Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Cognitive control moderates parenting stress effects on children's diurnal cortisol

  • Laurel Raffington,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany, Center for Lifespan Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany, Institute of Medical Psychology, Charité—Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany

  • Florian Schmiedek,

    Roles Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Center for Lifespan Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany, German Institute for International Educational Research (DIPF), Frankfurt am Main, Germany

  • Christine Heim,

    Roles Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany, Institute of Medical Psychology, Charité—Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany, Department of Biobehavioral Health, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, United States of America

  • Yee Lee Shing

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    shing@psych.uni-frankfurt.de

    Affiliations Center for Lifespan Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany, Division of Psychology, University of Stirling, Stirling, United Kingdom, Department of Psychology, Goethe University Frankfurt, Frankfurt am Main, Germany

Abstract

This study investigated associations between parenting stress in parents and self-reported stress in children with children's diurnal cortisol secretion and whether these associations are moderated by known stress-regulating capacities, namely child cognitive control. Salivary cortisol concentrations were assessed from awakening to evening on two weekend days from 53 6-to-7-year-old children. Children completed a cognitive control task and a self-report stress questionnaire with an experimenter, while parents completed a parenting stress inventory. Hierarchical, linear mixed effects models revealed that higher parenting stress was associated with overall reduced cortisol secretion in children, and this effect was moderated by cognitive control. Specifically, parenting stress was associated with reduced diurnal cortisol levels in children with lower cognitive control ability and not in children with higher cognitive control ability. There were no effects of self-reported stress in children on their cortisol secretion, presumably because 6-to-7-year-old children cannot yet self-report on stress experiences. Our results suggest that higher cognitive control skills may buffer the effects of parenting stress in parents on their children’s stress regulation in middle childhood. This could indicate that training cognitive control skills in early life could be a target to prevent stress-related disorders.

Introduction

In middle childhood, parent-reported stress related to their parenting [13] and children’s self-reported impact of negative life events [46] have been shown to be viable indicators of children’s stress experiences. Research investigating the effects of stress in childhood has sought to better understand ‘how stress gets under the skin’ [7,8]. This is especially important in the developmental literature, because the developing brain is disproportionally more vulnerable to the adverse effects of stress than the adult brain [9]. Animal models have shown that glucocorticoid (GC) steroid hormones are causally affected by stress exposure, showing patterns of GC elevations or depression in response to stress, depending on the age of stressor exposure and presence of maternal care [1013]. In humans, the endocrine stress response is mediated by the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, of which the GC cortisol is the end product. Cortisol secretion follows a diurnal rhythm with levels increasing starkly in the first hour upon awakening (cortisol awakening response; CAR) and decreasing linearly throughout the rest of the day (diurnal slope) [14]. Cortisol secretion mobilizes energy, suppresses the immune system, and helps the organism to adapt to stress [15], but comes at a cost with long-term activation [15]. Befitting cortisol secretion is necessary for optimal adaption to stress and both abnormally high and low cortisol levels have adverse effects on health [16]. Thus, HPA axis alterations in childhood and adolescence have been suggested to be a risk factor for later illness and psychiatric disorders although results have been somewhat inconsistent [6,17,18]. For instance, a recent meta-analysis found a significant association between flatter diurnal cortisol slopes and poorer health across multiple health domains [19].

A growing literature has linked environmental stressors to children’s cortisol secretion. Yet, commonly encountered stressors have been associated with both higher [2022] and lower basal cortisol levels [2327]. However, these studies have been methodologically limited by disregarding known diurnal secretory patterns in measuring a few serial ‘basal’ cortisol levels at differing times of the day. Furthermore, basal cortisol measures show considerably lower intra-individual stability than multiple measures of diurnal cortisol secretion collected across several days [28,29]. For example, confounding variables that influence cortisol levels, such as food intake or acute experiences of stress, affect basal cortisol levels more than multiple response measures. Recent recommendations have been made to improve collection and to control for confounds (see [30,31] for an expert consensus paper on measuring the CAR). Nevertheless, these basal cortisol secretion studies provide initial evidence for the notion that stressors affect children’s HPA axis functioning.

Measurement of more reliable dynamic cortisol secretion patterns has focused primarily on severe stress exposure, such as maltreatment [32,33] and early institutionalized care [34,35], finding flattened diurnal cortisol secretory patterns. One hypothesis is that flatter cortisol patterns may follow a phase of hyperreactivity of the HPA axis that leads to the down-regulation of the HPA axis to protect the developing system from overexposure to GC [12,36,37]. A small number of studies on less severe stress exposure indicate that higher parenting stress is associated with flatter diurnal cortisol profiles in 3-to-6-year-old children [38] and 9-to-12-year olds [39], but higher late morning cortisol levels in 3-to-5-year-old children [26]. Parents remain co-regulators of children’s cortisol stress responses to acute challenges into middle childhood [40], however stress renders parents less able to effectively co-regulate their child’s stress response [41,42] and affects parenting behaviors that can create stress for the child [1,2,43,44]. Lastly, parent-child interactions can in some cases elicit a cortisol stress response depending on the relationship quality [45]. Overall, these studies suggest that parenting stress in the parents is an important predictor of the child’s HPA axis function, although it is unclear in what way the diurnal cortisol secretion pattern is affected. Potentially, higher parenting stress could be associated with blunted diurnal cortisol secretion in children, mirroring effects of severe stress exposure.

Arguably a more valid measure of children’s stress experiences would be to evaluate children’s self-reported stress perception. The large majority of studies have focused on the effects of objective stressors (e.g., poverty) on stress physiology, disregarding the child’s subjective stress perception. Yet, stress occurs when an individual perceives an inability to cope with the demands of the environment [46]. The few studies to date suggest that higher self-reported stress perceptions in these multiple domains are associated with lower cortisol levels after awaking in 9-to-12-year-olds [4] and flatter diurnal cortisol slopes in 9-to-17-year-old children [5]. Furthermore, self-reported negative life events predicted the onset of depression in 9-to-14-year-old girls with higher levels of total cortisol levels [6]. Thus, daily stress perception may increase the risk for later psychiatric disorders by HPA axis dysregulation. In sum, it is not yet sufficiently established whether higher stress perceptions are associated with reductions or increases in diurnal cortisol secretion in childhood and whether higher or lower levels confer more health risk. Furthermore, the lack of correlation between parental and child stress reports may imply that young children are not reliable self-reporters of their stress experiences [4750], in contrast to children over 9 years [46]. We know of no studies investigating self-reported stress and HPA axis activity focusing on middle childhood as a further test of the validity of using young children’s stress reports.

Importantly, not all children exposed to stress show HPA axis alterations [51]. This may be related to issues of reliability in basal cortisol measurements [29] or derive from moderation effects [51,52]. Psychological vulnerability and resilience factors are thought to play a vital modulatory role in the embedding of stress exposure [53]. For example, executive functions and prefrontal cortex (PFC) function have been suggested to protect against the development of behavioral problems and developmental disorders [54]. Additionally, a very large recent study suggests that high effortful control, low negative affect, and low emotional reactivity mitigated the negative associations between socioeconomic risk (marked by high stress level) and both reading and math development [55]. Therefore, executive functions may act as a moderator of the embedding of stress exposure, with high executive function abilities providing protective or resilience effects.

While no study to date has tested for executive function moderation effects on children’s cortisol secretion, previous research shows that behavioral self-regulation is correlated with cortisol secretion at daycare in early childhood [5659]. For instance, preschool children with poorer self-control and more aggression showed greater increases in cortisol over the course of the day spent at daycare, suggesting that immature self-regulatory skills may stimulate cortisol elevations among young children [56]. Self-regulation is largely considered a cognitive regulatory skill and depends on executive functions including working memory, attention, and cognitive control, which can be defined here as top-down control of goal-directed action [60,61]. Indeed, the association of self-regulation and cortisol secretion was accounted for by an executive functions composite, including cognitive control [59]. Developmentally, cognitive control shows marked improvement in middle childhood [62,63] that intersects with functioning in multiple health, behavioral and physiological outcomes across the lifespan [6467]. Furthermore, cognitive control abilities are reflected in PFC development [68], which is also critically involved in providing feedback control to the HPA axis in regulating cortisol secretion [69]. Therefore, we hypothesize that cognitive control may act as a moderator of the effects of stress exposure on HPA axis activity. This may partly explain mixed results associating cortisol levels with executive function that have found both higher [4,20,26,56,70] and lower cortisol levels [7173] associated with outcomes of poorer executive functions.

To test this hypothesis, we investigated whether parenting stress and self-reported stress are associated with children’s diurnal cortisol secretion and whether this relationship is moderated by cognitive control capacity in middle childhood. First, we expected higher parenting stress and higher self-reported stress to be associated with flattened diurnal cortisol levels and a flatter diurnal slope, mirroring findings on previous diurnal cortisol studies and severe stress samples. Second, we predicted lower cognitive control to be associated with lower cortisol level. Thirdly, we expected higher cognitive control to buffer the negative effects of stress on diurnal cortisol secretion.

Methods

Participants

Participants were 53 children and their parents (29 female; age: M = 6.68; SD = .41) who took part in a longitudinal neuroimaging study (called the HippoKID study) that examined neurocognitive development with functional and structural magnetic resonance imaging. Data for the current study, other than the cognitive task, was collected only at the second time point of data collection (one year after baseline). Children were recruited at daycare centers and were either attending first grade or daycare during time point two testing. Strict health and behavioral exclusion criteria were specified, involving prenatal and perinatal histories, medical and psychiatric disorders, and history of steroid medication use. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the German Psychological Society (Deutsche Gesellschaft für Psychologie). All parents of participants provided written consent and children verbal assent.

Procedure

Distributed across three sessions, computerized cognitive testing as well as scanning was completed individually in the presence of a trained experimenter at the laboratory. While participants were being tested, parents waited in a separate room and filled out demographic and parenting stress questionnaires. During session two, after cognitive testing, the experimenter read out items in the children’s self-report stress questionnaire and the child pointed to smiley faces corresponding to the response options. At the end of session two, parents were trained in collecting saliva samples from their children. Saliva samples were stored in parents’ home freezer and picked up by an experimenter within three weeks.

Parents were asked to collect samples on two weekend days to estimate stable trait-like profiles [29], since children tend to spend more time with their families on weekends thus maximizing potential effects of family and minimizing differences in daycare versus school attendance. Mean number of days between the collection days was 4.71 (SD = 6.45, range 1–28). Because the children were too young to collect their own samples, parents were asked to wake their child shortly before the time they would normally wake up to reduce the risk of prior awakening and take the saliva sample right away. Being woken-up has been shown not to affect the CAR in adults [74]. Following samples were taken 30 minutes after awakening, at 12:00, and right before dinner to minimize effects due to eating large meals [75]. The strict adherence to the study protocol, especially regarding the timing of sampling, was especially emphasized. Parents were trained to use a timer alarming them of sampling times. It was also stressed that saliva sampling had to be postponed to the next possible weekend day if the child had woken up spontaneously or fell ill. Parents were told to withhold food, drinks, and brushing teeth prior to the sample 30 minutes after awakening and to withhold meals and caffeine 2 hours prior to the 12:00 and pre-dinner samples. They were also asked to fill in a protocol recording sampling times and any problems.

Saliva samples were stored in parents’ home freezer and picked up by an experimenter within three weeks. They were then stored at -80°C until assayed. Samples were brought to room temperature and centrifuged at 3000 rpm for 15 minutes. All samples were assayed for cortisol concentrations at the Institute of Medical Psychology at Charité—Universitätsmedizin Berlin using a highly sensitive enzyme immunoassay (Salimetrics, Suffolk, UK). The test has a detection range from 0,012 μg/dL– 3 μg/dL with a lower sensitivity limit of 0,007 μg/dL and average intra- and inter-assay coefficients of variation were 6.64% and 4.19%, respectively. There were no cortisol samples below detection limit. All samples were assayed in duplicate and the average of duplicates was used in all analyses.

Measures

Cortisol levels

Cortisol collection and pre-processing steps followed recommendations made by an expert consensus as far as possible [31]. Time between first and second collections ranged from 20 to 40 minutes (M = 31.08, SD = 1.92). Given the rapid change in morning cortisol levels, this is not ideal even though every parent reported to be within a 15-minute compliance window [76]. Therefore, preliminary analyses were conducted to identify outliers in cortisol concentrations and values more than 4 SD from the mean and were ‘winsorized’, i.e. replaced with the value at the 99.7th percentile [77]. This affected six samples from Day 1 and two samples from Day 2. Missing samples (one from Day 1 and one from Day 2) were replaced by the mean of that sample. Because cortisol measures displayed skewness and kurtosis, a log transformation was applied to these concentrations after winsorizing to normalize their distributions and meet assumptions for statistical analyses. Cortisol values and collection times since awakening in minutes were mean-centered for statistical modeling. Raw cortisol values showed moderate correlations across time (see Table 1).

thumbnail
Table 1. Raw cortisol values, covariates and their correlations.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191215.t001

Parenting stress inventory

The Parenting Stress Inventory [78] is a widely used questionnaire that assesses stress as a consequence of parental role. Five subscales measure perceived stress due to child characteristics (Distractibility/Hyperactivity, Adaptability, Demandingness, Mood, Acceptability) and seven subscales measure parental characteristics and situational variables (Competence, Isolation, Attachment, Health, Role Restriction, Spouse/Parenting Partner Relationship, Depression). The validated German version of the questionnaire [79] was filled out by the parent who generally spent more time with the child and with the participating child in mind. Parents responded to 48 items on a 6-point scale ranging from strongly agree to (0) strongly disagree (5). One missing sample was mean replaced. The total score, which has good reliability (α = .91) [78], was divided by number of subscales (normally 12 subscales, but only 11 if the parent has no partner) and mean-centered for statistical modeling. Higher scores indicate increased parenting stress.

Children’s stress questionnaire

The Children’s Stress Questionnaire has 50 items in five subscales and has been found to be reliable and valid in an Australian sample of children aged 7-to-9-years [80]. Although the full scale shows good reliability (α = .91), some of the subscales’ reliabilities are not so high (α < .60), suggesting that some caution may be needed using these scales [80]. Nevertheless, its validity has been established longitudinally by predicting scales of depression and anxiety [80]. Our analyses were restricted to the full scale. Five subscales measure pervasive hassles beyond normal control (every-day events such as not having enough money to buy things), relationship with parents (e.g., parents prefer siblings), experiences of transition (e.g., death of a family member), problems in school/daycare (e.g., bullying), and family dissonance (e.g., parental divorce). The questionnaire was translated into German by a Native German-English bilingual and then translated back into English by another Native German-English bilingual to highlight divergences. Disagreements were resolved through discussion. For each item, children pointed to the smiley corresponding to their self-reported impact, visualized as a crossed-out face (0 = ‘This did not happen to me’), neutral face (1 = ‘This happened, but did not affect me’), a very slight frown (2 = ‘This made me a little bit sad/angry’), a frown (3 = ‘This made me somewhat sad/angry’), a more exaggerated frown (4 = ‘This made me very sad/angry’) concerning their stressor experience over the past year. Raw scores were log transformed to correct for significant positive skew (W = 1.86, p < 0.05) and mean-centered for statistical modeling.

Cognitive control

Cognitive control and flexibility was assessed using the Hearts and Flowers task [62]. On each trial, a heart (as the congruent stimulus) or a flower (as the incongruent stimulus) appeared on either the right or left side of a computer screen. The task started with a fixed order of blocks, starting from congruent, incongruent, to mixed blocks. In the congruent block, only trials with the heart stimulus were shown and the child was instructed to press the button on the same side as the heart. In the incongruent condition, only trials with the flower stimulus were shown and children were told to press the button opposite the flower. In the mixed block, trials with flower or heart were randomly shown, and the child was asked to press the congruent button when the heart appears and the opposite button when the flower appears. Each test block was preceded by a practice block, which was continued until the child achieved at least 62,5% correct. Stimuli were presented for 1500 ms, with an interstimulus interval of 500 ms. Incorrect responses or response latency less than 200 ms were excluded in the analysis. The variables of interest were mean accuracy and mean response latency on correct trials in the mixed condition divided by accuracy/latency on correct trials in the congruent condition. One missing sample was mean replaced. The latency outcome variable was multiplied by -1 so that a higher score indicated better cognitive control response speed and both variables were mean-centered for statistical modeling. See Table 2 for descriptive statistics of main variables and their correlation.

thumbnail
Table 2. Descriptive statistics of stress and cognitive control and their correlations.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191215.t002

Data analysis

As the first step, a model of cortisol secretion was estimated using mixed effects hierarchical modeling, which has several strengths compared to more common repeated-measures ANOVA and difference scores reviewed elsewhere [31,81,82]. For example, the number of cortisol outcome variables and thereby the number of statistical comparisons is significantly reduced. In these hierarchical cortisol models [34,40,77], Level 1 represents repeated cortisol samples as a function of time, which are nested within subjects at Level 2. Our modeling was informed by previous studies that have shown that the combination of a linear time term (i.e., time since awakening), a quadratic time term (i.e., time since awakening–squared), and an imposed peak value at 30 min post-awakening provide good fit to diurnal cortisol data [77,83]. The linear time term represents the instantaneous rate of cortisol change and the quadratic time term represents the cortisol trajectory curvature. The peak term is a dummy variable indicating whether the value was the second sample or not to superimpose upon the quadratic model the typically observed peak elevation 30 minutes after awakening [77,83]. The intercept, linear time, and quadratic time terms were entered as random effects consecutively and retained if they improved model fit. Entering linear time, quadratic time, and peak variables without interactions best fit the data and adding random effects for intercept and linear time terms and their covariance significantly improved fit. Those random effects capture between-person differences in the overall cortisol level (intercept term) and cortisol trajectory over time.

In a second step, we explored whether potential covariates significantly predicted cortisol secretion by adding them consecutively to the model and testing for interactions with time. None of the covariates (age, gender, awakening times, week/weekend day, number of days between samples, school versus daycare attendance) showed significant main effects or interactions with time variables.

In a third step, we added stress variables (parenting stress and self-reported stress in separate models) as main fixed effects, and explored their interactions with time variables, only retaining significant effects. We finally added cognitive control, separately for response speed and accuracy, exploring both main effects and interactions with time and stress variables.

Models were implemented using the lme4 package [84] for linear mixed modeling in R and compared via likelihood ratio tests. Significance (at the α level of .05) of individual parameter estimates were evaluated by comparing the log-likelihoods of the full model and models leaving out the corresponding effect using χ2 tests. Significant effects were bootstrapped with 5000 iterations and we report bootstrapped confidence intervals (CI). Specific values for the parameter estimates were used to follow up on significant interactions and interpret directionality of effects through simple slope analysis [85].

Finally, to aid comparability to other studies and following recommendations by Stalder and colleagues [31], we further describe effects of stress on the CAR and diurnal slope by running linear regression models on the first cortisol level at awakening, the mean morning increase (0–30 min delta), and the diurnal slope (30 min–evening delta). These models averaged outcome variables across the two days and included awakening time and time since awakening as covariates.

Results

Cognitive control response speed

We first tested whether parenting stress and cognitive control speed, as well as their interaction, was related to cortisol secretion in hierarchical models (see Tables 1 and 2 for descriptive statistics of main variables of interest and their correlations). Confirming the first hypothesis, the negative main effect of parenting stress on cortisol secretion was significant, suggesting reduced diurnal cortisol level with higher parenting stress (see Fig 1; see Table 3 for parameter estimates). There was a significant negative main effect of cognitive control response speed, which contradicts the second hypothesis. There were no significant interactions of stress or cognitive control variables with linear, quadratic, or peak time effects. However, there was a significant parenting stress x cognitive control response speed interaction (see Fig 2A). For children with lower cognitive control, higher parenting stress was related to reduced diurnal cortisol level (Simple slope at Z = -1: -2.19, t(50) = 3.61, p < 0.05), whereas for children with higher cognitive control, parenting stress was not related to cortisol level (Simple slope at Z = 1: -0.08, t(50) = 0.11, p = 0.91).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Diurnal cortisol levels in children with parents of high or low parenting stress.

Mean raw diurnal cortisol levels in children of parents with high (+1 SD) and low (-1 SD) parenting stress. Actual models included parenting stress as a continuous variable. Higher parenting stress was associated with lower total cortisol levels over the day and there were no significant time-sensitive effects.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191215.g001

thumbnail
Fig 2. Cognitive control moderates parenting stress effects on diurnal cortisol levels.

Relationship between parenting stress and diurnal cortisol levels in children with high and low cognitive control, indicated by response speed (A) and accuracy (B).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191215.g002

thumbnail
Table 3. Parameter estimates and model indices for cortisol secretion with parenting stress and cognitive control response speed as predictors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191215.t003

Next, we examined whether self-reported stress and cognitive control speed, as well as their interaction, was related to cortisol secretion in hierarchical models. Contrary to our hypothesis, self-reported stress was not related to cortisol secretion. There were no significant interactions of self-reported stress with linear, quadratic, or peak time effects and no significant self-reported stress x cognitive control response speed interaction (see Table 4).

thumbnail
Table 4. Parameter estimates and model indices for cortisol secretion with self-reported stress and cognitive control response speed as predictors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191215.t004

Cognitive control accuracy

Subsequently, we tested whether parenting stress and cognitive control accuracy, as well as their interaction, was related to cortisol secretion in hierarchical models. Again contradicting the second hypothesis, there was no main effect of cognitive control accuracy on cortisol levels (see Table 5 for parameter estimates). There were no significant interactions of stress or cognitive control variables with linear, quadratic, or peak time effects. However, as in the response speed model, there was a significant parenting stress x cognitive control accuracy interaction (see Fig 2B). For children with lower cognitive control accuracy, higher parenting stress was related to reduced diurnal cortisol levels (Simple slope at Z = -1: -3.12, t(50) = 2.81, p < 0.05), whereas for children with higher cognitive control, parenting stress was not related to cortisol levels (Simple slope at Z = 1: 0.10, t(50) = 0.15, p = 0.88).

thumbnail
Table 5. Parameter estimates and model indices for cortisol secretion with parenting stress and cognitive control accuracy as predictor.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191215.t005

Next, we examined whether self-reported stress and cognitive control accuracy, as well as their interaction, was related to cortisol secretion in hierarchical models. Contrary to our hypothesis and mirroring speed results, self-reported stress was not related to cortisol secretion. There were no significant interactions of self-reported stress with linear, quadratic, or peak time effects and no significant self-reported stress x cognitive control response accuracy interaction (see Table 6).

thumbnail
Table 6. Parameter estimates and model indices for cortisol secretion with self-reported stress and cognitive control response accuracy as predictors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191215.t006

In addition, we further investigated effects of parenting stress on diurnal cortisol secretion using more common CAR and diurnal slope indices in linear regression models to aid comparability to other studies. Parenting stress was associated with a significantly lower cortisol level at awakening (β = -0.04, SE = 0.02, CI = -0.09 - -0.01, p < 0.05), but not mean morning increase (0–30 min delta) (β = -0.03, SE = 0.02, CI = -0.08–0.01 p = 0.17). Further, parenting stress was associated with a flatter diurnal slope (β = 0.07, SE = 0.02, CI = 0.02–0.12, p < 0.05). However, this was reduced to a non-significant trend (β = -0.04, SE = 0.02, CI = -0.01–0.10, p = 0.07), when including the significant effect of cortisol level at awakening (β = -0.05, SE = 0.17, CI = -0.80 –-0.10, p < 0.05) [31]. Therefore, the conservative interpretation of our data derived from converging these regression results with the lack of time-sensitive effects in the hierarchical model is that parenting stress is associated with total cortisol levels starting with lower morning cortisol levels that persist to be lower over the rest of the day, but is not significantly associated with the CAR or diurnal slopes.

Discussion

This study investigated associations between parenting stress and self-reported stress with children's diurnal cortisol secretion, and whether this is moderated by children’s cognitive control. Partially confirming our first hypothesis, higher parenting stress, but not self-reported stress, was associated with lower diurnal cortisol levels. Additionally, we found that cognitive control moderated this relationship, such that higher parenting stress was associated with reduced diurnal cortisol levels only for children with lower cognitive control.

First, our finding that prepubescent children’s cortisol secretion under parenting stress is profiled by a reduction in total diurnal cortisol secretion is in line with other studies finding total output reductions and flatter diurnal slopes [5,38,39,86], although we observed no reliable time-sensitive differences in CAR or diurnal slope. We speculate that we did not replicate time-sensitive slope differences over the day, because our sample had limited stress variance and a moderate sample size. Nevertheless, relative to a large norming sample of German mothers [79], parenting stress in our study ranged from the <2 to <98th percentile with the mean falling into the 62nd percentile. Since total cortisol levels has been shown to have the highest level of stability compared to the CAR and diurnal slope [29,87], this may explain why parenting stress is most reliably associated with total output in our study. However, another study found a positive association with higher late morning cortisol levels in 3-to-5-year-old children being related to higher parenting stress [26]. Critically, these children were younger and morning cortisol secretion was measured at daycare, where the parents are not present and parental presence seems to moderate cortisol secretion in young children and rodents [12,40]. Therefore, the null effect of parenting stress on CAR and diurnal slope should be interpreted cautiously. Future studies should measure diurnal cortisol secretion in children and adolescents on 4 days to enhance reliability including bedtime levels [29].

The reduction of diurnal cortisol secretion we and others have found in middle and later childhood is in line with patterns seen in children living in severe instances of stress, such as maltreatment [32,88]. The mechanisms involved in reduced diurnal cortisol secretion are not clear. Potentially, low cortisol levels are the result of sustained increases in cortisol secretion in response to chronic stress that result in flattened diurnal rhythm to reduce neural damage compensating for overexposure to cortisol [12,15,16,89]. It remains to be established whether cumulative chronic stress and heightened chronic cortisol levels [9093] precede flattened diurnal cortisol secretion in more stressed children, a chronic stress mechanism that has garnered some support in mediating higher and lower basal cortisol levels [94,95]. Especially aspects of early childhood caregiving may be operative in shaping HPA axis activity in middle childhood [96], which may further explain findings of higher cortisol levels in younger children [26].

Contrary to our first hypothesis and unlike previous studies, we found no effects of self-reported stress of children on cortisol secretion [46]. One major difference between these studies and the present study is that the children in our sample were several years younger and may not yet be reliable reporters of their stress experiences. Self-reported stress was on the lower end of the scale—the mean was approximately 20 points lower than the 7-9-year-old Australian sample with which the questionnaire was devised [80]—and did not correlate with parenting stress similar to previous studies [4750]. Given the positive skew in the scale, this indicates that 6-to-7-year-old children cannot report their stress experiences or that there is little variance in stress perceptions. However, even children aged 7–15 years have reported lower perceived stress compared to adults in response to the same acute stress task despite comparable cortisol reactivity levels [97]. Thus, the association of stress perception on diurnal cortisol secretion in childhood is likely to be dependent on age and should be distinguished from parental reports.

Second, the association of cognitive control and cortisol secretion was inconsistent. While lower cognitive control response speed was associated with higher, not lower, cortisol, cognitive control accuracy was not associated with cortisol at all. This mirrors the mixed literature with some studies suggesting a negative cortisol and executive functions association [4,20,26,56,70] and other studies finding a positive association, as well as null associations within these studies [26]. Therefore, the literature on executive functions and cortisol is mixed. Since not all children exposed to stress show HPA axis alterations or develop cognitive deficits, these mixed results may in part stem from lacking a consideration of moderation effects.

In support of this notion and confirming the third hypothesis, we found that cognitive control modified the relationship between parenting stress and diurnal cortisol. In particular, higher parenting stress was associated with reduced diurnal cortisol output in children with lower cognitive control, but not in children with higher cognitive control. Our results suggest that higher cognitive control skills may buffer the effects of parenting stress on their children’s stress physiology in middle childhood. Presumably, higher cognitive control skills are related to higher emotional self-regulation [59], which enables these children to ward off stress responses otherwise transferred by their parents. These finding suggests that executive functions need to also be considered as moderators, not just outcomes, of stress physiology in the developmental stress literature. Even more so, if cognitive control is both a moderator and outcome of chronic stress exposure, this could indicate a snowball effect of psychological vulnerability leading to worse cognition, such that worse cognitive control facilitates the embedding of chronic stress, leading to worse cognitive control and so on. Cross-lagged longitudinal studies could test for such bidirectional dynamics. Given the role of HPA axis in health, these results have larger implications for the risk of psychiatric and health disorders [6]. Therefore, investigating psychological vulnerability and resilience factors to stress is an important area of ongoing research [53]. An important challenge will be to relate such cognitive moderators to genetic effects, such as those hypothesized to make some individuals more susceptible to their environments.

This study has several limitations. The cross-sectional nature of the data did not allow for cause-effect inferences to be made concerning parenting stress affecting child diurnal cortisol secretion/levels or vice versa. It is also possible that children with lower diurnal cortisol and cognitive control may increase their parents’ stress, because of behavioral difficulties; or that those children share genes with their parents that reduces diurnal cortisol secretion, making them both more stress prone. While we know of no longitudinal HPA axis studies investigating bidirectional effects with parenting stress, a related longitudinal study found that child emotion regulation and externalizing behaviors predicted parenting stress longitudinally at 2, 4, and 5 years [98]. Thus, bidirectional influences of children’s emotion regulation, HPA axis and parenting are plausible. Alternatively, both higher parenting stress and lower diurnal cortisol could derive from genetic similarities between children and their parents that influence stress reactivity. More longitudinal research is needed to understand the lead-lag interplay of children’s HPA axis functioning and their parents’ stress, preferably in consideration of heritability.

Second, small variations in timing can lead to large differences in cortisol levels, which is why the use of objective measures of awakening times using movement trackers and sampling times using electronic time monitoring devices have been recommended for use, but were not available at the present data collection [31,99]. Finally, this study focused on healthy children in a rather small sample and excluded children with diagnosed psychiatric disorders; hence our results are not readily generalizable to wider populations and possibly revealed only the lower bound of associations of stress and HPA axis functioning.

In conclusion, our study showed that higher parenting stress was associated with lower diurnal cortisol output in 6-to-7-year-old children. This effect was moderated by cognitive control, suggesting that children with lower cognitive control had reduced diurnal cortisol output under high parenting stress, whereas children with higher cognitive control showed no associations of cortisol and parenting stress. There were no effects of self-reported stress on cortisol secretion. In conclusion, cognitive control is an important individual difference characteristic to be considered in future studies that potentially modulates the effects of parenting stress on child’s stress physiology. Importantly, executive function and self-regulation is modifiable to interventions, especially in at-risk children, thus training these skills could reduce the occurrence of stress-related disorders.

Supporting information

S1 Data. Anonymized raw data text file of variables of interest.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191215.s001

(TXT)

S2 Data. Variable key and description of raw data text file S1 Data.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191215.s002

(TXT)

Acknowledgments

The study was supported by a Minerva Research Group to YLS from the Max Planck Society. Laurel Raffington was supported by the Berlin School of Mind and Brain Graduate Fellowship.

References

  1. 1. Murgatroyd CA, Nephew BC. Effects of early life social stress on maternal behavior and neuroendocrinology. Psychoneuroendocrinology. Elsevier Ltd; 2013;38: 219–228. pmid:22770862
  2. 2. Finegood ED, Blair C. Poverty, parent stress, and emerging executive functions in young children. In: Deater-Deckard K, Panneton R, editors. Parental Stress and Early Child Development. Cham: Springer; 2017. pp. 181–207. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-55376-4
  3. 3. Gershoff ET, Aber JL, Raver CC, Lennon MC. Income is not enough: Incorporating material hardship into models of income associations with parenting and child development. Child Dev. 2007;78: 70–95. pmid:17328694
  4. 4. Maldonado EF, Fernandez FJ, Trianes MV, Wesnes K, Petrini O, Zangara A, et al. Cognitive performance and morning levels of salivary cortisol and alpha-amylase in children reporting high vs. low daily stress perception. Span J Psychol. 2008;11: 3–15. pmid:18630643
  5. 5. Wolf JM, Nicholls E, Chen E. Chronic stress, salivary cortisol, and α-amylase in children with asthma and healthy children. Biol Psychol. 2008;78: 20–28. pmid:18243483
  6. 6. LeMoult J, Ordaz SJ, Kircanski K, Singh MK, Gotlib IH. Predicting first onset of depression in young girls: Interaction of diurnal cortisol and negative life events. J Abnorm Psychol. 2015;124: 850–859. pmid:26595472
  7. 7. McEwen BS. Brain on stress: How the social environment gets under the skin. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2012;109: 17180–17185. pmid:23045648
  8. 8. Lupien SJ, King S, Meaney MJ, McEwen BS. Can poverty get under your skin? Basal cortisol levels and cognitive function in children from low and high socioeconomic status. Dev Psychopathol. 2001;13: 653–676. pmid:11523853
  9. 9. Knudsen EI, Heckman JJ, Cameron JL, Shonkoff JP. Economic, neurobiological, and behavioral perspectives on building America’s future workforce. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103: 10155–10162. pmid:16801553
  10. 10. Meaney MJ. Maternal care, gene expression, and the transmission of individual differences in stress reactivity across generations. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24: 1161–1192. pmid:11520931
  11. 11. Cadet R, Pradier P, Dalle M, Delost P. Effects of prenatal maternal stress on the pituitary adrenocortical reactivity in guinea-pig pups. J Dev Physiol. 1986;8: 467–475. pmid:3031149
  12. 12. Levine S. The ontogeny of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. The influence of maternal factors. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2006;746: 275–288.
  13. 13. Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim CM. Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10: 434–445. pmid:19401723
  14. 14. Wüst S, Federenko I, Hellhammer DH, Kirschbaum C. Genetic factors, perceived chronic stress, and the free cortisol response to awakening. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2000;25: 707–720. pmid:10938450
  15. 15. Danese A, McEwen BS. Adverse childhood experiences, allostasis, allostatic load, and age-related disease. Physiol Behav. Elsevier Inc.; 2012;106: 29–39. pmid:21888923
  16. 16. Gunnar MR, Quevedo K. The neurobiology of stress and development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2007;58: 145–173. pmid:16903808
  17. 17. Carnegie R, Araya R, Ben-Shlomo Y, Glover V, O’Connor TG, O’Donnell KJ, et al. Cortisol awakening response and subsequent depression: prospective longitudinal study. Br J Psychiatry. 2014;204: 137–143. pmid:24311550
  18. 18. Guerry JD, Hastings PD. In Search of HPA Axis Dysregulation in Child and Adolescent Depression. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2011;14: 135–160. pmid:21290178
  19. 19. Adam EK, Quinn ME, Tavernier R, McQuillan MT, Dahlke KA, Gilbert KE. Diurnal cortisol slopes and mental and physical health outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology. Elsevier Ltd; 2017;83: 25–41. pmid:28578301
  20. 20. Blair C, Granger DA, Willoughby M, Mills-Koonce R, Cox M, Greenberg MT, et al. Salivary cortisol mediates effects of poverty and parenting on executive functions in early childhood. Child Dev. 2011;82: 1970–1984. pmid:22026915
  21. 21. Lupien SJ, King S, Meaney MJ, McEwen BS. Child’s stress hormone levels correlate with mother’s socioeconomic status and depressive state. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;48: 976–980. pmid:11082471
  22. 22. Chen E, Cohen S, Miller GE. How low socioeconomic status affects 2-year hormonal trajectories in children. Psychol Sci. 2010;21: 31–37. pmid:20424019
  23. 23. Badanes LS, Watamura SE, Hankin BL. Hypocortisolism as a potential marker of allostatic load in children: Associations with family risk and internalizing disorders. Dev Psychopathol. 2011;23: 881–896. pmid:21756439
  24. 24. Chen E, Paterson LQ. Neighborhood, family, and subjective socioeconomic status: How do they relate to adolescent health? Heal Psychol. 2006;25: 704–714. pmid:17100499
  25. 25. Kliewer W, Reid-Quinones K, Shields BJ, Foutz L. Multiple risks, emotion regulation skill, and cortisol in low-income African American youth: A prospective study. J Black Psychol. 2008;35: 24–43.
  26. 26. Wagner SL, Cepeda I, Krieger D, Maggi S, D’Angiulli A, Weinberg J, et al. Higher cortisol is associated with poorer executive functioning in preschool children: The role of parenting stress, parent coping and quality of daycare. Child Neuropsychol. 2016;22: 853–869. pmid:26335047
  27. 27. Pagliaccio D, Luby JL, Bogdan R, Agrawal A, Gaffrey MS, Belden AC, et al. Stress-system genes and life stress predict cortisol levels and amygdala and hippocampal volumes in children. Neuropsychopharmacology. Nature Publishing Group; 2014;39: 1245–1253. pmid:24304824
  28. 28. Schmidt-Reinwald A, Pruessner JC, Hellhammer DH, Federenko I, Rohleder N, Schürmeyer TH, et al. The cortisol response to awakening in relation to different challenge tests and a 12-hour cortisol rhythm. Life Sci. 1999;64: 1653–1660. pmid:10328525
  29. 29. Rotenberg S, McGrath JJ, Roy-Gagnon M-H, Tu MT. Stability of the diurnal cortisol profile in children and adolescents. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37: 1981–1989. pmid:22658393
  30. 30. Smyth N, Hucklebridge F, Thorn L, Evans P, Clow A. Salivary cortisol as a biomarker in social science research. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2013;7: 605–625.
  31. 31. Stalder T, Kirschbaum C, Kudielka BM, Adam EK, Pruessner JC, Wüst S, et al. Assessment of the cortisol awakening response: Expert consensus guidelines. Psychoneuroendocrinology. Elsevier Ltd; 2015;63: 414–432. pmid:26563991
  32. 32. Peckins MK, Susman EJ, Negriff S, Noll J, Trickett PK. Cortisol profiles: A test for adaptive calibration of the stress response system in maltreated and nonmaltreated youth. Dev Psychopathol. 2015;27: 1461–1470. pmid:26535937
  33. 33. Bernard K, Butzin-Dozier Z, Rittenhouse J, Dozier M. Cortisol production patterns in young children living with birth parents vs children placed in foster care following involvement of child protective services. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2010;164: 438–443. pmid:20439794
  34. 34. Quevedo K, Johnson AE, Loman MM, LaFavor TL, Gunnar MR. The confluence of adverse early experience and puberty on the cortisol awakening response. Int J Behav Dev. 2012;36: 19–28. pmid:22383860
  35. 35. Koss KJ, Hostinar CE, Donzella B, Gunnar MR. Social deprivation and the HPA axis in early development. Psychoneuroendocrinology. Elsevier Ltd; 2014;50: 1–13. pmid:25150507
  36. 36. Gunnar MR, Fisher PA. Bringing basic research on early experience and stress neurobiology to bear on preventive interventions for neglected and maltreated children. Dev Psychopathol. 2006;18: 651–677. pmid:17152395
  37. 37. Koss KJ, Mliner SB, Donzella B, Gunnar MR. Early adversity, hypocortisolism, and behavior problems at school entry: A study of internationally adopted children. Psychoneuroendocrinology. Elsevier Ltd; 2016;66: 31–38. pmid:26773398
  38. 38. Fisher PA, Stoolmiller M. Intervention effects on foster parent stress: Associations with child cortisol levels. Dev Psychopathol. 2008;20: 1003–1021. pmid:18606041
  39. 39. Martin CG, Kim HK, Fisher PA. Differential sensitization of parenting on early adolescent cortisol: Moderation by profiles of maternal stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology. Elsevier Ltd; 2016;67: 18–26. pmid:26859701
  40. 40. Hostinar CE, Johnson AE, Gunnar MR. Parent support is less effective in buffering cortisol stress reactivity for adolescents compared to children. Dev Sci. 2015;18: 281–297. pmid:24942038
  41. 41. Halberstadt AG, Crisp VW, Eaton KL. Family expressiveness: A retrospective and new directions for research. Editions de la Maison des Sciences de l’Homme; 1999.
  42. 42. Sroufe LA. Emotional development: The organization of emotional life in the early years [Internet]. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.; 1996. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511527661
  43. 43. Evans GW. The environment of childhood poverty. Am Psychol. 2004;59: 77–92. pmid:14992634
  44. 44. McLoyd VC. The impact of economic hardship on Black families and children: Psychological distress, parenting, and socioemotional development. Child Dev. 1990;61: 311. pmid:2188806
  45. 45. Gunnar MR, Talge NM, Herrera A. Stressor paradigms in developmental studies: What does and does not work to produce mean increases in salivary cortisol. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34: 953–967. pmid:19321267
  46. 46. Adler NE, Boyce T, Chesney MA, Cohen S, Folkman S, Kahn RL, et al. Socioeconomic status and health: The challenge of the gradient. Am Psychol. 1994;49: 15–24. pmid:8122813
  47. 47. Weissman MM, Orvaschel H, Padian N. Children’s symptom and social functioning self-report scales comparison of mothers’ and children’s reports. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1980;168: 736–740. pmid:7452212
  48. 48. Bögels SM, van Oosten A, Muris P, Smulders D. Familial correlates of social anxiety in children and adolescents. Behav Res Ther. 2001;39: 273–287. pmid:11227809
  49. 49. Caster JB, Inderbitzen HM, Hope D. Relationship between youth and parent perceptions of family environment and social anxiety. J Anxiety Disord. 1999;13: 237–251. pmid:10372340
  50. 50. Tein J-Y, Roosa MW, Michaels M. Agreement between parent and child reports on parental behaviors. J Marriage Fam. 1994;56: 341.
  51. 51. Del Giudice M, Ellis BJ, Shirtcliff EA. The Adaptive Calibration Model of stress responsivity. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. Elsevier Ltd; 2011;35: 1562–1592. pmid:21145350
  52. 52. LaPrairie JL, Heim CM, Nemeroff C. The neuroendocrine consequence of early life trauma. In: Lanius RA, Vermetten E, Pain C, editors. The impact of early life trauma on health and disease: The hidden epidemic. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 2010. pp. 247–256.
  53. 53. Heim CM, Newport DJ, Mletzko T, Miller AH, Nemeroff CB. The link between childhood trauma and depression: Insights from HPA axis studies in humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2008;33: 693–710. pmid:18602762
  54. 54. Johnson MH. Executive function and developmental disorders: the flip side of the coin. Trends Cogn Sci. Elsevier Ltd; 2012;16: 454–457. pmid:22835639
  55. 55. Wang Y, Zhang L, Kong X, Hong Y, Cheon B, Liu J. Pathway to neural resilience: Self-esteem buffers against deleterious effects of poverty on the hippocampus. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016;37: 3757–3766. pmid:27239046
  56. 56. Dettling AC, Gunnar MR, Donzella B. Cortisol levels of young children in full-day childcare centers: Relations with age and temperament. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1999;24: 519–536. pmid:10378239
  57. 57. Dettling AC, Parker SW, Lane S, Sebanc A, Gunnar MR. Quality of care and temperament determine changes in cortisol concentrations over the day for young children in childcare. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2000;25: 819–836. pmid:10996476
  58. 58. Gunnar MR, Tout K, de Haan M, Pierce S, Stansbury K. Temperament, social competence, and adrenocortical activity in preschoolers. Dev Psychobiol. 1997;31: 65–85. pmid:9222117
  59. 59. Blair C, Granger DA, Razza RP. Cortisol reactivity is positively related to executive function in preschool children attending Head Start. Child Dev. 2005;76: 554–567. pmid:15892778
  60. 60. Gross JJ, Thompson R. Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In: Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of Emotion Regulation. New York: Guilford Press; 2007. pp. 3–24.
  61. 61. Ochsner KN, Gross JJ. The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9: 242–249. pmid:15866151
  62. 62. Davidson MC, Amso D, Anderson LC, Diamond A. Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44: 2037–2078. pmid:16580701
  63. 63. Wright A, Diamond A. An effect of inhibitory load in children while keeping working memory load constant. Front Psychol. 2014;5: 1–9.
  64. 64. Koenen KC, Moffitt TE, Roberts AL, Martin LT, Kubzansky L, Harrington H, et al. Childhood IQ and adult mental disorders: A test of the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Am J Psychiatry. 2009;166: 50–57. pmid:19047325
  65. 65. Deary IJ. Why do intelligent people live longer? Nature. 2008;456: 175–176. pmid:19005537
  66. 66. Deary IJ, Weiss A, Batty GD. Intelligence and personality as predictors of illness and death: How researchers in differential psychology and chronic disease epidemiology are collaborating to understand and address health inequalities. Psychol Sci Public Interes. 2010;11: 53–79. pmid:26168413
  67. 67. Briley DA, Tucker-Drob EM. Comparing the developmental genetics of cognition and personality over the life span. J Pers. 2017;85: 51–64. pmid:26045299
  68. 68. Luna B, Padmanabhan A, O’Hearn K. What has fMRI told us about the development of cognitive control through adolescence? Brain Cogn. Elsevier Inc.; 2010;72: 101–113. pmid:19765880
  69. 69. Dedovic K, Duchesne A, Andrews J, Engert V, Pruessner JC. The brain and the stress axis: the neural correlates of cortisol regulation in response to stress. Neuroimage. Elsevier Inc.; 2009;47: 864–71. pmid:19500680
  70. 70. Watamura SE, Donzella B, Kertes DA, Gunnar MR. Developmental changes in baseline cortisol activity in early childhood: Relations with napping and effortful control. Dev Psychobiol. 2004;45: 125–133. pmid:15505801
  71. 71. Bäumler D, Kliegel M, Kirschbaum C, Miller R, Alexander N, Stalder T. Effect of a naturalistic prospective memory-related task on the cortisol awakening response in young children. Biol Psychol. Elsevier B.V.; 2014; 59–61. pmid:25149142
  72. 72. Zalewski M, Lengua LJ, Fisher PA, Trancik A, Bush NR, Meltzoff AN. Poverty and single parenting: Relations with preschoolers’cortisol and effortful control. Infant Child Dev. 2012;21: 537–554.
  73. 73. Davis EP, Bruce J, Gunnar MR. The anterior attention network: Associations with temperament and neuroendocrine activity in 6-year-old children. Dev Psychobiol. 2002;40: 43–56. pmid:11835150
  74. 74. Stalder T, Hucklebridge F, Evans P, Clow A. Use of a single case study design to examine state variation in the cortisol awakening response: Relationship with time of awakening. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34: 607–614. pmid:19059727
  75. 75. de Kloet ER. Brain corticosteroid receptor balance and homeostatic control. Front Neuroendocrinol. 1991;12: 95–164.
  76. 76. Broderick JE, Arnold D, Kudielka BM, Kirschbaum C. Salivary cortisol sampling compliance: Comparison of patients and healthy volunteers. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004;29: 636–650. pmid:15041086
  77. 77. Hostinar CE, Johnson AE, Gunnar MR. Early social deprivation and the social buffering of cortisol stress responses in late childhood: An experimental study. Dev Psychol. 2015;51: 1597–1608. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000029 pmid:26322485
  78. 78. Abidin R. Parenting Stress Index (PSI). Charlottesville, VA: Pediatric Psychology Press; 1990.
  79. 79. Tröster H. Eltern-Belastungs-Inventar (EBI). Hogrefe; 2011.
  80. 80. Byrne DG, Thomas KA, Burchell JL, Olive LS, Mirabito NS. Stressor experience in primary school-aged children: Development of a scale to assess profiles of exposure and effects on psychological well-being. Int J Stress Manag. 2011;18: 88–111.
  81. 81. Lehman BJ, Kirsch JA, Jones DR. Effectively analyzing change over time in laboratory research on stress and health: A multilevel modeling approach. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2015;10: 551–566.
  82. 82. Llabre MM, Spitzer S, Siegel S, Saab PG, Schneiderman N. Applying latent growth curve modeling to the investigation of individual differences in cardiovascular recovery from stress. Psychosom Med. 2004;66: 29–41. pmid:14747635
  83. 83. Hostinar CE, Sullivan RM, Gunnar MR. Psychobiological mechanisms underlying the social buffering of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis: a review of animal models and human studies across development. Psychol Bull. 2014;140: 256–82. pmid:23607429
  84. 84. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Package “Ime4” [Internet]. 2015. Available: http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4/lme4.pdf.
  85. 85. Aiken L, West S. Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1991.
  86. 86. Martin CG, Kim HK, Bruce J, Fisher PA. Child diurnal cortisol rhythms, parenting quality, and externalizing behaviors in preadolescence. Psychoneuroendocrinology. Elsevier Ltd; 2014;40: 170–180. pmid:24485489
  87. 87. Ross KM, Murphy MLM, Adam EK, Chen E, Miller GE. How stable are diurnal cortisol activity indices in healthy individuals? Evidence from three multi-wave studies. Psychoneuroendocrinology. Elsevier Ltd; 2014;39: 184–193. pmid:24119668
  88. 88. Blair C, Berry DJ, Mills-Koonce WR, Granger DA. Cumulative effects of early poverty on cortisol in young children: Moderation by autonomic nervous system activity. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38: 2666–2675. pmid:23890719
  89. 89. Fries E, Dettenborn L, Kirschbaum C. The cortisol awakening response (CAR): Facts and future directions. Int J Psychophysiol. Elsevier B.V.; 2009;72: 67–73. pmid:18854200
  90. 90. Vaghri Z, Guhn M, Weinberg J, Grunau RE, Yu W, Hertzman C. Hair cortisol reflects socio-economic factors and hair zinc in preschoolers. Psychoneuroendocrinology. Elsevier Ltd; 2013;38: 331–340. pmid:22809790
  91. 91. Flom M, St. John AM, Meyer JS, Tarullo AR. Infant hair cortisol: Associations with salivary cortisol and environmental context. Dev Psychobiol. 2017;59: 26–38. pmid:27472986
  92. 92. Tucker-Drob EM, Grotzinger AD, Briley DA, Engelhardt LE, Mann FD, Patterson M, et al. Genetic influences on hormonal markers of chronic hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal function in human hair. Psychol Med. 2017;47: 1389–1401. pmid:28100283
  93. 93. Rippe RCA, Noppe G, Windhorst DA, Tiemeier H, van Rossum EFC, Jaddoe VWV, et al. Splitting hair for cortisol? Associations of socio-economic status, ethnicity, hair color, gender and other child characteristics with hair cortisol and cortisone. Psychoneuroendocrinology. Elsevier Ltd; 2016;66: 56–64. pmid:26773401
  94. 94. Suor JH, Sturge-Apple ML, Davies PT, Cicchetti D, Manning LG. Tracing differential pathways of risk: Associations among family adversity, cortisol, and cognitive functioning in childhood. Child Dev. 2015;86: 1142–1158. pmid:26081792
  95. 95. Blair C, Raver CC, Granger DA, Mills-Koonce WR, Hibel LC. Allostasis and allostatic load in the context of poverty in early childhood. Dev Psychopathol. 2011;23: 845–857. pmid:21756436
  96. 96. Blair C, Raver CC. Child development in the context of adversity: Experiential canalization of brain and behavior. Am Psychol. 2012;67: 309–318. pmid:22390355
  97. 97. Yim IS, Quas JA, Rush EB, Granger DA, Skoluda N. Experimental manipulation of the Trier Social Stress Test-Modified (TSST-M) to vary arousal across development. Psychoneuroendocrinology. Elsevier Ltd; 2015;57: 61–71. pmid:25885544
  98. 98. Williford AP, Calkins SD, Keane SP. Predicting change in parenting stress across early childhood: Child and maternal factors. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2007;35: 251–263. pmid:17186365
  99. 99. Bäumler D, Kirschbaum C, Kliegel M, Alexander N, Stalder T. The cortisol awakening response in toddlers and young children. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38: 2485–92. pmid:23768972