Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Longitudinal Visuomotor Development in a Malaria Endemic Area: Cerebral Malaria and Beyond

  • Paul C. Knox ,

    pcknox@liv.ac.uk

    Affiliation Department of Eye and Vision Science, Institute of Ageing and Chronic Disease, University of Liverpool, William Duncan Building, Liverpool, L7 ATX, United Kingdom

  • Ian J. C. MacCormick,

    Affiliations Department of Eye and Vision Science, Institute of Ageing and Chronic Disease, University of Liverpool, William Duncan Building, Liverpool, L7 ATX, United Kingdom, Malawi-Liverpool-Wellcome Trust Clinical Research Programme, PO Box 30096, Blantyre, Malawi

  • Emme Mbale,

    Affiliation University of Malawi College of Medicine, College of Medicine, P/Bag 360 Chichiri, Blantyre, Malawi

  • Macpherson Malewa,

    Affiliation University of Malawi College of Medicine, College of Medicine, P/Bag 360 Chichiri, Blantyre, Malawi

  • Gabriela Czanner,

    Affiliations Department of Eye and Vision Science, Institute of Ageing and Chronic Disease, University of Liverpool, William Duncan Building, Liverpool, L7 ATX, United Kingdom, Department of Biostatistics, University of Liverpool, Waterhouse Building, Liverpool, L69 3GL, United Kingdom

  • Simon P. Harding

    Affiliation Department of Eye and Vision Science, Institute of Ageing and Chronic Disease, University of Liverpool, William Duncan Building, Liverpool, L7 ATX, United Kingdom

Abstract

Paediatric cerebral malaria is the most serious complication of Plasmodium falciparum infection. While the majority recover, long-term cognitive impairment has been highlighted as a significant and neglected problem. Persistent or serious deficits in processes such as attention or behavioural inhibition should be manifest in changes to performance on oculomotor tasks. Therefore we investigated the impact of cerebral malaria on the development of reflexive pro-saccades and antisaccades. In a longitudinal study, 47 children previously admitted with retinopathy-confirmed cerebral malaria (mean age at admission 54 months), were compared with 37 local healthy controls (mean ages at first study visit 117 and 110 months respectively). In each of three or four test sessions, over a period of up to 32 months, participants completed 100 prosaccade tasks and 100 antisaccade tasks. Eye movements were recorded using infrared reflectance oculography; prosaccade, correct antisaccade and error prosaccade latency, and antisaccade directional error rate were calculated. Hierarchical linear modelling was used to investigate the effect of age and the influence of cerebral malaria on these parameters. Data were also collected from an independent, older group (mean age 183 months) of 37 local healthy participants in a separate cross-sectional study. Longitudinal data exhibited the expected decrease in latency with age for all saccade types, and a decrease in the antisaccade directional error rate. Hierarchical linear modelling confirmed that age had a statistically significant effect on all parameters (p< = 0.001). However, there were no statistically significant differences between the cerebral malaria and control groups. Combining groups, comparison with the literature demonstrated that antisaccade directional error rate for the Malawi sample was significantly higher than expected, while latencies for all saccade types were indistinguishable from published. The high directional error rate was also confirmed in the older, healthy Malawian participants from the cross sectional study. Our observation of similar oculomotor performance in cerebral malaria and control groups at long follow-up periods suggests that cerebral malaria survivors are not at a generally increased risk of persistent cognitive deficits. Our data raise questions about the prevailing hypothesis that cerebral malaria has gross impacts on the development of processes such as attention and behavioural inhibition. More importantly, our novel finding of a clear difference in antisaccade performance between all of the Malawi participants and published data suggests that the Malawian paediatric population as a whole faces serious challenges to cognitive development beyond cerebral malaria.

Introduction

Malaria presents a substantial global health challenge, with an estimated annual incidence of approximately two million cases and over 600 000 deaths per year[1]. Most deaths occur in children under five years of age, particularly in sub-Saharan Africa. Paediatric cerebral malaria is one of several severe malarial syndromes and is diagnosed when peripheral parasitaemia is accompanied by impaired consciousness in the absence of another identifiable cause of coma. Greatly improved diagnostic specificity is achieved if malarial retinopathy is observed [2,3]. The case fatality rate of cerebral malaria with treatment is approximately 15% and survivors exhibit a range of neurological deficits at discharge [46]. More recently attention has turned to cognitive impairment following infection (the “hidden burden of malaria” [7]). It has been reported that impairments in aspects of executive function such as attention and working memory are both relatively common and long term [810].

Studying cognition in cerebral malaria survivors is challenging. Neuropsychological tests developed in one cultural context cannot simply be transferred to another, even once linguistic barriers have been overcome [11,12]. Cognitive tests developed in the West cannot be used in sub-Saharan Africa without adaptation, the effects of which are not always clear [1315]. Measuring neural function more directly with electrophysiological techniques [15] or brain imaging [16,17] is rarely an option in sub-Saharan Africa.

An alternative is the measurement of the parameters of saccadic eye movements, an approach widely used to investigate cognition and specific neural circuits [18] [19] and development [2025]. There are well established links between specific oculomotor parameters and cognitive processes [26], including attention [27,28], working memory [29,30] and inhibitory control [31,32], all key aspects of executive function. Clinically, the antisaccade task (in which participants are instructed to execute a saccade to the mirror-image position of a suddenly appearing target [33]) has increasingly been used to detect neuropathological and cognitive deficits in various patient groups [34,35] and has been used to investigate neuro-developmental conditions such as ADHD [3639].

If cerebral malaria survivors are at risk of long-term impairments in cognition, particularly related to executive function, then we would expect to see performance deficits in saccade tasks compared to a non-cerebral malaria local control group. We therefore conducted a longitudinal case control study of retinopathy-confirmed cerebral malaria using both prosaccade and antisaccade tasks. As many of our participants were relatively young even at the end of the longitudinal study, a smaller cross-sectional study was conducted in a group of normally developing, older participants to confirm the developmental course of the parameters of interest in the population we were examining.

Materials and Methods

Ethics Statement

All experiments were performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and were approved by the University of Malawi College of Medicine Research Ethics Committee. Study information was provided to parents, guardians and participants in the local language and written, informed consent obtained.

Participants

Participants were recruited from two pre-existing cohorts of children from previous studies in Blantyre, Malawi. The Blantyre Malaria Project Epilepsy Study (BMPES [40]) recruited retinopathy-confirmed cerebral malaria survivors from children admitted to the Queen Elizabeth Central Hospital (QECH), Blantyre, Malawi, between 2005 and 2007. The control group in BMPES consisted of children who were admitted to general paediatric wards in QECH, with a normal level of consciousness, and no history of unprovoked seizures or severe malaria. Children from both of these groups who had not been lost to follow-up since the original study, who did not suffer from epilepsy and who were a minimum of 6 years of age when testing in the current study began (November 2011) were approached. Those without apparent gross neurological or visual deficits were recruited to our longitudinal study. Once recruited, they attended QECH for testing on multiple occasions.

We also examined a second group of participants who were originally recruited for the Brain Imaging in Normal Kids (BRINK) study [41]. The BRINK study recruited a community-based, representative sample of normally developing children from in and around Blantyre, and each child underwent a neurological examination and brain MRI. From this cohort we were able to contact, approach and recruit an older age-group compared to the longitudinal study. We tested those participants who had not suffered any serious infection or injury since participating in BRINK, and who were willing to consent.

Equipment

Horizontal eye movements were recorded binocularly with a miniaturised head-mounted infrared saccadometer (Advanced Clinical Instrumentation, Cambridge, UK). This sampled infrared reflectance signals at 1KHz, and low-pass filtered them at 250 Hz with 12-bit resolution. The device incorporated three low-power red lasers projecting red 13 cd/m2 target spots subtending approximately 0.1°, in a horizontal line, centrally and at 10° to left and right of centre. As the stimuli moved with the head, participants were not head-fixed; they sat in a comfortable position approximately 1.5m in front of a near-white surface. For both pro- and antisaccades the stimulus was identical. After a randomised fixation period (1s-2s), the central fixation target was extinguished, and a single eccentric target appeared at 10° to either the left or right (this was also randomised). Note that in both prosaccade and antisaccade tasks the target appeared immediately after fixation target extinction; synchronous rather than gap tasks were used.

Procedures

In the longitudinal study, participants completed 100 prosaccade and 100 antisaccade trials in two separate blocks in each testing session. While initially it had been hoped to counterbalance block order, participants found this confusing. So for the bulk of testing the prosaccade block was completed first. Prior to each block, participants completed 10 calibration trials, and were given detailed task instructions and stepped through the task. They were asked to describe, by pointing with their finger, what they had been instructed to do. For prosaccade trials, they were instructed to look at targets appearing to the left and right of centre as soon as they saw them. Other than this they were given no instructions about speed or accuracy. For antisaccade trials, care was taken to explain that they were to look to the mirror image position of the target, that is in the opposite direction to the target, but the same distance from fixation as the target. This was done while demonstrating the relative positions of the central fixation target, the target position, and where they were required to look. Participants were carefully observed throughout testing. If there was any doubt that they had understood the tasks instructions, the run was stopped, and the task explained again. Exactly the same procedures were followed during the single test session in the cross-sectional study on the BRINK cohort.

Analysis

Oculomotor data were stored on the Saccadometer handset, and downloaded using Latency Meter 4 software. Parameters were collated using MS Excel. For each participant and each testing session we computed the median prosaccade (Pro), correct antisaccade (CorAS) and error prosaccade (ErrPS) latency. We also calculated mean saccade amplitude for each type of saccade. For each participant and session, the antisaccade directional error rate (DER) was calculated.

In an initial analysis of the longitudinal data, and to aid visualization, least squares linear regressions of each participant’s session data on age were calculated, and the slope and Pearson correlation coefficient used to summarize each regression. To explicitly investigate the influence of cerebral malaria, the latency of each type of saccade (Pro; CorAS and ErrPS from the antisaccade task) and the DER were analysed using growth curve modelling [42,43]. This describes all of the data in a single analysis allowing the characterisation of general patterns, tests for individual differences in growth curves and the further characterisation of within participant differences. A hierarchical linear model (also referred to as a linear mixed-effects model) was used. First, linear, quadratic and inverse unconditional growth curves were fitted to the data. The optimally fitting model was chosen using the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) and the fit confirmed using standard goodness-of-fit residual analysis. Using the analysis of DER as an example, the final model selected was:

Level 1 describes the values of DER on the level of all participants (i.e. the between participant differences), while Level 2 describes the values of DER at the level of individual participants (i.e. the within-participant differences). In this model Malaria is a dichotomous predictor (0 = control, 1 = case) and Age is centered at 132 months. The parameter π1i reflects i-th child slope of DER profile curve, π2 reflects the mean increase in DER due Malaria, β00 reflects the grand mean DER at the centred age of the sample, and β01 reflects the grand mean slope of the child’s DER profile. The variability of the random terms (r0i and r1i) show the amount of variability within participants (of their intercepts and slopes). The models were fit using the restricted maximisation likelihood in the R statistical package (function “lme”).

Results

In the longitudinal study, data were available from 47 retinopathy-confirmed cerebral malaria survivors and 37 control participants (Table 1), all with normal visual acuity. Cerebral malaria survivors were tested on four occasions over a mean period of 31.2 months, or on three occasions over 20.3 months. The controls were tested on either four occasions over 32.1 months, or three occasions over 21.9 months. For the cerebral malaria group, the first test session took place on average 68.1 months after their admission in a malaria coma, at which time malarial retinopathy had been confirmed.

Latency Results

We observed the expected development in performance in prosaccade tasks (Fig 1A and 1B); as age increased Pro latency tended to decrease. To capture within-participant development, we calculated least-squares linear regressions of latency on age (in months). The mean (±SD) slope of these functions was -1.27±2.20 for the cerebral malaria group compared to -1.92±2.69 in the control group (t = 1.2, p = 0.22, two-tailed), suggesting that on average there was a similar decrease in latency with age in both groups. Slopes were generally steeper and more variable among younger participants consistent with more rapid development, which slowed at older ages. Data from the antisaccade task followed the same pattern. For CorAS (Fig 1C and 1D) the average slope of participants’ regression functions was greater than for Pro but very similar between groups (cerebral malaria: -3.10±5.90; control: -3.13±5.12). ErrPS latency (Fig 1E and 1F) also exhibited the same general pattern of reductions with age both within and between participants.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Effect of age on the latency of three types of saccade.

Prosaccades: A,B; correct antisaccades: C,D; error prosaccades from antisaccade tasks: E, F. Control (CON A,C,E) and Cerebral Malaria (CM B,D,F) groups. Data from each participant in each testing session is plotted, and summarised using a least-squares linear regression line. Where data is available for four test sessions it is plotted in red, data from participants tested over three sessions is plotted in blue. On each plot the mean (±SD) Pearson correlation coefficient (r) and gradient of the regression lines (m) is shown, averaged over all participants in the group. Note that the y-axis scale differs for C and D.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0164885.g001

As it takes additional time to programme and execute an antisaccade compared to a prosaccade, CorAS latency is expected to be greater than ErrPS latency. This pattern was observed in our dataset, and did not differ between groups (Fig 2A). We computed the difference between each participant’s median CorAS and ErrPS latency, and examined the effect of age (using each participant’s mean age across testing sessions) on this difference. While the general patterns were similar between groups (Fig 2B) there were some cerebral malaria participants who generated negative differences in this analysis (ie did not exhibit, on average, shorter ErrPS latency compared to the CorAS latency). However, where DER is either very high or very low, the session estimates for these figures are based on very low numbers of observations. A very high DER implies very few CorAS from which to compute a median latency; a very low DER means few ErrPS. Within the small number of cerebral malaria participants who generated these negative differences, we observed both of these phenomena.

thumbnail
Fig 2. The latency of different types of saccade and their relationships.

A. Intersubject mean (95% CI–upper or lower error bars plotted for clarity) averaged across each testing session. CM: Data plotted in red. CON: Data plotted in blue. ● correct antisaccades (CorAS); ■ error prosaccades (ErrPS). B. Plot of the difference between mean correct antisaccade latency (averaged over testing sessions) and mean error prosaccade latency for each participant against their age (calculated as the mean age over testing sessions). CM: red, CON: blue as in A. C. Correlation between prosaccade (ProSac) latency and error prosaccade (ErrPS) latency for all participants and sessions. CM: red, CON: blue as in A. Parameters of least-squares linear regression functions and correlation coefficients are shown.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0164885.g002

As prosaccade errors in the antisaccade task are assumed to be ineffectively inhibited reflexive responses toward target onsets, there should be a clear correlation across participants between ErrPS latency and the latency of saccades in the prosaccade task itself (Pro). For both groups we observed statistically significant correlations between the latencies of these two types of saccade across participants (Fig 2C; cerebral malaria: r = 0.53, p<0.001; controls: r = 0.56, p<0.001). The slope of the least squares linear regression lines was 0.59 for both, consistent with ErrPS latency being consistently less than Pro latency. There was no difference between the groups.

Antisaccade Directional Error Rate

Antisaccade DER also tended to decrease with age, both within and between participants, as expected (Fig 3). For the cerebral malaria group, individual regression slopes were negative in 39 of 47 participants (83%), and the overall mean of all regression slopes was -0.48±0.72. For the control group, 26 of 37 individual participants had negative regression slopes (70%) with a mean of -0.54±0.85.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Effect of age on antisaccade directional error rate (DER).

Presented calculated as a percentage for control (A) and cerebral malairia (B) groups. Plotting conventions as for Fig 1.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0164885.g003

We used a hierarchical linear model to confirm that ageing had the expected general effects on all the parameters investigated both within and between participants, and to assess in a more critical manner whether there was evidence of significant differences between the cerebral malaria and control groups (Table 2). The linear model provided the most optimal fit for the growth curves of DER, and Pro, CorAS and ErrPS latency (Table 2) as guided by the AIC values. The mean DER, Pro, CorAS and ErrPS of a participant of 11 years (based on the intercept of the model centered at this age) was 56.76±2.09%, 241.13±8.42ms, 387.38±14.13ms and 246.62±8.44ms, respectively. All four of these parameters were affected significantly by age (p< = 0.001; Table 2). However, there was no statistically significant effect of malaria (ie no statistically significant difference between the two groups) for DER, or Pro and CorAS latencies. The effect on ErrPS latency reached a marginal level of significance (p = 0.04). This was insignificant after Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons with a family-wise level of significance α = 0.05, which provided a corrected α = 0.0125. On this basis, none of the parameters reached significance.

thumbnail
Table 2. Analysis of data from the longitudinal study comparing cerebral malaria and control groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0164885.t002

Cross-sectional results from the BRINK cohort

The average age of 37 participants recruited from the BRINK cohort was 183±23months, with 12/37 (32%) aged 192 months (16y) or over at the time of testing. The intersubject mean of individual median Pro latencies was 237±45ms, while CorAS and ErrPS latencies were 351±117ms and 253±51ms respectively. The mean DER was 58±22%. We compared these data with older participants from the longitudinal study. Given that there was no difference in the performance of cerebral malaria and control groups, we calculated mean latencies and DER for all participants whose average age over testing sessions was greater than 144 months (N = 15; Fig 4). The group mean age for the participants from the sub-group from the longitudinal study was 173±24 months, and not statistically different from the age of the group from the BRINK cohort (t = 1.4, p = 0.16).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Comparison of cross-sectional and longitudinal data.

Cross-sectional data from the participants from the BRINK cohort (C) is compared to data from participants aged >144 months in the longitudinal study (L). In each plot individual participants’ data is shown, with the mean±95% CI. A. Prosaccade latency. B. Antisaccade directional error rate (DER). C. Correct antisaccade latency. D. Error prosaccade latency from the antisaccade task. Note different axis scales between A and C, D.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0164885.g004

For all of the saccade parameters, performance in the BRINK group tended to be worse (longer latencies, higher DER). For Pro and CorAS latency the group differences were not statistically significant (t = 1.72, p = 0.09 and t = 0.66, p = 0.51 respectively; p values uncorrected for multiple comparisons). For ErrPS latency and DER the differences were statistically significant (t = 3.83, p<0.001 and t = 2.64, p = 0.01 respectively).

Comparison with data in the literature

Given the absence of a difference between the cerebral malaria and control groups for DER, we fitted a single function to all of the raw data to provide a single description of the influence of age on DER. For this we used the mean from the linear hierarchical model (±95% CI) because it was our best fitting model of DER (Fig 5A). This single fit was very similar to the LOESS fit of the same data, which is shown for comparison. We identified a small number of studies in which a synchronous antisaccade task had been used (as opposed to either gap or overlap tasks), and which reported DER for specific age groups. Using a weighted mixed-effect linear model (with a random effect for the study to allow for correlations between data from same study) we generated a comparison plot of DER against age (±95% CI; Fig 5B). These two linear models had very different slopes; the DER was consistently higher in the longitudinal Malawi dataset than would be expected based on the literature.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Effect of age on DER in combined Malawi group, and comparrision with data from the literature.

A. Combined analysis of data from cerebral malaria (blue) and control (red) groups in the longitudinal study. Both hierarchical linear model (solid lines) and LOESS fits (dotted lines), ±95% CI are shown. B. Comparison of hierarchical linear model (solid lines) from longitudinal Malawi dataset, with a weighted mixed-effect linear model constructed from data extracted from four published studies in which a synchronous antisaccade task was used (dashed lines). Central estimates ±95% CI shown.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0164885.g005

We also compared Pro and CorAS latency from our data with data from the literature. This analysis was limited because, particularly for the antisaccade literature, latency is reported less consistently than DER. As listed in Table 3, we identified a small number of studies using synchronous saccade tasks, from which we could extract latency centred at the age of 132 months. From these we calculated a weighted mean (Table 3). Prosaccade latency from our data was 241.13ms (95% CI: 224.5, 257.7) compared to a weighted mean from the literature of 230.5 ms (95% CI: 223.6, 237.4). Given the observed overlap of the confidence intervals, we conclude that the two means are similar with at least 90% confidence. Our mean CorAS latency was 387.4ms (95% CI: 359.5, 415.2), compared to the literature weighted mean of 376.5ms (95% CI: 357.2, 395.9). Again, given the observed overlap of the confidence intervals, we conclude that the two means are similar with at least 90% confidence.

Discussion

The developmental trajectory of reflexive prosaccade (Pro) latency, correct antisaccade (CorAS) and error prosaccade (ErrPS) latency, and of antisaccade directional error rate (DER) have been widely reported. Latencies steadily decrease with age, reaching a minimum in mid to late adolescence; the antisaccade directional error rate begins to decline around the age of 8 years, and shows a particularly marked decrease up to the age of approximately 14 years [19,20,22,44]. These behavioral changes are related to the structural and functional maturation of the frontal and parietal cortices, and their interconnection with other structures such as the basal ganglia [45,46].

Longitudinal analysis of saccade data from retinopathy-confirmed cerebral malaria survivors and age-matched local controls, demonstrated the expected decrease in latency with age for all saccade types examined and no difference in saccade behaviour or in its longitudinal development between the groups. We confirmed detailed relationships between saccade latency parameters and again found that they did not differ between the groups. Antisaccade DER also declined with age with no apparent difference between the groups. Hierarchical linear modelling confirmed that age had a statistically significant effect on all parameters, while having had cerebral malaria did not.

While there is a clear qualitative (Figs 1 and 3) and statistical similarity in performance between groups, two related issues need to be addressed. Firstly, what magnitude of difference would have constituted a biologically or clinically significant difference between groups? Secondly, on the basis of our data, how confident can we be that there really is no functionally important difference? In ADHD, differences in antisaccade DER between cases and controls of the order of 12% to 14% have been reported in large studies of children aged approximately 11y, and have been taken to be relevant to understanding cognitive function in this condition [36,37]. In a smaller study of children aged around 10y, in which we were investigating the effect of preterm birth on antisaccade performance, we observed a difference of 20% between groups [47]. In studies of adults, proportionately larger group differences have been reported. In summarising the schizophrenia literature, Hutton [48] quotes a healthy control DER range of 2% to 25%, with 20% being typical of larger studies, compared to 25% to 70% in patients with schizophrenia. In much of this literature DER in cases is often two or three times that observed in controls. For current purposes therefore, had we observed a difference of the order of 10% to 15% then we would have concluded an important performance deficit was present, consistent with impaired cognition.

For our samples, over the age range of 138 to 150 months, the mean antisaccade DER was 58.9% and 56.9% for cases and controls respectively, with a standard deviation of 19.4%. If antisaccade DER between groups is compared using a 1-tailed alternative hypothesis t-test, assuming a level of significance of 0.05, then group differences of 10%, 15% or 20%, would be detectable with powers of 75.2%, 96.7% and 99.9% respectively. This provides some assurance that we have sufficient statistical power to detect a magnitude of group difference (15%) that would generally be considered functionally important. Note that this reduces the power calculation to point estimates at a specific age. The growth curve modelling extends this by taking the longitudinal nature of the data into account.

This lack of effect of cerebral malaria is surprising, and seemingly at odds with a body of literature suggesting a range of sequelae following cerebral malaria. However, there are important differences between this and previous studies. Many cerebral malaria studies have used standard clinical criteria [1,49] for participant recruitment: otherwise unexplained coma in a child with peripheral parasitaemia [8,9,50,51]. It has been known for some time that in up to 25% of children meeting these criteria, the cause of the coma may not be malaria [2]. We used the presence of malarial retinopathy on admission to improve diagnostic accuracy [52].

We also excluded participants with epilepsy in contrast to a number of studies reporting persistent neurocognitive impairments post-infection [53,54]. The evidence of poorer cognitive outcomes in those cases who suffer from seizures is mixed [55]; it has been suggested that it is specifically this group who need monitoring and management the most [56]. However, only 5% to 10% of cerebral malaria survivors develop epilepsy [40,57,58], and concern has been expressed about those survivors who do not suffer from obvious impairments, and yet may have difficulties that remain undetected [7]. Our motivation was therefore to study a group of children with a clear diagnosis of cerebral malaria (aided by the detection of retinopathy) but without gross impairments.

Studies also differ with respect to both the age of infection and coma in cases, and the timing of assessments post-coma. In malaria endemic areas in sub-Saharan Africa with high rates of transmission, cerebral malaria mainly occurs in children aged less than 4 years. The average age at admission in our cerebral malaria group was approximately 4.5y. This was partly determined by the timing of our study relative to the original recruitment of participants into the BMPES study, and our need to test participants in the age range where saccade behaviour is developing. In children <3 years of age, cerebral malaria (and severe malarial anemia) has been reported to adversely affect general cognition and attention [51], as well as in older children relative to local controls [8,59]. We cannot exclude the possibility that children who are younger at the time of infection and coma are impacted differently from older children, since we only recruited older children.

The timing of post-coma or post-discharge assessments and follow-up periods vary across studies; this may be important as there is a temporal pattern to the emergence of sequelae. Across a number of studies [9,51,60] a range of severe neurological sequelae that may be observed at discharge have been reported to resolve by 12 months, while it has been suggested cognitive impairments persist over longer periods. As noted in Table 1, in 3/42 (7%) of our cases serious sequelae were noted at discharge, which were not present at recruitment to the study. In many studies the age at the time of testing is lower than in our study, while the length of follow-up (typically 6 months to 2 years for those noted above) is less than our 5.5 years (the average period between coma and the first test in the longitudinal study). Our participants were therefore both older and examined much longer post-coma than has typically been the case. In one other previous study with a relatively long average follow-up period of 3.7 years there was no difference between the cerebral malaria and control groups on a battery of neurological and cognitive tasks [50].

Two studies examined outcomes in retinopathy positive cerebral malaria survivors from Blantyre, Malawi [40,61]. Birbeck et al [40], followed participants for a mean of 544 days (approximately 1.5y) using both an epilepsy and a neurological screening questionnaire. They reported multiple neurological sequelae which emerged sequentially (gross motor and sensory deficits first, then behavioural disorders, then epilepsy) affecting 32% of their cerebral malaria group. Boivin et al [61] used the Malawi Development Assessment Tool and the Auchenbach Child Behaviour Checklist a mean of 1.43 years post-discharge and reported that a statistically significantly higher proportion of cerebral malaria survivors exhibited evidence of developmental delay compared to controls. On the Auchenbach CBCL, used to investigate psychiatric and psychosocial symptoms, there was no statistically significant difference between groups. These studies are consistent with those performed elsewhere, suggesting that cerebral malaria survivors in Blantyre, when examined within 1–2 years of coma, exhibit a similar range of neurological and cognitive sequelae as found in other malaria endemic areas. However, this is not incompatible with our results. It leaves open the possibility that these various sequelae resolve subsequently leaving no detectable group differences after longer periods.

Timing of assessments is important because, particularly for cognitive functions such as attention, development continues into late adolescence and early adulthood [62,63]. It is important to recognize therefore, that measurement of these aspects of cognition in very young participants (as in a number of cerebral malaria studies), will miss much of this development. At the minimum, distinguishing between permanent deficits and developmental delays is made more difficult without data from older age groups.

The other surprising result the emerges from our analysis is that when we combined the saccade data from cerebral malaria and control participants in the longitudinal study into a single “Malawi” dataset, we found that antisaccade DER was much higher than expected. This was confirmed using a weighted mixed-effects linear model to compare our data with data extracted from the literature. Not only was DER in the Malawi data higher across the range of ages we studied, but for that part of the age range for which we have most data, the lack of overlap of the 95% confidence intervals suggests that the difference is robust. This was not replicated for prosaccade and antisaccade latency where comparisons demonstrated that prosaccade and antisaccade latency at 11 years of age was statistically indistinguishable between the Malawi dataset and data drawn from the literature. The main weakness in this analysis is the relatively small number of studies we were able to identify which had used a synchronous antisaccade task as opposed to a gap task. The type of task used is an important consideration as DER is higher in gap tasks. However, agreement between the studies we were able to identify was reasonable despite a degree of methodological heterogeneity.

Antisaccade DER reaches a plateau in late adolescence, as does saccade latency [21,64]. In the longitudinal study we had relatively few older participants meaning that estimates of performance at older ages were necessarily uncertain. However, by recruiting participants from the BRINK cohort [41] we were able to test older participants drawn from the same general population. These participants had been recruited from the community and screened as developing normally. Given this, and their older age, we would have expected lower DER and latency in this group. In fact, DER was higher compared to the 15 oldest participants from the longitudinal study, and the latencies were longer. These data are important and provide confirmation that performance on the antisaccade task in this population was worse (ie higher error rates) than would be expected from the saccade literature.

Our motivation in testing saccade development, and in particular in testing antisaccades, was to investigate cognition. It might be argued that saccade tasks are not associated closely enough with the aspects of cognitive function reported to be affected by cerebral malaria, compared to other types of neuropsychological testing. However, deficits in attention [10,59,65], working memory [51,65] and behavioural inhibition [10,66], all key aspects of executive function, have been reported to be affected by cerebral malaria. Deficits in precisely these aspects of cognition should impair performance on the antisaccade task which is dependent on all three [26]. Much higher rates of disruptive behaviour (akin to ADHD) have also been reported in cerebral malaria survivors compared to controls[61]. Antisaccade DER is increased in ADHD [36,37,39] as well as in other conditions in which attentional, working memory and inhibitory deficits are prominent (eg autism [67]; schizophrenia [68,69]). If therefore there were persistent deficits in one or some combination of attention, working memory and inhibition, we would expect this to be manifest in an increased antisaccade DER, and be particularly clear in older participants (ie >12y) in whom error rates should be approaching adult values.

Further complementary testing of cognition at older ages in well-characterised cerebral malaria survivors will be required to establish precisely the long-term risks of cerebral malaria in early childhood, particularly in those apparently making good recoveries. It would also be useful to have saccade data from other populations for comparison both with our Malawi data and that published in the literature which is dominated by participants drawn from Western populations.

While malaria in general and cerebral malaria in particular continue to pose serious health challenges, there is a need to study factors beyond cerebral malaria that might explain the poor antisaccade performance we have uncovered. While there are some population differences in saccade processing that do not appear to have an environmental cause [7072], these relate primarily to patterns of prosaccade latency, where there is little difference between the Malawi data and published values. In the population from which our participants were drawn, non-malaria factors that might have a bearing on cognitive development (eg poverty, nutrition[73,74]), along with the impact of other diseases, need to receive further attention.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Theresa Nnensa and Bright Banda for their assistance in recruiting and testing participants along with nurses from the Malaria Research Ward in Queen Elizabeth Central Hospital. We are also grateful to the COPS and BMPES investigators, Gretchen Birbeck, Michael Potchen and the other BRINK investigators and staff, and Karl Seydel, Terrie Taylor and Malcolm Molyneux for support, advice and help with recruitment throughout this project.

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: PCK SPH.
  2. Data curation: PCK IJCM EM GC.
  3. Formal analysis: PCK IJCM.
  4. Funding acquisition: PCK SPH.
  5. Investigation: PCK IJCM EM.
  6. Methodology: PCK IJCM MM.
  7. Project administration: PCK IJCM MM.
  8. Resources: PCK SPH MM.
  9. Supervision: PCK SPH MM.
  10. Visualization: PCK IJCM GC.
  11. Writing – original draft: PCK.
  12. Writing – review & editing: PCK IJCM MM EM GC.

References

  1. 1. WHO (2014) Severe Malaria. Tropical Medicine & International Health 19: 7–131.
  2. 2. Taylor TE, Fu WJ, Carr RA, Whitten RO, Mueller JG, Fosiko NG, et al. (2004) Differentiating the pathologies of cerebral malaria by postmortem parasite counts. Nat Med 10: 143–145. pmid:14745442
  3. 3. Beare NAV, Taylor TE, Harding SP, Lewallen S, Molyneux ME (2006) Malarial Retinopathy: A newly established diagnostic sign in severe malaria. The American Journal of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene 75: 790–797. pmid:17123967
  4. 4. Molyneux M, Taylor TE, Wirima JJ, Borgstein A (1989) Clinical features and prognostic indicators in paediatric cerebral malaria: a study of 131 comatose Malawian children. Quarterly Journal of Medicine 71: 441–459. pmid:2690177
  5. 5. Brewster DR, Kwiatkowski D, White NJ (1990) Neurological sequelae of cerebral malaria in children. The Lancet 336: 1039–1043.
  6. 6. Bondi FS (1992) The incidence and outcome of neurological abnormalities in childhood cerebral malaria: A long-term follow-up of 62 survivors. Transactions of The Royal Society of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene 86: 17–19. pmid:1566292
  7. 7. Fernando S, Rodrigo C, Rajapakse S (2010) The 'hidden' burden of malaria: cognitive impairment following infection. Malaria Journal 9: 366. pmid:21171998
  8. 8. Carter JA, Mung’ala-Odera V, Neville BGR, Murira G, Mturi N, Musumba C. (2005) Persistent neurocognitive impairments associated with severe falciparum malaria in Kenyan children. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 76: 476–481.
  9. 9. Boivin MJ, Bangirana P, Byarugaba J, Opoka RO, Idro R, Jurek AM (2007) Cognitive Impairment After Cerebral Malaria in Children: A Prospective Study. Pediatrics 119: e360–366. pmid:17224457
  10. 10. Kariuki S, Abubakar A, Newton C, Kihara M (2014) Impairment of executive function in Kenyan children exposed to severe falciparum malaria with neurological involvement. Malaria Journal 13: 365. pmid:25224247
  11. 11. Harkness S, Super CM (1977) Why African children are so hard to test. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 285: 326–331.
  12. 12. Greenfield PM (1997) You can't take it with you: Why ability assessments don't cross cultures. American Psychologist 52: 1115–1124.
  13. 13. Holding PA, Taylor HG, Kazungu SD, Mkala T, Gona J, Mwamute B, et al. (2004) Assessing cognitive outcomes in a rural African population: Development of a neuropsychological battery in Kilifi District, Kenya. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 10: 246–260. pmid:15012845
  14. 14. Kitsao-Wekulo PK, Holding PA, Taylor HG, Abubakar A, Connolly K (2013) Neuropsychological Testing in a Rural African School-Age Population: Evaluating Contributions to Variability in Test Performance. Assessment 20: 776–784. pmid:22936783
  15. 15. Kihara M, de Haan M, Garrashi HH, Neville BGR, Newton CRJC (2010) Atypical brain response to novelty in rural African children with a history of severe falciparum malaria. Journal of the Neurological Sciences 296: 88–95. pmid:20566207
  16. 16. Potchen MJ, Birbeck GL, DeMarco JK, Kampondeni SD, Beare N, Molyneux ME, et al. (2010) Neuroimaging findings in children with retinopathy-confirmed cerebral malaria. European Journal of Radiology 74: 262–268. pmid:19345538
  17. 17. Potchen MJ, Kampondeni SD, Seydel KB, Birbeck GL, Hammond CA, Bradley WG, et al. (2012) Acute brain MRI findings in 120 Malawian children with cerebral malaria: New insights into an ancient disease. American Journal of Neuroradiology 33: 1740–1746. pmid:22517285
  18. 18. Leigh RJ, Kennard C (2004) Using saccades as a research tool in the clinical neurosciences. Brain 127: 460–477. pmid:14607787
  19. 19. McDowell JE, Dyckman KA, Austin BP, Clementz BA (2008) Neurophysiology and neuroanatomy of reflexive and volitional saccades: Evidence from studies of humans. Brain and Cognition 68: 255–270. pmid:18835656
  20. 20. Fischer B, Biscaldi M, Gezeck S (1997) On the development of voluntary and reflexive components in human saccade generation. Brain Research 754: 285–297. pmid:9134986
  21. 21. Munoz DP, Broughton JR, Goldring JE, Armstrong IT (1998) Age-related performance of human subjects on saccadic eye movement tasks. Experimental Brain Research 121: 391–400. pmid:9746145
  22. 22. Klein C, Foerster F (2001) Development of prosaccade and antisaccade task performance in participants aged 6 to 26 years. Psychophysiology 38: 179–189. pmid:11347863
  23. 23. Luna Beatriz, Garver Krista E., Urban Trinity A., Lazar Nicole A., Sweeney John A. (2004) Maturation of cognitive processes from late childhood to adulthood. Child Development 75: 1357–1372. pmid:15369519
  24. 24. Karatekin C (2007) Eye tracking studies of normative and atypical development. Developmental Review 27: 283–348.
  25. 25. Bucci M, Seassau M (2012) Saccadic eye movements in children: a developmental study. Experimental Brain Research 222: 21–30. pmid:22836522
  26. 26. Hutton S (2008) Cognitive control of saccadic eye movements. Brain and Cognition 68: 327–340. pmid:19028265
  27. 27. Harwood MR, Madelain L, Krauzlis RJ, Wallman J (2008) The spatial scale of attention strongly modulates saccade latencies. J Neurophysiol 99: 1743–1757. pmid:18234988
  28. 28. Kowler E, Anderson E, Doshler B, Blaser E (1995) The role of attention in the programming of saccades. Vision Res 35: 1987–1961.
  29. 29. Mitchell JP, Macrae CN, Gilchrist ID (2002) Working memory and the suppression of reflexive saccades. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 14: 95–103. pmid:11798390
  30. 30. Crawford T, Parker E, Solis-Trapala I, Mayes J (2011) Is the relationship of prosaccade reaction times and antisaccade errors mediated by working memory? Experimental Brain Research 208: 385–397. pmid:21107543
  31. 31. Aron AR (2007) The neural basis of inhibition in cognitive control. The Neuroscientist 13: 214–228. pmid:17519365
  32. 32. Roberts W, Fillmore MT, Milich R (2011) Linking impulsivity and inhibitory control using manual and oculomotor response inhibition tasks. Acta Psychologica 138: 419–428. pmid:21982865
  33. 33. Munoz DP, Everling S (2004) Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement. Nat Rev Neurosci 5: 218–228. pmid:14976521
  34. 34. Hellmuth J, Mirsky J, Heuer HW, Matlin A, Jafari A, et al. (2012) Multicenter validation of a bedside antisaccade task as a measure of executive function. Neurology 78: 1824–1831. pmid:22573640
  35. 35. Heuer HW, Mirsky JB, Kong EL, Dickerson BC, Miller BL, Kramer JH, et al. (2013) Antisaccade task reflects cortical involvement in mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 81: 1235–1243. pmid:23986300
  36. 36. Klein C, Raschke A, Brandenbusch A (2003) Development of pro- and antisaccades in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and healthy controls. Psychophysiology 40: 17–28. pmid:12756978
  37. 37. Munoz DP, Armstrong IT, Hampton KA, Moore KD (2003) Altered control of visual fixation and saccadic eye movements in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Neurophysiology 90: 503–514. pmid:12672781
  38. 38. Carr L, Henderson J, Nigg JT (2010) Cognitive control and attentional selection in adolescents with ADHD versus ADD. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology 39: 726–740.
  39. 39. Goto Y, Hatakeyama K, Kitama T, Sato Y, Kanemura H, Aoyagi K, et al. (2010) Saccade eye movements as a quantitative measure of frontostriatal network in children with ADHD. Brain and Development 32: 347–355. pmid:19505783
  40. 40. Birbeck GL, Molyneux ME, Kaplan PW, Seydel KB, Chimalizeni YF, Kawaza K, et al. (2010) Blantyre Malaria Project Epilepsy Study (BMPES) of neurological outcomes in retinopathy-positive paediatric cerebral malaria survivors: a prospective cohort study. The Lancet Neurology 9: 1173–1181. pmid:21056005
  41. 41. Potchen MJ, Kampondeni SD, Mallewa M, Taylor TE, Birbeck GL (2013) Brain imaging in normal kids: a community-based MRI study in Malawian children. Tropical Medicine & International Health 18: 398–402.
  42. 42. Kail RV, Ferrer E (2007) Processing speed in childhood and adolescence: Longitudinal models for examining developmental change. Child Development 78: 1760–1770. pmid:17988319
  43. 43. Snijders T, Boske R (2012) Multilevel analysis: An introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling. London: Sage.
  44. 44. Munoz DP, Broughton JR, Goldring JE, Armstrong IT (1998) Age-related performance of human subjects on saccadic eye movement tasks. Experimental Brain Research 121: 391–400. pmid:9746145
  45. 45. Asato MR, Terwilliger R, Woo J, Luna B (2010) White matter development in adolescence: A DTI study. Cerebral Cortex 20: 2122–2131. pmid:20051363
  46. 46. Alahyane N, Brien DC, Coe BC, Stroman PW, Munoz DP (2014) Developmental improvements in voluntary control of behavior: Effect of preparation in the fronto-parietal network? NeuroImage 98: 103–117. pmid:24642280
  47. 47. Newsham D, Knox PC, Cooke RWI (2007) Oculomotor control in children who were born very prematurely. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 48: 2595–2601. pmid:17525189
  48. 48. Hutton SB, Ettinger U (2006) The anitsaccade task as a research tool in psychopathology: A critical review. Psychophysiology 43: 302–313. pmid:16805870
  49. 49. Newton CR, Taylor TE, Whitten RO (1998) Pathophysiology of fatal falciparum malaria in African children. The American Journal of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene 58: 673–683. pmid:9598460
  50. 50. Muntendam AH, Jaffar S, Bleichrodt N, van Hensbroek MB (1996) Absence of neuropsychological sequelae following cerebral malaria in Gambian children. Transactions of The Royal Society of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene 90: 391–394. pmid:8882184
  51. 51. Bangirana P, Opoka RO, Boivin MJ, Idro R, Hodges JS, Romero RA (2014) Severe malarial anemia is associated with long-term neurocognitive impairment. Clinical Infectious Diseases 59: 336–344. pmid:24771329
  52. 52. Beare NAV, Lewallen S, Taylor TE, Molyneux ME (2011) Redefining cerebral malaria by including malaria retinopathy. Future Microbiology 6: 349–355. pmid:21449844
  53. 53. Holding PA, Stevenson J, Peshu N, Marsh K (1999) Cognitive sequelae of severe malaria with impaired consciousness. Trans R Soc Trop Med Hyg 93(5): 529–34. pmid:10696414
  54. 54. Boivin MJ, Bangirana P, Byarugaba J, Opoka RO, Idro R, Jurek AM (2007) Cognitive impairment after cerebral malaria in children: a prospective study. Pediatrics 119(2): e360–6. pmid:17224457
  55. 55. Carter JA, Ross AJ, Neville BGR, Obiero E, Katana K, Mung’ala-Odera V (2005) Developmental impairments following severe falciparum malaria in children. Tropical Medicine & International Health 10: 3–10.
  56. 56. Kariuki SM, Abubakar A, Newton CR, Kihara M (2014) Impairment of executive function in Kenyan children exposed to severe falciparum malaria with neurological involvement. Malar J 13(1): 365
  57. 57. Carter JA, Neville BGR, White S, Ross AJ, Otieno G, Mturi N, et al. (2004) Increased prevalence of epilepsy associated with severe falciparum malaria in children. Epilepsia 45: 978–981. pmid:15270766
  58. 58. Ngoungou EB, Dulac O, Poudiougou B, Druet-Cabanac M, Dicko A, Mamadou Traore A, et al. (2006) Epilepsy as a consequence of cerebral malaria in area in which malaria Is endemic in Mali, West Africa. Epilepsia 47: 873–879.
  59. 59. John CC, Bangirana P, Byarugaba J, Opoka RO, Idro R, Jurek AM (2008) Cerebral Malaria in Children Is Associated With Long-term Cognitive Impairment. Pediatrics 122: e92–99. pmid:18541616
  60. 60. van Hensbroek MB, Palmer A, Jaffar S, Schneider G, Kwiatkowski D (1997) Residual neurologic sequelae after childhood cerebral malaria. The Journal of Pediatrics 131: 125–129. pmid:9255203
  61. 61. Boivin MJ, Gladstone MJ, Vokhiwa M, Birbeck GL, Magen JG, Page C, (2011) Developmental outcomes in Malawian children with retinopathy-positive cerebral malaria. Tropical Medicine & International Health 16: 263–271.
  62. 62. Rueda MR, Fan J, McCandliss BD, Halparin JD, Gruber DB, Lercari LP, et al. (2004) Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia 42: 1029–1040. pmid:15093142
  63. 63. Waszak F, Li S-C, Hommel B (2010) The development of attentional networks: Cross-sectional findings from a life span sample. Developmental Psychology 46: 337–349. pmid:20210494
  64. 64. Luna B, Garver KE, Urban TA, Lazar NA, Sweeney JA (2004) Maturation of cognitive processes from late childhood to adulthood. Child Development 75: 1357–1372. pmid:15369519
  65. 65. Bangirana P, Musisi S, Boivin M, Ehnvall A, John C, Bergemann TL (2011) Malaria with neurological involvement in Ugandan children: effect on cognitive ability, academic achievement and behaviour. Malaria Journal 10: 334 pmid:22047193
  66. 66. Idro R, Marsh K, John CC, Newton CRJ (2010) Cerebral Malaria: Mechanisms of brain injury and strategies for improved neurocognitive outcome. Pediatr Res 68: 267–274. pmid:20606600
  67. 67. Goldberg MC, Lasker AG, Zee DS, Garth E, Tien A, Landa RJ (2002) Deficits in the initiation of eye movements in the absence of a visual target in adolescents with high functioning autism. Neuropsychologia 40: 2039–2049. pmid:12208001
  68. 68. Reuter B, Herzog E, Kathmann N (2006) Antisaccade performance of schizophrenia patients: Evidence of reduced task-set activation and impaired error detection. Journal of Psychiatric Research 40: 122–130. pmid:16459170
  69. 69. Nieman DH, Bour LJ, Linszen DH, Goede J, Koelman JHTM, Gersons BPR, et al. (2000) Neuropsychological and clinical correlates of antisaccade task performance in schizophrenia. Neurology 54: 866–871. pmid:10690978
  70. 70. Amatya N, Gong Q, Knox PC (2011) Differing proportions of ‘express saccade makers’ in different human populations. Experimental Brain Research 210: 117–129. pmid:21374077
  71. 71. Knox PC, Amatya N, Jiang X, Gong Q (2012) Performance deficits in a voluntary saccade task in Chinese “express saccade makers”. PLOS One 7: e47688. pmid:23091639
  72. 72. Knox PC, Wolohan FDA (2014) Cultural diversity and saccade similarities: Culture does not explain saccade latency differences between Chinese and Caucasian participants. PLOS One 9: e94424. pmid:24709988
  73. 73. Banerjee TD, Middleton F, Faraone SV (2007) Environmental risk factors for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Acta Pædiatrica 96: 1269–1274. pmid:17718779
  74. 74. Bergen D (2008) Effects of poverty on cognitive function: A hidden neurologic epidemic. Neurology 71: 447–451. pmid:18678828