Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Etiological Subgroups of Small-for-Gestational-Age: Differential Neurodevelopmental Outcomes

  • Xiuhong Li,

    Affiliations Department of Maternal and Child Health, School of Public Health, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou, Guangdong 510080, China, Division of Behavioral Medicine, Department of Pediatrics, University at Buffalo, State University of New York, Buffalo, NY, 14214, United States of America

  • Rina D. Eiden,

    Affiliation Research Institute on Addictions, University at Buffalo, State University of New York, Buffalo, NY, 14203, United States of America

  • Leonard H. Epstein,

    Affiliation Division of Behavioral Medicine, Department of Pediatrics, University at Buffalo, State University of New York, Buffalo, NY, 14214, United States of America

  • Edmond D. Shenassa,

    Affiliations Maternal and Child Health Program, School of Public Health, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, 20742, United States of America, Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, School of Public Health, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, 20742, United States of America, Department of Epidemiology, School of Public Health, Brown University, Providence, RI, 02912, United States of America

  • Chuanbo Xie,

    Affiliations Division of Behavioral Medicine, Department of Pediatrics, University at Buffalo, State University of New York, Buffalo, NY, 14214, United States of America, Department of Cancer Prevention Research, Sun Yat-sen University Cancer Center, Guangzhou, Guangdong Province 510060, China

  • Xiaozhong Wen

    xiaozhongwen@hotmail.com

    Affiliation Division of Behavioral Medicine, Department of Pediatrics, University at Buffalo, State University of New York, Buffalo, NY, 14214, United States of America

Abstract

Objectives

It remains unclear why substantial variations in neurodevelopmental outcomes exist within small-for-gestational-age (SGA) children. We prospectively compared 5-y neurodevelopmental outcomes across SGA etiological subgroups.

Methods

Children born SGA (N = 1050) from U.S. Early Childhood Longitudinal Study-Birth Cohort (2001–2007) was divided into etiological subgroups by each of 7 well-established prenatal risk factors. We fit linear regression models to compare 5-y reading, math, gross motor and fine motor scores across SGA subgroups, adjusting for socio-demographic confounders.

Results

Compared to singleton SGA subgroup, multiple-birth SGA subgroup had lower mean reading (adjusted mean difference, -4.08 [95% confidence interval, -6.10, -2.06]) and math (-2.22 [-3.61, -0.84]) scores. These disadvantages in reading and math existed only among multiple-birth SGA subgroup without ovulation stimulation (reading, -4.50 [-6.64, -2.36]; math, -2.91 [-4.37, -1.44]), but not among those with ovulation stimulation (reading, -2.33 [-6.24, 1.57]; math 0.63 [-1.86, 3.12]). Compared to singleton SGA subgroup without maternal smoking and inadequate gestational weight gain, singleton SGA subgroup with co-occurrence of maternal smoking and inadequate gestational weight gain (GWG) had lower mean reading (-4.81 [-8.50, -1.12]) and math (-2.95 [-5.51, -0.38]) scores. These differences were not mediated by Apgar score.

Conclusions

Multiple-birth SGA subgroups (vs. singleton SGA) or singleton SGA subgroup with co-occurrence of smoking and inadequate GWG (vs. singleton SGA subgroup without maternal smoking and inadequate gestational weight gain) have poorer cognitive development up to 5 y.

Introduction

Body size at birth is well known to be a risk factor for perinatal morbidity and mortality, as well as poor neurodevelopment [1,2]. A newborn shorter or lighter than expected for a certain gestational age, i.e. 2 standard deviation (SD) below the average or below the 3rd or 10th percentile of newborn reference of the same sex and gestational age is often called small for gestational age (SGA). About 10% -16% of children in high-income countries [3] and 27% of children in low- and middle-income countries [4] are born small-for-gestational-age (SGA), due to various genetic and environmental causes. Some key prenatal risk factors for SGA include maternal pre-pregnancy underweight, short stature, smoking during pregnancy, alcohol use during pregnancy, inadequate gestational weight gain (GWG), hypertensive conditions, and multiple births [5].

On average, children born SGA are more likely to have worse neurodevelopment than children born appropriate-for-gestational-age (AGA) [6]. However, there is a great deal of heterogeneity in these adverse long-term outcomes among SGA newborns. It remains unclear why substantial variations in long-term neurodevelopmental outcomes exist within small-for-gestational-age (SGA) children. Neurodevelopmental disadvantages related to SGA are likely due to structural and/or functional impairment in brain development [7]. Research suggests that different risk factors of SGA may influence fetal brain development through different biological mechanisms. For example, SGA fetus with maternal smoking during pregnancy may have interferes directly by stimulation of neurotransmitters or indirectly by inducing brain hypoxia [8,9]; SGA fetus with maternal pre-pregnancy underweight or inadequate GWG may have insufficient supply of nutrition to brain during pregnancy [10,11]; SGA fetus with multiple birth often restricted by crowded space and nutrition supply [12,13]. In addition, some prenatal risk factors leading to SGA are likely to co-occur, which may cause “double hits” to the fetal brain and causes more severe impairments than each factor separately. Therefore, it’s reasonable to hypothesize different etiologies of SGA can help to understand these substantial variations. This hypothesis is supported by our previous research in which only SGA subgroups with maternal smoking during pregnancy have elevated risk of hypercholesterolemia in adults, while SGA subgroups without maternal smoking have similar risk as AGA children [14]. The death risk of a SGA fetus is higher if the mother is ≥175 cm tall, normotensive, nulliparous, or non-smoking [15].

In addition, the extent of neurodevelopmental impairment may vary considerably across SGA etiological subgroups, possibly due to the variation in Apgar score (an indicator for some SGA-related complications such as birth asphyxia and cerebral palsy [16]). This variable is associated with the incidence and severity of late neurodevelopmental impairment, possibly through reduction in brain weight and cell number [17]. Alternatively, the variation in neurological impairment across SGA subgroups may be due to the direct effects of some prenatal risk factors on the developing fetal brain. As shown in Fig 1, we hypothesized that etiological subgroups of SGA have different neurodevelopment measured by cognitive and motor outcomes, through the direct effects of SGA-related prenatal risk factors or mediation by Apgar score (indirect effects).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Life course framework for neurodevelopmental outcomes at 5 y of SGA etiological subgroups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160677.g001

Early screening and intervention may alter SGA children’s disadvantaged neurodevelopmental paths and thus improve their later outcomes [1820]. Dividing SGA children into etiological subgroups, can not only help to distinguish the perinatal etiology for SGA, but also identify high-risk subgroups of SGA that have poorer neurodevelopmental outcomes in later life and thus need early intervention. Therefore, in this study we aimed to 1) examine the differences in neurodevelopmental outcomes at 5 y across etiological subgroups of SGA based on 7 key prenatal risk factors including maternal pre-pregnancy underweight, short stature, smoking during pregnancy, alcohol use during pregnancy, inadequate gestational weight gain (GWG), hypertensive conditions, and multiple births; 2) examine the differences in neurodevelopmental outcomes at 5 y across etiological subgroups of SGA based on co-occurrence of significant risk factors selected in Aim 1; 3) examine the extent to which Apgar score would mediate the associations between SGA etiological subgroups and neurodevelopmental outcomes.

Materials and Methods

Data and sample

We used existing data from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study-Birth cohort (ECLS-B, 2001–2007, N = 10 700), a U.S. national longitudinal birth cohort with child assessments at birth, 9 months (m), 2 years (y), 4 y (preschool), 5 y (kindergarten 2006), and 6 y (kindergarten 2007) [21,22]. This analysis included ECLS-B children born SGA (N = 1050) and AGA (N = 4250) with complete data on birth weight and gestational age, the 7 key prenatal risk factors for SGA, neurodevelopmental outcomes at 5 y, and potential confounders (Table 1). Fig 2 shows the sample flow. All reported numbers about sample size are rounded to nearest 50 according to the confidentiality policy of U.S. Department of Education. This secondary data analysis was approved by the Social and Behavioral Sciences Institutional Review Board, State University of New York at Buffalo.

thumbnail
Table 1. Characteristics of analytic and excluded SGA and AGA samples (N = 9450).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160677.t001

Measures of exposures

In this study, the exposures were SGA etiological subgroups based on each or co-occurrence of maternal pre-pregnancy underweight, short stature, smoking and alcohol use during pregnancy, inadequate GWG, hypertensive conditions, and multiple births. We considered these 7 factors because they are well-established prenatal risk factors for SGA and could explain large proportion of SGA [5]. Also, they are potentially modifiable by nutritional, behavioral, and medical interventions.

SGA and AGA.

Birth weight in grams, last menstrual period (LMP), and delivery date were extracted from birth certificates of live births in 2001 provided by the State Vital Registration and Statistics Executives [23]. Birth weight in birth certificate has been shown valid and reliable (e.g. correlation with birth weight on birth record, 0.96) [24]. Gestational age in weeks was calculated as the interval between LMP and delivery date. Gestational age from birth certificate has been shown fairly valid (e.g. correlation with gestational age on birth record, 0.68) [24]. SGA was defined as birth weight below 10th percentile for the same sex and gestational age within a national reference population of U.S. newborns [25]. AGA was defined as birth weight between 10th-90th percentiles. For the purpose of this analysis, we excluded children with birth weight above 90th percentile (large-for-gestational-age, LGA).

Pre-pregnancy underweight and short stature.

Mother reported her height and pre-pregnancy weight in the 9-m postpartum interview. Pre-pregnancy body mass index (BMI) was calculated as weight (kg)/height (m) 2. Pre-pregnancy underweight was defined as pre-pregnancy BMI<18.5kg/m2 [26]. Short stature was defined as maternal height ≤157.5cm [27].

Smoking and alcohol use during pregnancy.

We extracted information on maternal smoking (# of cigarettes/day) and alcohol use (yes vs. no) during pregnancy from birth certificates. Although these two socially unfavorable behaviors were subject to under-report in birth certificate, it was acceptable to use them for exploratory studies like ECLS-B [28]. If the information on smoking (16.1%) and alcohol use (14.9%) was unavailable in birth certificate, we supplemented it with retrospective self-report in the 9-m postpartum interview. Besides binary variable (yes/no), we also classified maternal smoking into 4 categories: never smoking, quitted smoking (smoking before pregnancy, but not during pregnancy), moderate smoking (1–9 cigarettes/day during pregnancy) and heavy smoking (≥10 cigarettes/day during pregnancy).

Inadequate GWG.

The information on total GWG was obtained from birth certificates (77.9%) and also from maternal self-reports at 9-m postpartum interview (22.1%) if birth certificate data was unavailable. Inadequate GWG for singletons was defined total GWG less than 12.5 kg for underweight (pre-pregnancy BMI<18.5 kg/m2), 11.5 kg for normal weight (BMI, 18.5–24.9 kg/m2), 7 kg for overweight (BMI, 25–29.9 kg/m2), and 5 kg for obese women (BMI ≥30 kg/m2), respectively. For multiple births, inadequate GWG was defined total GWG less than 17 kg for underweight and normal weight, 14 kg for overweight, and 11 kg for obese women, respectively [26].

Hypertensive conditions.

The information on diagnoses of hypertensive conditions (i.e., chronic hypertension, gestational hypertension, preeclampsia, and eclampsia) was extracted from birth certificates.

Multiple births.

The information on singleton and multiple births (e.g., twins and triplets) was extracted from birth certificates. Ovulation stimulation is one of the most important reasons for recent rapid increase in rate of multiple births [29]. Thus we further divided multiple births into those with or without ovulation stimulation based on maternal retrospective recall at 9-m postpartum interview.

Measures of outcomes

Our key neurodevelopmental outcome measures included reading, math, gross and fine motor scores at 5 y assessed by certified fieldworkers (Table 2). Neurodevelopmental status assessed around the age for kindergarten has been shown to be strongly related to later school success up to high school [30]. Although longer-term follow-up was preferred, we decided not to use 6-y outcome data, as the 6-y ECLS-B sample was rather small (<20% of full sample) and highly selective [22].

thumbnail
Table 2. Comparison of 5-y neurodevelopmental outcomes between children born SGA and AGA (N = 5300).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160677.t002

Reading and math.

The assessment battery of reading and math in ECLS-B kindergarten wave was a combination of some items fielded as part of the ECLS-B preschool wave and some items used with kindergartners in another cohort, the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten Class of 1998–99 (ECLS-K) battery. Specifically, the 5-y reading assessment was based on the child’s performance on certain language-based items (receptive language/ Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test items) and the literacy items (e.g., conventions of print, letter recognition, understanding of letter-sound relationships, phonological awareness, sight word recognition, understanding words in the context of simple sentences). The 5-y math assessment included number sense, properties, operations, measurement, geometry and spatial sense, data analysis, statistics, probability, patterns, algebra, and functions. For reading and math, overall scale scores based on the full set of assessment items were calculated using item response theory (IRT) procedures [21]. IRT methods make it possible to calculate scores for a given domain that can be compared regardless of which version of the test battery a child received through adaptive testing. The mean±SD of IRT scale scores for reading and math were 38.60±14.84, 40.40±10.56, respectively. The mean±SE of IRT Theta ability estimates for reading and math were 0.33±0.34, 0.38±0.34, respectively. No floor or ceiling effects were found and reliability coefficients of IRT-based score were 0.92 for both the reading and math assessments in the kindergarten 2006 wave of ECLS-B [21,22]. No specific external validation for reading and math items was conducted in ECLS-B kindergarten 2006 sample, as these items had already validated in ECLS-B preschool wave and also ECLS-K kindergarten wave. For example, the ECLS-B preschool field test item pools were validated by concurrent administration of one of the six School Readiness subtests (i.e., colors, letters, numbers/counting, sizes, comparisons, and shapes) of the Bracken Basic Concept Scale-Revised [31]. The correlation of the Bracken Letters subtest with early reading of ECLS-B preschoolers was 0.82, while the correlation of the Bracken Numbers subtest mathematics field test ability estimate of ECLS-B preschoolers was 0.75.

Gross and fine motor.

The gross and fine motor skills were direct assessed using a battery mainly modified from the preschool and/or kindergarten versions of the Early Screening Inventory-Revised (ESI-R) [21], a reliable and valid instrument to evaluate a child’s ability to perform developmentally appropriate tasks [32,33]. Gross motor skills were assessed by certified field workers using 7 physical tasks including balancing (left and right feet), hopping (left and right feet), skipping, walking backwards, and bean bag catching, which were standardized in the Child Assessment Booklet. For the bean bag catching task, if the child caught the bag all the five times, we rated the task as “pass”, otherwise as “fail”. For the other 6 tasks, fieldworkers rated each of them as either pass or fail. We calculated the total number of passed physical tasks as the gross motor score (range, 0–7). Fine motor skill tests included “build a gate” test with blocks and copying 4 forms (square, triangle, asterisk and circle-square) [21]. “Build a gate” task was scored by fieldworker, and the drawing tasks were scored by specially trained coders centrally at RTI International. All items were scored as a “pass” (1 point) or “fail” (0 point) using standardized scoring rules from the ESI-R, kindergarten version. We calculated the total number of passed items and used it as the fine motor score (range, 0–5). The coding of fine motor items was rather reliable, as both inter-rater and standard-comparison agreements were above 85% [21].

Note that child intelligence quotient (IQ) assessment was unavailable in ECLS-B due to the study priority on the learning-related skills (i.e., reading and math) that were more directly related to school readiness, intent to measure cognitive dimensions that were comparable across different assessment waves at different ages (2 y, 4 y, 5 y, and 6 y), as well as several important constraints including short assessment time (<45 minutes), less controlled home settings, and less professional field examiners with only basic knowledge of child development [21].

Measures of mediators

We considered 5- minute Apgar score as a perinatal mediator related to poor neurodevelopment of some SGA etiological subgroups. Although 5- minute Apgar score is not designed to predict long term health and development, it can be a valuable indicator for some important fetal complications such as birth asphyxia and cerebral palsy [16].

Measures of confounders

The confounders in this analysis included family socioeconomic status (SES); maternal age at pregnancy, race/ethnicity, education level, marital status, method of delivery, and diabetes during pregnancy; and the child’s sex. Family SES score was derived from 5 available components including parents’ education, parents’ occupation, and household income [23].

Statistical analysis

In this study, we divided SGA children into etiological subgroup because it can better control for other prenatal risk factors than regular statistical methods such as multivariable regression modeling and thus offer clear separation of different etiologies of SGA. Although the subgroup approach is subject to lower statistical power due to reduced sample size, it fits our study purposes well.

Aim 1—single factor.

We fitted multivariable linear regression models to compare neurodevelopmental outcomes between the etiological SGA subgroup with a specific risk factor and the SGA subgroup without the corresponding risk factor (SGA internal reference), adjusting for confounders (Table 3). All regression models were fitted with generalized estimating equations to control for the correlation between multiple siblings (twins or triplets), by specifying exchangeable covariance matrix among siblings.

thumbnail
Table 3. Neurodevelopmental outcomes at 5 y of SGA etiological subgroups by single prenatal risk factor (N = 1050).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160677.t003

Aim 2- co-occurring factors.

Restricted by sample size, we only considered the co-occurrence of 3 relatively common risk factors (i.e., smoking during pregnancy, inadequate GWG, and multiple births) that were associated or marginally associated with neurodevelopmental outcomes in Aim 1 –single factor analysis (Table 4).

thumbnail
Table 4. 5-y neurodevelopmental outcomes of SGA subgroups by co-occurrence of maternal smoking, inadequate GWG, and multiple births (N = 1050).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160677.t004

Aim 3 –Mediation.

Based on the causal step method for medication analysis [34], we fitted 4 models with the same neurodevelopment outcome variable: the basic model (model 1) included SGA subgroups by the 3 co-occurring factors mentioned above and potential confounders, models 2 included the variables in model 1 and 5-minute Apgar score (S1 Table).

Supplemental analyses.

We also ran several supplemental analyses to assess the robustness of our findings or to gain deeper insights on some research questions. First, in Aim 1, we compared neurodevelopmental outcomes between the etiological SGA subgroup with a specific risk factor to AGA without the corresponding risk factor (normal reference), which could help to assess the departure from normal neurodevelopment of healthy children (S2 Table). Second, we calculated Pearson correlations across reading, math, gross and fine motor scores at 5 y, which could help to assess their internal reliability (S3 Table). Third, in Aim 2, we used the 4-category measure of maternal smoking to compare the 5-y neurodevelopmental outcomes across singleton SGA subgroups by co-occurrence of maternal smoking and inadequate GWG, which could provide insight on potential dose-response relationship (S4 Table). Fourth, in order to explore the potential reasons for the differences in neurodevelopmental outcomes between multiple-birth SGA subgroups with and without ovulation stimulation, we compared their socio-demographics and parenting characteristics (S5 Table).

Results

Sample characteristics

Compared with those in the excluded sample, mothers in the analytic sample were more educated, had higher family SES score, were more likely to have multiple births, and were less likely to smoke during pregnancy and be underweight; children in the analytic sample were less likely to be born preterm (Table 1).

Neurodevelopmental outcomes of SGA

Children born SGA had lower mean reading (confounders-adjusted mean difference, -1.05 [95% confidence interval or CI, -1.93, -0.16]), math (-0.84 [-1.45, -0.22]), gross (-0.22 [-0.34, -0.10]) and fine (-0.22 [-0.32, -0.12]) motor scores than children born AGA (Table 2).

Neurodevelopmental outcomes of SGA etiological subgroups by single factor (Aim 1)

Compared to the SGA subgroup with normal GWG, the SGA subgroup with inadequate GWG had lower mean reading (-2.26 [-3.98, -0.55]), math (-1.54 [-2.74, -0.35]) and fine motor (-0.19 [-0.38, 0.00]) scores (Table 3). Compared with the singleton SGA subgroup, the multiple-birth SGA subgroup had lower mean reading (-4.08 [-6.10, -2.06]) and math (-2.22 [-3.61, -0.84]) scores; these cognitive disadvantages were observed only among those without ovulation stimulation (reading, -4.50 [-6.64, -2.36]; math, -2.91 [-4.37, -1.44]) but not among those with ovulation stimulation (reading, -2.33 [-6.24, 1.57]; math, 0.63 [-1.86, 3.12]).

Neurodevelopmental outcomes of SGA etiological subgroups by co-occurring factors (Aim 2)

We further stratified SGA by the co-occurrence of smoking during pregnancy, inadequate GWG, and multiple births, as they were associated or marginally associated with neurodevelopmental outcomes in single factor analysis shown above. Compared with the reference SGA subgroup without smoking, inadequate GWG, or multiple births, the multiple-birth SGA subgroup without smoking and inadequate GWG had lower mean reading (-3.78 [-6.69, -0.88]) and math (-2.15 [-4.08, -0.21]) score at 5 y (Table 4). The singleton SGA subgroup with both, but not single, of maternal smoking and inadequate GWG had lower mean reading (-4.81 [-8.50, -1.12]) and math (-2.95 [-5.51, -0.38]) scores. The SGA subgroup with co-occurrence of maternal smoking and multiple births had lower mean math (-4.00 [-7.92, -0.07]) and fine motor (-0.68 [-1.28, -0.08]) scores. The SGA subgroup with co-occurrence of maternal inadequate GWG and multiple births (12.9%) had lower mean reading (-6.32 [-9.25, -3.39]) and math (-3.27 [-5.31, -1.22]) scores.

In addition, among singleton SGA children, the co-occurrence of inadequate GWG and moderate smoking during pregnancy (vs. absence of these 2 risk factors) was associated with lower mean reading score (-4.55 [-8.06, -1.03]), while the co-occurrence of inadequate GWG and heavy smoking during pregnancy was associated with lower mean math score (-3.47 [-6.71, -0.22]) (S4 Table).

Mediation analysis (Aim 3)

There was no significant difference in Apgar score across SGA subgroups by co-occurrence of maternal smoking, inadequate GWG, and multiple births (S1 Table). Therefore, Apgar score did not mediate the associations between SGA etiological subgroups and neurodevelopmental outcomes.

Discussion

Within a large US prospective birth cohort, we used the etiological subgroup approach to examine the differences in neurodevelopmental outcomes at 5 y across subgroups of SGA based on each and co-occurrence of several important prenatal risk factors. We also examined the extent to which Apgar score could mediate the associations between SGA etiological subgroups and neurodevelopmental outcomes. Our results suggested that 1) the SGA subgroup with co-occurrence of maternal smoking and inadequate GWG was particularly disadvantaged in reading and math skills at 5 y; 2) the multiple-birth SGA subgroup without ovulation stimulation was at an increased risk of developmental delay in reading at 5 y; 3) these cognitive disadvantages could not be explained by low Apgar score at birth. Our novel findings supported the importance of considering prenatal factors in the understanding of heterogeneity in neurodevelopmental outcomes among SGA children. It is necessary to screen SGA children based on their potential etiology and identify SGA subgroups at the greatest neurodevelopmental risk for referrals to early intervention.

Consistent with the literature [6], we observed that on average children born SGA had worse cognitive and motor outcomes at 5 y than children born AGA, even after adjusting for a series of socio-demographic and pregnancy confounders. SGA is associated with comprised brain development and differentiation due to reduced oxygen and/or nutrient delivery [7].

One of our most important findings was that the singleton SGA subgroup with co-occurrence of maternal smoking during pregnancy and inadequate GWG had worse reading and math outcomes up to 5 y. This cognitive disadvantage seemed not be explained by low Apgar score. Prenatal exposure to tobacco metabolites may alter and damage fetal brain development reflected by small head circumference, lower cortical grey matter and total parenchymal volumes in the offspring [35], and thus increase risk of cognition delay, poor language skills, and behavioral problems in later life [28]. GWG is a proxy for maternal nutritional status or energy balance during pregnancy and placental function [10]. Malnutrition during pregnancy has been linked to fetal brain dysfunctions with widely distributed brain pathology [36]. Smoking pregnant women are at risk of inadequate GWG, possibly due to dietary restrictions related to the anorexic effects of tobacco [37]. The effects of co-occurrence of maternal smoking and inadequate GWG on the developing brain remain largely understudied. Based on the existing evidence on their separate effects, we propose several possible explanations for our novel finding: 1) the co-occurrence of maternal smoking and inadequate GWG acts as “double hits” to the fetal brain and causes more severe impairments than each factor separately. For example, for a pregnant women with inadequate GWG due to low caloric intake, smoking may further interfere with the efficiency of calorie utilization [38,39]; 2) fetal malnutrition due to maternal inadequate GWG may increase vulnerability of fetal brain to smoking-related hypoxia-ischemia and toxic tobacco products such as nicotine and carbon monoxide, and then lead to worse cognitive development; 3) although we adjusted for maternal education and other socio-demographics in this analysis, there must be some residual confounding by genetics [40], poor parenting and disadvantaged family environmental factors that put the child at high risk of cognitive delay.

Our second novel finding was that the multiple-birth SGA subgroup without ovulation stimulation was at an increased risk of developmental delay in reading. This finding is consistent with the literature on neurodevelopmental disadvantages related to multiple births [41]. It is hypothesized that multiple births are often restricted by crowded space and nutrition supply, and more likely to experience some severe fetal complications such as asphyxia and cerebral palsy; thus, have high risk of brain damage [42]. However, in our mediation analysis, the mean difference in reading score between singleton and multiple-birth SGA subgroups seemed not be explained by Apgar score. Alternatively, the disadvantaged postnatal environments such as twins’ competition for parental time and attention may help to explain this [43]. Interestingly, we did not observe adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes in the multiple-birth SGA subgroup with ovulation stimulation. As expected, our supplemental analysis showed that mothers of multiple-birth SGA subgroup with ovulation stimulation had more positive parenting at 2 and 4 y than those of multiple-birth SGA subgroup without ovulation stimulation (S5 Table). Positive parenting may well offset the potential disadvantages related to multiple births. Finally, we could not rule out the possibility that mother of multiple-birth SGA subgroup with ovulation stimulation might have involved genetic screening or selection of healthier embryos, which could be associated with the better neurodevelopmental outcomes.

We noticed that SGA children with co-occurrence of maternal smoking, inadequate GWG, and multiple births did not have statistically significantly worse neurodevelopmental outcomes than those without any of these 3 risk factors. This finding was different from our hypothesis (this subgroup being the worst). Particular caution is needed to interpret this finding, given the very small sample size of this SGA subgroup (2.6% within SGA). It might be related to survival bias of live babies, i.e., SGA newborns who survive in harsh intrauterine environment tend to be healthier than other babies who otherwise die before birth such as miscarriage.

We found SGA children as a whole group had poorer gross and fine motor development than AGA children, which was consistent with findings of some previous studies [44] but not with others [45]. However, we did not observe any substantial difference in motor development across SGA etiological subgroups. This null finding suggests that SGA per se predicts poor motor development in childhood, which does not depend on the potential cause(s) of SGA.

Limitations

First, the attrition rate at 5-y visit was considerably high (~33%) in the ECLS-B sample. Our analytic sample was somewhat different from the excluded sample especially in maternal education, which could introduce selection bias. Second, the low prevalence of alcohol use limited statistical power to compare neurodevelopment across SGA subgroups by these factors. Also, we could not examine SGA subgroup due to some genetic factors such as maternal low birth weight as this information was unavailable in ECLS-B. Third, the relatively low reliability and validity of some non-medical measures on birth certificates needed to be taken in account when interpreting our findings [46]. For example, self-reported maternal smoking and alcohol use during pregnancy on birth certificates were subject to recall bias especially under-report since they are social undesirable behaviors [47]. Some pregnant women might not recall their pre-pregnancy weight accurately [48]. Fourth, we were unable to control for parental neurodevelopmental status such as IQ. But maternal education could, at least partially, reflect the level of maternal IQ. We could not explore other SGA-related perinatal complications as potential mediators due to insufficient power (e.g., meconium aspiration) or unavailable information (e.g., hypoglycemia and polycythemia).

Conclusion

In summary, we found that multiple-birth SGA subgroup or singleton SGA subgroup with co-occurrence of smoking and inadequate GWG were particularly disadvantaged in cognitive development up to 5 y. From a clinical perspective, the effect size of these disadvantages appeared moderate (Cohen’s d<0.4) at individual level, however, a downward shift in mean neurodevelopmental level might result in many children with neurodevelopmental disabilities at population level [49]. If our novel findings can be replicated in other cohorts, further intervention research should assess if quitting smoking or avoiding inadequate GWG among smoking pregnant women could improve offspring’s neurodevelopmental outcomes. Also, we are concerned about the recent rapid increase in the prevalence of multiple births, as it seems to convey some cognitive disadvantages in the offspring.

Supporting Information

S1 Table. Differences in Apgar score across SGA subgroups by co-occurrence of maternal smoking, inadequate GWG, and multiple births (N = 1050).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160677.s001

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Neurodevelopmental outcomes at 5 y of SGA vs AGA subgroups by single prenatal risk factor (N = 5300).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160677.s002

(DOC)

S3 Table. Pearson correlations between neurodevelopmental outcomes at 5 y (N = 5300).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160677.s003

(DOC)

S4 Table. 5-y neurodevelopmental outcomes of singleton SGA subgroups by co-occurrence of maternal smoking and inadequate GWG (N = 700).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160677.s004

(DOCX)

S5 Table. Comparisons of socio-demographics and parenting characteristics between singleton, multiple-birth SGA subgroups with and without ovulation stimulation (N = 1050).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160677.s005

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The authors appreciate the staff in the Institute of Education Sciences (IES) of the United States Department of Education for providing access to restricted-use data of ECLS-B cohort (License No. 12080032) and reviewing the manuscript. Special thanks are also given to the parents, children, and schools for their participation in ECLS-B cohort.

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: XL, RDE, LHE, EDS, CX, XW.
  2. Data curation: XL, XW.
  3. Formal analysis: XL, XW.
  4. Funding acquisition: XW, XL.
  5. Investigation: XL, XW.
  6. Methodology: XL, RDE, LHE, EDS, CX, XW.
  7. Project administration: XW.
  8. Resources: XW.
  9. Software: XW.
  10. Supervision: XW.
  11. Visualization: XL.
  12. Writing - original draft: XL, XW.
  13. Writing - review & editing: XL, RDE, LHE, EDS, CX, XW.

References

  1. 1. Lawn JE, Blencowe H, Oza S, You D, Lee AC, Waiswa P, et al. (2014) Every Newborn: progress, priorities, and potential beyond survival. Lancet 384: 189–205. pmid:24853593
  2. 2. Arcangeli T, Thilaganathan B, Hooper R, Khan KS, Bhide A (2012) Neurodevelopmental delay in small babies at term: a systematic review. Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol 40: 267–275. pmid:22302630
  3. 3. Ota E, Ganchimeg T, Morisaki N, Vogel JP, Pileggi C, Ortiz-Panozo E, et al. (2014) Risk Factors and Adverse Perinatal Outcomes among Term and Preterm Infants Born Small-for-Gestational-Age: Secondary Analyses of the WHO Multi-Country Survey on Maternal and Newborn Health. PLoS One 9: e105155. pmid:25119107
  4. 4. Lee AC, Katz J, Blencowe H, Cousens S, Kozuki N, Vogel JP, et al. (2013) National and regional estimates of term and preterm babies born small for gestational age in 138 low-income and middle-income countries in 2010. Lancet Glob Health 1: e26–36. pmid:25103583
  5. 5. Kramer MS (1987) Intrauterine growth and gestational duration determinants. Pediatrics 80: 502–511. pmid:3658568
  6. 6. O'Keeffe MJ, O'Callaghan M, Williams GM, Najman JM, Bor W (2003) Learning, cognitive, and attentional problems in adolescents born small for gestational age. Pediatrics 112: 301–307. pmid:12897278
  7. 7. de Kieviet JF, Zoetebier L, van Elburg RM, Vermeulen RJ, Oosterlaan J (2012) Brain development of very preterm and very low-birthweight children in childhood and adolescence: a meta-analysis. Dev Med Child Neurol 54: 313–323. pmid:22283622
  8. 8. Marret S (2005) [Effects of maternal smoking during pregnancy on fetal brain development]. J Gynecol Obstet Biol Reprod (Paris) 34 Spec No 1: 3S230–233.
  9. 9. Durazzo TC, Meyerhoff DJ, Nixon SJ (2010) Chronic cigarette smoking: implications for neurocognition and brain neurobiology. Int J Environ Res Public Health 7: 3760–3791. pmid:21139859
  10. 10. Davis RR, Hofferth SL (2012) The association between inadequate gestational weight gain and infant mortality among U.S. infants born in 2002. Matern Child Health J 16: 119–124. pmid:21086155
  11. 11. Flidel-Rimon O, Rhea DJ, Keith LG, Shinwell ES, Blickstein I (2005) Early adequate maternal weight gain is associated with fewer small for gestational age triplets. J Perinat Med 33: 379–382. pmid:16238531
  12. 12. Lung FW, Shu BC, Chiang TL, Lin SJ (2009) Twin-singleton influence on infant development: a national birth cohort study. Child Care Health Dev 35: 409–418. pmid:19397604
  13. 13. Blickstein I (2004) Is it normal for multiples to be smaller than singletons? Best Pract Res Clin Obstet Gynaecol 18: 613–623. pmid:15279820
  14. 14. Wen X, Triche EW, Hogan JW, Shenassa ED, Buka SL (2010) Birth weight and adult hypercholesterolemia: subgroups of small-for-gestational-age based on maternal smoking status during pregnancy. Epidemiology 21: 786–790. pmid:20798636
  15. 15. Cnattingius S, Haglund B, Kramer MS (1998) Differences in late fetal death rates in association with determinants of small for gestational age fetuses: population based cohort study. BMJ 316: 1483–1487. pmid:9582131
  16. 16. Casey BM, McIntire DD, Leveno KJ (2001) The continuing value of the Apgar score for the assessment of newborn infants. N Engl J Med 344: 467–471. pmid:11172187
  17. 17. Nelson KB, Ellenberg JH (1981) Apgar scores as predictors of chronic neurologic disability. Pediatrics 68: 36–44. pmid:7243507
  18. 18. Berlin LJ, Brooks-Gunn J, McCarton C, McCormick MC (1998) The effectiveness of early intervention: examining risk factors and pathways to enhanced development. Prev Med 27: 238–245. pmid:9579002
  19. 19. Reynolds AJ, Temple JA, Robertson DL, Mann EA (2001) Long-term effects of an early childhood intervention on educational achievement and juvenile arrest: A 15-year follow-up of low-income children in public schools. Jama 285: 2339–2346. pmid:11343481
  20. 20. Dearing E, McCartney K, Taylor BA (2009) Does higher quality early child care promote low-income children's math and reading achievement in middle childhood? Child Dev 80: 1329–1349. pmid:19765003
  21. 21. Najarian M, Snow K, Lennon J, Kinsey S (2010) Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS), Preschool-Kindergarten 2007 Psychometric Report (NCES 2010–009). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics, Institute for Education Sciences, US Dept of Education.
  22. 22. Snow K, Derecho A, Wheeless S, Lennon J, Rosen J, Rogers J, et al. (2009) Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS-B), Kindergarten 2006 and 2007, Data File User's Manual (2010–010). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, US Department of Education.
  23. 23. Nord C, Edwards B, Hilpert R, Branden L, Andreassen C, Elmore A, et al. (2004) Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS-B), User’s Manual for the ECLS-B Nine-Month Restricted-Use Data File and Electronic Code Book (NCES 2004–092). National Center for Education Statistics.
  24. 24. Reichman NE, Hade EM (2001) Validation of birth certificate data. A study of women in New Jersey's HealthStart program. Ann Epidemiol 11: 186–193. pmid:11248582
  25. 25. Oken E, Kleinman KP, Rich-Edwards J, Gillman MW (2003) A nearly continuous measure of birth weight for gestational age using a United States national reference. BMC Pediatr 3: 6. pmid:12848901
  26. 26. Institute of Medicine and National Research Council (2009) Weight Gain During Pregnancy: Reexamining the Guidelines. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
  27. 27. Kramer MS, Morin I, Yang H, Platt RW, Usher R, McNamara H, et al. (2002) Why are babies getting bigger? Temporal trends in fetal growth and its determinants. J Pediatr 141: 538–542. pmid:12378194
  28. 28. Boutwell BB, Beaver KM (2010) Maternal cigarette smoking during pregnancy and offspring externalizing behavioral problems: a propensity score matching analysis. Int J Environ Res Public Health 7: 146–163. pmid:20195438
  29. 29. Collins J (2007) Global epidemiology of multiple birth. Reprod Biomed Online 15 Suppl 3: 45–52.
  30. 30. Ahnert J, Schneider W, Bös K (2009) Developmental changes and individual stability of motor abilities from the preschool period to young adulthood. Human development from early childhood to early adulthood: Evidence from the Munich Longitudinal Study on the Genesis of Individual Competencies (LOGIC): 35–62.
  31. 31. Bracken B (1998) Bracken basic concept scale-revised. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace and Company.
  32. 32. Tourangeau K, Burke J, Le T, Wan S, Weant M, Brown E, et al. (2001) ECLS-K Base Year Public-use Data Files and Electronic Codebook: User’s Manual. NCES.
  33. 33. Meisels SJ, Henderson LW, Liaw F-r, Browning K, Have TT (1993) New evidence for the effectiveness of the early screening inventory. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 8: 327–346.
  34. 34. Baron RM, Kenny DA (1986) The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 51: 1173–1182. pmid:3806354
  35. 35. Rivkin MJ, Davis PE, Lemaster JL, Cabral HJ, Warfield SK, Mulkern RV, et al. (2008) Volumetric MRI study of brain in children with intrauterine exposure to cocaine, alcohol, tobacco, and marijuana. Pediatrics 121: 741–750. pmid:18381539
  36. 36. Guerrini I, Thomson AD, Gurling HD (2007) The importance of alcohol misuse, malnutrition and genetic susceptibility on brain growth and plasticity. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 31: 212–220. pmid:16908066
  37. 37. Levine MD, Cheng Y, Cluss PA, Marcus MD, Kalarchian MA (2013) Prenatal smoking cessation intervention and gestational weight gain. Womens Health Issues 23: e389–393. pmid:24183413
  38. 38. Picone TA, Allen LH, Olsen PN, Ferris ME (1982) Pregnancy outcome in North American women. II. Effects of diet, cigarette smoking, stress, and weight gain on placentas, and on neonatal physical and behavioral characteristics. Am J Clin Nutr 36: 1214–1224. pmid:7148740
  39. 39. Picone TA, Allen LH, Schramm MM, Olsen PN (1982) Pregnancy outcome in North American women. I. Effects of diet, cigarette smoking, and psychological stress on maternal weight gain. Am J Clin Nutr 36: 1205–1213. pmid:7148739
  40. 40. D'Onofrio BM, Singh AL, Iliadou A, Lambe M, Hultman CM, Neiderhiser JM, et al. (2010) A quasi-experimental study of maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring academic achievement. Child Dev 81: 80–100. pmid:20331655
  41. 41. Sutcliffe AG, Derom C (2006) Follow-up of twins: health, behaviour, speech, language outcomes and implications for parents. Early Hum Dev 82: 379–386. pmid:16690232
  42. 42. Rutter M, Thorpe K, Greenwood R, Northstone K, Golding J (2003) Twins as a natural experiment to study the causes of mild language delay: I: Design; twin-singleton differences in language, and obstetric risks. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 44: 326–341. pmid:12635964
  43. 43. Thorpe K, Rutter M, Greenwood R (2003) Twins as a natural experiment to study the causes of mild language delay: II: Family interaction risk factors. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 44: 342–355. pmid:12635965
  44. 44. Sommerfelt K, Sonnander K, Skranes J, Andersson HW, Ahlsten G, Ellertsen B, et al. (2002) Neuropsychologic and motor function in small-for-gestation preschoolers. Pediatr Neurol 26: 186–191. pmid:11955924
  45. 45. Pinello L, Manea S, Visona Dalla Pozza L, Mazzarolo M, Facchin P (2013) Visual, motor, and psychomotor development in small-for-gestational-age preterm infants. J aapos 17: 352–356. pmid:23993714
  46. 46. Northam S, Knapp TR (2006) The reliability and validity of birth certificates. J Obstet Gynecol Neonatal Nurs 35: 3–12. pmid:16466348
  47. 47. Pickett KE, Kasza K, Biesecker G, Wright RJ, Wakschlag LS (2009) Women who remember, women who do not: a methodological study of maternal recall of smoking in pregnancy. Nicotine Tob Res 11: 1166–1174. pmid:19640836
  48. 48. Russell A, Gillespie S, Satya S, Gaudet LM (2013) Assessing the accuracy of pregnant women in recalling pre-pregnancy weight and gestational weight gain. J Obstet Gynaecol Can 35: 802–809. pmid:24099445
  49. 49. Rohlman DS, Lucchini R, Anger WK, Bellinger DC, van Thriel C (2008) Neurobehavioral testing in human risk assessment. Neurotoxicology 29: 556–567. pmid:18539229