatory equivalent method [19, 20].

HR (beats · min-1) was continuously recorded before and throughout the test using an HR monitor (RS400, Polar Electro™, Kempele, Finland). VO2, carbon dioxide production (VCO2), and pulmonary ventilation (VE) were measured using a portable gas analysis system (K4b2, Cosmed, Rome, Italy). The system was calibrated using room air (21% O2, 0.03% CO2) and a certified gas mixture (16% O2, 5% CO2; Scott Medical Products, Plumsteadville, USA) prior to each test. The turbine flowmeter was periodically calibrated using a 3-L syringe according to the manufacturer’s instructions.

Psychometric Measures

Perceived Exertion, Affective Valence, and Perceived Activation.

The 6–20 Borg Rate of Perceived Exertion (RPE) scale [21] was used as a measure of whole-body perceived exertion during exercise. This scale is a 15-point single-item measure, ranging from 6 to 20, with verbal anchors ranging from “no exertion at all” (6) to “maximal exertion” (20). Participants were previously anchored to the scale during the orientation session using memory-anchoring procedures [22]. The ratings of perceived exertion were estimated at the end of each 3-min stage of the maximal graded exercise test and at every 15-min interval of the exercise intensity trials.

Affective valence was measured using the Feeling Scale (FS) [23]. The FS is a single-item indicator of affective valence scored on an 11-point scale ranging from -5 “very bad” to +5 “very good”. The “core” or “basic” affective valence measured by the FS is a major component of the circumplex model, presented by Russell et al. [24], which incorporates affective valence and activation as orthogonal and bipolar dimensions of the affective space. In the present study, the Felt Arousal Scale (FAS) [25] was used as a measure of perceived activation, which is considered a global index of “somatic” arousal. The FAS is a 6-point, single-item, bipolar measure, ranging from 1 to 6, with verbal anchors of “low arousal” (1) and “high arousal” (6). In a counterbalanced manner 6–20 Borg RPE, FS, and FAS scales were administered at the end of each 3-min stage of the maximal graded exercise test and at every 15-min interval of the exercise intensity trials. Additionally, these scales were administered at two points in the exercise intensity trial: (i) immediately after the warm up (0 min) and (ii) immediately after the cool down. Participants rated “how” and “what” they felt at these particular moments. Standard definitions of perceived exertion, affective valence, and perceived activation, along with separate instructional sets for the 6–20 Borg RPE, FS, and FAS scales, were read to the participants before the maximal graded exercise test and exercise intensity trials [6,22,25]. During the maximal graded exercise test, the 6–20 Borg RPE, FS, and FAS scales were in full view of the participants at all times of the test.

Self-Reported Preference for Exercise Intensity.

Preference was defined as a predisposition to select a particular level of exercise intensity when given the opportunity [26]. The Preference for and Tolerance of the Intensity of Exercise Questionnaire (PRETIE-Q) was used as a measure of self-reported preference for exercise intensity. Developed by Ekkekakis et al. [26], the eight-item PRETIE-Q Preference Scale contains four items that tap preference for high intensity (e.g., “I would rather have a short, intense workout than a long, low-intensity workout”) and four that tap preference for low intensity and are reverse-scored (e.g., “when I exercise, I usually prefer a slow, steady pace”). Each item is scored on a 5-point response scale ranging from “I totally disagree” (1) to “I totally agree” (5). At the end of the orientation session, the participants were given copies of the eight-item PRETIE-Q Preference Scale and were asked to complete and return them upon arrival for the next session. Given the recent exercise experience among these participants, it was possible for them to provide meaningful responses to the questions of the scale, which inquires about usual responses to the bodily stimulation generated during exercise [27].

Self-Efficacy.

Self-efficacy is a concept relevant only to specifically delineated tasks, so it is assessed using task-specific, rather than domain-general, measures [28]. However, given the myriad of situations in which self-efficacy is a concern, attempting to develop countless scales for every conceivable situation seems unrealistic [29]. In this study, the Physical Self Efficacy Scale (PSES) was used as a measure of two differentially meaningful components: perceived physical ability (PPA) and physical self-presentation confidence (PSPC). The PPA subscale appraises the perceptions of individuals’ competence in performing tasks that utilize physical skills, while the PSPC subscale measures their confidence as reflected by their physical demeanor in the presence of others [30]. Developed by Ryckman et al. [30], the PSES contains 10 items that tap PPA (e.g., “I have a strong grip”) and 12 items that tap PSPC (e.g., “I find that I am not accident prone”) and are reverse-scored. Each item is scored on a 6-point response scale ranging from “strongly agree” (1) to “strongly disagree” (6), with higher scores reflecting stronger PPA and PSPC. At the end of the orientation session, the participants were given copies of the 22-item PSES and were asked to complete and return them upon arrival for the next session.

Statistical Analysis

Data are shown as means ± standard deviations or standard errors. Independent t-tests were used to examine gender differences in anthropometric characteristics and physiological responses during the graded exercise test (p < 0.05). A series of three-factor [gender (men and women) × exercise intensity trial (moderate and vigorous intensities) × time (0 min, 15 min, 30 min, 45 min, and cool down)] repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted on %HRR, perceived exertion, affective valence, and perceived activation. Initially, gender was included as a between-subject factor in all analyses conducted on the dependent variables (i.e., %HRR, perceived exertion, affective valence, and perceived activation). However, none of the main or interaction effects of gender (e.g., gender × exercise intensity trial, gender × time, and gender × exercise intensity trial × time) were statistically significant. As a consequence, gender was omitted. Whenever the sphericity assumption was violated in the ANOVA models, degrees of freedom were adjusted using the Greenhouse-Geisser or Huynh Feldt epsilon corrections. Partial eta squared (η2p) was used to estimate the sizes of the effects [15]. For each ANOVA model with a significant exercise intensity trial × time interaction, the simple effects of time were further analyzed within each exercise intensity trial. Significant simple effects of time were followed by planned contrasts, in which 15 min, 30 min, 45 min, and cool down values were compared with their 0 min (i.e., end of warm up) value. Because these comparisons increase the risk for type I error (i.e., to reject the null hypothesis when it should not be rejected), the p value for post hoc analyses was adjusted according to the Bonferroni correction to 0.05/4 = 0.0125. Additionally, Pearson correlation r coefficients were calculated to examine the relationship of intensity preference and the self-efficacy components (i.e., PPA and PSPC) with the psychophysiological responses (i.e., %HRR, perceived exertion, affective valence, and perceived activation) for each of the two exercise intensity trials. All data were analyzed using SPSS 17.0 for Windows (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, USA).

Results

Descriptive data for the participants are shown in Table 1. There were no gender differences in age, VT, HRRest, and HRmax. However, the men were significantly heavier and taller (p < 0.001) and had greater BMIs and VO2max values than the women (p < 0.001).

The %HRR responses to the two different exercise intensity trials are presented in Fig 1. There were significant main effects of exercise intensity trial, F(1,23) = 5.788, p = 0.025, η2p = 0.201, and time, F(2.659,61.155) = 122.740, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.842. There was also a significant interaction between the exercise intensity trial and the time, F(2.028,46.646) = 4.073, p = 0.023, η2p = 0.150. This finding demonstrates that the changes in %HRR responses to exercise differed as a function of the exercise intensity trial. Therefore, the interaction was decomposed into the simple effects of time within each exercise intensity trial. For the moderate-intensity trial, there was a significant effect of time, F(4,96) = 54.670, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.695. Planned contrasts on the %HRR data indicated that the %HRR response was significantly lower at 0 min (i.e., end of warm up) compared with those at 15 min, 30 min, and 45 min (p < 0.0125), but was similar to that at cool down. There was also a significant effect of time for the vigorous-intensity trial, F(4,108) = 99.406, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.786. Planned contrasts on the %HRR data indicated that the %HRR response was significantly lower at 0 min (i.e., end of warm up) compared with that at 15 min (p < 0.0125), and was similar to those at 30 min, 45 min, and cool down.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Physiological (%HRR) responses to the two exercise intensity trials.

Data are shown as means ± standard error. * p < 0.01, moderate-exercise intensity significantly different from vigorous-exercise intensity.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0149997.g001

The exertional responses to the two different exercise intensity trials are presented in Fig 2. There were significant main effects of exercise intensity trial, F(1,26) = 18.238, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.412, and time, F(2.955,71.634) = 147.761, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.850. There was also a significant interaction between the exercise intensity trial and the time, F(2.643,68.725) = 15.123, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.368. This finding demonstrates that the changes in perceived exertion during the exercise sessions differed as a function of the exercise intensity trial. Therefore, the interaction was decomposed into the simple effects of time within each exercise intensity trial. For the moderate-intensity trial, there was a significant effect of time, F(4,112) = 50.881, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.645. Planned contrasts on the perceived exertion data indicated that the exertional response was significantly lower at 0 min (i.e., end of warm up) compared with those at 15 min, 30 min, and 45 min (p < 0.0125), but was similar to that at cool down. There was also a significant effect of time for the vigorous-intensity trial, F(4,104) = 92.092, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.780. Planned contrasts on the perceived exertion data indicated significant increases in perceived exertion (in comparison with 0-min values) at 15 min, 30 min, and 45 min (p < 0.0125), but not at cool down. The exertional response was also significantly higher at 15 min compared with those at 30 min and 45 min (p < 0.0125).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Perceived exertion responses to the two exercise intensity trials.

Data are shown as means ± standard error. * p < 0.01, moderate-exercise intensity significantly different from vigorous-exercise intensity.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0149997.g002

The affective responses to the two different exercise intensity trials are presented in Fig 3. There were significant main effects of exercise intensity trial, F(1,26) = 3.808, p = 0.062, η2p = 0.128, and time, F(2.207,57.386) = 25.717, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.497. In particular, a more positive affective response occurred during the moderate-intensity trial than during the vigorous-intensity trial. However, there was a significant interaction between the exercise intensity trial and the time, F(2.427,63.090) = 5.343, p = 0.004, η2p = 0.170. This finding demonstrates that the changes in affective responses to exercise differed as a function of the exercise intensity trial. Therefore, the interaction was decomposed into the simple effects of time within each exercise intensity trial. For the moderate-intensity trial, there was a significant effect of time, F(4,112) = 11.658, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.294. Planned contrasts on the “core” affective valence data indicated that the affective response was significantly more positive at 0 min (i.e., end of warm up) compared with those at 15 min and 30 min (p < 0.0125), but was similar to those at 45 min and cool down. There was also a significant effect of time for the vigorous-intensity trial, F(4,104) = 17.775, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.406. Planned contrasts on the “core” affective valence data indicated that the affective response was significantly more positive at 0 min (i.e., end of warm up) compared with those at 15 min, 30 min, and 45 min (p < 0.0125), but was similar to that at cool down in both trials.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Temporal dynamics of the affective valence and perceived activation responses to the two exercise intensity trials.

Data are shown as means ± standard error. * p < 0.01, moderate-exercise intensity significantly different from vigorous-exercise intensity.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0149997.g003

The “somatic” arousal responses to the two different exercise intensity trials are presented in Fig 3. There was a significant main effect of the time F(1.731, 45.001) = 19.221, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.425. In contrast, there was no significant main effect of exercise intensity trial (p = 0.60) or a significant exercise intensity trial × time interaction (p = 0.80). These findings demonstrate that the changes in perceived activation during the exercise sessions were similar for the two exercise intensity trials. As a result, the exercise intensity trials were collapsed for planned contrasts that compared the 0-min (i.e., end of warm up) value with the 15-min, 30-min, 45-min, and cool down values. These analyses indicated significant increases in perceived activation (compared with 0-min values) at 15 min and 30 min (p < 0.001), but not at 45 min and cool down, for the two exercise intensity trials.

The correlations between the intensity preference and self-efficacy components with the psychophysiological responses at each of the two exercise intensity trials are presented in Table 2. The correlation r coefficients for each variable were consistent across the five time points (i.e., 0 min, 15 min, 30 min, 45 min, and cool down) within each exercise intensity trial. This consistency is important because the stability of correlational statistics can be a point of concern with small sample sizes (e.g., < 100) [27]. The results indicate that there were no significant correlations of the intensity preference and the self-efficacy components with the psychophysiological responses in any of the two exercise intensity trials (p > 0.05) (Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Pearson average correlation r coefficients of preference for and the self-efficacy components with psychophysiological responses at each of the two exercise intensity trials.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0149997.t002

Discussion

This is the first study to examine the psychophysiological responses to group exercise training sessions and to verify whether these responses are intensity-dependent. To examine these objectives, the present research design involved participants performing two group exercise training sessions at different intensities on separate days, which were administered in a counterbalanced order. Specifically, participants were instructed to synchronize their body movements according to music tempo and the trainer’s choreography during moderate (120 beats·min-1) and vigorous (145 beats·min-1) sessions. As proposed by Karageorghis et al. [17], synchronous music at these tempos may lead participants to exercise at moderate and vigorous intensities, respectively. The group exercise training trials used in this study resulted in different %HRR values (by approximately 15%). Importantly, the %HRR responses to group exercise training trials during both the moderate (from 59% to 79% HRR) and vigorous (from 80% to 90% HRR) sessions were within the exercise intensity range recommended by the ACSM [1] to promote fitness and health benefits (Fig 1). These results are in accordance with the findings from previous studies [10,11] and thereby support the notion of group exercise training sessions as a valuable and effective method to improve health and fitness in active, young adults.

The results of the present study also corroborate with previous findings indicating that vigorous intensities produce less positive affective responses [6,7,14] and demonstrate that the effects of exercise intensity on affective responses are independent of the self-efficacy and self-reported preference for exercise intensity of the participants. These responses are consistent with the fundamental assumptions of the dual mode model [6], which suggest an interplay of cognitive appraisal processes and interoceptive cues in the generation of affective responses during exercise. According to this model, cognitive appraisal processes are the primary determinants of affective responses as the exercise intensity is below or near to the VT. At these exercise intensities, affective responses are pleasant by most people but also unpleasant by many other people. Once the exercise intensity is beyond the VT, interoceptive cues gain salience and become the primary determinant of affective responses. At exercise intensities exceeding the VT, affective responses tend to be homogeneously negative.

The reduction in the affective valence began at the first assessment during exercise and continued until completion of the exercise (Fig 3). Interestingly, there was no significant effect of exercise intensity on post-exercise affect and arousal. Both trials resulted in similar changes in the perceived activation and affective valence immediately after exercise. The reasons for this phenomenon are unclear. However, it is likely that the effects of exercise intensity on affect during exercise are unrelated to affect after exercise. This repeated assessment of “core” affect could be considered a strength of this investigation, as few studies have utilized this methodological approach [31,32]. Additionally, the approach underscores the importance of both assessing and comparing affective responses, both during and after exercise.

The findings of the present study may have implications for exercise prescription in group exercise training programs that differ in intensity. As highlighted, the prescription of vigorous-intensity activities is correlated with less favorable affective responses during exercise. Importantly, the amount of pleasure one experiences during exercise might play a key role in later participation in physical activity [6,5,8]. A more positive affective response experienced during exercise might lead to a greater enjoyment of the activity, creating a positive memory of the exercise experience that may increase the motivation to participate in physical activity in the future. A recent prospective study by Williams et al. [8] confirmed that affective responses to a moderately intense stimulus were predictive of participation in physical activity up to 6 and 12 months later. Thus, interventionists are encouraged to consider that even a very subtle increase in intensity during group exercise training sessions can be enough to make an individual feel less positive about their exercise experience. Given the proposed link between affect and adherence [8], the influence that this "extra" exercise intensity can have on affective responses may be enough to discourage future exercise participation because the experience was less pleasant than one is willing to tolerate.

Although the affective responses experienced during exercise may play a role in predicting adherence to physical activity programs, the exertional responses also have consequences for future physical activity behaviors [8,31]. In the present study, both affective ("how" a person feels) and exertional ("what" a person feels) responses were assessed to provide a more complete picture of the subjective exercise experience. This combination is useful, as the affect and perceived exertion are not isomorphic constructs [23]. As expected, the results from the current study indicate that the vigorous-intensity trial produced more strenuous exertional responses. Additionally, the results demonstrate that the effects of exercise intensity on perceived exertion are unrelated to the self-efficacy and self-reported intensity preference of the participants. Although both trials resulted in similar increases in perceived exertion, the vigorous-intensity trial resulted in a significant increase in effort perception. This increase began at the first assessment during exercise and continued until completion of the exercise (Fig 2). Based on the principles of Borg's [21] model of effort continua, the higher exertional responses reported by the participants during the vigorous-intensity trial are likely to be attributable, at least in part, to the greater physiological requirements of the trial. However, there was no significant effect of exercise intensity on post-exercise perceived exertion. Both trials resulted in similar changes in perceived exertion immediately after exercise. The reasons for this “rebound” effect in the temporal dynamics of the exertional responses are unclear, but the influences of cognitive-social factors [33] and dissociative cues [17] may be plausible explanations. Further research is warranted to elucidate how specific cognitive-social factors and associative/dissociative cues affect exertional and affective responses to group exercise training sessions.

Limitations

In interpreting the results of this study, readers should take into account its inherent limitations. The sample is the primary limitation. The current sample is rather narrow in terms of age, fitness level, weight status, and socio-educational status. On the one hand, this cohort may not be representative of the adult population at large. On the other hand, this population is appropriate for a preliminary exploration of the effects of exercise intensity on psychophysiological responses to group exercise training sessions. Therefore, the present findings cannot be applied to older adults or individuals with chronic diseases. Similar research with more diverse populations is needed. Secondly, this study did not control for the effects of dispositional variables (extraversion, neuroticism, behavioral inhibition) on affective valence and perceived exertion [33]. It is likely that a degree of the observed variation in the psychophysiological responses to group exercise training sessions may be accounted for by dispositional variables20.

Conclusions

In summary, this study has provided evidence that psychophysiological responses to group exercise training sessions are intensity-dependent, regardless of differences in self-efficacy beliefs and intensity preferences. Specifically, a moderate-intensity session resulted in lower exercise intensity and perceived activation, less strenuous perceived exertion, and more pleasant affective valence when compared with a vigorous-intensity session performed by active, young adults. From a theoretical perspective, the results of this study provides useful insight into how exercise intensity may influence the associated affective and exertional responses. More importantly, from a practical perspective, it appears that individuals report more positive affective and exertional responses during moderate-intensity exercise sessions, which could create a positive memory of the activity and could hopefully lead to increased motivation for future participation in physical activity.

Implications

The strength of this investigation lies in the implications of the data for professionals involved in exercise prescription. Interventionists can be confident that group exercise training sessions may elicit physiological requirements that fall within the exercise intensity range recommended by the American College of Sports Medicine to promote fitness and health benefits. On the basis of emerging evidence [10,11], it seems appropriate to highlight the potential role of group exercise training programs as a valuable and effective strategy for improving health and fitness in active, young adults. Interventionists can also be confident that group exercise training sessions at moderate intensities are more helpful for maximizing affective responses and minimizing exertional responses than group exercise training sessions at vigorous intensities, regardless of the self-efficacy beliefs or predispositions for certain exercise intensities among the participants. Such recommendations support the growing sentiment that vigorous-intensity exercise may negatively affect future participation in physical activity [8,14]. That is, vigorous intensities may elicit less pleasant exercise experiences, discouraging the continuation of participation in such strenuous activities in favor of more moderate intensities that result in more positive experiences.

Author Contributions

  1. Conceived and designed the experiments: MV EC LM LC MB CFB.
  2. Performed the experiments: MV EC LM LC MB CFB.
  3. Analyzed the data: MV EC LM CFB.
  4. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: MV EC LM LC MB CFB.
  5. Wrote the paper: MV EC MB CFB.

References

  1. 1. American College of Sports Medicine. ACSM’s Guidelines for Exercise Testing and Prescription. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2006.
  2. 2. World Health Organization. A global strategy for diet, physical activity, and health. Geneva: Technical Report Series; 2004.
  3. 3. Dishman RK, Buckworth J. Increasing physical activity: a quantitative synthesis. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1996;28: 706–719. pmid:8784759
  4. 4. Sallis JF, Haskell WL, Fortmann SP, Vranizan KM, Taylor CB, Solomon DS. Predictors of adoption and maintenance of physical activity in a community sample. Prev Med. 1986;15: 331–341. pmid:3763558
  5. 5. Perri MG, Anton SD, Durning PE, Ketterson TU, Sydeman SJ, Berlant NE, et al. Adherence to exercise prescriptions: effects of prescribing moderate versus higher levels of intensity and frequency. Health Psychol. 2002;21: 452–458. pmid:12211512
  6. 6. Ekkekakis P. Pleasure and displeasure from the body: perspectives from exercise. Cogn Emot. 2003;17: 213–239.
  7. 7. Rose EA, Parfitt G. Can the Feeling Scale be used to regulate exercise intensity? Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2008;40: 1852–1860. pmid:18799997
  8. 8. Williams DM, Dunsiger S, Ciccolo JT, Lewis BA, Albrecht AE, Marcus BH. Acute affective response to a moderate-intensity exercise stimulus predicts physical activity participation 6 and 12 months later. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2008;9: 231–245. pmid:18496608
  9. 9. Kahneman D, Fredrickson BL, Schreiber CA, Redelmeier DA. When more pain is preferred to less: Adding a better end. Psychol Sci. 1993;4: 401–405.
  10. 10. Oliveira AS, Greco CC, Pereira MP, Figueira TR, Ruas VDA, Gonçalves M, et al. Physiological and neuromuscular profile during a bodypump session: Acute responses during a high-resistance training session. J Strength Cond Res. 2009;23: 579–586. pmid:19209078
  11. 11. Greco CC, Oliveira AS, Pereira MP, Figueira TR, Ruas VDA, Gonçalves M, et al. Improvements in metabolic and neuromuscular fitness after 12-week bodypump training. J Strength Cond Res. 2011;25: 3422–3431. pmid:22080312
  12. 12. D’Amico EJ, Neilands TB, Zambarano R. Power analysis for multivariate and repeated measures analysis: a flexible approach using the SPSS MANOVA procedure. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 2001;33, 479–484. pmid:11816450
  13. 13. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Meth. 2007;39, 175–191.
  14. 14. Kilpatrick M, Kraemer R, Bartholomew J, Acevedo E, Jarreau D. Affective responses to exercise are dependent on intensity rather than total work. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2007;39: 1417–1422. pmid:17762376
  15. 15. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale: Erlbaum; 1988.
  16. 16. Simpson SD, Karageorghis CI. The effects of synchronous music on 400-m sprint performance. J Sports Sci. 2006;24: 1095–1102. pmid:17115524
  17. 17. Karageorghis CI, Jones L, Low DC. Relationship between exercise heart rate and music tempo preference. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2006;77: 240–250. pmid:16898279
  18. 18. Swain DP, Leutholtz BC, King ME, Haas LA, Branch DJ. Relationship between % heart rate reserve and %VO2 reserve in treadmill exercise. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1998;30: 318–321. pmid:9502363
  19. 19. Caiozzo VJ, Davis JA, Ellis JF, Azus JL, Vandagriff R, Prietto CA, et al. A comparison of gas exchange indices used to detect the anaerobic threshold. J Appl Physiol. 1982;53: 1184–1189. pmid:7174412
  20. 20. Gaskill SE, Ruby BC, Walker AJ, Sanchez OA, Serfass RC, Leon AS. Validity and reliability of combining three methods to determine ventilatory threshold. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2001;33: 1841–1848. pmid:11689733
  21. 21. Borg GAV. Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1982;14: 377–381. pmid:7154893
  22. 22. Noble BJ, Robertson RJ. Perceived exertion. Champaign: Human Kinetics; 1996.
  23. 23. Hardy CJ, Rejeski WJ. Not what, but how one feels: the measurement of affect during exercise. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 1989;11: 304–317.
  24. 24. Russell JA. A circumplex model of affect. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1980;39: 1161–1178.
  25. 25. Svebak S, Murgatroyd S. Metamotivational dominance: a multimethod validation of reversal theory constructs. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1985;48: 107–116.
  26. 26. Ekkekakis P, Hall EE, Petruzzello SJ. Some like it vigorous: Measuring individual differences in the preference for and tolerance of exercise intensity. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2005;27: 350–374.
  27. 27. Ekkekakis P, Lind E, Joens-Matre RR. Can self-reported preference for exercise intensity predict physiologically defined self-selected exercise intensity? Res Q Exerc Sport. 2006;77, 81–90. pmid:16646355
  28. 28. Bandura A. Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W. H. Freeman; 1997.
  29. 29. McAuley E, Gill D. Reliability and validity of the physical self-efficacy scale in a competitive sport setting. J Sport Psychol. 1983;5: 410–418.
  30. 30. Ryckman RM, Robbens MA, Thornton B, Cantrell P. Development and validation of a physical self-efficacy scale. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1982;42: 891–900.
  31. 31. DaSilva SG, Guidetti L, Buzzachera CF, Elsangedy HM, Krinski K, De Campos W, et al. Psychophysiological responses to self-paced treadmill and overground exercise. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2011;43: 1114–1124. pmid:21088625
  32. 32. Ekkekakis P, Petruzzello SJ. Analysis of the affect measurement conundrum in exercise psychology: iv. A conceptual case for the affect circumplex. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2002;3: 35–63.
  33. 33. Hall EE, Ekkekakis P, Petruzzello SJ. Is the relationship of RPE to psychological factors intensity-dependent? Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2005;37: 1365–1373. pmid:16118584