Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Augmented Pain Processing in Primary and Secondary Somatosensory Cortex in Fibromyalgia: A Magnetoencephalography Study Using Intra-Epidermal Electrical Stimulation

  • Manyoel Lim,

    Affiliation Neuroscience Research Institute, Seoul National University College of Medicine, Seoul, Korea

  • Meyke Roosink,

    Affiliations Neuroscience Research Institute, Seoul National University College of Medicine, Seoul, Korea, Department of Rehabilitation, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, the Netherlands

  • June Sic Kim,

    Affiliation Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Seoul National University College of Natural Sciences, Seoul, Korea

  • Hye Won Kim,

    Affiliations Division of Rheumatology, Department of Internal Medicine, Seoul National University College of Medicine, Seoul, Korea, Division of Rheumatology, Department of Internal Medicine, Eulji University School of Medicine, Eulji General Hospital, Seoul, Korea

  • Eun Bong Lee,

    Affiliation Division of Rheumatology, Department of Internal Medicine, Seoul National University College of Medicine, Seoul, Korea

  • Kyeong Min Son,

    Affiliation Department of Internal Medicine, Hallym University College of Medicine, Chuncheon, Korea

  • Hyun Ah Kim,

    Affiliations Department of Internal Medicine, Hallym University College of Medicine, Chuncheon, Korea, Division of Rheumatology, Department of Internal Medicine, Hallym University Sacred Heart Hospital, Anyang, Korea

  • Chun Kee Chung

    chungc@snu.ac.kr

    Affiliations Neuroscience Research Institute, Seoul National University College of Medicine, Seoul, Korea, Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Seoul National University College of Natural Sciences, Seoul, Korea, Department of Neurosurgery, Seoul National University Hospital, Seoul, Korea

Abstract

The aim of this study was to investigate augmented pain processing in the cortical somatosensory system in patients with fibromyalgia (FM). Cortical evoked responses were recorded in FM (n = 19) and healthy subjects (n = 21) using magnetoencephalography after noxious intra-epidermal electrical stimulation (IES) of the hand dorsum (pain rating 6 on a numeric rating scale, perceptually-equivalent). In addition, healthy subjects were stimulated using the amplitude corresponding to the average stimulus intensity rated 6 in patients with FM (intensity-equivalent). Quantitative sensory testing was performed on the hand dorsum or thenar muscle (neutral site) and over the trapezius muscle (tender point), using IES (thresholds, ratings, temporal summation of pain, stimulus-response curve) and mechanical stimuli (threshold, ratings). Increased amplitude of cortical responses was found in patients with FM as compared to healthy subjects. These included the contralateral primary (S1) and bilateral secondary somatosensory cortices (S2) in response to intensity-equivalent stimuli and the contralateral S1 and S2 in response to perceptually-equivalent stimuli. The amplitude of the contralateral S2 response in patients with FM was positively correlated with average pain intensity over the last week. Quantitative sensory testing results showed that patients with FM were more sensitive to painful IES as well as to mechanical stimulation, regardless of whether the stimulation site was the hand or the trapezius muscle. Interestingly, the slope of the stimulus-response relationship as well as temporal summation of pain in response to IES was not different between groups. Together, these results suggest that the observed pain augmentation in response to IES in patients with FM could be due to sensitization or disinhibition of the cortical somatosensory system. Since the S2 has been shown to play a role in higher-order functions, further studies are needed to clarify the role of augmented S2 response in clinical characteristics of FM.

Introduction

Fibromyalgia (FM) is commonly associated with chronic wide-spread pain, but is notorious for its multi-modal symptoms such as anxiety and depression, sleep disturbances, fatigue and cognitive symptoms [1]. Typically, patients report pain upon palpation of pre-defined tender point sites, and the number of painful tender points (at least 11 out of 18) has been commonly used as a diagnostic criterion for FM (American College of Rheumatology, 1990) [2].

Previous studies of FM have shown that pain sensitivity [37], temporal summation of pain [811], and cortical evoked responses [1218] are generally increased in patients with FM as compared to healthy control (HC) subjects, and that patients with FM often report longer and different after-sensations [3, 8]. Moreover, patients with FM may show reduced endogenous pain inhibitory functions [5, 6, 1921] and abnormal responses to innocuous somatosensory [5, 2224], auditory [2527], and olfactory stimuli [28]. Together, these findings suggest that sensory augmentation could be “generalized”, i.e. occurring at the central level and not being specific for pain, although some inconsistencies remain [4, 2931].

What remains largely unclear from studies assessing sensory symptoms and signs in patients with FM is how different outcome measures relate to each other. This is particularly important since patients with FM are notorious for their heterogeneity, and symptoms tend to evolve over time. Also, the pathophysiology of FM, and in particular the relative contributions of different pain mechanisms (e.g. increased facilitation and/or decreased inhibition) at different neural levels (i.e. peripheral afferent, spinal cord and/or brain), remains a matter of debate [1, 28, 3234]. A better understanding of the mechanisms underlying pain augmentation may help to develop pathophysiological models of FM and may contribute to improved diagnosis and treatment.

With respect to the stimulation method, a recent magnetoencephalography (MEG) study used painful and non-painful mechanical pressure stimuli that were subjectively matched between patients with FM and HC subjects [17]. Since painful mechanical pressure stimulation typically activates Aβ-, Aδ- and C-fibers, it is necessary to subtract the Aβ-fiber mediated response to assess the pain-related brain response, as in previous functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies in FM [13]. Otherwise, both intra-epidermal electrical stimulation (IES) [3540] and laser stimulation [4144] have been regarded as validated methods for selective activation of Aδ nociceptors in the epidermis. Prior MEG studies in combination with IES [40, 45, 46] or laser stimulation [4244, 47, 48] have reported activation of several brain areas involved in the processing of the sensory-discriminative component of pain, such as the primary (S1) and secondary (S2) somatosensory cortices and/or posterior parietal cortex (PPC). Also recently, Mouraux et al. (2014) showed that trains of repeated IES with a short inter-stimulus interval can be used to increase the intensity of perception and brain responses [38].

The present study aimed to investigate the central augmentation of pain processing in patients FM using MEG and IES which can selectively stimulate cutaneous Aδ nociceptors [35]. Quantitative sensory testing was performed on the hand dorsum or thenar muscle (neutral site) and over the trapezius muscle (tender point), using IES (thresholds, ratings, temporal summation of pain) and mechanical stimuli (threshold, ratings). In addition, the nature of pain augmentation (e.g. higher offset and/or increased slope) was assessed using a stimulus-response curve for standardized IES on the hand dorsum. Cortical pain processing of noxious stimuli was prioritized for the estimation in the S1 and S2 [46].

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Subjects were right-handed (as confirmed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory) [49], females and aged between 30 and 60 years (during which patients were more likely to be diagnosed as having primary FM). Subjects were excluded from participation if they presented with contraindications for MEG and/or MRI assessment, pregnancy, a medical history of a psychiatric disease or a disorder affecting the central nervous system (i.e. cerebrovascular accident, multiple sclerosis, Parkinson’s disease), or signs of peripheral neuropathy of the upper extremities (e.g. related to trauma, polyneuropathy). Additional inclusion and exclusion criteria were specified for patients and HC subjects separately as mentioned below.

Patients.

Patients were recruited from the outpatient clinics of the rheumatology departments of the Seoul National University Hospital and Hallym University Sacred Heart Hospital. Patients were eligible for participation if they were clinically diagnosed as having primary FM (according to the American College of Rheumatology 1990 criteria [2] that are still commonly used for research purposes [21, 23, 50, 51]), their disease duration was at least 3 months, they reported an average pain intensity of at least 40 on a 0−100 mm visual analogue scale (VAS; 0 mm = no pain, 100 mm = worst pain imaginable) during the past week, and they were willing to stop taking medications known to influence the somatosensory system (e.g. analgesics, antidepressants, anticonvulsants) at least 3 days prior to assessments. In addition to the general exclusion criteria, patient-specific exclusion criteria were secondary FM, concomitant chronic pain of other etiology (i.e. rheumatoid arthritis, osteoarthritis), concomitant acute pain in the upper extremities (e.g. due to injury).

Healthy controls.

HC subjects were recruited through local advertisements and were matched for age and gender to the FM group. In addition to the general exclusion criteria, control-specific exclusion criteria were chronic pain complaints of any kind, and acute pain at the time of the assessments.

Ethics.

The study protocol was approved by the institutional review board at Seoul National University Hospital (H-1107-013-367) and Hallym University Sacred Heart Hospital (2011-I048) and the study was conducted in compliance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All subjects received verbal information about the study protocol and signed the informed consent form prior to participation.

Demographics and medical data

For all subjects, age (years), marital status (single, married, separated/divorced), education (years), employment status (employed, unemployed/housewife), and medication use were registered. For patients with FM, the disease duration (since onset of wide-spread pain) and number of tender points (as assessed by the treating rheumatologist using manual palpation upon inclusion) were registered. The tender point count was (re-)assessed by a trained rheumatologist from Seoul National University Hospital (H.W.K) in both patients and HC subjects using a hand-held pressure algometer (Baseline Evaluation Instruments, Fabrication Enterprises, New York, USA). In addition, subjects filled out the Fibromyalgia Impact Questionnaire (FIQ, patients only) [52], Beck’s Anxiety Inventory (BAI) [53], Beck’s Depression Inventory (BDI) [54], and Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) [55].

The intensity of average pain over the last week was assessed using a VAS. The qualitative aspects of clinical pain perception were assessed using the short-form McGill pain questionnaire (SF-MPQ) [56].

Quantitative sensory testing

All tests were performed on the right (dominant) upper extremity (hand and trapezius muscle). The test order was fixed. All tests were performed before MEG recordings.

Mechanical stimuli.

The tactile detection threshold (TDT) was determined using 5 Semmes Weinstein monofilaments (sizes: 2.83, 3.61, 4.31, 4.56, 6.65; Baseline Evaluation Instruments, Fabrication Enterprises, New York, USA). The filaments were applied at 2 locations over the hand dorsum and at 2 locations over the trapezius muscle. For each body region, the TDT was defined as the smallest filament that could be perceived at each location. In addition, subjects rated the perceived painfulness of a standardized pinprick stimulus (PIN, 6.65 Semmes-Weinstein filament).

The pressure pain threshold (PPT) was determined using an experimenter-operated pressure algometer (Baseline Evaluation Instruments, Fabrication Enterprises, New York, USA). An in-house Teflon stimulation surface of 1 cm2 and a slope of 0.5 kg per second were used. Subjects were instructed to keep their arm in zero degrees of shoulder abduction and 90 degrees of elbow flexion. The PPT was then determined at 3 locations over the thenar eminence of the hand and at 3 locations over the trapezius muscle [4, 57, 58]. In response to the increasing pressure delivered by the experimenter, subjects were instructed to verbally indicate when they first perceived the pressure as painful. The 3 PPTs for each body region were averaged for further analysis.

For all mechanical thresholds, subjects rated the average perceived painfulness directly after threshold determination using a numeric rating scale (NRS; 0 = no pain, 10 = worst pain imaginable). All mechanical stimuli were delivered by a trained experimenter.

Electrical stimuli.

The electrical sensory thresholds were determined using an experimenter-operated Digitimer DS7AH, which is a high voltage constant current stimulator (Digitimer Ltd., United Kingdom), and an intra-epidermal electrode (Nihon-Kohden, Tokyo, Japan) (pulse width: 1 ms) [36, 39]. The intra-epidermal electrode was placed over the skin between the first and second meta-carpal bones of the hand dorsum (first assessment) or over the skin overlying the trapezius muscle (second assessment). The electrical sensation threshold (EST) was determined by manually increasing the stimulator current output in steps of 0.05 mA. The subject had to indicate verbally when she first perceived the stimulus (typically a faint sensation). The electrical pain threshold (EPT) was determined by manually increasing the stimulator current output in steps of 0.3 mA. The subject had to verbally indicate when she first perceived the stimulus as being sharp and painful. The electrical pain tolerance threshold (EPTT) was determined by manually increasing the stimulator current output in steps of 0.5 mA. The subject had to verbally indicate when she first perceived the stimulus as being very sharp and painful, and was not willing to tolerate a higher stimulus amplitude. Prior to the actual assessments, subjects were trained to determine these thresholds reliably at both locations. All thresholds were determined 3 times for each body region. After each third assessment, subjects rated the average perceived painfulness using an NRS. Thresholds were averaged for each body region separately for further analysis.

Stimulus-response curve.

A series of 12 standardized IES were presented to each subject on the hand dorsum. Stimulus intensities were 25%, 100%, 175% and 250% of the individual EPT, and were presented in a semi-randomized order, so that all intensities were used 3 times. The inter-stimulus interval was approximately 5–10 seconds. Subjects were asked to rate the pain intensity of each stimulus using an NRS. Ratings were averaged according to stimulus intensity for further analysis.

Temporal summation of pain.

Lastly, subjects were asked to rate the pain intensity of a single IES and the pain intensity of the last IES in a 1 Hz pulse train of 5 pulses using an NRS. Pulse trains were generated using Stim2 software (Compumedics Neuroscan, Charlotte, North Carolina, USA). The stimulus intensity equaled the individual EPT. Each series of pulses (single, train) was rated 3 times, and then averaged. Temporal summation of pain could not be determined in 1 patient with FM due to technical problems.

MEG responses to IES

Experimental conditions.

An intra-epidermal electrode connected to a Digitimer DS7AH (Digitimer Ltd., United Kingdom) operated by Stim2 Software (Compumedics Neuroscan, Charlotte, North Carolina, USA), was used to apply single pulse IES to the dorsum of the right hand [35]. A pulse width of 1 ms was used.

For patients, the stimulus intensity (mA) corresponded to moderately painful stimulation with a perceptual pain rating of 6 on the NRS. This was determined after assessing electrical quantitative sensory testing (QST) on the hand, by manually increasing the stimulator current output in steps of 0.3 mA. For HC subjects, a perceptually-equivalent (HC-PE) stimulus and an intensity-equivalent (HC-IE) stimulus were used. For HC-PE, the stimulus intensity corresponded to moderately painful stimulation with a perceptual pain rating of 6 on the NRS, and it was determined for each subject individually as described for patients with FM. For HC-IE, the stimulus intensity was fixed at 2.66 mA, corresponding to the stimulus intensity necessary to evoke an average pain rating of 6 on the NRS in patients with FM (n = 7). The order of the stimulus condition (HC-IE or HC-PE) was randomized across subjects.

For each stimulus condition, a total number of 100 trials were recorded. The inter-stimulus interval was 4–6 seconds (randomized). Subjects were instructed not to pay attention to the stimuli and to fix their gaze on a cross in the center of the front wall.

MEG recordings.

The MEG signals were recorded in a magnetically shielded room using a helmet-shaped whole-head neuromagnetometer with a sensor array of 102 identical triple sensors (VectorViewTM, Elektra Neuromag Oy, Helsinki, Finland). Each sensor consisted of 2 orthogonal planar gradiometers and 1 magnetometer. Subjects were seated comfortably under the helmet-shaped sensor array. The exact location of the head relative to the sensor array was determined by placing 4 head position indicator coils at known sites on the scalp, and the magnetic signal produced by electrical currents delivered to each of these 4 head position indicator coils was subsequently measured. The location of the head position indicator coils relative to 3 anatomical landmarks, the nasion and the 2 preauricular points, was measured using a 3-dimensional digitizer (FASTRAKTM, Polhemus, Colchester, Vermont, USA). In this way, MEG and MRI coordinate systems could be aligned [59]. The x-axis passed through the 2 preauricular points (right = positive), the y-axis (positive) passed through the nasion, and the z-axis (positive) pointed upward. High-resolution T1-weighted MR images were obtained from all subjects, except for one control subject due to claustrophobia, using a Siemens 3 T scanner (Siemens Magnetom TrioTim, Siemens, Erlangen, Germany).

The MEG signals were recorded with a band-pass filter of 0.1–300 Hz and were digitized at 1 kHz. Environmental and biological noises were reduced by applying the spatiotemporal space separation method to the acquired MEG signals using MaxFilter software version 2.2.10 (Elekta Neuromag Oy, Helsinki, Finland) [60].

MEG analysis.

Epochs with amplitudes exceeding 3000 fT/cm (MEG) or 150 μV (electrooculogram) during the time window for analysis (100 ms pre-stimulus to 400 ms post-stimulus) were excluded from the analysis. All remaining epochs were averaged according to the stimulation intensity level. A minimum of 80 epochs were needed for subjects to be included in the analyses.

Source modeling was based on the signals recorded by the 204 gradiometers in individual subjects in response to NRS6 (patients) and HC-PE (HC subjects) stimulation. The cortical sources were modelled in the time domain as equivalent current dipoles (ECD), using Elekta Neuromag software (Elekta Neuromag Oy, Helsinki, Finland) [59]. A spherical volume conductor was used to model the head. For the subject in whom brain MRI was not available, a sphere model was fitted to accurately digitized isotrak points using MaxFilter software [61]. The ECDs that best explained the measured data were determined by a least-squares search at the latency of the peak amplitude, using a subset of 18–24 gradiometer channels over the maximum response area. This resulted in the respective three-dimensional location, orientation and strength of the ECDs in a spherical volume conductor model of the head. Only ECDs with a goodness-of-fit ≥ 80% of the field variance were accepted for further analysis [47, 62].

A source corresponding to the contralateral S1 (cS1) was found in 14 FM and 15 HC subjects. Sources corresponding to the contralateral secondary (cS2) and ipsilateral secondary (iS2) somatosensory cortex were found in all but one FM subject. A source corresponding to the contralateral PPC was found in 2 FM and 2 HC subjects. For the subject in whom brain MRI was not available, sources were identified by considering coordinates, peak latencies and orientations of the modelled sources. After identifying the single dipoles for each subject, the entire time window for analysis and all channels were taken into account for computing an individual time-varying multi-dipole model with fixed ECDs. For those subjects in whom a PPC source was identified, this source was included in the multi-dipole model to improve its goodness-of-fit. However, no further analysis relating to PPC activation was performed since only 2 PPC data sets would be available per group. For HC subjects, the sources estimated based on the HC-PE condition were applied to the signals recorded for the HC-IE condition [63]. For each subject and stimulation intensity level, the response peak latencies and amplitudes of the ECDs corresponding to the cS1, cS2 and iS2 sources were determined and used for further analysis.

Individual MRIs were spatially normalized to the Talairach coordinates using BrainVoyager QX software (version 1.10; Brain Innovation, Maastricht, The Netherlands) according to Lim et al. 2011 and 2015 [23, 64]. Locations of the cS1, cS2 and iS2 sources in the head coordinates were transformed into Talairach coordinates using Brain Electrical Source Analysis software (version 5.1.8; MEGIS software, Munich, Germany). Twenty HC subjects whose MRI data were available were included in deriving the source location in the Talairach coordinates.

Statistical analysis

All statistical testing was performed using SPSS 13.0 for Windows (SPSS Inc. Chicago, IL, USA).

Demographic and medical data.

For all demographic and medical data and pain assessments, frequencies (binary data) or averages and standard deviations (continuous data) were calculated for each group, and compared using appropriate statistical tests (independent t-tests and X2-tests). All tests were 2-tailed. A p-value less than 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Quantitative sensory testing.

QST parameters (thresholds, ratings, stimulus-response curve, temporal summation of pain) were generally not normally distributed. As such, median and range were calculated for thresholds and threshold ratings. For presentation purposes, the stimulus-response curves and results for temporal summation of pain are presented using mean ± SEM. Prior to statistical analyses, all individual raw QST parameters were log-transformed to allow for testing under normality assumptions [58].

For QST thresholds and ratings (TDT, PIN, PPT, EST, EPT, EPTT), differences between groups were statistically tested using repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVA) with factors [Site] (hand, trapezius muscle) and [Group] (FM, HC). For the stimulus-response curve, ratings for the series of fixed stimulus amplitudes at 25%, 100%, 175% and 250% of the EPT were compared between groups using a repeated measures ANOVA with factors [Intensity] (25%, 50%, 100%, 175%) and [Group] (FM, HC). For temporal summation of pain, the ratings for the single pulse and pulse train stimulation at 100% of the EPT, were compared between groups using a repeated measures analysis with factors [Number of pulses] (1, 5) and [Group] (FM, HC). Differential effects of temporal summation of pain were tested as interactions between the number of pulses and group as in a previous study [10]. The influence of potential confounders was assessed using additional multivariate analyses with dependent [QST threshold and/or rating] (TDT, PIN, PPT, EST, EPT, EPTT), factor [Group] (FM, HC) and covariate [potential confounder] (BAI, BDI, PSQI). All factors were entered into the analyses separately. Although the covariates BDI (PPT) and PSQI (TDT, EST) were significant upon multivariate testing, associations for independent testing were not consistent. Moreover, in all cases, a significant effect of [Group] remained. We therefore considered systematic confounding by any of these variables highly unlikely.

MEG data.

The peak latencies and amplitudes of the bilateral S2 sources were analysed using repeated measures ANOVA with a within-subjects factor [Source] (cS2, iS2) and between-subjects factor [Group] (FM, HC-IE or HC-PE). Mauchly's test of sphericity was used to evaluate the assumption of sphericity. Significant between-subjects effects were further tested using independent t-tests. The between-group differences in peak latencies and amplitudes of the cS1 source were compared by independent t-tests with factor [Group] (FM, HC-IE or HC-PE). Talairach coordinates of the cS1, cS2 and iS2 sources were compared separately by means of repeated measures ANOVA with coordinate (x, y and z) as a within-subject factor and group (FM and HC) as a between-subject factor. The relationship between the amplitude of cortical responses and FM pain intensity as reported for the last week (VAS) was assessed using Pearson’s correlation coefficient (r). The influence of potential confounders on bilateral S2 source amplitudes was assessed using a multivariate analysis with a dependent variables [cS2, iS2], factor [Group] (FM, HC-IE or HC-PE) and covariate [potential confounder] (BAI, BDI and PSQI). All factors were entered into the analyses separately. There were no significant effects for any of the covariates. The influence of potential confounders on cS1 source amplitudes was assessed using a multivariate analysis with dependent variable [cS1], factor [Group] (FM, HC-IE or HC-PE) and covariate [potential confounder] (BAI, BDI and PSQI). All factors were entered into the analyses separately. Although the covariate BAI was significant, the significant effect of [Group] remained. In summary, it is unlikely that the amplitudes of cortical sources were confounded by group differences in clinical variables relating to anxiety, depression or sleep quality.

Results

A total of 19 patients with FM and 21 HC subjects participated in the experiments.

Demographics & medical data

A summary of demographics and medical data is presented in Table 1. Patients with FM showed higher anxiety scores (BAI), higher depression scores (BDI), reduced sleep quality (PSQI) (p < 0.001), and an increased number of painful tender points upon manual palpation or pressure algometry (p < 0.001).

thumbnail
Table 1. Demographics and clinical characteristics of the participants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151776.t001

Quantitative sensory testing

Thresholds and ratings.

A summary of the median and range of thresholds and ratings as obtained with QST is presented in Table 2. For all subjects, TDT thresholds were found to be within the normal range (Semmes Weinstein filament sizes 3.61 or 4.31) and were not analysed further. Statistical analyses revealed significant differences between groups, indicating that QST thresholds (PPT, EPT, EPTT) were lower and QST ratings (PIN, PPT, EST, EPT, EPTT) were higher in patients with FM as compared to HC subjects. With respect to the 2 stimulation sites, the PPT was found to be significantly lower (p < 0.001) and the PIN rating was significantly higher (p = 0.003) for stimulation of the trapezius muscle. No significant interaction effects were found (p > 0.05), suggesting that the effect of [Site] was similar in both groups.

Stimulus-response curve.

The mean NRS scores corresponding to fixed levels of IES are presented for both groups in Fig 1A. Statistical analysis showed that, compared to HC subjects, patients with FM rated the stimuli as more painful (p = 0.006). In both groups, NRS ratings increased when stimulus intensity was increased (p < 0.001). No significant interaction effects were found (p = 0.099), suggesting that this effect of stimulus intensity was similar in both groups.

thumbnail
Fig 1.

Stimulus-response curve (A) and temporal summation of pain (B) in response to standardized intra-epidermal electrical stimulation. Grey squares/bars represent patients with FM (A: n = 19, B: n = 18), white squares/bars represent HC subjects (n = 21). The levels of stimulation (x-axis) were based on individual pain thresholds. PT_25%, PT_100%, PT_175% and PT_250% correspond to 25%, 100%, 175% and 250% of the pain threshold intensity (mA), respectively. Data are expressed as the mean ± SEM.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151776.g001

Temporal summation of pain.

Fig 1B presents the NRS ratings in response to standardized electrical stimulation with 1 and 5 pulses. Statistical analysis showed that, compared to HC subjects, patients with FM rated the stimuli as more painful (p = 0.009). In both groups, NRS ratings increased when the number of pulses was increased (p = 0.001). No significant interaction effects were found (p = 0.315). As such, the effect of the number of pulses was similar in both groups, suggesting that temporal summation of pain in patients with FM was comparable to that in HC subjects.

MEG responses to IES

The mean stimulus intensity in patients with FM was 2.89 ± 0.89 mA (NRS6). The mean stimulus intensity in HC subjects was 5.60 ± 1.95 mA (NRS6 = HC-PE). Stimulus intensities for NRS 6 were significantly lower in patients with FM as compared to HC subjects (t = 5.562, p < 0.001). Fixed stimulus intensity of 2.66 mA in HC-IE was comparable to mean stimulus intensity in patients with FM (2.89 ± 0.89 mA, NRS6).

The spatial distribution of the somatosensory evoked fields in response to noxious IES of the right hand dorsum in representative FM (A) and HC (B) subjects is presented in Fig 2. In all subjects, clear long-latency responses were observed in the contralateral parietal area (Fig 2, insert a) and the bilateral temporoparietal areas (Fig 2, insert b and c). These responses could be located in the posterior wall of the central sulcus corresponding to the location of S1, and in the upper bank of the bilateral Sylvian fissure corresponding to S2, respectively. The averaged MEG data of all subjects is in S1 Dataset (see S1 Table for description).

thumbnail
Fig 2.

Spatial distribution of the MEG responses in a representative FM (A) and HC subjects (B). The head is viewed from the top. The trace of each gradiometer response pair represents the magnetic field derivation along the latitude (upper) and longitude (lower). The inserts on the right indicate the enlarged responses recorded from the contralateral S1 (a), contralateral S2 (b) and ipsilateral S2 (c) regions. The black lines represent the cortical response to the NRS 6 (FM) and perceptually-equivalent (HC) conditions. The grey line represents the cortical response to the intensity-equivalent condition (HC only). The vertical dotted lines indicate the stimulus onset.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151776.g002

Fig 3A shows the group mean location of the S1 and S2 dipole sources superimposed on the standard brain (Table 3). The results showed a main effect of coordinate (cS1, p < 0.001; cS2, p < 0.001; iS2, p < 0.001, respectively), whereas a main effect of group (cS1, p = 0.221; cS2, p = 0.355; iS2, p = 0.120, respectively) and a coordinate x group interaction effect (cS1, p = 0.242; cS2, p = 0.309; iS2, p = 0.103, respectively) were not observed. Fig 3B shows the grand averaged source waveforms based on the multi-dipole models as calculated for each group.

thumbnail
Fig 3.

Group mean source locations (A) and source waveforms (B). (A) The group mean source locations for the NRS6 condition (FM) in blue and for the perceptually-equivalent condition (HC-PE) in cyan superimposed on a standard brain. (B) The group mean source waveform as a function of time. The NRS6 condition (FM) in blue, HC-PE in cyan and the intensity-equivalent condition (HC-IE) in gray. The vertical line indicates the stimulus onset.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151776.g003

thumbnail
Table 3. Mean source locations for the NRS6 (FM) and perceptually-equivalent (HC-PE) conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151776.t003

Fig 4 shows the mean ± SEM amplitudes of the cS1 and bilateral S2 responses in both groups. For the amplitude of bilateral S2 responses, a significant main effect of group was found on comparing FM to both the HC-PE (F(1, 37) = 4.762, p = 0.036) and the HC-IE (F(1, 37) = 12.533, p = 0.001) conditions. The amplitudes of the S2 sources were significantly greater in patients with FM compared to HC subjects for both cS2 (HC-PE: t = 2.222, p = 0.032; HC-IE: t = 3.343, p = 0.002) and iS2 (HC-IE: t = 3.052, p = 0.004). The amplitude of cS1 responses was significantly greater in patients with FM compared to HC subjects for both the HC-PE (t = 2.496, p = 0.019) and HC-IE (t = 3.599, p = 0.001) conditions. Peak latencies of the S1 and S2 sources (Table 4) were not different between groups.

thumbnail
Fig 4. The amplitudes of the cS1, cS2 and iS2 in patients with FM and HC subjects.

Repeated measures ANOVAs and independent t-tests showed that cortical responses in response to intra-epidermal electrical stimulation of the hand were higher in patients with FM as compared to both perceptually-equivalent (HC-PE) and intensity-equivalent (HC-IE) conditions in HC subjects. Data are expressed as the mean ± SEM. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151776.g004

thumbnail
Table 4. Peak latencies and peak amplitudes of the S1 and S2 sources.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151776.t004

The amplitude of the cS2 response was positively correlated to the clinical pain intensity over the last week (r = 0.500, p = 0.035) (Fig 5). In an exploratory analysis, no significant correlations were found between the amplitude of the cortical response and the sensory and affective pain scores (SF-MPQ).

thumbnail
Fig 5. Relationship between the dipole amplitude in the cS2 and clinical pain intensity (VAS, mm).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151776.g005

Discussion

The present study aimed to investigate the central augmentation of pain processing in patients with FM using MEG and IES. Dipole amplitudes in the cS1, cS2 and iS2, were found to be increased in patients with FM as compared to HC subjects, and the amplitude of the cS2 response in patients with FM was positively correlated to the clinical pain intensity reported over the last week. In addition, QST findings showed that patients with FM were more sensitive to painful IES as well as to mechanical stimulation, regardless of whether the stimulation site was the hand or the trapezius muscle. Interestingly, the slope of the stimulus-response relationship as well as temporal summation of pain in response to IES was not different between groups, suggesting that the observed pain augmentation in response to IES in patients with FM could be due to sensitization or disinhibition at the cortical level.

Quantitative sensory testing

Our findings showing no differences in tactile as well as electrical detection threshold between the groups suggest that tactile sensitivity in patients with FM is generally normal. This result is in line with several other studies that showed no differences between FM patients and HC subjects in terms of tactile perception threshold [65] and electrical [66, 67] and mechanical detection threshold [4]. In contrast to the findings of tactile sensitivity, FM patients showed increased pain sensitivity to IES as well as to mechanical stimulation, regardless of whether they were stimulated at a tender point (trapezius) or a neutral site (hand), indicating the absence of specific changes at tender points. However, the stimulus-response curve slope was similar in patients with FM and HC subjects, suggesting that the increased pain sensitivity in patients with FM was due to a higher offset in stimulus intensity perception, rather than being intensity dependent. This is consistent with previous FM studies demonstrating a parallel leftward shift in the stimulus-response curve without a change in slope in response to painful heat and pressure stimuli [65, 68]. These results confirm and extend previous work reporting reduced pain thresholds and increased pain ratings in patients with FM in response to a variety of different stimuli at different body sites [37, 22, 69, 70], and point towards central sensitization or disinhibition. This finding is in contrast with several recent studies implicating a role for peripheral abnormalities in the maintenance of FM [71, 72]. Although peripheral mechanisms cannot be ruled out, our results suggest that peripheral sensitization is unlikely to explain the observed pain augmentation in response to IES in our patients with FM.

In addition to sensory thresholds and ratings, we assessed temporal summation of pain in response to IES, which could be considered as the electrical equivalent of a noxious pinprick stimulus, as used in studies by the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain [58]. Temporal summation of pain refers to the increase in perception of pain with repetitive noxious stimuli (≥0.3 Hz) that is thought to be related to a frequency-dependent increase in the excitability of spinal cord neurons, commonly known as wind-up [10, 73]. In the present study, patients with FM reported higher ratings in response to single and repeated IES, but temporal summation of pain was not significantly different from that observed in HC subjects. This is consistent with several other studies which showed that patients with FM were not significantly different from HC subjects when testing temporal summation of pain using pinprick [4, 74, 75] or thermal stimuli [30], or when testing reflex responses to nociceptive electrical stimulation [29]. In contrast, several other studies have reported increased temporal summation of pain in response to thermal stimuli [10, 11], and reduced reflex responses to nociceptive electrical stimulation in patients with FM [6, 76]. These mixed results are likely due to clinical heterogeneity of the FM symptoms and methodological differences in stimulation. It can be argued that the use of pain thresholds for the assessment of temporal summation can be problematic due to the low pain thresholds commonly observed in patients with FM [30]. Therefore, more carefully designed studies are needed to clarify the presence or absence of abnormal temporal summation of pain in patients with FM.

What is interesting about the findings in our study as compared to those in other studies is that we used the same type of stimulus (IES) for the assessment of temporal summation of pain, sensory thresholds and ratings, and cortical responses. The lack of differences between patients with FM and HC subjects with respect to temporal summation of pain in response to IES as observed in this study raises questions about the role of spinal sensitization in explaining the observed pain augmentation in response to IES in our patients with FM.

MEG responses to IES

After noxious stimulation of the skin, several cortical sources may be identified including the S1, S2, insular, and anterior cingulate cortex [77]. These cortical sources are considered to be part of the so-called ‘pain matrix’, a network of brain areas implicated in the processing of pain [78]. To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first research to combine MEG and IES in the assessment of patients with FM and several sources could be identified, including the bilateral S2 and cS1. Increased activity in the cS2 and cS1 was observed in patients with FM when compared to both perceptually and intensity controlled activation in HC subjects. This is consistent with a previous MEG study that used subjectively matched painful pressure stimuli in patients with FM and HC subjects [17]. Lower stimulus intensities were required to evoke pain in patients with FM. However, these lower stimulation intensities still resulted in increased evoked fields in the somatosensory, temporal and parietal areas at short latencies, and in prefrontal areas at both short and long latencies in patients with FM as compared to HC subjects [17]. Importantly, in our study, significantly increased activity in the ipsilateral S2 was observed, an area that has recently been implicated in mediating clinical pain in patients with FM [18]. Since the S2 has been shown to play a role in higher-order functions, such as attention [79] and integration of nociceptive and non-nociceptive sensory input [80], further studies are needed to clarify the role of augmented S2 response in clinical characteristics of FM.

Augmented cortical responses to nociceptive stimuli could be due to facilitation or disinhibition of nociceptive input at multiple levels in the cortical pain matrix. For example, it has been suggested that prolonged nociceptive input could lead to central sensitization and maladaptive neuroplasticity within the somatosensory and motor systems [81]. In our study by utilizing paired-pulse median nerve stimulation and MEG, we found that intracortical inhibition in the S1 is compromised in FM patients [23]. In addition, a growing body of evidence has suggested that deficits in endogenous inhibitory systems could play an essential role in FM [51, 8284]. A recent functional MRI study demonstrated that activity of the rostral anterior cingulate cortex, known to play a crucial role in descending pain inhibition, was attenuated in patients with FM upon painful pressure stimulation [82]. Moreover, patients with FM showed reduced functional connectivity between the rostral anterior cingulate cortex and other regions involved in the descending pain inhibitory network including the brainstem, rostral ventromedial medulla, amygdala, and hippocampus [83]. Lastly, several previous studies assessing pain augmentation in patients with FM have also reported decreased thalamic activation in response to both painful [13] and non-painful [12] stimuli. As such, the observed pain augmentation in the present study could have been due to altered filtering of nociceptive input in the thalamus.

Methodological considerations

In this study we used tools that have not been previously used (IES) or have been infrequently (MEG) applied in the study of FM. The advantage of MEG is that cortical responses can be measured in the time and space domain in great detail. However, only a subset of brain regions involved in pain processing can be reliably detected using this method. To obtain information about concomitant activation in other brain regions, other imaging methods as well as more complex analysis procedures [17, 85] may complement the present results.

A particular strong point of our study was the assessment of cortical responses to IES in the context of behavioral responses to the same type of stimulus. Still, in order to do so, different levels of stimulation were used. Previous studies often used IES at or just above pain threshold levels (<2.5 mA) [36, 37, 85], to ensure Aδ-fiber specific stimulation [37]. In some of our subjects (including both patients and HC subjects), higher stimulus intensities were occasionally needed, e.g. to assess the EPTT or to attain a perceptual pain rating of 6 on the NRS. This means that the data cannot be contributed to the Aδ-fiber function alone. If IES activates Aδ- and Aβ-fibers simultaneously, Aβ stimulation may have interfered with perception of pain elicited by noxious stimuli. A study by Inui et al. found that cortical responses to noxious IES can be inhibited by applying simultaneously innocuous transcutaneous electrical stimulation [86]. This effect may be more pronounced in case of HC-PE, where HC subjects required higher stimulus intensities to induce moderate pain. However, systematic studies examining to what extent the noxious evoked brain response was reduced by Aβ co-activation according to increasing intensity of IES have not yet been performed. In addition, the subjective sensation of the stimulus as well as the cortical response latencies comparable to those observed in studies using lower stimulus intensities suggested a primary activation of Aδ-fibers. Markedly increased amplitudes of cS1, cS2 and iS2 responses in FM compared to HC-IE condition further support our interpretation of pain augmentation in response to IES in FM. Taken together, although our results cannot exclude the possibility that a higher intensity stimulus activates other kinds of fibers, it is unlikely that this potential limitation affected the between-group differences in response to IES. In the light of recent studies reporting C-fiber abnormalities in patients with FM [71, 72], it could additionally be interesting to repeat the present study using a recently developed modified version of the IES electrode that selectively stimulates C-fibers [87].

Lastly, although the sample size in the present study was larger than that in previous clinical MEG studies, the sample size was still relatively small considering the heterogeneous presentation of FM. In addition, the unknown effects of medication (or rather the medication stoppage and limited washout period) limit the generalizability of the present results.

Conclusion

The present study investigated the central augmentation of pain processing in patients with FM using MEG and IES. Increased responses were found in the cS1, cS2 and iS2 in patients with FM, and the amplitude of the cS2 response was found to be positively correlated with clinical FM pain intensity. These results corresponded with generalized hypersensitivity to both IES and mechanical stimuli as assessed using QST. Temporal summation of pain in response to IES was not different between patients with FM and HC subjects. Together, these results suggest that the observed pain augmentation in response to IES in patients with FM occurred relatively independent of peripheral and spinal sensitization to IES. The observed pain augmentation in response to IES in patients with FM could be due to sensitization or disinhibition of the cortical somatosensory system. The combination of MEG and IES could therefore be particularly useful to explore the role of bilateral somatosensory cortices in FM pathophysiology.

Supporting Information

S1 Table. Description of variables in the MAT-file.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151776.s002

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The authors are thankful to professors Ryusuke Kakigi and Koji Inui at the Department of Integrative Physiology of the National Institute of Physiological Sciences in Okazaki, Japan, for providing the intra-epidermal electrodes.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: ML MR JSK HWK EBL KMS HAK CKC. Performed the experiments: ML MR JSK HWK EBL KMS HAK CKC. Analyzed the data: ML MR. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: ML. Wrote the paper: ML MR CKC.

References

  1. 1. Schmidt-Wilcke T, Clauw DJ. Fibromyalgia: From pathophysiology to therapy. Nat Rev Rheumatol. 2011;7(9):518–27. pmid:21769128
  2. 2. Wolfe F, Smythe HA, Yunus MB, Bennett RM, Bombardier C, Goldenberg DL, et al. The American College of Rheumatology 1990 Criteria for the Classification of Fibromyalgia. Report of the Multicenter Criteria Committee. Arthritis Rheum. 1990;33(2):160–72. pmid:2306288.
  3. 3. Berglund B, Harju EL, Kosek E, Lindblom U. Quantitative and qualitative perceptual analysis of cold dysesthesia and hyperalgesia in fibromyalgia. Pain. 2002;96(1–2):177–87. pmid:11932073
  4. 4. Blumenstiel K, Gerhardt A, Rolke R, Bieber C, Tesarz J, Friederich HC, et al. Quantitative sensory testing profiles in chronic back pain are distinct from those in fibromyalgia. Clin J Pain. 2011;27(8):682–90. pmid:21487289
  5. 5. Kosek E, Hansson P. Modulatory influence on somatosensory perception from vibration and heterotopic noxious conditioning stimulation (HNCS) in fibromyalgia patients and healthy subjects. Pain. 1997;70(1):41–51. pmid:9106808.
  6. 6. Desmeules JA, Cedraschi C, Rapiti E, Baumgartner E, Finckh A, Cohen P, et al. Neurophysiologic evidence for a central sensitization in patients with fibromyalgia. Arthritis Rheum. 2003;48(5):1420–9. pmid:12746916
  7. 7. Staud R, Weyl EE, Price DD, Robinson ME. Mechanical and heat hyperalgesia highly predict clinical pain intensity in patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain syndromes. J Pain. 2012;13(8):725–35. pmid:22739051
  8. 8. Staud R. Abnormal Pain Modulation in Patients with Spatially Distributed Chronic Pain: Fibromyalgia. Rheum Dis Clin N Am. 2009;35(2):263–74.
  9. 9. Staud R, Weyl EE, Riley JL III, Fillingim RB. Slow temporal summation of pain for assessment of central pain sensitivity and clinical pain of fibromyalgia patients. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(2):e89086. Epub 2014/02/22. PONE-D-13-30378 [pii]. pmid:24558475; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3928405.
  10. 10. Price DD, Staud R, Robinson ME, Mauderli AP, Cannon R, Vierck CJ. Enhanced temporal summation of second pain and its central modulation in fibromyalgia patients. Pain. 2002;99(1–2):49–59. pmid:12237183.
  11. 11. Staud R, Vierck CJ, Cannon RL, Mauderli AP, Price DD. Abnormal sensitization and temporal summation of second pain (wind-up) in patients with fibromyalgia syndrome. Pain. 2001;91(1–2):165–75. pmid:11240089.
  12. 12. Cook DB, Lange G, Ciccone DS, Liu WC, Steffener J, Natelson BH. Functional Imaging of Pain in Patients with Primary Fibromyalgia. J Rheumatol. 2004;31(2):364–78. pmid:14760810
  13. 13. Gracely RH, Petzke F, Wolf JM, Clauw DJ. Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence of augmented pain processing in fibromyalgia. Arthritis Rheum. 2002;46(5):1333–43. Epub 2002/07/13. pmid:12115241.
  14. 14. Pujol J, López-Solà M, Ortiz H, Vilanova JC, Harrison BJ, Yücel M, et al. Mapping brain response to pain in fibromyalgia patients using temporal analysis of fMRI. PLoS ONE. 2009;4(4).
  15. 15. Lorenz J, Grasedyck K, Bromm B. Middle and long latency somatosensory evoked potentials after painful laser stimulation in patients with fibromyalgia syndrome. Electroen Clin Neuro. 1996;100(2):165–8.
  16. 16. Burgmer M, Pogatzki-Zahn E, Gaubitz M, Wessoleck E, Heuft G, Pfleiderer B. Altered brain activity during pain processing in fibromyalgia. NeuroImage. 2009;44(2):502–8. pmid:18848998
  17. 17. Maestu C, Cortes A, Vazquez JM, del Rio D, Gomez-Arguelles JM, del Pozo F, et al. Increased brain responses during subjectively-matched mechanical pain stimulation in fibromyalgia patients as evidenced by MEG. Clin Neurophysiol. 2013;124(4):752–60. pmid:23121899
  18. 18. Fallon N, Chiu YH, Li X, Nurmikko TJ, Stancak A. Ipsilateral cortical activation in fibromyalgia patients during brushing correlates with symptom severity. Clin Neurophysiol. 2013;124(1):154–63. pmid:22819596
  19. 19. Lannersten L, Kosek E. Dysfunction of endogenous pain inhibition during exercise with painful muscles in patients with shoulder myalgia and fibromyalgia. Pain. 2010;151(1):77–86. pmid:20621420
  20. 20. Staud R, Robinson ME, Vierck CJ Jr, Price DD. Diffuse noxious inhibitory controls (DNIC) attenuate temporal summation of second pain in normal males but not in normal females or fibromyalgia patients. Pain. 2003;101(1–2):167–74. pmid:12507711
  21. 21. Pujol J, Macia D, Garcia-Fontanals A, Blanco-Hinojo L, Lopez-Sola M, Garcia-Blanco S, et al. The contribution of sensory system functional connectivity reduction to clinical pain in fibromyalgia. Pain. 2014;155(8):1492–503. pmid:24792477.
  22. 22. Lautenbacher S, Rollman GB, McCain GA. Multi-method assessment of experimental and clinical pain in patients with fibromyalgia. Pain. 1994;59(1):45–53. Epub 1994/10/01. doi: 0304-3959(94)90046-9 [pii]. pmid:7854801.
  23. 23. Lim M, Roosink M, Kim JS, Kim DJ, Kim HW, Lee EB, et al. Disinhibition of the primary somatosensory cortex in patients with fibromyalgia. Pain. 2015;156(4):666–74. pmid:25630027.
  24. 24. Montoya P, Sitges C, Garcia-Herrera M, Rodriguez-Cotes A, Izquierdo R, Truyols M, et al. Reduced brain habituation to somatosensory stimulation in patients with fibromyalgia. Arthritis Rheum. 2006;54(6):1995–2003. Epub 2006/05/30. pmid:16732548.
  25. 25. Geisser ME, Glass JM, Rajcevska LD, Clauw DJ, Williams DA, Kileny PR, et al. A Psychophysical Study of Auditory and Pressure Sensitivity in Patients With Fibromyalgia and Healthy Controls. J Pain. 2008;9(5):417–22. pmid:18280211
  26. 26. McDermid AJ, Rollman GB, McCain GA. Generalized hypervigilance in fibromyalgia: evidence of perceptual amplification. Pain. 1996;66(2–3):133–44. Epub 1996/08/01. doi: 0304-3959(96)03059-X [pii]. pmid:8880834.
  27. 27. Choi W, Lim M, Kim JS, Kim DJ, Chung CK. Impaired pre-attentive auditory processing in fibromyalgia: A mismatch negativity (MMN) study. Clin Neurophysiol. 2015;126(7):1310–8. pmid:25453609.
  28. 28. Schweinhardt P, Sauro KM, Bushnell MC. Fibromyalgia: A disorder of the brain? Neuroscientist. 2008;14(5):415–21. pmid:18270311
  29. 29. Rhudy JL, DelVentura JL, Terry EL, Bartley EJ, Olech E, Palit S, et al. Emotional modulation of pain and spinal nociception in fibromyalgia. Pain. 2013;154(7):1045–56. pmid:23622762
  30. 30. Potvin S, Paul-Savoie E, Morin M, lanie , Bourgault P, Marchand S. Temporal Summation of Pain Is Not Amplified in a Large Proportion of Fibromyalgia Patients. Pain Res Treat. 2012;2012:#938595.
  31. 31. Wolfe F, Rasker JJ, Hauser W. Hearing loss in fibromyalgia? Somatic sensory and non-sensory symptoms in patients with fibromyalgia and other rheumatic disorders. Clin Exp Rheumatol. 2012;30(6 Suppl 74):88–93. Epub 2012/11/10. doi: 6315 [pii]. pmid:23137751.
  32. 32. Arendt-Nielsen L, Henriksson KG. Pathophysiological mechanisms in chronic musculoskeletal pain (fibromyalgia): the role of central and peripheral sensitization and pain disinhibition. Best Pract Res Clin Rheumatol. 2007;21(3):465–80. pmid:17602994
  33. 33. Vierck CJ Jr. Mechanisms underlying development of spatially distributed chronic pain (fibromyalgia). Pain. 2006;124(3):242–63. Epub 2006/07/18. doi: S0304-3959(06)00308-3 [pii] pmid:16842915.
  34. 34. Staud R. Evidence for shared pain mechanisms in osteoarthritis, low back pain, and fibromyalgia. Curr Rheum Rep. 2011;13(6):513–20.
  35. 35. Inui K, Kakigi R. Pain perception in humans: use of intraepidermal electrical stimulation. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2012;83(5):551–6. Epub 2011/12/06. doi: jnnp-2011-301484 [pii] pmid:22138180.
  36. 36. Inui K, Tran TD, Hoshiyama M, Kakigi R. Preferential stimulation of Aδ fibers by intra-epidermal needle electrode in humans. Pain. 2002;96(3):247–52. pmid:11972996
  37. 37. Mouraux A, Iannetti GD, Plaghki L. Low intensity intra-epidermal electrical stimulation can activate Aδ-nociceptors selectively. Pain. 2010;150(1):199–207. pmid:20510515
  38. 38. Mouraux A, Marot E, Legrain V. Short trains of intra-epidermal electrical stimulation to elicit reliable behavioral and electrophysiological responses to the selective activation of nociceptors in humans. Neurosci Lett. 2014;561:69–73. pmid:24361132.
  39. 39. Nakata H, Sakamoto K, Inui K, Hoshiyama M, Kakigi R. The characteristics of no-go potentials with intraepidermal stimulation. NeuroReport. 2009;20(13):1149–54. pmid:19617857
  40. 40. Omori S, Isose S, Otsuru N, Nishihara M, Kuwabara S, Inui K, et al. Somatotopic representation of pain in the primary somatosensory cortex (S1) in humans. Clin Neurophysiol. 2013;124(7):1422–30. pmid:23415452.
  41. 41. Caetano G, Olausson H, Cole J, Jousmaki V, Hari R. Cortical responses to Adelta-fiber stimulation: magnetoencephalographic recordings in a subject lacking large myelinated afferents. Cereb Cortex. 2010;20(8):1898–903. pmid:19959562; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC2901021.
  42. 42. Forss N, Raij TT, Seppa M, Hari R. Common cortical network for first and second pain. Neuroimage. 2005;24(1):132–42. pmid:15588604.
  43. 43. Kirveskari E, Vartiainen NV, Kallio-Laine K, Kalso E, Forss N. Normal laser-evoked cortical responses in patients with chronic hemibody pain. Eur J Pain. 2015;19(8):1168–76. pmid:25523148.
  44. 44. Ploner M, Schmitz F, Freund HJ, Schnitzler A. Parallel activation of primary and secondary somatosensory cortices in human pain processing. J Neurophysiol. 1999;81(6):3100–4. pmid:10368426.
  45. 45. Ogino Y, Nemoto H, Goto F. Somatotopy in human primary somatosensory cortex in pain system. Anesthesiology. 2005;103(4):821–7. pmid:16192775.
  46. 46. Inui K, Wang X, Qiu Y, Nguyen BT, Ojima S, Tamura Y, et al. Pain processing within the primary somatosensory cortex in humans. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;18(10):2859–66. pmid:14656335.
  47. 47. Nakata H, Tamura Y, Sakamoto K, Akatsuka K, Hirai M, Inui K, et al. Evoked magnetic fields following noxious laser stimulation of the thigh in humans. NeuroImage. 2008;42(2):858–68. pmid:18585060.
  48. 48. Vartiainen NV, Kirveskari E, Forss N. Central processing of tactile and nociceptive stimuli in complex regional pain syndrome. Clin Neurophysiol. 2008;119(10):2380–8. pmid:18723393.
  49. 49. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9(1):97–113.
  50. 50. Kim DJ, Lim M, Kim JS, Son KM, Kim HA, Chung CK. Altered white matter integrity in the corpus callosum in fibromyalgia patients identified by tract-based spatial statistical analysis. Arthritis Rheumatol. 2014;66(11):3190–9. pmid:25225152.
  51. 51. Jensen KB, Srinivasan P, Spaeth R, Tan Y, Kosek E, Petzke F, et al. Overlapping structural and functional brain changes in patients with long-term exposure to fibromyalgia pain. Arthritis Rheum. 2013;65(12):3293–303. pmid:23982850.
  52. 52. Burckhardt CS, Clark SR, Bennett RM. The fibromyalgia impact questionnaire: development and validation. J Rheumatol. 1991;18(5):728–33. Epub 1991/05/01. pmid:1865419.
  53. 53. Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer RA. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1988;56(6):893–7. Epub 1988/12/01. pmid:3204199.
  54. 54. Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch gen psych. 1961;4:561–71.
  55. 55. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF III, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28(2):193–213. pmid:2748771
  56. 56. Melzack R. The short-form McGill Pain Questionnaire. Pain. 1987;30(2):191–7. Epub 1987/08/01. doi: 0304-3959(87)91074-8 [pii]. pmid:3670870.
  57. 57. Persson AL, Brogardh C, Sjolund BH. Tender or not tender: test-retest repeatability of pressure pain thresholds in the trapezius and deltoid muscles of healthy women. J Rehabil Med. 2004;36(1):17–27. pmid:15074434.
  58. 58. Rolke R, Baron R, Maier C, Tolle TR, Treede RD, Beyer A, et al. Quantitative sensory testing in the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS): standardized protocol and reference values. Pain. 2006;123(3):231–43. Epub 2006/05/16. doi: S0304-3959(06)00152-7 [pii] pmid:16697110.
  59. 59. Hämäläinen M, Hari R, Ilmoniemi RJ, Knuutila J, Lounasmaa OV. Magnetoencephalography—Theory, instrumentation, and applications to noninvasive studies of the working human brain. Rev Mod Phys. 1993;65(2):413–97.
  60. 60. Taulu S, Simola J. Spatiotemporal signal space separation method for rejecting nearby interference in MEG measurements. Phys Med Biol. 2006;51(7):1759–68. Epub 2006/03/23. doi: S0031-9155(06)09790-9 [pii] pmid:16552102.
  61. 61. Gross J, Baillet S, Barnes GR, Henson RN, Hillebrand A, Jensen O, et al. Good practice for conducting and reporting MEG research. NeuroImage. 2013;65:349–63. pmid:23046981.
  62. 62. Nakata H, Inui K, Wasaka T, Tamura Y, Tran TD, Qiu Y, et al. Movements modulate cortical activities evoked by noxious stimulation. Pain. 2004;107(1–2):91–8. pmid:14715394
  63. 63. Lim M, Kim JS, Chung CK. Modulation of somatosensory evoked magnetic fields by intensity of interfering stimuli in human somatosensory cortex: an MEG study. NeuroImage. 2012;61(3):660–9. pmid:22516368.
  64. 64. Lim M, Kim JS, Chung CK. Oscillatory interaction between the hand area of human primary motor cortex and finger muscles during steady-state isometric contraction. Clin Neurophysiol. 2011;122(11):2246–53. pmid:21493129.
  65. 65. Kosek E, Ekholm J, Hansson P. Sensory dysfunction in fibromyalgia patients with implications for pathogenic mechanisms. Pain. 1996;68(2–3):375–83. pmid:9121827.
  66. 66. Lautenbacher S, Rollman GB. Possible deficiencies of pain modulation in fibromyalgia. Clin J Pain. 1997;13(3):189–96. pmid:9303250.
  67. 67. Pollok B, Krause V, Legrain V, Ploner M, Freynhagen R, Melchior I, et al. Differential effects of painful and non-painful stimulation on tactile processing in fibromyalgia syndrome and subjects with masochistic behaviour. PLoS One. 2010;5(12):e15804. pmid:21203391; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3011001.
  68. 68. Petzke F, Clauw DJ, Ambrose K, Khine A, Gracely RH. Increased pain sensitivity in fibromyalgia: effects of stimulus type and mode of presentation. Pain. 2003;105(3):403–13. pmid:14527701.
  69. 69. Tampin B, Briffa NK, Slater H. Self-reported sensory descriptors are associated with quantitative sensory testing parameters in patients with cervical radiculopathy, but not in patients with fibromyalgia. Eur J Pain. 2013;17(4):621–33. Epub 2012/10/31. pmid:23108871.
  70. 70. Pfau DB, Rolke R, Nickel R, Treede RD, Daublaender M. Somatosensory profiles in subgroups of patients with myogenic temporomandibular disorders and fibromyalgia syndrome. Pain. 2009;147(1–3):72–83. pmid:19767146.
  71. 71. Serra J, Collado A, Sola R, Antonelli F, Torres X, Salgueiro M, et al. Hyperexcitable C nociceptors in fibromyalgia. Ann Neurol. 2014;75(2):196–208. pmid:24243538.
  72. 72. Uceyler N, Zeller D, Kahn AK, Kewenig S, Kittel-Schneider S, Schmid A, et al. Small fibre pathology in patients with fibromyalgia syndrome. Brain. 2013;136(Pt 6):1857–67. pmid:23474848.
  73. 73. Price DD, Hu JW, Dubner R, Gracely RH. Peripheral suppression of first pain and central summation of second pain evoked by noxious heat pulses. Pain. 1977;3(1):57–68. pmid:876667.
  74. 74. Pfau DB, Geber C, Birklein F, Treede RD. Quantitative Sensory Testing of Neuropathic Pain Patients: Potential Mechanistic and Therapeutic Implications. Curr Pain Headache Rep. 2012:1–8.
  75. 75. Tampin B, Slater H, Hall T, Lee G, Briffa NK. Quantitative sensory testing somatosensory profiles in patients with cervical radiculopathy are distinct from those in patients with nonspecific neck-arm pain. Pain. 2012;153(12):2403–14. Epub 2012/09/18. doi: S0304-3959(12)00478-2 [pii] pmid:22980746.
  76. 76. Banic B, Petersen-Felix S, Andersen OK, Radanov BP, Villiger PM, Arendt-Nielsen L, et al. Evidence for spinal cord hypersensitivity in chronic pain after whiplash injury and in fibromyalgia. Pain. 2004;107(1–2):7–15. pmid:14715383
  77. 77. Schnitzler A, Ploner M. Neurophysiology and functional neuroanatomy of pain perception. J Clin Neurophysiol. 2000;17(6):592–603. pmid:11151977.
  78. 78. Apkarian AV, Bushnell MC, Treede RD, Zubieta JK. Human brain mechanisms of pain perception and regulation in health and disease. Eur J Pain. 2005;9(4):463–84. pmid:15979027.
  79. 79. Mima T, Nagamine T, Nakamura K, Shibasaki H. Attention modulates both primary and second somatosensory cortical activities in humans: a magnetoencephalographic study. J Neurophysiol. 1998;80(4):2215–21. pmid:9772274.
  80. 80. Torquati K, Pizzella V, Della Penna S, Franciotti R, Babiloni C, Romani GL, et al. "Gating" effects of simultaneous peripheral electrical stimulations on human secondary somatosensory cortex: A whole-head MEG study. NeuroImage. 2003;20(3):1704–13. pmid:14642480
  81. 81. Seifert F, Maihofner C. Functional and structural imaging of pain-induced neuroplasticity. Curr Opin Anaesthesiol. 2011;24(5):515–23. pmid:21822136.
  82. 82. Jensen KB, Kosek E, Petzke F, Carville S, Fransson P, Marcus H, et al. Evidence of dysfunctional pain inhibition in Fibromyalgia reflected in rACC during provoked pain. Pain. 2009;144(1–2):95–100. pmid:19410366
  83. 83. Jensen KB, Loitoile R, Kosek E, Petzke F, Carville S, Fransson P, et al. Patients with fibromyalgia display less functional connectivity in the brain's pain inhibitory network. Mol Pain. 2012;8:32. pmid:22537768; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3404927.
  84. 84. Julien N, Goffaux P, Arsenault P, Marchand S. Widespread pain in fibromyalgia is related to a deficit of endogenous pain inhibition. Pain. 2005;114(1–2):295–302. pmid:15733656.
  85. 85. Inui K, Tran TD, Qiu Y, Wang X, Hoshiyama M, Kakigi R. A comparative magnetoencephalographic study of cortical activations evoked by noxious and innocuous somatosensory stimulations. Neuroscience. 2003;120(1):235–48. pmid:12849756
  86. 86. Inui K, Tsuji T, Kakigi R. Temporal analysis of cortical mechanisms for pain relief by tactile stimuli in humans. Cereb Cortex. 2006;16(3):355–65. pmid:15901650.
  87. 87. Motogi J, Kodaira M, Muragaki Y, Inui K, Kakigi R. Cortical responses to C-fiber stimulation by intra-epidermal electrical stimulation: an MEG study. Neurosci Lett. 2014;570:69–74. pmid:24731795.