Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Bilateral tDCS on Primary Motor Cortex: Effects on Fast Arm Reaching Tasks

  • Pablo Arias ,

    pabloarias.neurocom@udc.es)

    Affiliation Neuroscience and Motor Control Group (NEUROcom), Department of Medicine, INEF Galicia and Biomedical Research Institute of A Coruña (INIBIC), University of A Coruña, A Coruña, Spain

  • Yoanna Corral-Bergantiños,

    Affiliation Neuroscience and Motor Control Group (NEUROcom), Department of Medicine, INEF Galicia and Biomedical Research Institute of A Coruña (INIBIC), University of A Coruña, A Coruña, Spain

  • Verónica Robles-García,

    Affiliation Neuroscience and Motor Control Group (NEUROcom), Department of Medicine, INEF Galicia and Biomedical Research Institute of A Coruña (INIBIC), University of A Coruña, A Coruña, Spain

  • Antonio Madrid,

    Affiliation Neuroscience and Motor Control Group (NEUROcom), Department of Medicine, INEF Galicia and Biomedical Research Institute of A Coruña (INIBIC), University of A Coruña, A Coruña, Spain

  • Antonio Oliviero,

    Affiliation FENNSI Group, Hospital Nacional de Parapléjicos, SESCAM, Toledo, Spain

  • Javier Cudeiro

    Affiliations Neuroscience and Motor Control Group (NEUROcom), Department of Medicine, INEF Galicia and Biomedical Research Institute of A Coruña (INIBIC), University of A Coruña, A Coruña, Spain, Centro de Estimulación Cerebral de Galicia, A Coruña, Spain

Correction

22 Sep 2016: The PLOS ONE Staff (2016) Correction: Bilateral tDCS on Primary Motor Cortex: Effects on Fast Arm Reaching Tasks. PLOS ONE 11(9): e0163714. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0163714 View correction

Abstract

Background

The effects produced by transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) applied to the motor system have been widely studied in the past, chiefly focused on primary motor cortex (M1) excitability. However, the effects on functional tasks are less well documented.

Objective

This study aims to evaluate the effect of tDCS-M1 on goal-oriented actions (i.e., arm-reaching movements; ARM), in a reaction-time protocol.

Methods

13 healthy subjects executed dominant ARM as fast as possible to one of two targets in front of them while surface EMG was recorded. Participants performed three different sessions. In each session they first executed ARM (Pre), then received tDCS, and finally executed Post, similar to Pre. Subjects received three different types of tDCS, one per session: In one session the anode was on right-M1 (AR), and the cathode on the left-M1 (CL), thus termed AR-CL; AL-CR reversed the montage; and Sham session was applied likewise. Real stimulation was 1mA-10min while subjects at rest. Three different variables and their coefficients of variation (CV) were analyzed: Premotor times (PMT), reaction-times (RT) and movement-times (MT).

Results

triceps-PMT were significantly increased at Post-Sham, suggesting fatigue. Results obtained with real tDCS were not different depending on the montage used, in both cases PMT were significantly reduced in all recorded muscles. RT and MT did not change for real or sham stimulation. RT-CV and PMT-CV were reduced after all stimulation protocols.

Conclusion

tDCS reduces premotor time and fatigability during the execution of fast motor tasks. Possible underlying mechanisms are discussed.

Introduction

Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) is a promising tool for neurorehabilitation purposes. Over the past few years a solid background has been built on the capacity of tDCS to modulate functional brain networks and guidelines have been formulated for its safe use on humans [1, 2]. tDCS permits a transient modulation of cortical excitability by the application of currents in a non-invasive way. tDCS is able to produce long lasting depolarization or hyperpolarization of cell membranes depending on stimulation polarity [3]; these aftereffects are thought to be mediated by calcium-dependent plasticity of glutamatergic neurons [4]. As a result, anodal tDCS applied to the primary motor cortex (M1) increases cortico-spinal excitability and cathodal stimulation produces the opposite effect [3]. These effects depend on the intensity and duration of the stimulation [3], but just ten minutes at 1mA induces consistent aftereffects, lasting for a period of minutes [5, 6].

The classic electrode montage to modulate the excitability of the motor cortex places one electrode over M1 and the other at the contralateral supraorbital forehead [3]. However, simultaneous bilateral M1 stimulation induces a similar effect in terms of excitability modulation, weaker in magnitude than unilateral montages [7, 8], but with smaller inter-subject variability [7]. The use of bilateral tDCS-M1 is appealing in pathologies like stroke, where the interhemispheric imbalance might be controlled by up-regulating the excitability of the ipsilesional motor cortex, while down-regulating the contralesional M1 [9]. Also, it is believed that bilateral tDCS-M1 produces better effects than uni-hemispheric tDCS on behavioral motor tasks [10].

Despite what has been mentioned above, tDCS effects on functional motor tasks have received less attention than effects related to cortical excitability; however, its applicability seems suitable for rehabilitation and motor learning [11, 12]. Remarkably, tDCS of M1 modulates excitability at sites distant from the cortex [13], such as the propriospinal circuits within the spinal cord [14, 15]. This particular point should be considered when evaluating the effect of tDCS on goal-oriented motor tasks (including movement preparation and execution), because of a inter-play between M1 and spinal cord excitability precedes the execution of movements [1618]. In fact, cathodal tDCS-M1 reduces cortical excitability but does not change spinal excitability [3, 5, 14], whereas anodal tDCS increases excitability in M1 [3, 6] but also modulates the excitability of propriospinal circuits [14, 15]. Therefore, it seems that the understanding of bilateral tDCS effects on motor functional activities is of relevance in order to design new strategies aimed to improve function in certain pathologies [1921]. In this sense, one essential task in daily living activities is arm reaching.

A classical view of the parieto-frontal network considers several loci to study the effect of tDCS on arm reaching towards a target in the peri-personal space. The posterior parietal cortex receives input from visual areas through the dorsal stream [22] and generates representations of eye, head, body and surrounding frameworks for early visuomotor planning [23, 24] of reaching and grasping [22]; some of these functions are shared by M1 [25] and other areas [26]. The prefrontal cortex (PFC) has a role in the control of reaching, especially for choice reaching tasks [27, 28] in intimate relation with the dorsal premotor cortex (d)PMC [23, 29]; while the ventral premotor cortex (vPMC) may contribute to transformations from extrinsic to intrinsic coordinates to guide movement directed to objects in peri-personal space [23].

However, a modern view of this network is much wider, challenging the “serial” assumption that selection (decision making) occurs before specification (movement planning), and rather advocates that these processes operate simultaneously and in an integrated manner [30]. It is now suggested that potential reaching actions are specified by the dorsal stream, while their selection involves cortical (like PFC [30] or dPMC[31, 32]) and subcortical structures (like the Basal Ganglia [33, 34]) engaged in the evaluation of the opportunity of executing a reaching movement while minimizing costs and maximizing profits [35]. In this sense the network predicts future outcomes and cancels not sufficiently valuable actions [30]. Within the whole network M1 is a crucial area to study the effect of tDCS on reaching tasks for its role in planning and releasing the movement [23, 25, 3538], as well on cancelling those that are on-going [32, 35]. M1 (amongst other areas) is also involved in choosing between alternative actions to get the goal and the generation of their motor commands [35, 39].

The effect of tDCS-M1 on the planning and execution of goal-oriented actions can be inferred from the reaction times (RT) and the movement times (MT), respectively [40]. In this study we have evaluated the effects of bilateral tDCS-M1 on arm reaching movements. We used RT tasks of different complexity while controlling the effect of polarity, and placebo.

Our research hypothesis is that tDCS modulates motor behavior in a polarity dependent manner. We predict faster responses after bilateral tDCS, when anodal stimulation is applied to the M1 contralateral to the dominant (executing) arm, as a result of the increased excitability of M1 circuits. Conversely, cathodal stimulation shall render opposite effects.

Experimental Procedures

All experimental subjects signed consent forms. The protocol conformed to the declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of A Coruña (Spain). The individuals whose experimental data were included in this manuscript have given written informed consent (as outlined in PLOS consent form) allowing the use of photographs to illustrate the figures.

Subjects

Thirteen healthy subjects participated (six women, age range 20-37yrs). None took medication or undertook hard physical work in the week prior the experimental sessions. Subjects were right-handed [41] and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision.

Procedure

Each subject performed three sessions, one week apart. In each session they executed the reaching tasks before and after the tDCS (Pre and Post, respectively). A different tDCS protocol was applied in each session; the order was randomized. Subjects reached to one of two round targets placed at gaze height in front of them (see below). Reaching was always performed as fast as possible with the right (dominant) hand on a frontoparallel plane (Fig 1). No instructions were provided on how to touch the target, apart from asking subjects to touch the centre of the target with the hand as fast as possible. All subjects chose to touch with finger joints extended.

thumbnail
Fig 1.

(A) Experimental setting and tDCS electrode montage in the 3 experimental sessions. The pictures show two subjects receiving tDCS at rest. (B) A single trial lasted 10s; the response signal was presented 500, 1000 or 1500ms after the warning cue. (C) Example of one recording reflecting the sequential activation of the three muscles evaluated. Recordings are synchronized to the response signal (marked as the blue vertical line). The dashed area is enlarged at the right to clarify the sequential muscle responses. The individuals in these pictures have given written informed consent (as outlined in PLOS consent form) to publish the images.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160063.g001

Each of the three sessions comprised three different reaching tasks randomized in order, and each task included several trials. In all cases a low-tone audible warning signal was presented as a cue prior to the response-signal. The turning-on of an array of red LED’s at the edge of the target was the response-signal. Without any other purpose than making the response-signal time unpredictable (with regards to the warning cue) we used three foreperiods (delays) between warning and response signals; 500, 1000, or 1500ms, and their presentation order was randomized within trials. The sequence of events was controlled by Signal-4 software via a CED 1401mkII (Cambridge Electronic Design, UK). Subjects were asked to fixate on a central point between the two targets until the appearance of the response-signal (Fig 1) after which they made the response.

Tasks.

T1: Subjects were informed they had to reach the target ipsilateral to their dominant (executing) hand (i.e., single-ipsilateral response).

T2: Subjects were told that they had to reach the target in the contralateral space to their dominant arm (single-contralateral).

T3: In this task (choice) subjects were informed they would reach either the ipsilateral or the contralateral target to the dominant arm, depending on what target was lit (in randomized order).

Each of the two single-tasks (single-ipsilateral and single-contralateral) included 12 reaching trials, plus 1 catch trial, all randomized. In the catch trial the response signal did not appear after the warning cue.

The choice-task included 12 trials to the ipsilateral target (choice-ipsilateral) and 12 trials to the contralateral target (choice-contralateral) and these also included a catch trial. The 25 trials were randomized in order.

In all cases the inter-trial interval was 10s, and a one minute rest was given between the different tasks.

Pre and Post testing lasted 12min 10s each; Post started 1min after the end of the tDCS. The order of presentation of the three tasks in Pre was randomized, and reproduced in Post.

Experimental Setting

The round targets were 15cm diameter; their centers were 32cm apart from each other, and halfway between them a small black spot (1 cm-diameter) served as the fixation spot. The subjects were seated on an adjustable chair, containing chest-straps to avoid trunk movements, but allowing unrestrained shoulder movements. The chair height was adapted to make eye level at fixation height; the distance from the subject to the targets was adjusted such that allowing the reaching to the contralateral target with nearly full-elbow extension, minimizing leg involvement in the task [42]. Subjects’ hands were on contact-plates. After setting, subjects made three fast ARM’s to each target, as practice. The entire setup (Fig 1) was reproduced in each of the different test days.

Motor Outcomes

Signals were acquired by means of Biometrics-Data-Link, Digitimer D-360 amplifiers and the CED 1401 (3-3000Hz; 10KHz sampling rate, 1000 gain). Electromyographic (EMG) recordings determined the pre-motor time (PMT) [17], as the time lags from the response-signal to the movement-related EMG-onset [43]. EMG recorded the activity of the deltoid, biceps brachii and triceps brachii. Surface electrodes were placed in a belly-tendon montage on the anterior deltoid; large biceps head; and lateral triceps head (del; bic; tri), always after skin preparation. EMG-onset was determined automatically (and visually checked [44]) by applying the double-threshold method [45]. Thus the EMG signals were rectified and the EMG onset was considered at the first of ten consecutive samples (one threshold) above a given EMG amplitude (the other threshold); the latter threshold was equal to the mean EMG-background activity amplitude plus one standard deviation, which was calculated in the time-window just 50ms prior the response signal. Such threshold was obtained for each of the three muscles independently. The time elapsing from the LEDs flash to lifting the hand and leaving the contact-plate was computed as the RT; and the time from leaving the contact-plate to touch the target determined the MT. Customized MatLab programmes (The Mathworks, Ltd) were used to process the data.

Brain Stimulation

tDCS was applied bilaterally on M1’s with a Neuroconn DC-Stimulator connected to a pair of 5x7cm saline-soaked electrodes. In all sessions, one electrode was placed on the left M1, and the other on the right M1; corresponding to C3 and C4 of the International 10–20 EEG system.

In one session the anode was on the left M1, and the cathode on the right M1, and this is referred as AL-CR montage. In other session the anode was on the right M1, and the cathode on the left M1 (AR-CL). Sham montage randomized electrode positions. For real stimulation 1mA-intensity was applied for 10min (current fade-in and out was ramped and lasted the initial and final 8s). The Sham protocol lasted the same time but the current was applied for 30s and then ceased [7]. Subject remained restful during the stimulation sessions.

Statistical Methods

The mean of the responses for each experimental condition and subject was the outcome-value introduced in the analysis. The mean was computed considering all events from each experimental condition, but those which PMT <80ms or >800ms; thus those events with PMT <80ms were considered anticipations, and their proportion in the different stimulation protocols, conditions and testing time-points (pre and post) were evaluated with the Fisher Exact Probability Test. The threshold of 80ms was set based on the latencies of a visual-evoked potential to a flashing LED recorded in the primary visual cortex (V1), plus the minimum latency for an interaction between V1 and M1, plus the latency from M1 activation to EMG onset on the studied muscles [4648]. Events with PMT >800ms were discarded as sign of un-attentiveness [49], though this happened just once in all subjects and conditions. PMT, RT and MT from events within those thresholds were normalized to control variability related to daily differences in the experimental setting (see above), though care was taken to minimize it. Normalization was performed as follows: For each session the average for each variable was calculated from data including Pre outcome-values for all tasks and subjects (Table 1). This normalizing value was used to divide all subjects’ responses at Pre and Post for the corresponding day. This normalization procedure respects inter-subject variability, while normalizing the responses to the daily pooled Pre-testing.

thumbnail
Table 1. PMT, RT and MT at Pre in the three days of the protocol; mean (SE) considering all subjects.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160063.t001

After checking the normality of the distributions with a Kolgomorov-Smirnov test for one sample, an ANOVA with repeated measures analyzed the effect of tDCS on the variables considering the 13 subjects.

We have used two different ANOVA designs: one for the PMT and one for RT and MT. The former is a five factors ANOVA with STIM (three levels: AR-CL, AL-CR, and Sham); Time (two levels: Pre and Post); LATERALITY (two levels: Target Ipsilateral or Contralateral to the dominant–executing- hand); OPTION (two levels: single and choice responses) and MUSCLE (three levels: deltoid, biceps and triceps). The ANOVA on RT and MT had the same design except for the factor MUSCLE which was not included since RT and MT derived from contact plates.

The W-Mauchly test checked the sphericity for ANOVA, if sphericity was violated the ANOVA degrees of freedom were corrected by means of the Greenhouse-Geisser coefficients. Effect sides were calculated by partial etha and etha squared (ηp2, η2). Significance was considered if p<0.05.

Results

Table 1 shows the mean PMT, RT, and MT at Pre under each experimental condition (values serving as normalizing factors and equivalent to the units in the y-axes of the corresponding graph). Subjects made no responses during the catch trials. See also S1 Data.

Effects of Brain Stimulation on PMT

Table 2 shows the mean values for the different levels of all factors in a pre-post basis.

thumbnail
Table 2. Mean values in ms (and SE) for the different levels of the different factors in a pre-post basis.

PMT: premotor times; RT: reaction times; MT: movement times.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160063.t002

Table 3 summarizes the effects of tDCS on PMT (ANOVA factors, and significant interactions of the factors with TIME). If interactions TIME x STIM were significant (first column of results in the table), we followed-up with ANOVA’s by pair of STIM modes and, if significance remained, by STIM mode in isolation (subsequent columns in Table 3).

thumbnail
Table 3. ANOVA’s for PMT.

Main effects and significant interactions with factor TIME. ANOVA’s were executed considering the three STIM modes. If significant interactions indicate different responses to STIM in the testing TIMEs, the ANOVA’s were followed-up by pairs and the by single STIM mode.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160063.t003

With the 3 STIM modes-ANOVA there were significant main effects showing that all PMT were faster in single compared to choice responses (ηp2 = 0.672; η2 = 0.061), in ipsilateral than contralteral responses (ηp2 = 0.368; η2 = 0.006); and presented a sequential muscle activation (ηp2 = 0.716 η2 = 0.393; the latter can be observed in a representative subject in Fig 1C).

The significant interactions between TIME and STIM with other factors were observed in this model with 3 stimulation modes (also in the rest of models, except when the two active tDCS protocols were compared), Table 3. This means that the responses to Sham were different to the responses of the other to stimulation modes, and that the responses obtained with the two active stimulation modes were not significantly different of each other; for such reason their effects are shown pooled in figures (green tones). The individual’s responses in Pre vs. Post basis are depicted in Fig 2.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Individuals’ responses for PMT.

The y-axis unit indicates the mean response considering all subjects and conditions at Pre. It was equivalent to 201.8ms (sem 11.5) for AL-CR (A); 204.5ms (sem 12.3) for AR-CL (B); and 201.0ms (sem 10.5) for Sham sessions (C).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160063.g002

For the follow-up ANOVA including AR-CL and AL-CR stim modes the significant factor TIME, in absence of significant interactions with any other factor, indicates a small (ηp2 = 0.288 η2 = 0.002) but significant (F1,12 = 4.9 p<0.05TIME) reduction of 1.5% in the PMT at Post (Fig 3A). This effect was present in the three studied muscles and was independent on the laterality and options of the responses. Fig 2 indicates that in most of the subjects the reduction in the PMT was small, which explains the small (although significant) effect observed.

thumbnail
Fig 3.

(A) PMT at Post were differently modulated by Sham-tDCS compared to the other two active protocols, which did not differ each other (shown pooled in green tones). There was a significant decrease at Post after both active protocols. (B) Sham stimulation increased Post PMT, specifically in the triceps muscle. The y-axis unit indicates the mean response across all subjects and conditions at Pre. *p<0.05; **p<0.01.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160063.g003

For the ANOVA with Sham stimulation, the effects were rather the opposite. The effect of Sham stimulation considering the three muscle together was a very mild (surely non significant) increase of PMT in the Post condition (Fig 3A). However the Sham effects where different for the three muscles (F2,24 = 9.5Ԑ = 0.6 p = 0.005TIMExMUS ηp2 = 0.443 η2 = 0.009), and PMT increased significantly (4%) at Post in the triceps (post-hoc p = 0.002) (Fig 3B).

Effects of Brain Stimulation on RT and MT

Table 2 shows the mean values for the different levels of the different factors in a pre-post basis, in the RT and MT.

Table 4 shows significant main effects, indicating that the responses were faster in the case of the single than in choice tasks for RT (ηp2 = 0.659; η2 = 0.069). On the other hand, ipsilateral were faster than contralateral responses, this was shown by a significant main effect of factor “laterality” for RT (ηp2 = 0.291; η2 = 0.010) and also for MT (ηp2 = 0.873; η2 = 0.495).

thumbnail
Table 4. ANOVA’s for variables which response was not different for the three stimulation modes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160063.t004

Table 4 also shows that RT and MT were not modified by the different stimulation modes. For both variables factor TIME was never significant and it did not interact significantly with any other factor.

Effects of Brain Stimulation on CV’s of PMT, RT and MT

Table 4 indicates a change in the CV’s of PMT and RT at post compared to pre; but not in the CV of MT. Since there was a significant main effect of factor “time” for CV-PMT and for CV-RT (Fig 4A and 4B respectively), but there were not significant interactions with any other factor, this means that the significant reductions in the CV’s after tDCS (from 18.3 to 17.0% in PMT; and from 13.9 to 12.6% in RT) were observed in all tasks (and muscles for PMT); and also in all stimulation protocols, including Sham (ηp2 = 0.677, η2 = 0.01 for PMT; and ηp2 = 0.373, η2 = 0.014 for RT).

thumbnail
Fig 4.

(A) The CV of PMT was reduced significantly at Post, regardless stimulation modes, tasks or muscles (so that shown pooled). (B) A similar pattern was found for the CV of RT. *p<0.05; ***p<0.001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160063.g004

Effects of Brain Stimulation on Anticipatory Responses

A total of 70 reaching movements were anticipatory responses, this is 5.6% of the 1,248 movements executed, in all subjects. The proportion of anticipations at Pre and Post was not significant different (Fisher Exact Probability Test p>0.05). Fragmentation of this analysis for the different tasks at Pre and Post was not considered due to the reduced number of anticipations.

Discussion

In the tasks performed in our experiments we have observed some well known features of reaction responses: i.) faster reactions to single than to choice options [50, 51]; ii.) faster reactions with ipsilateral than contralateral movements [52] and iii.) sequential muscle activation during reaching tasks [5355]. However, the main finding of this work is that real tDCS of M1 reduces the PMT of reaching movements. Remarkably, the effect of tDCS was not different for the two active electrode montages. The significant increase in triceps PMT Post-Sham might be explained by fatigability, since the muscle has a main role in the fast projection of the hand towards the target. Fatigability (i.e., reduction of performance at Post) was not only avoided by real stimulation, but the net effect was a step further and reduced PMT (in all muscles). Admittedly the reduction of PMT after active tDCS was small but statistically significant.

Sham Effects on PMT and RT

The increment in PMT after Sham stimulation was localized at the level of the triceps muscle, without affecting the deltoids and biceps. These results might provide some insights on the operational mechanisms at post-Sham. The role of the triceps is of capital importance in the projection of the hand to the target, as an elbow extensor, while, on the other hand, the biceps is an elbow and shoulder flexor, this latter function shared with anterior head of deltoids. Thus, deltoids and biceps main involvement in the tasks is to lift the hand from the plates; later on the triceps is activated to project the hand towards the target [54]. Such a sequential activation is reflected in all our EMG recordings. It is tempting to speculate that, in our protocol, fatigue only affects the more demanding muscular activity and, consequently, only the triceps was affected post Sham (active stimulation affected all muscles, see discussion below). These results matched the fact that RT (recorded at plate lift) were unaffected by the Sham stimulation. Therefore task progression might induce fatigue which is reflected in an alteration in muscle recruitment patterns.

Remarkably fatigue might arise from different sources. Muscle fatigue is defined as a progressive failure of muscle-output generating capacity during and after the tasks. Its origin appears to lie in a deficit of the neural motor system to generate or propagate the action potentials to the muscle in an efficient manner [56]—this is a form of central fatigue (CF). However, CF might also emerge from other (non motor) factors (i.e., mental fatigue) [57, 58] where the increase in PMT during a prolonged period of attention might be a marker of cognitive functionality waning [57, 58]. Bilateral tDCS of prefrontal areas (regardless polarity) has been shown to improve cognitive processes like attention [59], and it seems that tDCS improved attention might be a key element to reduce high order expressions of fatigability (such as mental fatigue) [57].

Despite that with our protocol we cannot discard that the reduction in PMT after bilateral tDCS could be related to modulations of cognitive processing, we believe that mental fatigue expression on PMT should have been shown in all the three muscles recorded, and not only in the triceps. Note also that the anticipatory responses (likely to be related with mental fatigue) were not modified by the protocols, and that very delayed PMT reflecting loss of attention (longer than 800ms) were present only once across subjects, which weakens the possibility that the effects of our tDCS protocol are related to cognitive processing.

Active tDCS Reduces PMT Regardless Polarity

Contrary to our initial hypothesis, the effects observed with real stimulation were polarity independent. Our hypothesis followed the “contralaterality of motor control”[60]; this considers the presence of inter-hemispheric interactions between both motor areas, with a stronger inhibition on the non-dominant M1 emerging from the dominant motor cortex [6064]. Considering such asymmetry, it should never be expected that both active tDCS montages produce a same effect. However, it should also be considered that there are a number of ipsilateral projections from the motor areas to the spinal cord [65]. These ipsilateral projections likely target a great proportion of motoneuron pools controlling proximal limb muscles (like those evaluated in our study deltoids, biceps or triceps) [66], which might contribute to our polarity independent results. In fact, there is a growing amount of data showing that when we move a hand or an arm the activity in both the contralateral and the ipsilateral hemispheres are simultaneously activated. The neurophysiological significance of the bilateral activation of the motor cortices remains unclear. Kobayashi et al., [64] suggested that “ipsilateral activation during non-dominant hand movements could reflect an increased inhibition exerted by the right over the left hemisphere through callosal fibers”. Others support the idea that all movements are initiated in the dominant hemisphere with the non-dominant would be responsible just for the execution of the command issued by the dominant hemisphere [60]. Interestingly, we started with an hypothesis which would sustain in principle the theory of contralaterality of motor control (i.e. “we predict a shortening of the MT and RT after bilateral tDCS, when anodal stimulation is applied to the M1 contralateral to the executing arm, as a result of the increased excitability of M1 circuits”), but we ended up with something completely different which seem to follow the new stream of thought.

Effects of tDCS on RT and MT

Our results indicate that active tDCS reduces PMT but not RT. At a first glance these results seems incompatible. Reynolds and Ashby [67] have shown that PMT are periods characterized by a progressive reduction of intracortical inhibition. This is to say that the level of intracortical inhibition is minimal from the last ≈80ms preceding EMG-onset to end of the PMT [67], as well as during muscle contraction [68]. As mentioned before, this inhibitory process is likely to be modulated by tDCS [69]. However, RT (once finished PMT) corresponds with periods where inhibition is not altered much and, perhaps not affected by tDCS.

Likewise, MT were unaffected by tDCS. Since the reaction-time protocols reflect both, cognitive and motor components, the lack of changes in MT seems to favor a specific effect of tDCS on the cognitive component of the response. However, some evidences might not support this possibility. Firstly, PMT was differently affected in the three muscles after Sham. This suggests an alteration in muscle recruitment rather than in cognitive processing. Second, some previous work has indicated that MT variations depend on the speed of reactions; faster reactions produce increments in MT and vice versa [33, 52]; thus, perhaps the option that tDCS prevents the increment of MT should be taken into account.

Effects of tDCS on Variability of Responses

Some previous work indicates that tDCS on parietal cortex reduces the variability of time estimation if applied to the left hemisphere, or impacts the accuracy of time estimation after right hemisphere stimulation [70]. However, in our case, the reduction of the variability (in PMT and RT, but not of MT) occurred after all stimulation protocols, either active or Sham. The reduction of variability during repetitive reaching movements is not a new finding and has been related to progressive appearance of fatigue along the task. In this situation the kinematics of reaching movements becomes more stereotyped [71] and movements less variable. However, we believe that a reduction of CV’s of PMT and RT simply due to motor practice is also possible. The protocol was not conceived to test this hypothesis but this is a possibility to be considered since the reduction in CV was observed in all muscles (regardless they expressed fatigue -i.e., triceps- post Sham or not -i.e., deltoids and biceps-) and in all stimulation conditions. Though this possibility does not explain why the CV of MT was not reduced with practice, it is plausible that different expressions of a motor act might need different levels of practice to reduce their variability.

Limitations of the Study

Our study only explored the effects of tDCS on the dominant limb. It is possible that effect sizes would have been greater in the case of studying the non-dominant hand, because in healthy subjects motor execution with the dominant arm is likely to experience ceiling-effects. In fact, some reports indicate greater effects of tDCS for the non-dominant limb [72]. In addition our sample size is modest, therefore future studies should clarify the effect of tDCS on reaching movements executed with dominant/non-dominant arms, on greater samples.

Conclusions

In our hands, bilateral tDCS-M1 reduced PMT and avoided fatigability in a functional reaching task; the effects were polarity independent. Futures studies might include different reaction time protocols to disentangle the cognitive and motor effects produced by tDCS on these kinds of tasks.

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Kenneth Grieve for his useful comments on this work.

Author Contributions

  1. Conceived and designed the experiments: PA AO JC.
  2. Performed the experiments: PA YC VR AM.
  3. Analyzed the data: PA.
  4. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: AO JC.
  5. Wrote the paper: PA YC VR AM AO JC.

References

  1. 1. Nitsche MA, Liebetanz D, Lang N, Antal A, Tergau F, Paulus W. Safety criteria for transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) in humans. Clin Neurophysiol. 2003;114(11):2220–2; author reply 2–3. pmid:14580622
  2. 2. Poreisz C, Boros K, Antal A, Paulus W. Safety aspects of transcranial direct current stimulation concerning healthy subjects and patients. Brain Res Bull. 2007;72(4–6):208–14. pmid:17452283
  3. 3. Nitsche MA, Paulus W. Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J Physiol. 2000;527 Pt 3:633–9. pmid:10990547
  4. 4. Nitsche MA, Fricke K, Henschke U, Schlitterlau A, Liebetanz D, Lang N, et al. Pharmacological modulation of cortical excitability shifts induced by transcranial direct current stimulation in humans. J Physiol. 2003;553(Pt 1):293–301. pmid:12949224
  5. 5. Nitsche MA, Nitsche MS, Klein CC, Tergau F, Rothwell JC, Paulus W. Level of action of cathodal DC polarisation induced inhibition of the human motor cortex. Clin Neurophysiol. 2003;114(4):600–4. pmid:12686268
  6. 6. Nitsche MA, Paulus W. Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology. 2001;57(10):1899–901. pmid:11723286
  7. 7. Mordillo-Mateos L, Turpin-Fenoll L, Millan-Pascual J, Nunez-Perez N, Panyavin I, Gomez-Arguelles JM, et al. Effects of simultaneous bilateral tDCS of the human motor cortex. Brain Stimul. 2012;5(3):214–22. pmid:21782545
  8. 8. Kidgell DJ, Goodwill AM, Frazer AK, Daly RM. Induction of cortical plasticity and improved motor performance following unilateral and bilateral transcranial direct current stimulation of the primary motor cortex. BMC Neurosci. 2013;14:64. pmid:23815634
  9. 9. Schlaug G, Renga V, Nair D. Transcranial direct current stimulation in stroke recovery. Arch Neurol. 2008;65(12):1571–6. pmid:19064743
  10. 10. Vines BW, Cerruti C, Schlaug G. Dual-hemisphere tDCS facilitates greater improvements for healthy subjects' non-dominant hand compared to uni-hemisphere stimulation. BMC Neurosci. 2008;9:103. pmid:18957075
  11. 11. Lindenberg R, Renga V, Zhu LL, Nair D, Schlaug G. Bihemispheric brain stimulation facilitates motor recovery in chronic stroke patients. Neurology. 2010;75(24):2176–84. pmid:21068427
  12. 12. Waters-Metenier S, Husain M, Wiestler T, Diedrichsen J. Bihemispheric transcranial direct current stimulation enhances effector-independent representations of motor synergy and sequence learning. J Neurosci. 2014;34(3):1037–50. pmid:24431461
  13. 13. Stagg CJ, O'Shea J, Kincses ZT, Woolrich M, Matthews PM, Johansen-Berg H. Modulation of movement-associated cortical activation by transcranial direct current stimulation. Eur J Neurosci. 2009;30(7):1412–23. pmid:19788568
  14. 14. Roche N, Lackmy A, Achache V, Bussel B, Katz R. Impact of transcranial direct current stimulation on spinal network excitability in humans. J Physiol. 2009;587(Pt 23):5653–64. pmid:19805746
  15. 15. Roche N, Lackmy A, Achache V, Bussel B, Katz R. Effects of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation over the leg motor area on lumbar spinal network excitability in healthy subjects. J Physiol. 2011;589(Pt 11):2813–26. pmid:21502292
  16. 16. Mellah S, Rispal-Padel L, Riviere G. Changes in excitability of motor units during preparation for movement. Exp Brain Res. 1990;82(1):178–86. pmid:2257902
  17. 17. Riedo R, Ruegg DG. Origin of the specific H reflex facilitation preceding a voluntary movement in man. J Physiol. 1988;397:371–88. pmid:3411511
  18. 18. Frank JS. Spinal motor preparation in humans. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1986;63(4):361–70. pmid:2419094
  19. 19. Hummel F, Celnik P, Giraux P, Floel A, Wu WH, Gerloff C, et al. Effects of non-invasive cortical stimulation on skilled motor function in chronic stroke. Brain. 2005;128(Pt 3):490–9. pmid:15634731
  20. 20. Hummel FC, Cohen LG. Non-invasive brain stimulation: a new strategy to improve neurorehabilitation after stroke? Lancet Neurol. 2006;5(8):708–12. pmid:16857577
  21. 21. Saiote C, Goldschmidt T, Timaus C, Steenwijk MD, Opitz A, Antal A, et al. Impact of transcranial direct current stimulation on fatigue in multiple sclerosis. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2014;32(3):423–36. pmid:24531295
  22. 22. Galletti C, Kutz DF, Gamberini M, Breveglieri R, Fattori P. Role of the medial parieto-occipital cortex in the control of reaching and grasping movements. Exp Brain Res. 2003;153(2):158–70. pmid:14517595
  23. 23. Kalaska JF, Scott SH, Cisek P, Sergio LE. Cortical control of reaching movements. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1997;7(6):849–59. pmid:9464979
  24. 24. Kalaska JF, Caminiti R, Georgopoulos AP. Cortical mechanisms related to the direction of two-dimensional arm movements: relations in parietal area 5 and comparison with motor cortex. Exp Brain Res. 1983;51(2):247–60. pmid:6617794
  25. 25. Georgopoulos AP, Schwartz AB, Kettner RE. Neuronal population coding of movement direction. Science. 1986;233(4771):1416–9. pmid:3749885
  26. 26. Battaglia-Mayer A, Caminiti R, Lacquaniti F, Zago M. Multiple levels of representation of reaching in the parieto-frontal network. Cereb Cortex. 2003;13(10):1009–22. pmid:12967918
  27. 27. Goto K, Hoshi Y, Sata M, Kawahara M, Takahashi M, Murohashi H. Role of the prefrontal cortex in the cognitive control of reaching movements: near-infrared spectroscopy study. J Biomed Opt. 2011;16(12):127003. pmid:22191933
  28. 28. Katsuki F, Saito M, Constantinidis C. Influence of monkey dorsolateral prefrontal and posterior parietal activity on behavioral choice during attention tasks. Eur J Neurosci. 2014.
  29. 29. Hanakawa T. Rostral premotor cortex as a gateway between motor and cognitive networks. Neurosci Res. 2011;70(2):144–54. pmid:21382425
  30. 30. Cisek P, Kalaska JF. Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2010;33:269–98. pmid:20345247
  31. 31. Mattia M, Pani P, Mirabella G, Costa S, Del Giudice P, Ferraina S. Heterogeneous attractor cell assemblies for motor planning in premotor cortex. J Neurosci. 2013;33(27):11155–68. pmid:23825419
  32. 32. Mattia M, Spadacenta S, Pavone L, Quarato P, Esposito V, Sparano A, et al. Stop-event-related potentials from intracranial electrodes reveal a key role of premotor and motor cortices in stopping ongoing movements. Front Neuroeng. 2012;5:12. pmid:22754525
  33. 33. Mirabella G, Iaconelli S, Modugno N, Giannini G, Lena F, Cantore G. Stimulation of subthalamic nuclei restores a near normal planning strategy in Parkinson's patients. PLoS One. 2013;8(5):e62793. pmid:23658775
  34. 34. Mirabella G, Iaconelli S, Romanelli P, Modugno N, Lena F, Manfredi M, et al. Deep brain stimulation of subthalamic nuclei affects arm response inhibition in Parkinson's patients. Cereb Cortex. 2012;22(5):1124–32. pmid:21810782
  35. 35. Mirabella G. Should I stay or should I go? Conceptual underpinnings of goal-directed actions. Front Syst Neurosci. 2014;8:206. pmid:25404898
  36. 36. Georgopoulos AP. Current issues in directional motor control. Trends Neurosci. 1995;18(11):506–10. pmid:8592761
  37. 37. Glover S, Wall MB, Smith AT. Distinct cortical networks support the planning and online control of reaching-to-grasp in humans. Eur J Neurosci. 2012;35(6):909–15. pmid:22429244
  38. 38. Fu QG, Suarez JI, Ebner TJ. Neuronal specification of direction and distance during reaching movements in the superior precentral premotor area and primary motor cortex of monkeys. J Neurophysiol. 1993;70(5):2097–116. pmid:8294972
  39. 39. Tanji J, Evarts EV. Anticipatory activity of motor cortex neurons in relation to direction of an intended movement. J Neurophysiol. 1976;39(5):1062–8. pmid:824409
  40. 40. Zhang J, Riehle A, Requin J, Kornblum S. Dynamics of single neuron activity in monkey primary motor cortex related to sensorimotor transformation. J Neurosci. 1997;17(6):2227–46. pmid:9045746
  41. 41. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9(1):97–113. pmid:5146491
  42. 42. Dean C, Shepherd R, Adams R. Sitting balance I: trunk-arm coordination and the contribution of the lower limbs during self-paced reaching in sitting. Gait Posture. 1999;10(2):135–46. pmid:10502647
  43. 43. Eichenberger A, Ruegg DG. Relation between the specific H reflex facilitation preceding a voluntary movement and movement parameters in man. J Physiol. 1984;347:545–59. pmid:6707967
  44. 44. Hodges PW, Bui BH. A comparison of computer-based methods for the determination of onset of muscle contraction using electromyography. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1996;101(6):511–9. pmid:9020824
  45. 45. Andrews RK, Schabrun SM, Ridding MC, Galea MP, Hodges PW, Chipchase LS. The effect of electrical stimulation on corticospinal excitability is dependent on application duration: a same subject pre-post test design. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2013;10:51. pmid:23758902
  46. 46. Strigaro G, Ruge D, Chen JC, Marshall L, Desikan M, Cantello R, et al. Interaction between visual and motor cortex: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. J Physiol. 2015;593(10):2365–77. pmid:25762215
  47. 47. Cantello R, Strigaro G, Prandi P, Varrasi C, Mula M, Monaco F. Paired-pulse flash-visual evoked potentials: new methods revive an old test. Clin Neurophysiol. 2011;122(8):1622–8. pmid:21306943
  48. 48. Schieppati M, Trompetto C, Abbruzzese G. Selective facilitation of responses to cortical stimulation of proximal and distal arm muscles by precision tasks in man. J Physiol. 1996;491 (Pt 2):551–62. pmid:8866878
  49. 49. Brass M, Bekkering H, Prinz W. Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2001;106(1–2):3–22.
  50. 50. Gottsdanker R, Shragg GP. Verification of Donders' subtraction method. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1985;11(6):765–76. pmid:2934507
  51. 51. Donders FC. On the speed of mental processes. Acta Psychol (Amst). 1969;30:412–31.
  52. 52. Mirabella G, Pani P, Ferraina S. Context influences on the preparation and execution of reaching movements. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2008;25(7–8):996–1010. pmid:19378414
  53. 53. Flanders M. Temporal patterns of muscle activation for arm movements in three-dimensional space. J Neurosci. 1991;11(9):2680–93. pmid:1880544
  54. 54. Flanders M, Pellegrini JJ, Geisler SD. Basic features of phasic activation for reaching in vertical planes. Exp Brain Res. 1996;110(1):67–79. pmid:8817258
  55. 55. d'Avella A, Lacquaniti F. Control of reaching movements by muscle synergy combinations. Front Comput Neurosci. 2013;7:42. pmid:23626534
  56. 56. Gandevia SC. Spinal and supraspinal factors in human muscle fatigue. Physiol Rev. 2001;81(4):1725–89. pmid:11581501
  57. 57. Tanaka M, Ishii A, Watanabe Y. Neural effects of mental fatigue caused by continuous attention load: a magnetoencephalography study. Brain Res. 2014;1561:60–6. pmid:24642273
  58. 58. Faber LG, Maurits NM, Lorist MM. Mental fatigue affects visual selective attention. PLoS One. 2012;7(10):e48073. pmid:23118927
  59. 59. Nelson JT, McKinley RA, Golob EJ, Warm JS, Parasuraman R. Enhancing vigilance in operators with prefrontal cortex transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Neuroimage. 2014;85 Pt 3:909–17. pmid:23235272
  60. 60. Derakhshan I. Callosum and movement control: case reports. Neurol Res. 2003;25(5):538–42. pmid:12866205
  61. 61. Ferbert A, Priori A, Rothwell JC, Day BL, Colebatch JG, Marsden CD. Interhemispheric inhibition of the human motor cortex. J Physiol. 1992;453:525–46. pmid:1464843
  62. 62. Gorsler A, Baumer T, Weiller C, Munchau A, Liepert J. Interhemispheric effects of high and low frequency rTMS in healthy humans. Clin Neurophysiol. 2003;114(10):1800–7. pmid:14499741
  63. 63. Netz J, Ziemann U, Homberg V. Hemispheric asymmetry of transcallosal inhibition in man. Exp Brain Res. 1995;104(3):527–33. pmid:7589304
  64. 64. Kobayashi M, Hutchinson S, Schlaug G, Pascual-Leone A. Ipsilateral motor cortex activation on functional magnetic resonance imaging during unilateral hand movements is related to interhemispheric interactions. Neuroimage. 2003;20(4):2259–70. pmid:14683727
  65. 65. Ziemann U, Ishii K, Borgheresi A, Yaseen Z, Battaglia F, Hallett M, et al. Dissociation of the pathways mediating ipsilateral and contralateral motor-evoked potentials in human hand and arm muscles. J Physiol. 1999;518 (Pt 3):895–906.
  66. 66. Colebatch JG, Gandevia SC. The distribution of muscular weakness in upper motor neuron lesions affecting the arm. Brain. 1989;112 (Pt 3):749–63. pmid:2731028
  67. 67. Reynolds C, Ashby P. Inhibition in the human motor cortex is reduced just before a voluntary contraction. Neurology. 1999;53(4):730–5. pmid:10489033
  68. 68. Reis J, Swayne OB, Vandermeeren Y, Camus M, Dimyan MA, Harris-Love M, et al. Contribution of transcranial magnetic stimulation to the understanding of cortical mechanisms involved in motor control. J Physiol. 2008;586(2):325–51. pmid:17974592
  69. 69. Cengiz B, Murase N, Rothwell JC. Opposite effects of weak transcranial direct current stimulation on different phases of short interval intracortical inhibition (SICI). Exp Brain Res. 2013;225(3):321–31. pmid:23292100
  70. 70. Vicario CM, Martino D, Koch G. Temporal accuracy and variability in the left and right posterior parietal cortex. Neuroscience. 2013;245:121–8. pmid:23628777
  71. 71. Lomond KV, Cote JN. Movement timing and reach to reach variability during a repetitive reaching task in persons with chronic neck/shoulder pain and healthy subjects. Exp Brain Res. 2010;206(3):271–82. pmid:20848276
  72. 72. Marquez J, Conley A, Karayanidis F, Lagopoulos J, Parsons M. Anodal direct current stimulation in the healthy aged: Effects determined by the hemisphere stimulated. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2015;33(4):509–19. pmid:26409409