Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Getting to the Heart of Emotion Regulation in Youth: The Role of Interoceptive Sensitivity, Heart Rate Variability, and Parental Psychopathology

Abstract

Emotion regulation and associated autonomic activation develop throughout childhood and adolescence under the influence of the family environment. Specifically, physiological indicators of autonomic nervous system activity such as interoceptive sensitivity and vagally mediated heart rate variability (HRV) can inform on emotion regulation. Although the effect of parental emotion socialization on emotion regulation appears to be influenced by autonomic processes, research on physiological regulation and the influence of parental factors remains scarce. This study investigated the relationship between self-reported habitual emotion regulation strategies and HRV at rest as well as interoceptive sensitivity in forty-six youngsters (27 female; age: M = 13.00, SD = 2.13). Secondly, the association between these autonomic correlates and parental psychopathology was also studied. Whereas better interoceptive sensitivity was related to reduced maladaptive emotion regulation, specifically rumination, high HRV was related to more use of external emotion regulation strategies (i.e., support seeking). In addition, increased HRV and decreased interoceptive sensitivity were associated with maternal internalizing and there was evidence for a possible mediation effect of HRV in the relationship between maternal internalizing and child external emotion regulation. This study elucidates the link between cognitive emotion regulation strategies and underlying physiological regulation in adolescents but also indicates a putative influence of maternal internalizing symptoms on emotion regulation in their offspring.

Introduction

During late childhood and adolescence, emotion processing abilities are continuously developing in interaction with the family context [1]. Parents strongly shape emotional experience and emotion regulation (ER) in their children, but, interestingly, the effect of parental emotion socialization on ER appears to be partially mediated by autonomic processes [2]. How parental influences interact with a maturing autonomic nervous system is however still under study [3]. Previous studies already found indications that the development of emotional regulation during adolescence is associated with changes in autonomic activation [3, 4] and that such associations might be predictive of well-being and development of psychopathology including anxiety disorders and depression [5]. However, research on autonomic processes related to emotional regulation in youth remains limited and the precise role of parental factors in child autonomic nervous system activity and conscious awareness of this activity is insufficiently researched.

There is a long history of theories attributing importance to the role of the autonomic nervous system and bodily arousal in the experience of emotional states [610]. Bottom-up processes such as central nervous system processing of peripheral physiological reactions to emotional stimuli influence the subjective experience of emotional intensity [10]. However, individuals actively engage with the environment and apply regulatory control to an individually different extent, which makes both bottom-up and top-down processes key for emotional experience and regulation. Emotion regulation (ER) can be defined as “the extrinsic and intrinsic processes responsible for monitoring, evaluating, and modifying emotional reactions, especially their intensive and temporal features, to accomplish one’s goals” ([11]; pp 27). While the adaptive nature of a specific strategy also depends on situational factors and environmental demands [12], the large spectrum of ER strategies can generally be categorized into adaptive, maladaptive, and external ER. A well-known example of an adaptive strategy is cognitive reappraisal, the cognitive reinterpretation of situations to change the emotional response to them [13]. Rumination on the other hand is an example of a maladaptive strategy and is characterized by a repetitive and passive focus on symptoms of distress and possible causes and consequences [14]. Finally, interpersonal strategies such as support seeking [3] are termed external ER strategies. There is currently no consensus on whether external strategies are beneficial or detrimental to wellbeing [15]. A meta-analytic review showed that although ER is far less studied in youth than adulthood and youngsters might be less able to use more demanding strategies such as problem solving, children do already use both adaptive and maladaptive strategies to manage their emotions [16]. However, the psychophysiological correlates of these ER strategies in youth have not yet been fully characterized.

Top-down regulation of emotional states has often been linked to a specific autonomic component, i.e. vagally mediated intrinsic variability in heart rate, by both theory and research. Vagally mediated heart rate variability (HRV) refers to the beat-to-beat variability in heart rate and is controlled by the parasympathetic nervous system through the vagus nerve. It is an index of healthy functioning and flexibility to adapt to complex environmental demands [17]. The neurovisceral integration model states that vagally mediated and prefrontally modulated cortico-cardiac interaction at resting state can be considered a proxy for prefrontal inhibitory capacity and hence for ER capacity [17, 18]. In adults, higher vagally mediated resting HRV corresponds to higher ability to react and return to homeostasis (i.e. regulate) [19] and is related to lower levels of affective instability [20], greater (increase in) behavioral regulatory control [21, 22], improved cognitive control over unwanted memories [23], better fear extinction [24], and less difficulties in self-reported ER [25]. By contrast, research in children is more limited and to date inconsistent. While Vasilev et al. [5] showed that developmental changes in resting HRV were related to self-reported ER abilities in children and two other studies showed that higher resting HRV is associated with better effortful control and increased perceived control over anxiety [26, 27], Gentzler et al. [28] did not find an association between baseline HRV and adaptive or maladaptive ER in youth. Interestingly, a second theory, the polyvagal perspective, supports the supposition that vagally mediated HRV is related to regulation, but additionally proposes that it is associated with social behavior. The polyvagal theory states that the vagus nerve is involved in the inhibition of primitive neural fight and flight mechanisms and the promotion of social behavior [9, 29]. Therefore, increased vagus control is associated with higher HRV as well as emotion expression, social competence and active engagement with the environment [30, 31]. Again, some studies in adults have already supported the relationship between HRV and social support seeking [29, 32], however no studies on the association between resting HRV and external ER in youngsters could be found. In conclusion, while HRV has been reliably shown to be associated with (interpersonal) self-regulation by both theory and empirical research in adults, this association requires further validation in adolescents.

As opposed to top-down emotional control processes, the psychophysiological correlates of bottom-up processes are often disregarded in the ER literature. However, these bottom-up emotional processes have also been linked to a specific physiological process, i.e. interoceptive sensitivity (IS), by the somatic marker hypothesis [10]. The somatic marker hypothesis states that the subjective emotional experience is shaped by bodily conditions [33]. Sensitivity towards these bodily conditions and more specifically, towards changes in one’s physiological state as represented by IS to one’s heartbeat, has been shown to moderate the effect of bodily responses on emotional arousal [34, 35]. This is further related to individual differences in subjectively experienced emotional intensity [36, 37]. Research in adults indicates that high IS is linked to greater emotional arousal [38, 39], less alexithymia [40], and, interestingly, also better downregulation of negative affect via cognitive reappraisal [41]. Research on the associations between IS and emotional states in children is limited. One available study in primary school children showed that IS was significantly associated with interpersonal emotional intelligence and adaptability (i.e. ability for behavioral adjustment in a changing environment; [42]). Although initially IS was conceptualized as being involved in bottom-up emotion experience, other available evidence links it to regulation as well [41]. Higher IS could be related to more precise bodily feedback, i.e., better information and central representation of the visceral states caused by emotions, and therefore regulation might be applied more effectively. While no studies investigated the relationship between parental factors and child IS, the biological sensitivity to context theory suggests that individual differences in child psychophysiology emerge from, and constantly interact with, the family environment to predict psychiatric and biomedical outcomes [12, 43].

The family environment might not only create a context which puts children at increased risk for maladaptive outcomes [44], but parental factors might also interact with psychophysiological processes such as HRV in the offspring. While high vagal control is generally considered to be related to positive outcomes such as ER capacity and regulatory control [17, 18], it has also been reported to exacerbate the negative effect of maternal depressive symptoms on child ER [45]. Furthermore, direct effects of parental psychopathology suggest that offspring of patients with an anxiety disorder or depression have decreased cardiac vagal function [46, 47]. However, these studies all recruited participants in early to mid-childhood and age has proven to be an important determinant of HRV. Evidence suggests that individuals may be more likely to show HRV differences in certain developmental periods critical for the augmentation of the autonomic nervous system or self-regulatory skills [48]. In early childhood, at-risk children with a parental history of mood disorders and low-risk peers show a similar level of HRV functioning. By comparison, in late childhood and adolescence, sensitivity to environmental influences of stress and challenges to self-regulatory skills increase and this is reflected in higher inter-individual variability in HRV between high-risk and low-risk children [48]. The relationship between individual differences in parents and adolescent psychophysiology and ER is complex and insufficiently addressed in previous research. Furthermore, the precise roles of HRV and IS in ER remain unclear.

Therefore, this study had three goals. First, we aimed to examine the link between HRV and IS to ER strategies (adaptive, maladaptive, external) in late childhood and adolescence. Second, to investigate the influence of parental factors on physiological reactivity in their offspring in late childhood and adolescence. Third, to explore whether autonomic responses could mediate the relationship between parental internalizing (i.e., anxiety and depressive) symptoms and child ER. Based on recent evidence, we anticipated that better resting HRV (e.g., [21]) and IS [41] would be associated with better regulatory control and adaptive ER and, in case of HRV, also with increased use of external ER (e.g., [29]). Second, limited previous evidence suggests that anxiety disorders in the parents are correlated with HRV in the child [46]. Based on this prior study, one would expect that increased parental internalizing psychopathology is associated with decreased HRV in the child. Furthermore, we would like to investigate the relationship between parental internalizing psychopathology and child IS.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Forty-six healthy youngsters ages 9 to 16 years (22 female; Age: M = 13.00, SD = 2.13) participated in this study. Initially, 50 youngsters (mostly of Caucasian ethnicity from a middle-class background) were recruited from the extended urban area of Ghent (Belgium) by advertisement in local schools and youth organizations. These participants were invited for participation by telephone and all participants as well as their parents filled out informed consent forms (parents) and assent forms (youths). All participants received a fee for expense reimbursement (€ 20). Ethical approval was obtained from the ethical committee of Ghent University Hospital. Exclusion criteria consisted of an internalizing DSM-5 disorder or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and severe medical or neurological illnesses. Four participants had to be excluded due to the presence of a DSM-5 disorder (n = 3) or a neurological illness (n = 1). To detect the presence of DSM disorders the Dutch Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV, Child Edition (KID-SCID [49, 50]) was conducted by a psychologist or psychology student trained in conducting the KID-SCID. Since this interview was constructed to assess DSM-IV criteria, several questions were added to ensure coverage of all DSM-5 criteria. None of the mothers were confronted with current or past psychiatric disorders, one father suffered from bipolar disorder.

Psychophysiological measures

Heart rate and resting heart rate variability.

The ECG was collected at a sampling rate of 1000 Hz with de ECG100C module of the Biopac MP150 acquisition system and electrodes (small stress test electrode EL501) were positioned in a modified Einthoven lead II configuration (Biopac systems, Goleta, CA, United States of America). One electrode was placed just below the right clavicle, the second on the left lower torso, and the ground electrode was placed on the right lower torso. Heart rate was recorded during 8 consecutive minutes while watching a segment of an animated movie (Wall-E) that contained very few emotionally salient events or speech. To increase reliability (related to settling into the task, or activity of the experimenter) of these 8 minutes, the first 150 and last 30 seconds were excluded to get a reliable measure of 5 consecutive minutes. In case there were large artifacts (in the form of irregularities or movement artifacts that could not be corrected by hand) in the middle 5 minutes, the entire 8 minutes were inspected and the 5 minutes of the highest quality (i.e. the smallest amount of measurement artifacts) were selected. Inter beat intervals and HRV were calculated using ARTiiFACT software [51]. We analyzed the two most frequently used HRV indices of the time- and frequency domain, respectively the root mean square of successive differences (RMSSD) and the high frequency component (HF; Fast-Fourier-Transformation, bandwidth 0.15 to 0.4 Hz) [52].

Interoceptive sensitivity.

Previous research has already shown that the heartbeat detection task (used to assess IS) can capture individual differences in children even though children do experience more difficulty in executing and/or feeling their heartbeat, as shown by lower mean IS scores relative to adults [42]. In this task, participants were instructed to count their heartbeats (without taking their pulse) in the interval between two auditory signals. To account for (and rule out) counting ability as a potential confounding factor in our young sample, participants were instructed to count the number of heartbeats in an auditory sample of a heart beating (not their own) before doing the actual IS task. In addition, to make the task more engaging, participants could listen to their own heartbeat through a stethoscope before commencing the task. Participants completed one practice trial of 20 seconds followed by 6 experimental trials of 25, 35, and 45 seconds (precise order: 25, 35, 45, 35, 45, 25). Each trial was followed by two questions: ‘How many heartbeats did you count?’ and ‘How sure are you on a scale of 1 (not sure at all) to 9 (very sure)?’. The second question explores the metacognitive belief in ones interoceptive abilities (also referred to as interoceptive awareness [53]). The number of actual heartbeats in each trial was recorded with ECG (see section 2.2.1.). The IS score was calculated as follows, with a higher score indicating better performance [42]:

Questionnaires

Emotion regulation.

To assess ER strategies the FEEL-KJ was used [15, 54]. The validity of this questionnaire has already been proven by previous studies [55, 56]. The Dutch version of the FEEL-KJ contains 90 items that assess to what extent children and adolescents habitually use adaptive, maladaptive, and external ER strategies [15]. The scale of adaptive strategies contains the subscales problem-oriented action, cognitive problem-solving, acceptance, forgetting, distraction, reevaluation, and evoking positive mood. Maladaptive strategies are represented by giving up, aggression, withdrawal, self-devaluation, and rumination. Finally, external ER consists of interpersonal strategies to regulate emotions and is represented by the subscales social support seeking, expression, and emotional control. While the subscale expression refers to openly displaying how you feel, emotional control concerns to what extent emotions are being concealed from others and is consequently reverse scored. Each ER strategy is represented by two items that are repeated in three different emotion categories (angry, scared, and sad) and participants are asked to rate the frequency of use of each item on a 5-point scale. The FEEL-KJ questionnaire has a satisfactory overall validity and reliability (Cronbach’s alpha of 0.86 for the entire questionnaire in this sample).

Regular exercise and pubertal development.

Since both HRV and IS could be influenced by the level of physical fitness [52, 57], participants were asked to report whether they regularly exercise (yes or no) and list sportive activities. In addition, previous research has shown that pubertal development has an impact on cardiac contractility [58] and HRV [59, 60]. Furthermore, the influence of pubertal development on HRV should be considered, certainly in the context of parental psychopathology [48]. Therefore, a self-rated version (consisting of 9 items for girls and 8 items for boys) of the pubertal development scale (PDS [61]) was used. The reliability of this scale was adequate (Cronbach’s alpha in this sample of .83 and .75, for boys and girls, respectively) and previous research had already shown that the PDS is a good scale for the assessment of physical maturity [62].

Parental psychopathology.

A Dutch translation of the 14-item Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS; [63, 64]) was used to measure symptoms of anxiety and depression in the parents. Although the Dutch version of this scale has been validated in previous research, a better balance between sensitivity and positive predictive value has been found in the total HADS score than in the two factor solution (based on 7 items each [64]). Therefore, a total internalizing sum score was calculated (Cronbach’s alpha of .84 in this sample).

Procedure

Before testing, children and adolescents completed the FEEL-KJ and filled out the two questions regarding physical exercise and sports. Similarly, both parents were asked to fill out the HADS and the questions regarding psychiatric disorders before coming to the laboratory. All questionnaires were completed on a secure online platform hosted by the Department of Developmental, Personality and Social Psychology of Ghent University. On the day of testing, participants started with participating in the DSM-5 adapted KID-SCID interview. Subsequently, they were weighed and measured and prepared for the physiological recordings. After recording ECG at rest, the PDS was completed. Subsequently, participants were asked whether they had ever heard their heartbeat and given the chance to listen to their heart with a stethoscope, right before they executed the heart beat detection task. The study protocol also included an ER training and an experimental Physiological Indicators of Emotion Regulation (PIER) task that were collected after the present data. However, given that the training and task investigated a different research question, they will be reported elsewhere.

Data analysis

Primary analysis: correlations of emotion regulation abilities with psychophysiology.

To investigate the relationship between the physiological correlates (HRV and IS) and ER as well as parental psychopathology, partial correlation tests were calculated (alpha = .05, two-tailed) with the SPSS software package (version 20, IBM, Chicago, IL, USA). Both HRV measures violated the assumption of normality and were log-transformed (log10) to achieve normal distribution. These log-transformed scores were used in all further analyses. There were some missing values in IS score (n = 2, 2 female), PDS score (n = 1, male), regular exercise score (n = 1, female) and HADS internalizing in the mother (n = 1, male) and father (n = 4, 2 female) due to equipment failure, non-compliance, or unavailability of parents. Data was missing completely at random and therefore we imputed the missing values through expectation maximization (by use of the SPSS missing values analysis module) to provide unbiased parameter estimates and improve the statistical power of the analyses. In accordance with previous research, gender could confound results for both IS (r(46) = .33, p = .02; higher IS in males vs. females) and HRV (RMSSD: r(46) = .27, p = .07; higher HRV in males vs. females) [42, 65], regular exercise could additionally distort HRV outcomes (RMSSD: r(45) = .30, p = .05; higher HRV in high vs. low regular exercise) [66], and body mass index (BMI) is a possible confound for IS (r(46) = -.29, p = .05; higher IS in low vs. high BMI) [67]. Consequently, these factors were included as potential confounders in the partial correlations. Furthermore, since there was a large age range in the sample and consequently varying levels of pubertal development, all following analyses were additionally controlled for PDS score.

Exploratory analysis: mediation model.

Although mediation effects were not initially hypothesized, the primary data analyses gave rise to some interesting research questions that deserved further investigation through mediation analyses. Therefore, to further interrogate our data and investigate how parental internalizing could have indirectly influenced child ER, exploratory mediation models were performed using the PROCESS 2.15 macro [68] in SPSS. For significance testing the bootstrap method (with 5000 samples) was preferred over the Sobel test since the latter is only suited for use in large samples and has lower power [6971]. Similar to the partial correlations, gender and PDS score were always entered as covariate in the mediation analyses, while regular exercise was added only in the case of HRV and BMI was only added for IS.

Results

Interoceptive sensitivity, resting HRV, and emotion regulation

Table 1 provides an overview of the demographics of the current sample. RMSSD (log10) and HF (log10) were significantly positively associated with external ER but not adaptive or maladaptive ER (Table 2). This correlation indicated that higher resting HRV (both RMSSD and HF) was associated with increased use of external ER strategies. By comparison, IS was significantly negatively correlated with maladaptive ER but not adaptive or external ER (Table 2). Better IS was associated with less use of maladaptive ER. Interestingly, the participant’s belief about interoceptive abilities was not correlated with IS nor adaptive, maladaptive, or external ER. Of note, IS and resting HRV were not correlated.

thumbnail
Table 2. Partial correlations between interoceptive sensitivity and heart rate variability and ER strategies and parental internalizing.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0164615.t002

Subtypes of emotion regulation

To assess from which specific subscales of ER strategies the significant correlations emerged, we conducted further analyses. These analyses indicated that the correlation between high HRV and external ER was mainly driven by high support seeking (Table 3). When looking at the correlation between maladaptive ER and IS, this correlation was driven by rumination and self-devaluation (Table 4), where higher IS was associated with decreased rumination and self-devaluation.

thumbnail
Table 3. Follow-up correlations between heart rate variability and the subscales of external emotion regulation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0164615.t003

thumbnail
Table 4. Follow-up correlations between interoceptive sensitivity and the subscales of maladaptive emotion regulation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0164615.t004

Parental psychopathology and child physiology

Correlations between internalizing scores of parents and physiological indices of the child revealed that maternal internalizing symptoms were significantly related to child RMSSD, HF, and IS, while paternal internalizing symptoms were not (Table 2). Specifically, high internalizing in the mother was associated with higher HRV and lower IS (Table 2, Fig 1). Maternal internalizing was significantly directly negatively correlated with adaptive ER (rpartial(43) = -.31, p = .04) and positively with maladaptive ER (rpartial(43) = .30, p < .05) in the child, but not external ER (rpartial(43) = -.04, p = .81). These correlations indicate that anxiety and depressive symptoms in the mother were associated with lower habitual use of adaptive and higher use of maladaptive ER strategies in the offspring. There were no significant correlations between paternal internalizing symptoms and adaptive (rpartial(43) = .002, p = 1.00), maladaptive (rpartial(43) = .26, p = .08), or external (rpartial(43) = -.07, p = .63) ER in the child.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Scatterplots depicting the significant correlations between parental internalizing symptoms and interoceptive sensitivity and heart rate variability (high frequency component) in the child.

While maternal internalizing symptoms were correlated with child autonomic components (left panes), this effect was absent for paternal internalizing symptoms (right panes). Bivariate correlations are shown for the purpose of clear visualization and interpretation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0164615.g001

Exploratory mediation analyses

Based on the correlations mentioned above, there are three mediation models that deserve investigation. Firstly, maternal internalizing could impact HRV which in turn could influence external ER (Fig 2). There was evidence for an indirect effect of maternal internalizing on external ER through high frequency HRV (log10) (b = 0.25, 95% bootstrap CI: 0.01–0.57). The completely standardized indirect effect was 0.14 (95% bootstrap CI: 0.01–0.31). There was also an indirect effect of maternal internalizing measure on external ER through RMSSD (log10) (b = 0.27, 95% bootstrap CI: 0.03–0.58. The completely standardized indirect effect was 0.15 (95% bootstrap CI: 0.01–0.31). Finally, maternal internalizing could have an impact on maladaptive ER through IS. However, mediation analysis showed that there was no indirect effect (b = 0.23, 95% bootstrap CI: -0.03–0.71). The completely standardized indirect effect was 0.10 (95% bootstrap CI: -0.01–0.30).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Visual representation of the significant indirect effect of maternal internalizing on external emotion regulation in the child through both measures of heart rate variability (the high frequency component and RMSSD).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0164615.g002

Discussion

Because research on autonomic processes related to emotional processing in youth is still in its infancy, the present study had three main goals. First, it aimed to examine the relationship between autonomic correlates (HRV at rest and IS) and habitual ER use in children and adolescents. Second, it investigated the relationship between parental internalizing psychopathology and the autonomic correlates of ER in their offspring. Finally, exploratory mediation analysis was performed to study possible indirect effects. With regards to the first goal, higher HRV was associated with increased use of external ER, and support seeking in particular. By contrast, higher IS was related to decreased use of maladaptive ER, specifically rumination and self-devaluation. As for the second goal, maternal but not paternal internalizing symptoms were related to increased HRV and decreased IS. Thirdly, we did find evidence for a possible mediation effect of HRV in the relationship between maternal internalizing and child external ER. These results highlight the importance of autonomic processes in the context of ER in youth.

According to the polyvagal theory, high vagal control of autonomic nervous system activity is associated with emotion experience, top-down regulation, and social behavior [9]. Consistent with this theory, HRV was positively associated with external ER, such as support seeking. This finding is also consistent with work in adults that supports the association between vagally mediated HRV at rest and prosocial behavior such as engagement coping and social wellbeing [29]. The present findings are consistent with this perspective and indicate that higher resting HRV in youth is associated with increased habitual use of strategies that require a social context, i.e., external ER. Given the continuous development of ER in adolescents, interpersonal regulation might not only aid in momentary emotion experience, but also allow adolescents to learn from the environment and develop a broader repertoire of ER skills for future purposes. Nevertheless, HRV did not correlate with adaptive ER, a finding which is in line with Gentzler et al. [28] but contrary to what other previous research has suggested [5, 21]. A possible cause of this inconsistency in the literature lies in the different interpretations of the role of resting HRV in ER. While in some studies HRV was related to adaptive ER specifically (e.g., [21]), others suggested that resting HRV is involved in the flexibility of using different ER strategies [72], while yet others propose that high resting HRV could facilitate the acquisition of different adaptive ER strategies in children [45]. While the perspective of Blandon et al. [45] is most in line with our findings regarding prosocial behavior, we cannot exclude nor confirm these perspectives since the FEEL-KJ only assesses habitual ER strategy use and not flexibility in ER use, acquisition of ER, or ER efficacy. A second possible explanation for the discrepancy between current and previous research can be found in previous limitations to focus on a limited selection of ER strategies/skills, whereas the current study registered routine use of a wider range of different ER strategies in daily life.

Similarly to HRV, IS also correlated with a specific category of ER strategies. Increased IS was significantly associated with decreased use of maladaptive ER strategies, particularly rumination. This association seems to be independent from the metacognitive belief of one’s interoceptive skills, which did not correlate with IS nor ER. Such a result is consistent with previous literature considering interoceptive sensitivity and awareness to be two dissociable constructs [73, 74]. Currently, the link between IS and (maladaptive) ER has rarely been investigated, however, there is evidence supporting specifically low IS as a vulnerability factor for adult psychopathology known to be characterized by maladaptive ER [16], such as depression [75], personality disorders [76], and possibly anxiety disorders [77]. The present association of better IS with lower use of self-devaluation and rumination suggests that increased accuracy for emotional states might play a role in the prevention of a repetitive negative thinking style, at least in healthy adolescents. However, given the scarcity of available literature, the relationship between IS and maladaptive ER requires further examination preferably in both clinical and non-clinical groups across various ages. Taken together, both higher IS and HRV contribute to healthy ER in a different way. Better bottom-up emotional awareness, in the form of IS, is associated with decreased use of maladaptive ER, while high top-down vagally mediated HRV contributes to interpersonal regulation.

The second aim concerned identifying the role of parental psychopathology in psychophysiological correlates of ER. While some evidence supports the importance of parental (mainly maternal) factors in child ER [2, 78], research on the effects of maternal and paternal psychopathology on the physiological components of ER in adolescence is rare. In the present study, we found an association between maternal, but not paternal, internalizing symptoms and child psychophysiology. Specifically, high maternal internalizing problems in healthy mothers were associated with higher resting HRV in youngsters. Although this finding is opposite to Srinivasan et al. [46], who reported decreased cardiac vagal function in children of mothers and fathers with panic disorder, a possible explanation for this discrepancy lies in the sample used. The current study, as opposed to the study by Srinivasan and colleagues [46], did not select parents based on prior psychiatric diagnosis. In fact, no mothers in the present study reported current or past psychiatric disorders. This suggests that protective factors, such as high HRV [79] or increased interpersonal regulation [80], could reduce disease burden in these mothers. These protective factors could in turn be passed on to the children. This hypothesis is supported by the additional, exploratory mediation analyses. These analyses indeed suggested that the effect of maternal internalizing on external ER could be mediated by HRV measures. In conclusion, healthy mothers with internalizing symptoms appear to have children with higher HRV, which in turn fosters strategies that promote adaptive functioning in the children.

In contrast to HRV, in this study, maternal internalizing symptoms were associated with lower IS in children. Previous studies have already shown decreased IS to be related to internalizing disorders in adults [75, 77]. Perhaps these mothers with internalizing symptoms might have compromised IS themselves and consequently transmit this vulnerability factor to their children, hereby increasing their offspring’s risk for developing maladaptive ER (and possibly psychopathology). However, the additional, exploratory mediation analysis does not support an indirect effect of maternal internalizing on maladaptive ER through IS. Similarly to HRV, paternal internalizing symptoms do not influence IS. We could speculate that since mothers are often the primary caregiver in the family, they could have a larger impact on child behavior. However, future research is needed to investigate which factors underlie this gender difference. Furthermore, while the means by which decreased IS skills are conveyed from mother to child remain unknown, it is known that HRV is influenced both by genetic and environmental factors [81, 82]. We speculate that next to genetic transmission problematic parent-child interactions and heightened family stress associated with parental psychopathology could result in general dysregulation and related psychophysiological response patterns in children [82]. However, this study does not allow us to specify to what extent parental influences are transmitted through genetics or such environmental processes. Further research investigating possible mediation effects of parenting factors or genetics in the relationship between parental psychopathology and IS and HRV is needed. In conclusion, mothers with internalizing symptoms appear to have children with low interoceptive skills and these children appear to make more use of maladaptive ER strategies. However, at the same time these mothers provide their children with (potentially) compensatory tools (i.e. HRV) that promote ER and healthy interactions with the environment. However, mediation analyses in larger samples need to confirm these results.

Some limitations require discussion. Contrary to resting HRV protocols in adults, in which participants are asked to sit still, relax, and focus on fixation point, a movie clip was shown during resting HRV to elicit stronger task compliance and avoid potential confounds related to movement in the youngsters. Although we cannot exclude that some emotional response was evoked, the chosen clip was low in emotional arousal and included very few speech components. Moreover, previous authors have also opted for using neutral movie clips to assess baseline reactivity in children (e.g., [45, 8385]). Likewise, to increase engagement and promote compliance for the IS task, children had the opportunity to listen to their heart with a stethoscope immediately prior to the task. However, this could increase insight into their heart rate and hereby improve the accuracy of the estimated number of heartbeats during the task. Future research could benefit from increasing the time between listening to the heart and actually performing the task to diminish the chance of this affecting task performance. Whereas prior work has indicated that regular exercise could impact IS [57] and BMI could influence HRV [86], the absence of correlations of these variables with IS and HRV indicates little influence in the present data. The current study is novel in investigating the associations between two complementary psychophysiological indices and ER and parental psychopathology in adolescence but it also raises some interesting questions for future research. Contrary to previous research [27, 28], the current study suggested that gender might influence the effects. Furthermore, this is a cross-sectional study in Caucasian middle class youngsters. Therefore, future research should explore the effects of gender and different ethnic and socio-economic backgrounds throughout development to increase generalizability. Additionally, given that ER is associated with psychopathology [87], it would be interesting to reproduce the current study in adolescents with psychiatric disorders.

In conclusion, the current study provided evidence on the complementary nature of two processes involved in physiological regulation and ER in adolescents. While high IS was associated with low maladaptive ER, high HRV was associated with external ER. Moreover, maternal internalizing symptoms were associated with both physiological indices in their children, specifically with higher HRV but lower IS, suggesting an interesting relationship between maternal psychological problems and autonomic processes related ER (as a potential protective factor) in their children. Future work will need to replicate these findings in psychiatric samples and examine how these factors could provide risk or resilience factors longitudinally.

Acknowledgments

SCM and NDW are supported by Ghent University (Multidisciplinary Research Partnership ‘‘The integrative neuroscience of behavioral control”).

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: NDW CB SS SCM.
  2. Data curation: NDW.
  3. Formal analysis: NDW.
  4. Funding acquisition: SCM.
  5. Investigation: NDW.
  6. Methodology: NDW SS SCM.
  7. Software: SS.
  8. Supervision: SCM CB.
  9. Visualization: NDW.
  10. Writing – original draft: NDW.
  11. Writing – review & editing: SCM CB SS.

References

  1. 1. Morris AS, Silk JS, Steinberg L, Myers SS, Robinson LR. The Role of the Family Context in the Development of Emotion Regulation. Soc Dev. 2007; 16(2):361–388. pmid:19756175
  2. 2. Williams SR, Woodruff-Borden J. Parent Emotion Socialization Practices and Child Self-regulation as Predictors of Child Anxiety: The Mediating Role of Cardiac Variability. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2014; 46:512–522. pmid:25204571.
  3. 3. Zeman J, Cassano M, Perry-Parrish C, Stegall S. Emotion regulation in children and adolescents. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2006; 27(2):155–168. pmid:16682883
  4. 4. Beauchaine TP, Gatzke-Kopp L, Mead HK. Polyvagal Theory and developmental psychopathology: emotion dysregulation and conduct problems from preschool to adolescence. Biol Psychol. 2007; 74(2):174–184. pmid:17045726
  5. 5. Vasilev CA, Crowell SE, Beauchaine TP, Mead HK, Gatzke-Kopp LM. Correspondence between physiological and self-report measures of emotion dysregulation: A longitudinal investigation of youth with and without psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psyc. 2009; 50(11):1357–1364. 000270829100005. pmid:19811585
  6. 6. Schachter S, Singer JE. Cognitive, Social, and Physiological Determinants of Emotional State. Psychol Rev. 1962; 69(5):379–399. A1962WN80300001.
  7. 7. James W. What is an emotion? Mind. 1884; 9:188–205.
  8. 8. Thayer JF, Lane RD. A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. J Affect Disord. 2000; 61(3):201–216. pmid:11163422
  9. 9. Porges SW. The polyvagal perspective. Biol Psychol. 2007; 74(2):116–143. pmid:17049418
  10. 10. Damasio AR. The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1996; 351(1346):1413–1420. pmid:8941953
  11. 11. Thompson RA. Emotion regulation: a theme in search of definition. Monogr Soc Res Child. 1994; 59(2–3):25–52. pmid:7984164.
  12. 12. Boyce WT, Ellis BJ. Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary-developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Dev Psychopathol. 2005; 17(2):271–301. pmid:16761546
  13. 13. McRae K, Gross JJ, Weber J, Robertson ER, Sokol-Hessner P, Ray RD, et al. The development of emotion regulation: an fMRI study of cognitive reappraisal in children, adolescents and young adults. Soc Cogn Affect Neur. 2012; 7(1):11–22. 000298891400002 pmid:22228751
  14. 14. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Wisco BE, Lyubomirsky S. Rethinking Rumination. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2008; 3(5):400–424. 000262000200005 pmid:26158958
  15. 15. Braet C, Cracco E, Theuwis L. Vragenlijst over emotieregulatie bij kinderen en jongeren [self-report questionnaire to assess children’s and adolescents’ emotion regulation strategies Dutch version]. Amsterdam: Hogrefe; 2013.
  16. 16. Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Schweizer S. Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010; 30(2):217–237. 000274319600007 pmid:20015584
  17. 17. Thayer JF, Ahs F, Fredrikson M, Sollers JJ, Wager TD. A meta-analysis of heart rate variability and neuroimaging studies: Implications for heart rate variability as a marker of stress and health. Neurosci Biobehav R. 2012; 36(2):747–756. 000300747600002 pmid:22178086
  18. 18. Thayer JF, Lane RD. Claude Bernard and the heart-brain connection: further elaboration of a model of neurovisceral integration. Neurosci Biobehav R. 2009; 33(2):81–88. pmid:18771686
  19. 19. Fabes RA, Eisenberg N. Regulatory control and adults' stress-related responses to daily life events. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1997; 73(5):1107–1117. pmid:9364764
  20. 20. Koval P, Ogrinz B, Kuppens P, Van den Bergh O, Tuerlinckx F, Sutterlin S. Affective instability in daily life is predicted by resting heart rate variability. PloS one. 2013; 8(11):e81536. pmid:24312315
  21. 21. Segerstrom SC, Nes LS. Heart rate variability reflects self-regulatory strength, effort, and fatigue. Psychol Sci. 2007; 18(3):275–281. pmid:17444926
  22. 22. Demaree HA, Robinson JL, Everhart DE, Schmeichel BJ. Resting RSA is associated with natural and self-regulated responses to negative emotional stimuli. Brain Cogn. 2004; 56(1):14–23. pmid:15380871
  23. 23. Gillie BL, Vasey MW, Thayer JF. Individual differences in resting heart rate variability moderate thought suppression success. Psychophysiology. 2015; 52:1149–6110. pmid:25917319.
  24. 24. Pappens M, Schroijen M, Sutterlin S, Smets E, Van den Bergh O, Thayer JF, et al. Resting heart rate variability predicts safety learning and fear extinction in an interoceptive fear conditioning paradigm. PloS one. 2014; 9(9):e105054. pmid:25181542
  25. 25. Williams DP, Cash C, Rankin C, Bernardi A, Koenig J, Thayer JF. Resting heart rate variability predicts self-reported difficulties in emotion regulation: a focus on different facets of emotion regulation. Front Psychol. 2015; 6:261. pmid:25806017
  26. 26. Chapman HA, Woltering S, Lamm C, Lewis MD. Hearts and minds: Coordination of neurocognitive and cardiovascular regulation in children and adolescents. Biol Psychol. 2010; 84(2):296–303. pmid:20223274
  27. 27. Scott BG, Weems CF. Resting vagal tone and vagal response to stress: associations with anxiety, aggression, and perceived anxiety control among youths. Psychophysiology. 2014; 51(8):718–727. pmid:24708059
  28. 28. Gentzler AL, Santucci AK, Kovacs M, Fox NA. Respiratory sinus arrhythmia reactivity predicts emotion regulation and depressive symptoms in at-risk and control children. Biol Psychol. 2009; 82(2):156–163. pmid:19596044
  29. 29. Geisler FCM, Kubiak T, Siewert K, Weber H. Cardiac vagal tone is associated with social engagement and self-regulation. Biol Psychol. 2013; 93(2):279–286. pmid:23466587
  30. 30. Beauchaine TP. Vagal tone, development, and Gray's motivational theory: toward an integrated model of autonomic nervous system functioning in psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol. 2001; 13(2):183–214. pmid:11393643
  31. 31. Blandon AY, Calkins SD, Keane SP, O'Brien M. Contributions of child's physiology and maternal behavior to children's trajectories of temperamental reactivity. Dev Psychol. 2010; 46(5):1089–1102. pmid:20822225
  32. 32. Kok BE, Fredrickson BL. Upward spirals of the heart: autonomic flexibility, as indexed by vagal tone, reciprocally and prospectively predicts positive emotions and social connectedness. Biol Psychol. 2010; 85(3):432–436. pmid:20851735
  33. 33. Bechara A, Naqvi N. Listening to your heart: interoceptive awareness as a gateway to feeling. Nat Neurosci. 2004; 7(2):102–103. pmid:14747831
  34. 34. Dunn BD, Galton HC, Morgan R, Evans D, Oliver C, Meyer M, et al. Listening to your heart. How interoception shapes emotion experience and intuitive decision making. Psychol Sci. 2010; 21(12):1835–1844. pmid:21106893
  35. 35. Dunn BD, Evans D, Makarova D, White J, Clark L. Gut feelings and the reaction to perceived inequity: the interplay between bodily responses, regulation, and perception shapes the rejection of unfair offers on the ultimatum game. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2012; 12(3):419–429. pmid:22618636
  36. 36. Critchley HD, Wiens S, Rotshtein P, Ohman A, Dolan RJ. Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat Neurosci. 2004; 7(2):189–195. pmid:14730305
  37. 37. Craig AD. How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002; 3(8):655–666. 000177447800016 pmid:12154366
  38. 38. Herbert BM, Pollatos O, Flor H, Enck P, Schandry R. Cardiac awareness and autonomic cardiac reactivity during emotional picture viewing and mental stress. Psychophysiology. 2010; 47(2):342–354. pmid:20003150
  39. 39. Pollatos O, Traut-Mattausch E, Schroeder H, Schandry R. Interoceptive awareness mediates the relationship between anxiety and the intensity of unpleasant feelings. J Anxiety Disord. 2007; 21(7):931–943. pmid:17257810
  40. 40. Herbert BM, Herbert C, Pollatos O. On the Relationship Between Interoceptive Awareness and Alexithymia: Is Interoceptive Awareness Related to Emotional Awareness? J Pers. 2011; 79(5):1149–1175. 000295227800010 pmid:21241306
  41. 41. Füstös J, Gramann K, Herbert BM, Pollatos O. On the embodiment of emotion regulation: interoceptive awareness facilitates reappraisal. Soc Cogn Affect Neur. 2013; 8(8):911–917. pmid:22933520
  42. 42. Koch A, Pollatos O. Cardiac sensitivity in children: Sex differences and its relationship to parameters of emotional processing. Psychophysiology. 2014; 51:932–941. pmid:24810627
  43. 43. Ellis BJ, Boyce WT. Differential susceptibility to the environment: toward an understanding of sensitivity to developmental experiences and context. Dev Psychopathol. 2011; 23(1):1–5. pmid:21262035
  44. 44. Shanahan L, Calkins SD, Keane SP, Kelleher R, Suffness R. Trajectories of internalizing symptoms across childhood: The roles of biological self-regulation and maternal psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol. 2014; 26:1353–1368. pmid:25422966
  45. 45. Blandon AY, Calkins SD, Keane SP, O'Brien M. Individual differences in trajectories of emotion regulation processes: the effects of maternal depressive symptomatology and children's physiological regulation. Dev Psychol. 2008; 44(4):1110–1123. pmid:18605838
  46. 46. Srinivasan K, Ashok MV, Vaz M, Yeragani VK. Decreased chaos of heart rate time series in children of patients with panic disorder. Depress Anxiety. 2002; 15(4):159–167. pmid:12112720
  47. 47. Field T, Diego M. Vagal activity, early growth and emotional development. Infant Behav Dev. 2008; 31(3):361–373. 000258741000003 pmid:18295898
  48. 48. Gentzler AL, Rottenberg J, Kovacs M, George CJ, Morey JN. Atypical development of resting respiratory sinus arrhythmia in children at high risk for depression. Developmental psychobiology. 2012; 54(5):556–567. pmid:21960203
  49. 49. Dreessen L, Stroux A, Weckx M. Nederlandse vertaling van het Gestructureerd Klinisch Interview voor DSM-IV—kind versie (KID-SCID; Versie 1.0) [Dutch translation of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV – Child edition]. Maastricht: Maastricht university; 1998.
  50. 50. Hien D, Matzner FJ, First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-Child edition (Versie, 1.0). New York: Colombia University; 1994.
  51. 51. Kaufmann T, Sütterlin S, Schulz SM, Vögele C. ARTiiFACT: a tool for heart rate artifact processing and heart rate variability analysis. Behav Res Methods. 2011; 43(4):1161–1170. pmid:21573720
  52. 52. Task Force of The European Society of Cardiology and The North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. Heart rate variability. Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Eur Heart J. 1996; 17:354–381. pmid:8737210
  53. 53. Garfinkel SN, Critchley HD. Interoception, emotion and brain: new insights link internal physiology to social behaviour. Commentary on: "Anterior insular cortex mediates bodily sensibility and social anxiety" by Terasawa et al. (2012). Soc Cogn Affect Neur. 2013; 8(3):231–234. pmid:23482658
  54. 54. Grob A, Smolenski C. Fragebogen zur Erhebung der Emotionsregulation bei Kindern und Jugendlichen (FEEL-KJ). Bern: Verlag Hans Huber; 2005.
  55. 55. Braet C, Cracco E, Theuwis L. Vragenlijst over emotieregulatie bij kinderen en jongeren [self-report questionnaire to assess children’s and adolescents’ emotion regulation strategies Dutch version]. Amsterdam: Hogrefe; 2013.
  56. 56. Schmitt K, Gold A, Rauch UA. Defizitäre adaptive Emotions- regulation bei Kindern mit ADHS [Deficient adaptive regulation of emotion in children with ADHD]. Z Kinder Jug-Psych. 2012; 40(2):95–103. 000300713400003 pmid:22354493
  57. 57. Cameron OG. Interoception: the inside story—a model for psychosomatic processes. Psychosom Med. 2001; 63(5):697–710. pmid:11573016
  58. 58. Milicevic G, Narancic NS, Steiner R, Rudan P. Increase in cardiac contractility during puberty. Coll Antropol. 2003; 27(1):335–341. pmid:12974163
  59. 59. Kowalewski M, Alifier M, Bochen D, Urban M. Heart rate turbulence in children -age and heart rate relationships. Pediatr Res. 2007; 62(6):710–714. pmid:17957160
  60. 60. Tanaka H, Borres M, Thulesius O, Tamai H, Ericson MO, Lindblad LE. Blood pressure and cardiovascular autonomic function in healthy children and adolescents. J Pediatr. 2000; 137(1):63–67. pmid:10891823
  61. 61. Petersen AC, Crockett L, Richards M, Boxer A. A self-report measure of pubertal status: Reliability, validity, and initial norms. J Youth Adolescence. 1988; 17(2):117–133. pmid:24277579
  62. 62. Hibberd EE, Hackney AC, Lane AR, Myers JB. Assessing biological maturity: chronological age and the pubertal development scale predict free testosterone in adolescent males. J Pediatr Endocrinol Metab. 2015; 28(3–4):381–386. pmid:25332291
  63. 63. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1983; 67(6):361–370. pmid:6880820
  64. 64. Spinhoven P, Ormel J, Sloekers PP, Kempen GI, Speckens AE, Van Hemert AM. A validation study of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) in different groups of Dutch subjects. Psychological medicine. 1997; 27(2):363–370. pmid:9089829.
  65. 65. Faulkner MS, Hathaway D, Tolley B. Cardiovascular autonomic function in healthy adolescents. Heart Lung. 2003; 32(1):10–22. pmid:12571544
  66. 66. Blom EH, Olsson EMG, Serlachius E, Ericson M, Ingvar M. Heart rate variability is related to self-reported physical activity in a healthy adolescent population. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2009; 106(6):877–883. pmid:19479275
  67. 67. Herbert BM, Pollatos O. Attenuated interoceptive sensitivity in overweight and obese individuals. Eat Behav. 2014; 15(3):445–458. pmid:25064297
  68. 68. Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: The Guilford Press; 2013.
  69. 69. Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav Res Methods. 2008; 40(3):879–891. pmid:18697684
  70. 70. Shrout PE, Bolger N. Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations. Psychol methods. 2002; 7(4):422–445. pmid:12530702
  71. 71. Hayes AF, Scharkow M. The relative trustworthiness of inferential tests of the indirect effect in statistical mediation analysis: does method really matter? Psychol Sci. 2013; 24(10):1918–1927. pmid:23955356
  72. 72. Pu J, Schmeichel BJ, Demaree HA. Cardiac vagal control predicts spontaneous regulation of negative emotional expression and subsequent cognitive performance. Biol Psychol. 2010; 84(3):531–540. 000279663000013
  73. 73. Garfinkel SN, Seth AK, Barrett AB, Suzuki K, Critchley HD. Knowing your own heart: Distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biol Psychol. 2015; 104:65–74. pmid:25451381
  74. 74. Meessen J, Mainz V, Gauggel S, Volz-Sidiropoulou E, Sütterlin S, Forkmann T. The relationship between interoception and metacognition: A pilot study. J Psychophysiol. 2016; 30:76–86.
  75. 75. Furman DJ, Waugh CE, Bhattacharjee K, Thompson RJ, Gotlib IH. Interoceptive awareness, positive affect, and decision making in major depressive disorder. J Affect Disord. 2013; 151(2):780–785. pmid:23972662
  76. 76. Mussgay L, Klinkenberg N, Ruddel H. Heart beat perception in patients with depressive, somatoform, and personality disorders. J Psychophysiol. 1999; 13(1):27–36. 000080878300004
  77. 77. Krautwurst S, Gerlach AL, Gomille L, Hiller W, Witthoft M. Health anxiety—an indicator of higher interoceptive sensitivity? J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2014; 45(2):303–309. pmid:24584036
  78. 78. Bariola E, Gullone E, Hughes EK. Child and adolescent emotion regulation: the role of parental emotion regulation and expression. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2011; 14(2):198–212. pmid:21424275
  79. 79. Lin Y, Lin C, Sun IW, Hsu CC, Fang CK, Lo MT, et al. Resting respiratory sinus arrhythmia is related to longer hospitalization in mood-disordered repetitive suicide attempters. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2015; 16(5):323–333 pmid:25839729
  80. 80. Marroquin B. Interpersonal emotion regulation as a mechanism of social support in depression. Clin Psychol Rev. 2011; 31(8):1276–1290. pmid:21983267
  81. 81. Singh JP, Larson MG, O'Donnell CJ, Levy D. Genetic factors contribute to the variance in frequency domain measures of heart rate variability. Auton Neurosci. 2001; 90(1–2):122–126. pmid:11485278
  82. 82. Ashman SB, Dawson G, Panagiotides H. Trajectories of maternal depression over 7 years: relations with child psychophysiology and behavior and role of contextual risks. Dev Psychopathol. 2008; 20(1):55–77. pmid:18211728
  83. 83. Calkins SD, Graziano PA, Keane SP. Cardiac vagal regulation differentiates among children at risk for behavior problems. Biol Psychol. 2007; 74(2):144–53. pmid:17055141
  84. 84. Kushki A, Drumm E, Pla Mobarak M, Tanel N, Dupuis A, Chau T, et al. Investigating the autonomic nervous system response to anxiety in children with autism spectrum disorders. PLoS One. 2013;8(4):e59730. pmid:23577072
  85. 85. Sulik MJ, Eisenberg N, Silva KM, Spinrad TL, Kupfer A. Respiratory sinus arrhythmia, shyness, and effortful control in preschool-age children. Biol Psychol. 2013; 92(2):241–8. pmid:23127725
  86. 86. Koenig J, Jarczok MN, Warth M, Ellis RJ, Bach C, Hillecke TK, et al. Body Mass Index Is Related to Autonomic Nervous System Activity as Measured by Heart Rate Variability-a Replication Using Short Term Measurements. J Nutr Health Aging. 2014; 18(3):300–302. 000334401800012 pmid:24626758
  87. 87. McLaughlin KA, Hatzenbuehler ML, Mennin DS, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Emotion dysregulation and adolescent psychopathology: a prospective study. Behav Res Ther. 2011; 49(9):544–554. pmid:21718967