Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

GABAA receptor-expressing neurons promote consumption in Drosophila melanogaster

Abstract

Feeding decisions are highly plastic and bidirectionally regulated by neurons that either promote or inhibit feeding. In Drosophila melanogaster, recent studies have identified four GABAergic interneurons that act as critical brakes to prevent incessant feeding. These GABAergic neurons may inhibit target neurons that drive consumption. Here, we tested this hypothesis by examining GABA receptors and neurons that promote consumption. We find that Resistance to dieldrin (RDL), a GABAA type receptor, is required for proper control of ingestion. Knockdown of Rdl in a subset of neurons causes overconsumption of tastants. Acute activation of these neurons is sufficient to drive consumption of appetitive substances and non-appetitive substances and acute silencing of these neurons decreases consumption. Taken together, these studies identify GABAA receptor-expressing neurons that promote Drosophila ingestive behavior and provide insight into feeding regulation.

Introduction

The ability to adjust feeding behaviors in different environments and contexts is essential for an animal to survive. Homeostatic mechanisms that regulate food intake balance caloric consumption with energy expenditure, which influences the health, fitness, and body weight of an organism [1]. Homeostatic regulation requires the integration of internal nutritional state, external sensory signals, and associations with past food-related experiences.

The fruit fly, Drosophila melanogaster, is an excellent model system to examine the neural circuits underlying the regulation of feeding behaviors; the fly brain contains a million fold fewer neurons compared to the human brain[2] and powerful genetic tools enable the manipulation of specific neurons. These features make the fly brain a tractable system for uncovering the neural circuitry for feeding regulation. The circuit principles uncovered in Drosophila may be shared throughout the animal kingdom.

The fly has stereotypical feeding behaviors involving a series of feeding subprograms. Feeding behavior initially involves detection of a potential food source with the legs or proboscis, which contain taste receptors neurons that allow the fly to make an evaluation before attempting consumption [3]. Detection of sugars drives feeding initiation, whereas detection of bitter compounds promotes rejection. Once a palatable food source is detected, a fly initiates feeding by using the proboscis extension response (PER). As the fly consumes, it detects appetitive substances with taste neurons of the pharynx or internal mouthparts [3,4]. The fly continues to ingest until internal cues signal satiety.

Some taste-responsive neural components of these feeding subprograms have been uncovered. Distinct classes of gustatory sensory neurons detect either water, sugars, bitter compounds, or pheromones [58]. Gustatory neurons on the legs, proboscis labellum, and mouthparts send projections to non-overlapping regions in the subesophageal zone (SEZ), a taste-processing region in the brain [6,9]. Two sucrose-responsive classes of local interneurons in the SEZ play a role in sucrose consumption: feeding neurons (FDG) respond to sucrose detection on the proboscis to promote initial consumption of sucrose [10], while ingestion neurons (IN1), respond to sucrose detection in the pharynx to promote sustained sucrose consumption in hungry flies [11]. In addition, candidate second-order neurons that send their projections to the antennal mechanosensory and motor center (AMMC) are sucrose responsive and trigger sucrose acceptance [12]. Finally, SEZ motor neurons that are necessary for feeding initiation and consumption have been identified [13,14].

How are the circuits that regulate feeding modulated by internal state? Although several studies have identified modulatory neurons that promote feeding when the fly is food-deprived [15,16], very few have examined how feeding is terminated when the fly is sated. A recent study identified a set of four interneurons, named DSOG1, that play an essential role in inhibiting consumption [17]. These neurons are GABAergic neurons that send wide arborizations throughout the SEZ. Remarkably, flies with silenced DSOG1 neurons consume vast quantities of appetitive and non-appetitive substances, even under sated conditions. This suggests that DSOG1 acts to inhibit indiscriminate consumption. To examine how DSOG1 neurons suppress consumption, we searched for downstream neurons that might receive the GABA signals from DSOG1. Here, we characterize neurons that express RDL, a GABAA type receptor, that promote consumption, providing insight into the circuits that dynamically regulate feeding.

Results

Rdl is required for wild-type feeding behavior

Since DSOG1 neurons require GABA production to inhibit consumption [17], we reasoned that they may inhibit GABA-receptor expressing neurons that are sufficient to drive ingestive behaviors. We therefore tested whether reducing expression of GABA receptors influenced feeding. There are five GABAergic receptors in Drosophila: a heteromultimeric cationic channel composed of the subunits GABA and glycine-like receptor of Drosophila (GRD) and ligand-gated chloride channel homolog 3 (LCCH3) [18]; a GABAA type receptor (RDL) [19]; and 3 GABAB receptors (GABAB R1, GABAB R2, GABAB R3) [20]. To determine which GABAergic receptor(s) regulate consumption, we knocked down expression of the existing GABAergic receptors pan-neuronally using nSyb-Gal4[21] to drive RNA interference[22] and measured the effect on the duration of water and bitter substance consumption (Fig 1A and 1B, Table 1).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Rdl modulates consumption regulation.

A) Diagram illustrating the temporal consumption assay. The time individual flies consumed various substances was recorded. B) GABAergic receptors were tested for their role in water and bitter (1 mM denatonium) consumption using RNAi to knock down expression pan-neuronally with nSyb-Gal4; UAS-Dcr2 flies. Box plots show median consumption time for water consumption and 1 mM denatonium in fed flies. Different RNAi lines (different sequences targeting the same receptor) are denoted in (#). Knockdown of Rdl resulted in overconsumption of water and bitter. Kruskal-Wallis test, Dunn’s post-hoc; ***p<0.0001 indicates significance to UAS-empty RNAi controls for water or bitter consumption, as appropriate; n = 10-33/genotype. C) Screen for water overconsumption candidates (mean ± SEM); Gal4 lines were tested for their role in water consumption through RNAi of Rdl. Bars show consumption times (mean ± SEM) for water in fed flies. On the X-axis, each column represents a Gal4 line or wild-type (wt) lines. Gal4 lines were ordered from least to most consumption time. Arrows point to wild-type (IsoD1), VT16839-Gal4, and VT27941-Gal4. n = 10-22/genotype. D) Screen for bitter overconsumption candidates; Gal4 lines were tested for their role in bitter consumption through RNAi of Rdl. Bars show consumption times (mean ± SEM) for 1 mM denatonium in fed flies. On the X-axis, each column represents a Gal4 line or wild-type (wt). Gal4 lines are ordered corresponding to Fig 1C. Arrows point to wild-type (IsoD1), VT16839-Gal4, and VT27941-Gal4. The majority of Gal4 lines did not show consumption of denatonium. n = 10-22/genotype.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0175177.g001

thumbnail
Table 1. RNAi lines used for the GABAergic receptor screen.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0175177.t001

Wild-type flies that are hydrated and fed do not drink water or the bitter compound denatonium[17](Fig 1C and 1D), whereas flies with inactivated DSOG1 neurons consume a vast amount of water or denatonium[17]. As expected, control flies did not consume water or denatonium, (when they were hydrated and fed). In contrast, fed and hydrated flies with Rdl knocked down using two different RNA interference lines (Rdl(3) and Rdl (4)) that target different sequences of Rdl, significantly overconsumed both water and denatonium (Fig 1B), similar to the DSOG1 phenotype. Knocking down Rdl using the other two Rdl lines (1 and 2) did not result in a significantly different consumption time of water and bitter compared to all other lines. These behavioral differences likely reflect differences in the ability of different RNAi lines to effectively reduce gene expression. In contrast, knockdown of other GABAergic receptors resulted in little or no consumption of water or denatonium. Overconsumption of denatonium and water indicates that the drive to consume in these flies overrides normal bitter taste inhibition of feeding and water satiation, respectively. This suggests that the RDL receptor is the only GABAergic receptor required to restrict consumption of water and bitter substances in fed states.

Although RDL plays an important role in regulating consumption, it is broadly expressed in the nervous system and is not specific for neural circuits that mediate consumption. To screen for candidate RDL neurons that regulate consumption, we knocked down Rdl, using the line with the most robust overconsumption phenotype, UAS-Rdl(3), in small subsets of neurons within the central nervous system, using existing Gal4 lines from the Clandinin, Flylight, and Vienna collections[2326] and assayed for increased consumption of water or bitter in fed flies. Water consumption was variable, either because many neural subsets require RDL for water consumption or because of differences in genetic background. Nevertheless, RDL knockdown in neurons downstream of DSOG1 would be expected to have defects in both bitter and water consumption. Therefore, we selected lines that showed increased water and bitter consumption compared to wild-type and tested for reproducibility by comparing consumption time in fed flies with RDL knockdown to sibling controls (S1 Fig). Out of 183 Gal4 lines, we identified two Gal4 lines, VT16839-Gal4 and VT27941-Gal4 that showed robust and reproducible overconsumption of both denatonium and water compared to all other lines in the screen (Fig 1C and 1D and S1 Fig). Interestingly, the enhancer segment of VT27941 is a sequence mapping to the intronic region of Rdl.

To determine which neurons are contained in the Gal4 lines, we crossed them to UAS-mCD8::GFP reporter flies and examined GFP expression in the brain and ventral nerve cord. Expression of Gal4-driven mCD8::GFP in VT27941-Gal4 was broad throughout the central nervous system (data not shown), whereas the expression in VT16839-Gal4 was sparse. VT16839-Gal4 labels ~50 neurons in the brain and ~50 neurons in the ventral nerve cord (Fig 2A). We therefore focused on VT16839-Gal4 for further study.

thumbnail
Fig 2. VT16839-Gal4 neurons trigger and are critical in consumption behavior.

A) VT16839-Gal4 drives expression of UAS-mCD8::GFP in the brain and VNC (scale bar = 100 μm) B) RNA interference knockdown of Rdl in VT16839-Gal4 neurons increased consumption time of water, 1 mM denatonium, 500 mM sucrose and 20% PEG. Box plot shows fed UAS-Rdli; UAS-Dcr2/VT16839-Gal4 flies spent more time consuming water, 1 mM denatonium, and 500 mM sucrose compared to fed +; UAS-Dcr2/VT16839-Gal4 flies Wilcoxon rank-sum test with continuity correction; ***p<0.0001; n = 22-72/genotype C) Activation of VT16839-Gal4 neurons using ReaChR caused increased consumption time of appetitive (500 mM sucrose or water) and non-appetitive (1 mM denatonium) substances. Box plot shows fed VT16839-Gal4; UAS-ReaChR flies consumed more appetitive and non-appetitive substances compared to fed VT16839-Gal4 and UAS-ReaChR flies; Kruskal-Wallis test, Dunn’s post-hoc; ***p<0.0001; n = 46-49/genotype D) Silencing of VT16839-Gal4 neurons using shibirets (shi(ts)) decreased consumption of sucrose (500 mM) in flies starved for 24 hours. Box plot shows 24 hour starved W1118(wild-type); UAS-shi(ts) spent more time consuming 500 mM sucrose compared to starved VT16839-Gal4 /UAS-shi(ts) flies; Wilcoxon rank-sum test with continuity correction; ***p<0.0001; NS = not significant; n = 50-55/genotype; Permissive temperature = 19–21°, restrictive temperature = 30–33°

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0175177.g002

Rdl expression in VT16839 neurons regulate consumption

The phenotype of DSOG1-mediated overconsumption is independent of taste quality or nutritional state[17]. To test whether knockdown of Rdl in VT16839 neurons also causes overconsumption of nutritive and non-nutritive substances, we used RNA interference and assayed consumption. Knocking down Rdl in VT16839-Gal4 neurons resulted in fed flies that overconsumed denatonium, water, and sucrose compared to control flies (Fig 2B). These results suggest that levels of Rdl are critical for limiting ingestion of non-appetitive and appetitive tastants. Since Rdl in VT16839 neurons is essential to reduce consumption of nutritive and non-nutritive taste compounds, we wondered if it was also necessary to prevent consumption of compounds not detected by the gustatory system. We tested if the tasteless compound, 20% polyethylene glycol (PEG) was sufficient to drive overconsumption. 20% PEG is tasteless because its high osmolarity inhibits water-sensing neurons [5], and it is not detected by bitter and sweet sensory cells. Fed flies with Rdl knocked down in VT16839 neurons consumed significantly more 20% PEG compared to controls (Fig 2B), suggesting that the overconsumption phenotype is independent of taste inputs from sugar, bitter, or water taste neurons. Interestingly, overconsumption of 20% PEG was comparable to overconsumption of water, denatonium, and sucrose (Fig 2B). These results demonstrate that flies overconsume independent of taste quality when Rdl is knocked down in VT16839-Gal4 neurons, indicating that GABA signaling through the RDL receptor acts to limit consumption under normal conditions.

VT16839 neurons are necessary and sufficient for consumption

Knockdown of the GABA receptor, Rdl, in VT16839 neurons increased consumption, likely by decreasing inhibition and thus increasing activity of VT16839 neurons. Therefore, we hypothesized that directly increasing activity in VT16839 neurons might also promote consumption behavior. To test this, we acutely activated VT16839 neurons using UAS-ReaChR, an optically-gated non-specific cation channel [27]. We tested if these neurons could trigger consumption of tastants upon activation with 635 nm light. Activation of VT16839 neurons in fed flies resulted in overconsumption of not only sucrose and water but also denatonium (Fig 2C). In contrast, control flies that did not express UAS-ReaChR or VT16839-Gal4, showed little consumption. This argues that neurons in the VT16839-Gal4 line drive consumption likely downstream of taste quality checkpoints.

We tested whether VT16839 neurons are necessary for consumption by acutely silencing these neurons in starved flies, conditions where the fly had a strong drive to consume. If VT16839 neurons are necessary for consumption, we would expect to see a reduction in consumption when VT16839 neurons are silenced. We acutely silenced VT16839 neurons in starved flies using the temperature sensitive, dominant-negative dynamin, shibirets [28] which prevents synaptic vesicle recycling at 33°C but not at 22°C. Starved flies with VT16839 neurons silenced significantly reduced consumption of 500 mM sucrose compared to room temperature controls (Fig 2D). Taken together, these results demonstrate that VT16839 neurons are necessary and sufficient for consumption behavior.

VT16839 neurons are sufficient to suppress DSOG1 mediated overconsumption

DSOG1 neurons are GABAergic interneurons that inhibit consumption. VT16839 neurons are GABA-receptor expressing neurons that trigger consumption and are necessary for normal consumption. One hypothesis is that GABA release from DSOG1 binds to Rdl on VT16839 neurons to inhibit their activity and decrease consumption. To test whether VT16839 neurons are downstream of DSOG1 we performed an epistasis experiment by co-silencing the two populations of neurons. As expected, acutely silencing DSOG1 neurons in fed flies, using the 98-Gal4 to drive expression of shibirets, results in overconsumption of both appetitive and aversive substances ([17], Fig 3A). As VT16839 neurons might be downstream of DSOG1, we tested whether blocking synaptic transmission in VT16839 neurons would suppress the overconsumption phenotype. We co-silenced DSOG1 and VT16839 neurons in fed flies using UAS-shibirets and measured consumption. Fed flies with DSOG1 and VT16839 neurons co-silenced consumed significantly less sucrose compared to DSOG1 silenced flies but did not consume significantly different from DSOG1 genetic control (98-Gal4) (Fig 3A). In addition, co-silencing both populations of neurons also resulted in a significant suppression of the DSOG1-mediated consumption of denatonium. Co-silencing both populations resulted in significantly more denatonium consumption compared to the DSOG1 genetic control, which may be because the strength of the VT16839 driver may not be strong enough to completely overcome DSOG1-mediated denatonium consumption. These results demonstrate that silencing VT16839 neurons can override overconsumption caused by inhibition of DSOG1 neurons. These data are consistent with the notion that VT16839 neurons act downstream of DSOG1 neurons (Fig 3B), although they do not rule out the possibility of parallel pathways.

thumbnail
Fig 3. VT16839-Gal4 overrides DSOG1-mediated overconsumption.

A) Epistasis experiment. Co-silencing DSOG1 neurons (98-Gal4) and VT16839-Gal4 neurons in fed flies decreased consumption time. Box plot shows 98-Gal4; UAS-shi(ts)/16839-Gal4 fed flies spent less time consuming 1 mM denatonium and 500 mM sucrose compared to 98-Gal4; UAS-shi(ts) (DSOG1 neurons silenced) and 98-Gal4 fed flies. Kruskal-Wallis test, Dunn’s post-hoc; ***p<0.0001; n = 19-47/genotype B) Model. Silencing VT16839-Gal4 overrides the overconsumption phenotype caused from silencing DSOG1 neurons.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0175177.g003

A Subpopulation of VT16839 neurons trigger consumption

We sought to narrow down the causal neurons in VT16839-Gal4 that trigger consumption by using an intersectional approach to limit expression to subsets of VT16839 neurons. Because knocking down Rdl in either VT16839-Gal4 or VT27941-Gal4 resulted in overconsumption of water and denatonium, we decided to look for neurons present in both Gal4 lines. We made split-Gal4 driver lines where the promoter sequence of each Gal4 line was used to drive expression of a complementary half of Gal4 (VT16839-split-Gal4). Only neurons that have both halves of the Gal4 will reconstitute a functional Gal4 to drive expression of effector proteins. Therefore, this approach limits functional Gal4 expression to the neurons common to both driver lines [29]. As a result, functional Gal4 expression of mCD8::GFP was limited to ~10 neurons in the brain and ~16 in the VNC (Fig 4A). In the brain, there was a class of neurons strongly labeled in higher brain whose projections span near the pars intercerebralis along with several dimly labeled neurons in the SEZ. An additional two populations were labeled in the abdominal segment of the VNC.

thumbnail
Fig 4. A subset of VT16839-Gal4 neurons trigger consumption.

A) VT16839-Gal4-DBD, VT27941-Gal4-AD flies drives expression of UAS-mCD8::GFP in the brain and VNC (scale bar = 100 μm) B) Knockdown of Rdl in VT16839-split neurons resulted in increased consumption time. Box plot shows fed UAS-Rdli; UAS-Dcr2/VT16839-split-Gal4(split-Gal4) flies spent more time consuming water, 1 mM denatonium, and 500 mM sucrose, compared to UAS-empty attp40; UAS-Dcr2/VT16839-split-Gal4flies (split-Gal4) Wilcoxon rank-sum test with continuity correction; ***p<0.0001; n = 64-70/genotype C) Activation of VT16839-Gal4 neurons using csChrimson increased consumption volume of appetitive (500 mM sucrose or water) and non-appetitive (1 mM denatonium) substances. Box plot shows fed VT16839-split-Gal4(split-Gal4); UAS-csChrimson flies consume more volume of appetitive and non-appetitive substances; Kruskal-Wallis test, Dunn’s post-hoc; ***p<0.0001; n = 21-63/genotype

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0175177.g004

We tested whether the VT16839-split-Gal4 driver lines do indeed contain neurons that trigger consumption by knocking down Rdl using RNAi. Knockdown of Rdl in the VT16839-split-Gal4 resulted in overconsumption of both appetitive and aversive stimuli, indicating that neurons that promote consumption are labeled by the VT16839-split-Gal4 (Fig 4B). Similarly, we tested whether the neurons labeled by the VT16839-split-Gal4 can trigger consumption through acute activation experiments. Since activation using ReaChR resulted only in a small increase in consumption (S2 Fig), we used an alternative optically-gated cation channel, csChrimson [30] to activate the neurons upon red light exposure and measured consumption time. Activation caused the majority of flies to constitutively pump their proboscis (45 of 51 flies) in the absence of any stimulus. The constitutive pumping behavior may indicate a drive to consume that does not require a stimulus, suggesting that csChrimson strongly activates the neurons.

In the csChrimson experiments, consumption was masked by constitutive pumping, making it difficult to reliably measure consumption time. As a result, we decided to quantify consumption by measuring volume ingested instead of time spent drinking. We estimated volumes of ingestion by utilizing capillary tubes and measuring the volumes before and after individual flies drank. When VT16839-split-Gal4 neurons were activated using csChrimson, fed flies consumed more appetitive and aversive tastants in our volumetric consumption assay (Fig 4C) compared to control fed flies. This indicates that VT16839-split-Gal4 neurons trigger consumption of both appetitive and non-appetitive substances. The VT16839-split-Gal4 thus labels a small number of GABAergic receptor neurons that promote consumption. Furthermore, these RDL expressing neurons promote consumption independent of taste inputs.

Discussion

The dissection of neural circuits that underlie consumption remains an important challenge toward understanding the regulation of feeding behavior. Our study identifies neurons that regulate the consumption of non-appetitive and appetitive substances, and depend on the expression of RDL receptor for proper regulation of consumption. These RDL receptor-expressing neurons are able to orchestrate consumption regardless of taste quality, as knockdown of Rdl expression within these neurons not only causes overconsumption of sugar, bitter, and water substances, but tasteless substances as well. Acute activation of these neurons also caused overconsumption of sweet, bitter and water substances, whereas blocking neurotransmission of these neurons results in decreased sucrose consumption in starved flies. These studies reveal a subset of neurons that play a critical role in promoting consumption.

Previous studies have identified two different classes of interneurons that trigger sucrose consumption. FDG neurons are located in the SEZ and respond to sugar stimulation on the proboscis [10] and the cholinergic IN1 neurons respond to sugar stimulation of the internal mouthparts [11]. These two classes of neurons respond selectively to sucrose, suggesting that there is a pathway selective for regulating sucrose consumption. Similarly, ectopic activation of these neurons increased consumption of sucrose but not water or bitter. These studies indicate that consumption of sucrose is regulated independently of consumption of water or bitter and argue for distinct circuits mediating consumption for each class of tastant. The RDL-expressing neurons differ from previously identified consumption neurons because either knockdown of Rdl or optogenetic activation of these neurons elicited consumption not only of appetitive substances, but also of non-appetitive substances. One model suggested by these studies that bears testing is that there may be distinct circuits for sweet, water, and bitter food sources that all converge on the RDL-expressing neurons.

Knockdown of Rdl results in increased consumption of water, sucrose and bitter substances. These RDL neurons may be inhibited by GABAergic neurons such as DSOG1. Previous studies indicate that DSOG1 neurons act as a tonic inhibitor of consumption[17]. Flies with silenced DSOG1 neurons overconsume all taste substances independent of taste quality and nutritional state, very similar to the phenotype observed when activating the RDL neurons in this study. An attractive model is that GABA release from DSOG1 inhibits the RDL neurons, restricting consumption. Indeed, our studies show that RDL neuronal silencing is able to suppress the DSOG1-silencing phenotype. Although our data are consistent with the model that DSOG1 acts on the RDL neurons, it remains possible that the RDL neurons and DSOG1 influence parallel pathways. Further characterization of the RDL neurons that promote consumption and the DSOG1 neurons that inhibit consumption will enable us to distinguish these models.

Together, our study demonstrates that RDL function in a subset of neurons is critical for the regulation of consumption of all substances, regardless of taste modality. Further studies characterizing these neurons and their interactions with the different neurons that regulate feeding will provide insight into the temporal dynamics and plasticity in feeding decisions.

Materials and methods

Experimental animals

The Gal4 lines used for the behavioral screens were from the following collections: the Janelia Flylight collection [23], Vienna VT collection [25,26] and the Clandinin Gal4 collection [24]. GABAergic receptor RNAi lines were from the TRiP and VDRC collection [22]. The following fly lines were used: nSyb-Gal4 [21], UAS-ReachR (II) [27], UAS-csChrimson (X) [30], UAS-Shibirets [28]. UAS- Dicer2 (III) (BDSC #24651) was used in all RNA interference experiments. 98-Gal4 is from the Clandinin Gal4 collection, while VT16839-Gal4 and VT27941-Gal4 are from the Vienna VT collection.

For VT16839-split-Gal4: VT16839-Gal4 and VT27941-Gal4 enhancer segments were amplified using genomic DNA from IsoD1 flies. VT16839-Gal4 and VT27941-Gal4 enhancer segments were cloned into pBPZpGAL4DBDUw (Addgene Plasmid #26233) plasmid and pBPp65ADZpUw (Addgene Plasmid #26234) plasmid, respectively. Cloned plasmid were injected into attp2 (VT16839-Gal4-DBD) and vk00027 sites (VT27941-Gal4-AD) (BestGene).

Behavioral experiments

Female flies were tested for behavior 5–10 days after eclosion. All flies with the exception for UAS-shibirets flies (20°C) were grown at 25°C and flipped onto fresh food 2 days prior to the experiment. All starved experimental flies were flipped into vials with wet kimwipes for allotted starvation time. Flies were glued on slides with nail polish and then were placed 2–4 hours in a humidified chamber. Temporal consumption assays were performed as previously described [17]. Time spent consuming was recorded until flies rejected the substance ten times. For RNAi experiments, UAS-Dicer2 (III) was used to boost efficiency of the knockdown. For Figs 1C, 1D, 2, 3 and 4, UAS-Rdl-RNAi (3) (Vienna# 41103) line was used to knock down Rdl.

For activation experiments using ReaChR and csChrimson, experimental flies were kept on 0.4 mM all-trans retinal (Sigma), 2–4 days prior to the experiment. Activation experiments were done under 635 nm light (LaserGlow) and assayed one fly at a time. Flies were habituated under the laser for 1–2 minute prior to assay. For shibirets experiments, mounted flies were incubated at 30–32°C on a peltier device and given water for 10–15 min prior to experiment.

For volumetric consumption assays, Drummond Wiretrol capillaries (CAT #5-000-1003) were filled with 5 μl of tastant mixed with blue dye (0.25mg/mL blue dye (Erioglaucine, Sigma)) and attached to tubing connected to a 1 ml syringe. One edge of the capillary was painted with vasoline to prevent spilling of tastant. Before and after pictures were taken in the presence of a standard ruler to correct for zoom. Difference in pixels before and after was measured using Photoshop. A standard pixel-to-volume conversion factor was calculated by pipetting volumes into various pipettes and creating a graph with a linear fit (S3 Fig). Volume was calculated by converting the pixels to mm lost before and after behavioral assay based on the standard pixel-to volume conversion factor.

Statistics

Data from Figs 1, 2, 3 and 4 are all non-parametric, therefore non-parametric statistical tests were used. When multiple samples were being compared, Kruskal-Wallis test with Dunn’s post-hoc was used (Figs 1B, 2C, 3A and 4C). When two samples were being compared, Wilcox rank-sum test was used with continuity correction (Figs 2B, 2D and 4B, and S1 and S2 Figs).

Immunohistochemistry

Antibody fixation and staining was performed as previously described[17].The following antibodies were used: rabbit anti-GFP (Invitrogen, 1:1000); mouse anti-nc82 (Hybriodoma bank 1:500). The following secondary antibodies were used (Invitrogen at 1:100): 488 anti-rabbit, 568 anti-mouse. All images were taken using a Zeiss confocal microscope. Brightness and contrast was adjusted for images using ImageJ.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. VT16839-Gal4 and VT27941-Gal4 lines show increased water consumption with Rdl knockdown.

Knockdown of Rdl in candidate Gal4 lines. Bar graph showing time consumption of water (mean ± SEM) in fed flies. Candidate Gal4 lines were tested against sibling flies (no RNAi) for reproducibility of overconsumption phenotype for water. Both VT16839-Gal4 and VT27941-Gal4 showed robust and reproducibility of overconsumption; Wilcoxon rank-sum test with continuity correction;**p<0.01;***p<0.0001 n = 11-35/genotype

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0175177.s001

(TIF)

S2 Fig. Activation of VT16839-split-Gal4 neurons result in a small increase in consumption.

Bar plot shows (mean ± SEM) activation of VT16839-split-Gal4 caused small increases in 1 mM denatonium and water consumption time in UAS-ReaChR; VT16839-split-Gal4 fed flies compared to VT16839-split-Gal4 fed flies. Wilcoxon rank-sum test with continuity correction; ***p<0.001; n = 27-31/genotype

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0175177.s002

(TIF)

S3 Fig. Pixel-to-volume conversion factor in the volumetric consumption assay.

Scatter plot shows the pixels to volume when standard volumes (5 ul and 1 ul) were pipetted into capillaries. A curve was fit linearly to find the factor (y = 0.0058x) to convert the pixels changes to volume changes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0175177.s003

(TIF)

Acknowledgments

The authors thank members of the Scott lab for critical discussions. This work was supported by a grant to the NIDDK (R01DK098747) to KS.

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: SKC KS.
  2. Formal analysis: SKC.
  3. Funding acquisition: KS.
  4. Investigation: SKC.
  5. Methodology: SKC.
  6. Project administration: SKC KS.
  7. Resources: KS.
  8. Supervision: SKC KS.
  9. Validation: SKC.
  10. Visualization: SKC.
  11. Writing – original draft: SKC.
  12. Writing – review & editing: SKC KS.

References

  1. 1. Guh DP, Zhang W, Bansback N, Amarsi Z, Birmingham CL, Anis AH. The incidence of co-morbidities related to obesity and overweight: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Public Health. 2009;9: 88. pmid:19320986
  2. 2. Edwards TN, Meinertzhagen I a. The functional organisation of glia in the adult brain of Drosophila and other insects. Prog Neurobiol. Elsevier Ltd; 2010;90: 471–97. pmid:20109517
  3. 3. Stocker RF. The organization of the chemosensory system in Drosophila melanogaster: a review. Cell Tissue Res. 1994;275: 3–26. pmid:8118845
  4. 4. LeDue EE, Chen Y-C, Jung AY, Dahanukar A, Gordon MD. Pharyngeal sense organs drive robust sugar consumption in Drosophila. Nat Commun. Nature Publishing Group; 2015;6: 6667. pmid:25807033
  5. 5. Cameron P, Hiroi M, Ngai J, Scott K. The molecular basis for water taste in Drosophila. Nature. Nature Publishing Group; 2010;465: 91–5. pmid:20364123
  6. 6. Wang Z, Singhvi A, Kong P, Scott K. Taste representations in the Drosophila brain. Cell. 2004;117: 981–91. pmid:15210117
  7. 7. Thistle R, Cameron P, Ghorayshi A, Dennison L, Scott K. Contact chemoreceptors mediate male-male repulsion and male-female attraction during drosophila courtship. Cell. Elsevier Inc.; 2012;149: 1140–1151. pmid:22632976
  8. 8. Thorne N, Chromey C, Bray S, Amrein H. Taste Perception and Coding in Drosophila. Curr Biol. 2004;14: 1065–1079. pmid:15202999
  9. 9. Marella S, Fischler W, Kong P, Asgarian S, Rueckert E, Scott K. Imaging taste responses in the fly brain reveals a functional map of taste category and behavior. Neuron. 2006;49: 285–95. pmid:16423701
  10. 10. Flood TF, Iguchi S, Gorczyca M, White B, Ito K, Yoshihara M. A single pair of interneurons commands the Drosophila feeding motor program. Nature. Nature Publishing Group; 2013;499: 83–87. pmid:23748445
  11. 11. Yapici N, Cohn R, Schusterreiter C, Ruta V, Vosshall LB. A Taste Circuit that Regulates Ingestion by Integrating Food and Hunger Signals. Cell. Elsevier Inc.; 2016;165: 715–729. pmid:27040496
  12. 12. Kain P, Dahanukar A. Secondary Taste Neurons that Convey Sweet Taste and Starvation in the Drosophila Brain. Neuron. Elsevier Inc.; 2015;85: 819–832. pmid:25661186
  13. 13. Gordon MD, Scott K. Motor control in a Drosophila taste circuit. Neuron. Elsevier Ltd; 2009;61: 373–84. pmid:19217375
  14. 14. Manzo A, Silies M, Gohl DM, Scott K. Motor neurons controlling fluid ingestion in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2012; 6307–6312. pmid:22474379
  15. 15. Marella S, Mann K, Scott K. Dopaminergic Modulation of Sucrose Acceptance Behavior in Drosophila. Neuron. Elsevier Inc.; 2012;73: 941–950. pmid:22405204
  16. 16. Jourjine N, Mullaney BC, Mann K, Scott K. Coupled Sensing of Hunger and Thirst Signals Balances Sugar and Water Consumption. Cell. Elsevier Inc.; 2016;166: 855–866. pmid:27477513
  17. 17. Pool A-H, Kvello P, Mann K, Cheung SK, Gordon MD, Wang L, et al. Four GABAergic interneurons impose feeding restraint in Drosophila. Neuron. Elsevier Inc.; 2014;83: 164–77. pmid:24991960
  18. 18. Gisselmann G, Plonka J, Pusch H, Hatt H. Drosophila melanogaster GRD and LCCH3 subunits form heteromultimeric GABA-gated cation channels. Br J Pharmacol. 2004;142: 409–13. pmid:15148245
  19. 19. Buckingham SD, Hosie AM, Roush RL, Sattelle DB. Actions of agonists and convulsant antagonists on a Drosophila melanogaster GABA receptor (Rdl) homo-oligomer expressed in Xenopus oocytes. Neurosci Lett. 1994;181: 137–140. pmid:7898754
  20. 20. Mezler M, Müller T, Raming K. Cloning and functional expression of GABA(B) receptors from Drosophila. Eur J Neurosci. 2001;13: 477–86. pmid:11168554
  21. 21. Pauli A, Althoff F, Oliveira RA, Heidmann S, Schuldiner O, Lehner CF, et al. Cell-Type-Specific TEV Protease Cleavage Reveals Cohesin Functions in Drosophila Neurons. Dev Cell. Elsevier Inc.; 2008;14: 239–251. pmid:18267092
  22. 22. Dietzl G, Chen D, Schnorrer F, Su K-C, Barinova Y, Fellner M, et al. A genome-wide transgenic RNAi library for conditional gene inactivation in Drosophila. Nature. 2007;448: 151–6. pmid:17625558
  23. 23. Jenett A, Rubin GM, Ngo TTB, Shepherd D, Murphy C, Dionne H, et al. A GAL4-Driver Line Resource for Drosophila Neurobiology. Cell Rep. 2012;2: 991–1001. pmid:23063364
  24. 24. Gohl DM, Silies M, Gao XJ, Bhalerao S, Luongo J. F, Lin C-C, et al. A versatile in vivo system for directed dissection of gene expression patterns. Nat Methods. 2011;8: 231–237. pmid:21473015
  25. 25. Dickson BJ. VT collection, unpublished.
  26. 26. Kvon EZ, Kazmar TK, Stampfel GS, Yanez-Cuna JO, Pagani M, Schernhuber K, et al. Genome-scale functional characterization of Drosophila developmental enhancers in vivo. Nature. 2014;512: 91–95. pmid:24896182
  27. 27. Inagaki HK, Jung Y, Hoopfer ED, Wong AM, Mishra N, Lin JY, et al. Optogenetic control of Drosophila using a red-shifted channelrhodopsin reveals experience-dependent influences on courtship. Nat Methods. 2014;11: 325–32. pmid:24363022
  28. 28. Kitamoto T. Conditional modification of behavior in Drosophila by targeted expression of a temperature-sensitive shibire allele in defined neurons. J Neurobiol. 2001;47: 81–92. pmid:11291099
  29. 29. Pfeiffer BD, Ngo TTB, Hibbard KL, Murphy C, Jenett A, Truman JW, et al. Refinement of tools for targeted gene expression in Drosophila. Genetics. 2010;186: 735–755. pmid:20697123
  30. 30. Klapoetke NC, Murata Y, Kim SS, Pulver SR, Birdsey-Benson A, Cho YK, et al. Independent optical excitation of distinct neural populations. Nat Methods. 2014;11: 338–46. pmid:24509633