Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Walking Behavior of Zoo Elephants: Associations between GPS-Measured Daily Walking Distances and Environmental Factors, Social Factors, and Welfare Indicators

  • Matthew R. Holdgate ,

    mholdgate@gmail.com

    Affiliations Department of Biology, Portland State University, Portland, Oregon, United States of America, Conservation Research Division, Oregon Zoo, Portland, Oregon, United States of America

  • Cheryl L. Meehan,

    Affiliation AWARE Institute, Portland, Oregon, United States of America

  • Jennifer N. Hogan,

    Affiliation AWARE Institute, Portland, Oregon, United States of America

  • Lance J. Miller,

    Affiliation Chicago Zoological Society—Brookfield Zoo, Brookfield, Illinois, United States of America

  • Joseph Soltis,

    Affiliation Department of Education & Science, Disney’s Animal Kingdom, Lake Buena Vista, Florida, United States of America

  • Jeff Andrews,

    Affiliation Zoological Operations, Busch Gardens, Tampa, Florida, United States of America

  • David J. Shepherdson

    Affiliations Department of Biology, Portland State University, Portland, Oregon, United States of America, Conservation Research Division, Oregon Zoo, Portland, Oregon, United States of America

Abstract

Research with humans and other animals suggests that walking benefits physical health. Perhaps because these links have been demonstrated in other species, it has been suggested that walking is important to elephant welfare, and that zoo elephant exhibits should be designed to allow for more walking. Our study is the first to address this suggestion empirically by measuring the mean daily walking distance of elephants in North American zoos, determining the factors that are associated with variations in walking distance, and testing for associations between walking and welfare indicators. We used anklets equipped with GPS data loggers to measure outdoor daily walking distance in 56 adult female African (n = 33) and Asian (n = 23) elephants housed in 30 North American zoos. We collected 259 days of data and determined associations between distance walked and social, housing, management, and demographic factors. Elephants walked an average of 5.3 km/day with no significant difference between species. In our multivariable model, more diverse feeding regimens were correlated with increased walking, and elephants who were fed on a temporally unpredictable feeding schedule walked 1.29 km/day more than elephants fed on a predictable schedule. Distance walked was also positively correlated with an increase in the number of social groupings and negatively correlated with age. We found a small but significant negative correlation between distance walked and nighttime Space Experience, but no other associations between walking distances and exhibit size were found. Finally, distance walked was not related to health or behavioral outcomes including foot health, joint health, body condition, and the performance of stereotypic behavior, suggesting that more research is necessary to determine explicitly how differences in walking may impact elephant welfare.

Introduction

The distance that zoo elephants walk each day has been proposed as a biologically meaningful metric for measuring the success of zoo elephant programs in providing good welfare for their elephants [1]. Implicit in this recommendation is an assumption that elephants are strongly motivated and physiologically adapted to walk long distances, and that the welfare of zoo elephants is therefore compromised when walking distance is constrained, for example due to the size of the exhibit. The walking behavior of wild elephants has been measured under various conditions using a variety of techniques (summarized in [2]). However, there is significant variation in measured distances both within and between these studies, with reported daily walking distances ranging from 3.2 to 12.0 km/day [2]. Variation in wild elephant movement is affected by a variety of factors including the age and sex of the individual, season, social groupings, and the distribution and availability of resources [35]. In some cases these relationships encourage greater amounts of walking; elephants can travel long distances to seek out fruiting events [6], sodium [78], and green vegetation [9]. However, when conditions dictate, elephants’ locomotion is more limited. In the dry season, African elephant family units are unable to range far from water, and bulls in musth remain close to family units to maximize mating opportunities [10]. The link between resource availability and walking distances suggests that walking is an effective means for meeting social or nutritional needs and is performed flexibly in response to external conditions. In zoo settings, social and nutritional needs are addressed through management practices and therefore, the functional need for walking in this context is reduced. However, it is also possible that walking is important because it supports exploratory behavior which has an information-gathering function and may be rewarding to animals even when not directly linked to acquisition of resources [1112]. Rats, for example, have been found to forgo sucrose rewards in order to physically investigate their environment [13], but studies of elephants’ motivation to explore via locomotion have not been conducted.

In order to better understand the walking behavior of zoo elephants and its importance to welfare, it is necessary to develop methods to reliably collect data on the distances elephants walk under a range of conditions, and to determine the demographic, social, environmental, and management-related factors that are associated with differences in distances walked. Finally, it is important to test for associations between distances walked and other behavioral or health indicators to validate walking as a measure of welfare [14].

Physical exercise (including, but not limited to walking) is known to be associated with good health in humans (e.g., [1518]), and this relationship has also been measured in other species. For example, dogs that experienced vigorous exercise sessions had fewer behavioral and physiological signs of stress than their unexercised counterparts [19]. With respect to elephants, a survey-based study of 78 zoos found that zoos providing their elephants with more daily exercise reported fewer incidents of foot pathology [20]; another study [21] found that zoo elephants who experienced 14 hours per week of exercise had a lower likelihood of being overweight or obese. The challenge in applying the results of these studies to the relationship between walking and zoo elephant welfare is that they evaluated human-led exercise, which could have different associations with welfare than self-directed exercise. Human-led exercise sessions were associated with an increase in the performance of stereotypic behavior in horses [22]. In preference tests, horses reliably chose to remain in their stalls rather than participate in human-led exercise [23], but also preferred voluntary exercise (via release into a large paddock) over remaining in their stall [23]. The health outcomes of voluntary walking have been studied in cattle, and foot and musculoskeletal problems, mastitis, and dystocia were found to be significantly lower for cows that were given access to pasture to areas large enough to allow free-choice exercise [24]. There are no published studies investigating the relationships between walking distances and the welfare of zoo elephants.

The objectives for the current study were to: 1) use GPS tracking to quantify and describe the outdoor daily walking distances for both African and Asian elephants from a variety of zoos, 2) determine the associations between housing, social, management, and demographic factors and distance walked, and 3) test for associations between distance walked and a variety of health and behavioral welfare indicators including: foot health and musculoskeletal health, performance of stereotypic behavior, and body condition. Our study is the first large-scale multi-species investigation of zoo elephant walking and was as a component of the Using Science to Understand Zoo Elephant Welfare project, a multi-institutional collaborative effort to produce scientific data that will support decision making with regard to best practices in elephant management [25].

Methods

Ethics statement

This study was authorized by the management at each participating zoo and, where applicable, was reviewed and approved by zoo research committees. In addition, the study protocol was reviewed and approved by the Zoological Society of San Diego Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee N.I.H. Assurance A3675-01; Protocol 11–203. The study was non-invasive.

Subjects and facilities

Zoos that were accredited members of the Association of Zoos and Aquariums in 2012 were eligible for participation in this study provided that they managed African or Asian elephants in a non-mixed species herd, and that their herd included at least two adult female elephants who were not pregnant or experiencing severe illness or injury. A total of 49 zoos participated in the study. We used simplified random sampling to select two adult females (age ≥ 12 years) as subjects from each zoo; however, 26 zoos only had two eligible subjects so there was no randomization. In one case there were four subjects from one zoo because this zoo housed African and Asian elephants in separate exhibits. Three subjects were removed from the dataset prior to analysis because they were transferred between zoos or died during the 2012 study year.

Data collection

All data were collected between May 2012 and November 2012. We used historical weather data [26] to select a one month data collection period at each location that minimized inter-zoo variation in predicted daily maximum temperature (range: 22.3 C to 34.1 C). We instructed zoos to collect five non-consecutive days of data (24 hours/day) from each subject within that one-month timeframe. Zoos could collect data from both subjects on the same day, or use an alternating schedule. Zoos also completed detailed housing logs on the days of data collection indicating the areas to which elephants had access to and whether these areas were indoors, outdoors, or a mixed (indoor-outdoor) environment.

Leather anklets (Excelsior Leather, California, USA) [2728] were custom-fit to elephants using measurements provided by participating zoos. The ends of the anklets had D-rings to which shackles and brummel hooks were attached. This hardware was used to secure the anklet in place without causing constriction. A pouch attached to each anklet contained a waterproof case (OtterBox Drybox OTR3-1000S, OtterBox, Colorado, USA) inside of which was an accelerometer (used to collect data for a related study [29]) and a BT-Q1000XT GPS Travel Recorder (QStarz International Co., Taipei, Taiwan) (for anklet photo see [28]). The BT-Q1000XT has been used in previous zoo elephant research [28] and tests indicate that this device can be used to accurately assess elephant movement in a zoo environment [28]. The total weight of the unit was approximately 1.2 kg depending on the anklet size and number of shackles used. We shipped the anklets to the zoos and elephant care staff attached the anklets to one of the front legs of each subject. Most studies of zoo elephant movement have used collars to attach GPS devices [3,27,3032]; anklets were preferred for our multi-institutional study because they require less time for elephants to habituate to them and they are safer for zoo staff to place on the elephants.

We programmed the GPS units to record data points at five second intervals in accordance with a previous study using these units [28]. Each data point includes the date, time, latitude, longitude, and two indices of location estimate quality: the number of satellites used (NSAT) and the geometry of satellites used (HDOP: horizontal dilution of precision; [33]) in each location estimate. The device also received real-time positional corrections to improve accuracy via wide area augmentation system (WAAS; [34]). Finally, we requested that zoos place anklets in an exposed outdoor area fifteen minutes prior to data collection to provide sufficient time for the device to download satellite constellation information.

Data processing

Of the 49 original participating zoos, 40 zoos successfully collected data from 72 elephants. We downloaded the data using QTravel software (v 1.41, QStarz International Co.) and exported it into Microsoft Excel (Microsoft Corporation, Washington, USA). Since the GPS units do not reliably collect data when indoors, we removed data points that were known to have occurred while the elephant was housed indoors by using detailed reports on indoor/outdoor access provided by the zoos. We then mapped the data using ArcMap (v. 10.1, Environmental Systems Research Institute, California, USA) and used the clip function to remove any remaining data from indoor areas. We also clipped any data that fell outside of exhibit boundaries; in one test case we found that these data had a minimal effect (3%) on distance traveled, but clipping the data ensured that we underestimated rather than overestimated the distance. We then filtered the data using a series of macros. Data points were removed if they failed to meet any of the following requirements: NSAT ≥4, HDOP <2, WAAS-enabled.

Following this processing, the amount of valid, outdoor walking data collected from the elephants in our study varied greatly. Thus, we applied additional exclusion criteria to ensure that all elephants proceeding to the analysis stage met a minimum standard. First, in order for their data to be included in the analysis elephants were required to have outdoor access for at least 20/24 hours; any 24 hour period that did not meet this criterion was excluded. This helped minimize the potential effect of long bouts of confinement indoors, since post-inhibitory “rebound” effects can cause increased locomotor activity following confinement in some species [35]. Second, elephants were required to have a total of at least 60 minutes of valid data for each one-hour period across all days of data collection (except for the 0–4 hours they did not have outdoor access). This allowed us to remove any bias due to elephants who had consistently missing data from the same time periods each day. Finally, elephants with less than 3 days of data remaining after quality control filtering were excluded from analysis. A total of 16 elephants were removed during data processing; our final dataset included 56 elephants from 30 zoos. For these elephants we calculated the Euclidean distance between consecutive data points and we summed the distances for each day to calculate a daily distance traveled (km). Finally, we averaged these daily values to calculate mean daily walking distance (MDWD) (km/day) for each elephant.

Independent variables

Independent variables were selected based on hypotheses regarding their potential association with MDWD. Definitions for the variables selected for testing in this study are described in Table 1. Details on the collection and calculation of independent variables are presented by [29, 3638].

thumbnail
Table 1. Definitions of independent variables tested for correlation with mean daily walking distance.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0150331.t001

A novel variable called Space Experience warrants further attention. Space Experience was calculated by taking the size of each environment in which an elephant spent time, multiplying it by the percentage of time the elephant spent in that environment and then averaging these weighted environment sizes [36]. This allows us to account for the complex housing conditions of zoo elephants, in which they may be shifted between environments of different sizes for varying amounts of time, including at night.

We checked all continuous independent variables for outliers and removed any values that were greater than three standard deviations away from the mean. We adjusted some variables from continuous to binary because of zero-values for a high number of subjects within the sub-population of elephants in our study. Adjusted variables included two space variables (Space Experience In/Out Choice and Percent Time In/Out Choice), two flooring variables (Percent Time Hard Substrate and Percent Time Soft Substrate) and two social variables (Percent Time Housed Separately and Percent Time Juveniles). The Space Experience variables were adjusted to a value of “per 500 ft2” to aid in the interpretation of Beta values.

Statistical analysis

The regression models were fitted using generalized estimating equations (GEE), which allow for the individual elephant to be used as the unit of analysis, account for clustering of individuals within zoos, and support repeated measurement [3940]. Zoos were treated as random effects and an independent correlation structure was specified [41]. We first built multi-variable regression models by assessing individual predictors at the univariate level and then at the bivariate level with demographic variables (age, species and origin). Based on the fact that age, species and origin were likely to have an effect on both outcome and the tested input variable, we tested these variables as potential confounding variables [4243]. Confounding variables (those that altered the beta values of input variables by more than 10% during bivariate analysis) were included in all models, and any variables that predicted recumbence (P < 0.15) following the univariate and bivariate assessments were retained for evaluation in the hierarchical model building process. The hierarchical selection was based on quasi-likelihood under the independence model criterion (QIC) values and parameter estimates of explanatory variables.

Models exhibiting multi-collinearity, as defined by a variance inflation factor (VIF) of greater than 10 and a Condition Index (CI) of greater than 30, were not considered for further analysis. The model used an independent correlation matrix type. Statistical analyses were conducted by using SAS software, version 9.3 [PROC GENMOD, with options REPEATED, CORR = IND, and DIST = NORMAL; SAS Institute, Inc., Cary, NC].

Regression analyses were used to assess the association between MDWD as an input parameter and other outcomes measured in associated studies. These are Foot Health Score [44], Musculoskeletal Health Score [44], Stereotypic Behavior Rate [45], and Body Condition Score [21]. Models to assess these outcomes were specific to the distribution of the outcome. Foot health consisted of a count score of 0–12, and was tested using Poisson distribution, log link, and deviance scale d. Stereotypy consisted of the average of active scans in January and July, and was tested using Negative Binomial distribution, log link, and deviance scale d. Musculoskeletal health consisted of a 0–3 scale associated with presence of abnormal joints or range of motion, and was tested using multinomial logistic and a cumulative logit link function, with 0 as the reference value. Body condition consisted of assessing “ideal” body condition score 3 compared to elevated body condition scores of 4 or 5, and was tested using a multinomial logistic distribution and a cumulative logit link function. All regression methods utilized an independent correlation matrix. In addition to testing for associations between MDWD and these outcomes at the univariate level, bivariate analyses with age and with species were also conducted.

Results

Summary of walking data

Our final dataset included a total of 259 days of data, collected between May 7, 2012 and November 1, 2012, from 56 elephants at 30 zoos. Five days of data were collected for the majority of elephants (43/56), but for some elephants there were only four days (5/56) or three days (8/56) of data. The 56 elephants included 33 African elephants (58.9%) and 23 Asian elephants (41.1%) (Fig 1). The mean age of the African elephants was 33.2 years (range = 20 to 52) and the mean age of Asian elephants was 40.4 years (range = 16 to 61).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Mean daily walking distance in zoo elephants.

Black bars indicate African (n = 33), grey bars indicate Asian (n = 23).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0150331.g001

The mean daily walking distance (MDWD) for all elephants was 5.3 km/day. African elephants walked an average of 5.4 km/day and Asian elephants walked an average of 5.3 km/day; there was no significant difference between the species (P = 0.831) (Table 2). There was a large amount of individual variation in MDWD (SD = 2.4 km/day) (Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Summary of Mean Daily Walking Distance (km/day) for African and Asian zoo elephants.

A t-test used to test for a difference in the means between species was not significant (P = 0.831).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0150331.t002

Univariate analyses

We evaluated a variety of demographic, housing, social, and management factors for association with MDWD (Table 1). Univariate correlations were observed between MDWD and a number of variables (Table 3). Those variables with correlations where p<0.015 were included in the multi-variable modeling process. MDWD was correlated with 12 housing (space and flooring), eight social variables, and four management variables. MDWD was also correlated with Origin, however, due to the small number of elephants who had been imported from a range country (6/56) it was not possible to interpret these results. The population level descriptive statistics for the variables included in the multi-variable analyses are shown in Table 4.

thumbnail
Table 3. Univariate correlations between mean daily walking distance and independent variables.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0150331.t003

thumbnail
Table 4. Descriptive statistics for independent variables included in the multi-variable modeling process.

The sample size and mean age of elephants used in the correlation is provided.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0150331.t004

Multi-variable model

The MDWD multi-variable model (Table 5) includes Beta estimates for Age, Social Group Contact, Space Experience Total Night, Feeding Diversity, and Feeding Schedule. Beta estimates reflect the magnitude of the effect of the independent variables on walking as described below, but it is important to note that this effect is conditional on the effects of the other independent variables in each model. Two feeding variables were included in the multi-variable model. First, elephants with more diverse feeding programs tended to have significantly greater MDWD (P = 0.027). Second, while MDWD did not differ significantly between elephants with predictable feeding schedules and semi-predictable feeding schedules (P = 0.631), elephants with unpredictable feeding schedules walked 1.286 km/day more than elephants with a predictable feeding schedule (P = 0.044). Elephants increased MDWD by 0.422 km/day with every additional social group (P < 0.001), and also decreased their MDWD by 0.023 km for every additional 500 ft2 of Space Experience at night (P = 0.009). The model also showed that MDWD decreased by 0.09 km/day for each additional year in age (P = 0.014). Age was included in the models as a confounder of Social Group Contact and Feeding Diversity. Species was included as a confounder of Age.

thumbnail
Table 5. Mean daily walking distance multi-variable model (*P < 0.05).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0150331.t005

Mean Daily Walking Distance as an Independent Variable

The results of our assessment of the association between MDWD and elephant health and behavioral outcomes are shown in Table 6. We found no significant regression models at the univariate or bivariate (with age or with species) level.

thumbnail
Table 6. Mean daily walking distance as input parameter in Generalized Linear Regression Analyses (*P < 0.05).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0150331.t006

Discussion

There have been several previous single-zoo studies that have assessed zoo elephant walking distances (reviewed in [2]), but different data collection methods and conditions make it difficult to draw comparisons between these studies and our own. In addition, while our methods were certainly informed by previous studies [3,2728,30], our study is unique in terms of the technology and data processing methods employed and with respect to the breadth of the population studied.

Our results demonstrate that zoo elephants, when housed outdoors for at least 20 hours in a 24 hour period, walk between 1.2 and 17.3 km/day, with individual differences in walking distance associated with demographic, social, housing, and feeding-related variables.

The fact that two feeding-related factors played a significant role in our final model indicates that resource access influences how far zoo elephants walk in a day. The simplest hypothesis regarding this relationship is that when food items are distributed more in space, elephants will to walk more to acquire them. While we were unable to test this relationship explicitly because we could not quantify distances between feeding sites, we did not find support for this hypothesis when we tested the variable Spread, which measured the frequency with which food was distributed across the exhibit rather than concentrated in specific areas. By contrast, the features of feeding programs that were found to be positively associated with walking were the degree to which the timing of feedings was unpredictable and the diversity in the methods used to present food. One explanation for this combination of results is that that more dynamic feeding regimens lead to an increase in exploratory behavior, which elephants express via locomotion. Shepherdson et al. [12] found that leopard cats that were switched from a temporally predictable feeding schedule to an unpredictable feeding schedule with food items hidden throughout the enclosure increased their exploratory behavior. Higher feeding diversity scores, which indicate use of browse, food puzzles, and hanging food items in addition to trough or floor feeding, were also associated with increased locomotion, possibly because diversity in feeding opportunities promotes physical exploration of the exhibit. However, it is also possible that the increase in walking that we found related to feeding predictability and diversity is a response to increased arousal or frustration caused by the dynamic feeding regimens.

There was one social variable in the multi-variable model as well. Social Group Contact is a variable that quantifies the common management practice of dividing the herd into various sub-groups, with individual elephants spending time in one or more of these social configurations [36]. In our study population, the elephants spent time in between 1 and 10 different social groups. As an elephant’s Social Group Contact score went up, walking distances increased. A larger number of social groups could better replicate a fission-fusion society in which core social groups temporarily divide and reunite over the course of hours or days [4650]; increased walking could be the result of elephants walking more to interact socially as social groups change and could be a form of social exploration. However, in a related study, Brown et al. [51] found that Social Group Contact was positively associated with hyperprolactinemia in female elephants, which the authors interpreted as a physiological response to social stress caused by dynamic social management. Future studies could delve more deeply into the correlation we found by recording both the quality of social interactions and the amount of walking associated with different social management regimens.

Perhaps most interesting is the fact that none of our measures of space either at the zoo level (Exhibit) or the individual elephant level (Space Experience) were positively associated with distance walked in the multi-variable model. In fact, we found increased Space Experience at night was negatively correlated with distance walked. However, the magnitude of this effect was quite small. This finding challenges the idea that elephants will voluntarily walk greater distances if provided with more room to do so, and in combination with our feeding related results, indicates that exhibit space may be important to walking distances mainly to the extent that it supports the application of diverse and dynamic feeding programs. In considering this lack of effect, is important to note that the range of exhibit sizes in our study population may not have been sufficient to demonstrate the impact that extensive areas can have on walking distances. Studies of wild elephants in non-extreme environmental conditions indicate that average daily walking distances range from 5–10 km/day when there is extensive space availability. Since our measured walking distances are at the lower end of this range, it is possible that elephants would walk more if housed in exhibits larger than those currently represented in the North American zoos participating in this study.

However, setting wild walking distances as a benchmark for managed elephant programs or operating under the assumption that walking distances should be increased as a goal of zoo elephant management is only appropriate if it can be demonstrated that walking is important to elephant welfare. Because our study was conducted as part of the Using Science to Understand Zoo Elephant Welfare Project [25], we were able to test for associations between walking distances and several welfare indicators including foot health, joint health, body condition and stereotypic behavior.

Stereotypic behavior is considered a strong indicator of compromised welfare and is used extensively to evaluate the welfare of animals in a variety of managed settings [5253]. Locomotor stereotypies such as pacing are a concern for many zoo-housed species [5455] and long bouts of pacing by elephants could have inflated our measurements of distance walked. Since we did not combine our GPS data collection with direct behavioral observations, we cannot state conclusively that the elephants in our study did not perform locomotor stereotypy. However, in a related study that included 33 of our study elephants, Greco et al. [45] demonstrated that locomotor stereotypy is infrequently performed by North American zoo elephants and that the vast majority (over 90%) of the stereotypy elephants perform is non-ambulatory and consists of rocking and swaying. These most frequently performed forms of stereotypy should have limited effect on distance measurements collected with GPS anklets [28]. Additionally, the development of stereotypic behaviors in animals is associated with environments that inhibit the performance of highly motivated behaviors [53], and as such, if elephants are motivated to walk and cannot due to environmental restrictions, stereotypic behaviors could develop as a result. However our analysis of walking as an independent variable for stereotypic behavior showed no correlation. As such, we found no evidence to support the conclusion that elephants who walk shorter distances are more likely to perform higher rates of stereotypic behavior.

We also tested the relationship between walking distances and foot and musculoskeletal health because foot and joint conditions are the most commonly reported health issues affecting African and Asian elephants in zoos [20, 56] and it has been suggested that limited walking is a risk factor for these conditions [57]. We found no correlations between walking distance and either foot or musculoskeletal health. Similarly, Miller et al. determined that age and time spent on hard flooring substrates, not distances walked, were the primary predictors of both foot and joint problems in a related study [44]. It must be noted that while the foot and musculoskeletal health data and our walking distance data were collected on the same elephants during the same calendar year, they were not collected at exactly the same time. Although foot problems have been found to be persistent over time for a large proportion of this population [43], it is possible that measures of foot and musculoskeletal health taken coincident with walking distance measurements could show associations that we did not find.

Finally, we tested for an association between mean daily distances walked by elephants and body condition scores. These scores were assessed on a scale of 1–5 (see [21]) during the same calendar year as walking distances were measured. Body condition is a measure of fat deposition and is considered a welfare indicator for elephants because of plausible associations with conditions such as cardiovascular disease, arthritis and foot problems, and ovarian cycle abnormalities [20,5764], and greater walking distances have been predicted to be associated with increased likelihood of ideal body condition [65]. However, our assessment showed that elephants who walked more were not more likely to have ideal body condition. This lack of association could potentially be explained by the importance of the feeding program variable, Feeding Diversity, to both walking distances and risk of elevated body condition. Our models show that greater walking distances are associated with higher Feeding Diversity scores, but interestingly, Morfeld et al. [21] report that elephants who experience more diverse feeding programs are at higher risk of being overweight. If Feeding Diversity scores are a proxy for the amount of food an elephant eats (as hypothesized by Morfeld et al.), then perhaps the energetic expenditure of walking is being offset by caloric intake and therefore the predicted association between walking and ideal body condition was not found.

Overall, we found that the distances zoo elephants walk are influenced most significantly by feeding-related factors, and these associations suggest that walking in zoo elephants may be an expression of exploratory behavior. However more research is necessary to determine explicitly how differences in opportunity to explore via walking may impact elephant welfare, as we found no associations between distances walked and the behavioral or health outcomes we tested.

Acknowledgments

This project was part of a large scale collaboration titled “Using Science to Understand Zoo Elephant Welfare”. The authors would like to acknowledge the significant efforts of the full project team: Christy Alligood, Anne Baker, Jeff Bolling, Mary Bonaparte-Saller, Janine Brown, Kathy Carlstead, Anne-Marie de Passillé, Candice Dorsey, Brian Greco, Greg Guagnano, Mike Keele, Katherine Leighty, John Lehnhardt, Georgia Mason, Jill Mellen, Joy Mench, Michele Miller, Kari Morfeld, Steve Paris, Harry Peachey, Josh Plotnik, Beth Posta, Natalia Prado-Oviedo, Jeff Rushen, Daniel Sneed, Nadja Wielebnowski, and James Witte.

In addition, special thanks to the AZA Elephant TAG and TAG Chair Martha Fischer for logistical support, Jackie Ogden for communications support, and Vistalogic, Inc. for technological support and software services.

We thank Debbie Ethell, research associate at the Oregon Zoo, for assistance with data management and processing, and Joy Mench for helpful critique of this manuscript.

Finally, sincere thanks to the people and elephants at each of the following zoos for incredible participation and support of the project:

Africam Safari, Albuquerque Biological Park, Audubon Institute, Birmingham Zoo, BREC's Baton Rouge Zoo, Buffalo Zoological Gardens, Busch Gardens, Buttonwood Park Zoo, Caldwell Zoo, Calgary Zoo, Cameron Park Zoo, Cheyenne Mountain Zoological Park, Cincinnati Zoo & Botanical Garden, Cleveland Metroparks Zoo, Columbus Zoo, Dallas Zoo, Denver Zoo, Dickerson Park Zoo, Disney's Animal Kingdom, El Paso Zoo, Fresno Chaffee Zoo, Greenville Zoo, Honolulu Zoo, Houston Zoological Gardens, Indianapolis Zoological Society, Inc., Jacksonville Zoological Gardens, Knoxville Zoological Gardens, Lee Richardson Zoo, Little Rock Zoological Garden, Los Angeles Zoo and Botanical Gardens, Louisville Zoological Garden, Lowry Park Zoological Garden, Maryland Zoo, Memphis Zoological Garden and Aquarium, Metropolitan Toronto Zoo, Milwaukee County Zoological Gardens, Montgomery Zoo, Nashville Zoo, National Zoo, Niabi Zoo, North Carolina Zoological Park, Oakland Zoo, Oklahoma City Zoological Park, Oregon Zoo, Parque Zoologico de Leon, Phoenix Zoo, Point Defiance Zoo and Aquarium, Reid Park Zoo, Riverbanks Zoological Park, Roger Williams Park Zoo, Rosamond Gifford Zoo at Burnet Park, San Antonio Zoological Gardens & Aquarium, San Diego Safari Park, San Diego Zoo, Santa Barbara Zoological Gardens, Sedgwick County Zoo, Seneca Park Zoo, Saint Louis Zoo, The Kansas City Zoo, Topeka Zoological Park, Tulsa Zoological Park, Utah's Hogle Zoo, Virginia Zoological Park, Wildlife Conservation Society—Bronx Zoo, Wildlife Safari, Woodland Park Zoo, Zoo Atlanta, Zoo de Granby, Zoo Miami.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: MRH CLM JNH LJM JS JA DJS. Performed the experiments: MRH. Analyzed the data: MRH CLM JNH DJS. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: LJM JS JA DJS. Wrote the paper: MRH CLM JNH LJM JS JA DJS.

References

  1. 1. Hutchins M. Variation in nature: its implications for zoo elephant management. Zoo Biol. 2006; 25: 161–171.
  2. 2. Rowell Z. Locomotion in captive Asian elephants (Elephas maximus). Journal of Zoo and Aquarium Research. 2014; 2(4): 130–135.
  3. 3. Leighty KA, Soltis J, Wesolek CM, Savage A, Mellen J, Lehnhardt J. GPS determination of walking rates in captive African elephants (Loxodonta africana). Zoo Biol. 2009; 28(1): 16–28. pmid:19358315
  4. 4. Slotow R, van Dyk G. Ranging of older male elephants introduced to an existing small population without older males: Pilanesburg National Park. Koedoe. 2004; 47(2): 91–104.
  5. 5. Whitehouse AM & Schoeman DS. Ranging behaviour of elephants within a small fenced area in Addo Elephant National Park, South Africa. Afr Zool. 2003; 38: 95–108.
  6. 6. Gadd ME. The impact of elephants on the marula tree Sclerocarya birrea. Afr J Ecol. 2002; 40: 328–336.
  7. 7. Weir JS. Chemical properties and occurrence on Kalahari sand of salt licks created by elephants. J Zool. 1969; 158: 293–310.
  8. 8. Weir JS. Spatial distribution of elephants in an African National Park in relation to environmental sodium. Oikos. 1972; 23: 1–13.
  9. 9. Loarie SR, van Aarde RJ, Pimm SL. Elephant seasonal vegetation preferences across dry and wet savannas. Biol Conserv. 2009; 142(12): 3099–3107.
  10. 10. Stokke S, du Toit JT. Sexual segregation in habitat use by elephants in Chobe National Park, Botswana. Afr J Ecol. 2002; 40: 360–371.
  11. 11. Mench JA. Environmental enrichment and the importance of exploratory behavior. In: Shepherdson DJ, Mellen JD, Hutchins M, editors. Second Nature: Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals. Washington DC: Smithsonian Institution Press; 1998. p. 30–46.
  12. 12. Shepherdson DJ, Carlstead K, Mellen JD, Seidensticker J. The influence of food presentation on the behavior of small cats in confined environments. Zoo Biol. 1993; 12: 203–216.
  13. 13. Franks B, Champagne FA, Higgins ET. How enrichment affects exploration trade-offs in rats: Implications for welfare and well-being. PLoS One. 2013; 8(12): e83578. pmid:24376721
  14. 14. Rushen J. Changing concepts of farm animal welfare: Bridging the gap between applied and basic research. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2003; 81: 199–214.
  15. 15. Hassmen P, Koivula N, Uutela A. Physical exercise and psychological well-being: A population study in Finland. Prev Med. 2000; 30: 17–25. pmid:10642456
  16. 16. Salmon P. Effects of physical exercise on anxiety, depression and sensitivity to stress—A unifying theory. Clin Psychol Rev. 2001; 21(1): 33–61. pmid:11148895
  17. 17. Kopp M, Steinlechner M, Ruedl G, Ledochowski L, Rumpold G, Taylor AH. Acute effects of brisk walking on affect and psychological well-being in individuals with type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Res Clin Pract. 2012; 95(1): 25–9. pmid:21995867
  18. 18. Ekelund U, Ward HA, Norat T, Luan J, May AM, Weiderpass E, et al. Physical activity and all-cause mortality across levels of overall and abdominal adiposity in European men and women: the European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition Study (EPIC). Am J Clin Nutr. 2015;
  19. 19. Menor-Campos DJ, Molleda-Carbonell JM, López-Rodríguez R. Effects of exercise and human contact on animal welfare in a dog shelter. Vet Rec. 2011; 169: 388. pmid:21862471
  20. 20. Lewis KD, Shepherdson DJ, Owens TM, Keele M. A Survey of Elephant Husbandry and Foot Health in North American Zoos. Zoo Biol. 2010; 29: 221–236. pmid:20014111
  21. 21. Morfeld KA, Meehan CL, Hogan JN, Brown JL. Asseessment of body condition in African (Loxodonta africana) and Asian (Elephas maximus) elephants in North American zoos and management practices associated with high body condition scores. PLoS ONE 2016. doi: pone.0155146.
  22. 22. Whisher L, Raum M, Pina L, Pérez L, Erb H, Houpt C, Houpt K. Effects of environmental factors on cribbing activity by horses. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2011; 135: 63–69.
  23. 23. Lee J, Floyd T, Erb H, Houpt K. Preference and demand for exercise in stabled horses. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2011; 130: 91–100.
  24. 24. Popescu S, Borda C, Diugan EA, Spinu M, Groza IS, Sandru CD. Dairy cows welfare quality in tie-stall housing system with or without access to exercise. Acta Vet Scand. 2013; 55: 43. pmid:23724804
  25. 25. Carlstead K, Mench JA, Meehan CL, Brown JL. An epidemiological approach to welfare research in zoos: the Elephant Welfare Project. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. 2013; 16(4): 319–37. pmid:24079487
  26. 26. NOAA [Internet]. North Carolina: Quality Controlled Local Climatological Data (QCLCD) provided by NOAA/NESDIS/NCDC [cited 2011]. Available from: http://cdo.ncdc.noaa.gov/qclcd/QCLCD.
  27. 27. Rothwell ES, Bercovitch FB, Andrews JR, Anderson MJ. Estimating daily walking distance of captive African elephants using an accelerometer. Zoo Biol. 2011; 30(5): 579–591. pmid:21971913
  28. 28. Miller L, Andrews J, Anderson M. Validating methods to determine walking rates of elephants within a zoological institution. Anim Welf. 2012; 21(4): 577–582.
  29. 29. Holdgate MR, Meehan CL, Hogan JN, Miller LJ, Rushen J, de Passillé AM, Soltis J, Andrews J, Shepherdson DJ. Recumbence behavior in zoo elephants: determination of patterns and frequency of recumbent rest and associated environmental and social factors. PLoS ONE 2016. doi: pone.0153301.
  30. 30. Leighty KA, Soltis J, Savage A. GPS assessment of the use of exhibit space and resources by African elephants (Loxodonta africana). Zoo Biol. 2010; 29(2): 210–220. pmid:19418496
  31. 31. Soulsby KS. Use of a tri-axial accelerometer, behavioral observation, and GPS to monitor the activity of female Asian elephants in a zoo. M.Sc. Thesis, The University of Texas at Arlington. 2013.
  32. 32. Hacker CE, Horback KM, Miller LJ. GPS technology as a proxy tool for determining relationships in social animals: An example with African elephants. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2015; 163: 175–182.
  33. 33. Langley RB. Dilution of precision. GPS World. 1999; 10(5): 52–59.
  34. 34. Witte TH, Wilson AM. Accuracy of WAAS-enabled GPS for the determination of position and speed over ground. Journal of Biomechanics. 2005; 38(8): 1717–1722. pmid:15958230
  35. 35. Jones D, McGreevy JE. How much space does an elephant need? The impact of confinement on animal welfare. J Vet Behav. 2007; 2: 185–187.
  36. 36. Meehan CL, Hogan JN, Bonaparte-Saller M, Mench JA. Housing and social environments of African (Loxodonta africana) and Asian (Elephas maximus) elephants in North American zoos. PLoS ONE 2016. doi: pone.0146703.
  37. 37. Greco BJ, Meehan CL, Miller LJ, Shepherdson DJ, Morfeld KA, Andrews J, Baker AM, Carlstead K, Mench JA. Elephant management in North American zoological facilities: environmental enrichment, feeding, exercise, and training. PLoS ONE 2016. doi:pone.0152490.
  38. 38. Prado-Oviedo NA, Bonaparte-Saller MK, Malloy E, Meehan CL, Mench JA, Carlstead K. et al. Evaluation of demographics and social life events of Asian (Elephas maximus) and African elephants (Loxodonta africana) in North American zoos. PloS One 2016. doi: pone.0154750.
  39. 39. Hanley JA. Statistical Analysis of Correlated Data Using Generalized Estimating Equations: An Orientation. Am J Epidemiol. 2003;157(4):364–75. pmid:12578807
  40. 40. Hardin JW, Hilbe JM. Generalized Estimating Equations. 2 ed: Chapman and Hall / CRC; 2013.
  41. 41. Liang K, Zeger SL. Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models. Biometrika. 1986;73(1):13–22.
  42. 42. Stene LC, Magnus P, Lie RT, Søvik O, Joner G. Birth weight and childhood onset type 1 diabetes: population based cohort study. BMJ. 2001;322:889–92. pmid:11302899
  43. 43. Ozminkowski RJ, Ling D, Goetzel RZ, Bruno JA, Rutter KR, Isaac F, et al. Long-term impact of Johnson & Johnson's health and wellness program on health care utilization and expenditures. J Occup Environ Med. 2002;44:21–9. pmid:11802462
  44. 44. Miller MA, Hogan JN, Meehan CL. Housing and demographic risk factors impacting foot and musculoskeletal health in African elephants (Loxodonta africana) and Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) in North American zoos. PLoS ONE 2016. doi: pone.0155223.
  45. 45. Greco BJ, Meehan CL, Hogan JN, Leighty KA, Mellen J, Mason GJ, Mench JA. The days and nights of zoo elephants: using epidemiology to better understand stereotypic behavior of African elephants (Loxodonta africana) and Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) in North American zoos. PLoS ONE 2016. doi: pone.0144276.
  46. 46. Sukumar R. The living elephants: evolutionary ecology, behavior, and conservation. New York: Oxford University Press; 2003.
  47. 47. Wittemyer G, Douglas-Hamilton I, Getz WM. The socioecology of elephants: analysis of the processes creating multitiered social structures. Anim Behav. 2005; 69: 1357–1371.
  48. 48. Archie EA, Moss CJ, Alberts SC. The ties that bind: genetic relatedness predicts the fission and fusion of social groups in wild African elephants. Proc R Soc B. 2006; 273: 513–522. pmid:16537121
  49. 49. Moss CJ. Female social dynamics: fidelity and flexibility. In: Moss CJ, Croze H, Lee PC, editors. The Amboseli elephants: A long-term perspective on a long-lived mammal. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2011. p. 205–223.
  50. 50. De Silva S Ranjeewa ADG, Kryazhimskiy S. The dynamics of social networks among female Asian elephants. BMC Ecology. 2011; 11:17. pmid:21794147
  51. 51. Brown JL, Paris S, Prado-Oviedo NA, Meehan CL, Hogan JN, Morfeld KA, Carlstead K. Reproductive health assessment of female elephants in North American zoos and association of husbandry practices with reproductive dysfunction in African elephants (Loxodonta africana). PLoS ONE 2016. doi: pone.0145673.
  52. 52. Mason GJ. Sterotypies: A critical review. Anim Behav. 1991; 41: 1015–1037.
  53. 53. Mason GJ, Rushen J, editors. Stereotypic animal behaviour: Fundamentals and applications to welfare. 2nd ed. Cambridge MA: CABI; 2006.
  54. 54. Shepherdson D, Lewis KD, Bauman J, Carlstead K, Perrin N. Individual and environmental factors associated with stereotypic behavior and fecal glucocorticoid metabolite levels in zoo housed polar bears. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2013; 147: 268–277.
  55. 55. Clubb R, Mason G. Animal welfare: Captivity effects on wide-ranging carnivores. Nature. 2003; 425: 473–474. pmid:14523435
  56. 56. Mikota SK, Sargent EL, Ranglack G. Medical Management of the Elephant. Ithaca NY: Indira Publishing House; 1994.
  57. 57. Fowler ME. An overview of foot conditions in Asian and African elephants. In: Csuti B, Sargent EL, Bechert US, editors. The elephant's foot: Prevention and care of foot conditions in captive Asian and African elephants. Ames IA: John Wiley & Sons; 2001. pp. 3–7.
  58. 58. Taylor VJ, Poole TB. Captive breeding and infant mortality in Asian elephants: A comparison between twenty western zoos and three eastern elephant centers. Zoo Biol. 1998; 17: 311–332.
  59. 59. Brown JL. Reproductive endocrine monitoring of elephants: An essential tool for assisting captive management. Zoo Biol. 2000; 19: 347–368.
  60. 60. Brown JL, Walker S, Moeller T. Comparative endocrinology of cycling and noncycling Asian (Elephas maximus) and African (Loxodonta africana) elephants. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2004; 136: 360–370. pmid:15081836
  61. 61. Clubb R, Lee P, Mar KU, Moss C, Rowcliffe M, Mason GJ. Compromised survivorship in zoo elephants. Science. 2008; 322(5908): 1649. pmid:19074339
  62. 62. Clubb R, Rowcliffe M, Lee P, Mar KU, Moss C, Mason GJ. Fecundity and population viability in female zoo elephants: problems and possible solutions. Anim Welfare. 2009; 18(3): 237–247.
  63. 63. Mason GJ, Veasey JS. What do population-level welfare indices suggest about the well-being of zoo elephants? Zoo Biol. 2010; 29: 256–273. pmid:20140956
  64. 64. Morfeld KA, Lehnhardt J, Alligood C, Bolling J, Brown JL. Development of a body condition scoring index for female African elephants validated by ultrasound measurements of subcutaneous fat. PLoS One. 2014; 9: e93802. pmid:24718304
  65. 65. Barbour SW, Wakshlag JJ, Maley M, Farrell TJ, Struble AM, Panasevich MR et al. Use of pedometers to measure the relationship of dog walking to body condition score in obese and non-obese dogs. Brit J of Nutr. 2011; 106: S85–S89.