Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Effect of socioeconomic status on behavioral problems from preschool to early elementary school – A Japanese longitudinal study

  • Rikuya Hosokawa ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft

    rikuya@med.nagoya-cu.ac.jp

    Affiliations School of Nursing, Nagoya City University, Nagoya, Japan, Graduate School of Medicine, Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan

  • Toshiki Katsura

    Roles Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Graduate School of Medicine, Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan

Abstract

Purpose

Social inequalities are widely accepted to have a deleterious effect on children’s mental health, and those with lower socioeconomic status generally experience more mental health issues. In this study, we examine the impact of socioeconomic situations of children’s families during their early childhood on the children’s social adaptation in Japanese elementary school.

Methods

The current investigation consisted of two sets of data relating to two separate years (with a one-year interval). The participants included preschoolers aged five years at Time 1 (the first year) and first graders aged six years at Time 2 (the second year); 1,712 met the inclusion criteria for both years. Parents of the participants completed a self-reported questionnaire regarding their SES (i.e., family economy and mother’s education) and their children’s mental health. Mental health was assessed using the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18, Parent Report.

Results

For each SES indicator, we found an inverse relationship across all the symptom dimensions. Specifically, bivariate analyses revealed that lower family income, maternal education level, and paternal education level predict all three domains of behavioral problems (i.e., internalized problems, externalized problems, and total behavioral problems). Further, multivariate analyses revealed that lower family income consistently predicts all domains of behavioral problems, lower maternal education level predicted externalized problems and total behavioral problems, and paternal education level did not predict any clinically significant behavioral problems.

Conclusion

In this sample, we found that, for children, family income and parental education when entering preschool were significant predictors of mental health problems after elementary school enrollment; in particular, low income and low maternal educational achievement predicted a high probability of the development of a psychiatric disorder. A greater understanding of the mechanisms of these associations could contribute to improvements in interventions aimed at preventing child maladjustment.

Introduction

It is widely accepted that social inequalities increase children’s risk of developing mental health problems [14]. In order to provide a better understanding of this, numerous studies on social inequalities and mental health have focused on low socioeconomic status (SES) as the main causal variable. SES is a concept that summarizes an individual’s social position in society. As previously demonstrated in a number of studies, socioeconomic position in terms of family economics and educational aspects as SES indicators has a strong influence on a child’s health and well-being [57]. Further, SES has been found to have a high likelihood of interacting with other factors, mediating or moderating their influence on children’s development. For example, lower SES can adversely influence children’s development in terms of behavioral and cognitive domains through several avenues, including lower levels of parental resources, social support, parental mental health, and parental functioning [810].

While studies on this topic have been quite common in certain geographic regions such as North America and several European countries, there have been limited attempts to examine the impact that indicators of families’ socioeconomic situations have on child development in Japan. The main reason indicators of families’ socioeconomic circumstances have rarely been studied in Japan is that information at the individual and household levels is relatively unavailable in the country. Consequently, most of the previous research on this topic has relied on retrospective reports concerning people’s childhood circumstances; however, such reports are regularly compromised by poor recall and measurement errors [11]. As socio-cultural backgrounds differ between countries, precisely clarifying the relationship between SES and childhood psychopathology in Japan could contribute to providing insight into the etiology of mental health problems and to improving interventions designed to reduce the burden of such mental health problems and prevent child maladjustment.

It is difficult to claim that Japan is a country that upholds social equality. Although Japan has no official poverty line, in practice, the relative poverty rate of the OECD index is frequently used to calculate the poverty rate. The threshold, calculated based on the OECD standard, which is half of the median income of the total population, was defined as approximately 1.2 million JPY [12]. Japan’s relative poverty rates are higher than the OECD average, and are steadily increasing [13]; in particular, poverty among young adults and families with children has shown a marked increase [1416]. Notably, Japan is in the top third of OECD countries in terms of levels of average family income, but some children in the country do not enjoy the benefits of this position; specifically, child poverty in Japan, at approximately 14%, is higher than the OECD average of roughly 12%. In addition, the relative poverty rate among single-parent households in Japan is over 50%, which is the highest of all OECD nations. Concurrently, concerns in regard to the potential transmission of poverty from parents to children have become heightened in Japan, mainly fueled by the country’s widening income inequality and increasing poverty risks [17]. From a global perspective, absolute poverty is decreasing, but in the developed world, divides between the poor and the wealthy are widening [1416]; thus, relative child poverty is becoming a central issue that must be addressed not only in Japan but also in many developed countries.

Although absolute levels of affluence are critical variables for predicting child development, to better predict various social and behavioral health outcomes, an increasing emphasis is being placed on relative levels of affluence [18]. Several studies have suggested that relative deprivation, which is defined as individual SES relative to that of one’s peers in society, might have a closer relationship with child behavioral symptoms than absolute difference in income has with such symptoms [19,20]. In countries with high relative social inequality, such as Japan, people with lower SES may have lower levels of happiness across many dimensions than people with higher SES. Further, feelings of relative deprivation may contribute to worsening the conditions of people with lower SES, because they can be motivated to spend lavishly on goods and services that signify higher status in society. Such stressful experiences may outweigh the few existing parental resources, resulting in maladaptive coping mechanisms that foster conflict among family members and disrupted child-rearing skills, consequently predicting mental health problems in children. Thus, relative inequality, which is measured by analyzing the resources needed to maintain a particular social and economic lifestyle, is an important but often-neglected policy target. Therefore, in order to understand the mechanism of SES and child development, it is important to clarify the relationship between SES and children’s development in Japan.

On the other hand, many studies have measured SES by combining education status and other SES variables to create simple composite scores [21]. However, while several of these studies have found that lower parental education is related to lower levels of developmental outcomes in children, including psychological well-being and emotional and cognitive development [2225], there is limited evidence that parental education, as an SES indicator, independently predicts child developmental outcomes. Japan is a country with high educational standards: access to education is high across all levels, enrollment in pre-primary education is high, and first-time entry and graduation rates at the tertiary level are also high [26,27]. Further, roughly half of the working-age population in Japan is tertiary-educated, which is much higher than the OECD average of approximately 35%. In addition, based on current patterns of graduation, over 70% of young people in Japan are expected to graduate from tertiary education during their lifetimes, and this is also higher than the OECD average of approximately 50%. On the other hand, regarding the relationship between education and employment, approximately 80% of tertiary-educated adults in Japan are employed, while less than 75% of adults with lower levels of education have jobs. Concurrently, the unemployment rate among tertiary-educated adults decreased between 2000 and 2012 from 3.5% to 3.2%, while that of adults with upper secondary or post-secondary non-tertiary education increased from 4.7% to 5.1% during the same period. This shift means that the gap between adults with high and low levels of education has widened over the past 12 years. As shown by these statistics, the social inequalities resulting from parental educational achievement might negatively influence parents’ mental health and result in poor child developmental outcomes.

The effects of socioeconomic circumstances are likely to vary depending on the phase of life in which they are experienced. In particular, prior studies investigating this topic have suggested that such effects are most pronounced during preschool years. The reason for this may be that poverty interferes with the processes that lead to school readiness. Moreover, childhood SES is not the only aspect that has an impact on developmental outcomes and mental health in childhood, as hardships during childhood are also likely to be critical for explaining differences in developmental outcomes and mental health later in life. For instance, exposure to poverty during childhood appears to have a greater detrimental impact on childhood social and cognitive ability than experiencing poverty later in life [2831]. In addition, childhood SES has been found to be a relevant predictor of adult mental health status [32,33]. Furthermore, cumulative adversities experienced in childhood have also been found to be associated with psychological distress in adulthood [3436]; in other words, persistent economic deprivation appears to be more detrimental than transient poverty. Mental health problems experienced during childhood are known to be associated with both current and later impairment, and to result in maladjustment [37,38]. This realization has led to the development of numerous early childhood intervention programs, such as “Head Start,” targeting low-SES children, and this has been proven to be effective for promoting children’s development [3941]. Considering the above findings cumulatively, it seems that adversity in childhood becomes embodied at an early age, and the full impact of this then manifests itself later in adult life. Therefore, experiencing adversity and social inequalities early in life is likely to be more detrimental than experiencing them at any later point. Consequently, it is important to verify that this is the case: social inequalities in early childhood have a severe impact on development later in life.

In the current research, using a longitudinal design, we examined the impact SES in early childhood has on social adaptation in the first grade of elementary school in Japan. To achieve this, we included three different indicators of SES (i.e., family economy, paternal education level, and maternal education level). In addition, we included an assessment of mental health, including behavioral problems, in regard to the child’s everyday functioning. Consequently, our investigation was successful, and we determined that SES, particularly lower family income and lower maternal education, is a predictor of future behavioral problems. We anticipate that the results of this research will help inform policies and plans that support the development of children in countries with high relative poverty rates.

Methods

Participants

The current investigation consists of two sets of data relating to two different years (with a one-year interval between them) and is part of a longitudinal study examining the influence of family factors on children’s social developmental outcomes. At Time 1 (T1), which was conducted in 2014, we obtained a sample of preschool children, all of whom were five years old, from those enrolled in 52 kindergartens and 78 nursery schools in Nagoya City, which is a major urban area in Japan. To recruit these participants, self-reported questionnaires were distributed to all parents of targeted children (n = 5,024), and the parents were asked to complete the questionnaires (number of responses, n = 3,314). Then, at Time 2 (T2), which was conducted in 2015, the same children were recruited, now six years old and in the first grade. In other words, a similar questionnaire was provided to parents one year (12 months) after T1. Once again, the parents completed the questionnaires (number of responses, n = 1,787). The retention rate from T1 to T2 was 53.9%, meaning attrition from T1 to T2 was 46.1%, with 44.7% not responding at T2 and 1.4% (n = 46) having relocated. Comparing the non-returning participants with the returning participants on demographic features, regarding household income, 16.2% of the non-returning participants’ household income per year was below Ұ 2,999,999, while this was true of only 10.0% of returning participants. The household income of the non-returning participants was significantly lower than that of the returning participants, as measured by a chi-square test. In addition, regarding education level, 5.8% of the non-returning participants’ maternal educational background was compulsory education, while this was true of only 2.4% of returning participants. Furthermore, 7.6% of the non-returning participants’ paternal educational background was compulsory education, while this was true of only 4.7% of returning participants; both maternal and paternal education levels of non-returning participants were significantly lower than those of the returning participants, as measured by a chi-square test. Thus, the non-returning participants tended to have relatively lower SES (i.e., family income and parental education level) than did returning participants, meaning that there was a lower response rate for individuals with low SES compared to high SES. To accurately clarify the associations between SES and child developmental outcomes, we then chose to exclude children with developmental problems. Consequently, of the 1,787 children for whom data was received at both T1 and T2, 1,712 (95.8%) met the inclusion criteria.

Ethics statement

Researchers obtained written, informed consent from all participants. For the children, written, informed consent was obtained from parents on their behalf. Ethical approval for this study was obtained from Kyoto University’s Ethics Committee in Kyoto, Japan (E2322).

Measurements

Outcome variable: Child behavioral problems.

Behavioral problems were assessed using the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18 (CBCL), Parent Report, which targets children aged 4 to 18 years. [42]. For this study, the Japanese Edition of the CBCL was used, which consists of 113 items [43]. Each item was rated using a three-point scale ranging from 0 (“not true”) to 2 (“very true” or “often true”). Ratings for subsets of items were then summed to provide scores for the eight syndrome scales (i.e., withdrawn, somatic complaint, anxious/depressed, social problems, thought problems, attention problems, delinquent behavior, and aggressive behavior). Then, the scores of the syndrome scales were combined to determine scores for the higher-order domains of internalized, externalized, and total problems. Internalized factors include items relating to withdrawal, somatic complaints, and anxiety/depression, while externalized factors include items assessing delinquency and aggression. Next, the T-scores of the CBCL’s internalized, externalized, and total problem scores were calculated using standardized distribution among Japanese children, and T-scores greater than or equal to 63 in the internalized, externalized, and total problem scales were defined as indicating clinically significant “abnormal” symptoms [43]. The scale has been well standardized, and various studies have found it to have good reliability and validity [44,45]. In the current study, Cronbach’s α coefficient was found to be adequate, with the value for the externalized behaviors being .88, for internalized behaviors being .87, and for total behavior problems being .89.

Explanatory variable: Socioeconomic status.

In the next stage, SES was defined using information concerning family income and parental education. First, parents reported their annual equalized household income in Japanese Yen (JPY). Then, using this data, we created four categories of income: <3,000,000 JPY (approximately 30,000 USD), 3–4,000,000 JPY (approximately 30,000–40,000 USD), 5–6,000,000 JPY (approximately 50,000–60,000 USD), and ≥7,000,000 JPY (approximately 70,000 USD). Further, both parents of each child were also asked to report their education in years, as well as the highest educational level completed, using the following choices: elementary school (six years), junior high school (nine years), vocational or general upper secondary school (12 years), less than four years at college/university (13–15 years), four years at college/university (16 years), and graduate school (>16 years). It should be noted here that the Japanese education system comprises elementary school (six years), junior high school (three years), and high school (three years), and education is compulsory until the end of junior high school (nine years). Based on this data, we created four categories indicating education level: compulsory education (nine years), upper secondary school (12 years), up to four years of college/university (13–15 years), and over four years of college/university (>15 years).

Covariates: Demographic information.

Parents also provided background demographic information for their children, from which covariates were sourced. This information included sex, family composition (nuclear or expanded family), family status (two parents or single parent), number of siblings (one or more siblings or no siblings), and preschool institution attended (kindergarten or nursery school).

Data analyses.

Poisson regression analyses were conducted to assess the association between SES and diagnostic probability scores; this was due to the high prevalence of the outcomes (i.e., the CBCL clinically significant behavioral problems) [4648]. The dichotomized probability score for predicted behavioral problems was then entered as the dependent variable (non-abnormal = 0 and abnormal = 1), and family income and paternal and maternal education levels were entered as categorical predictors. Specifically, the associations between the children’s characteristics and SES at preschool and between SES and behavioral problems in the first grade were analyzed using a bivariate Poisson regression model; then, multivariate Poisson regression using significant variables identified from the bivariate regression was used to examine the independent associations between SES and behavioral problems. In the multivariate model, as the demographic factors were not significantly associated with behavioral problems in the bivariate analyses, we did not include the demographic factors in the multivariate analyses. On the other hand, as all socioeconomic status factors were significantly associated with behavioral problems in the bivariate analyses, we included all socioeconomic status factors in the analyses. More specifically, although not all indicators were significantly related to behavioral problems in the bivariate analyses, all indicators of annual household income, maternal education level, and paternal education level were covaried in the multivariate analyses.

In addition, to investigate how variables related to each other, we conducted path analyses; the path analyses were conducted to estimate direct and indirect paths between family income, maternal and paternal education levels, and behavioral problems (see S2 Fig, S3 Fig, and S4 Fig). The hypothesized model is presented in S1 Fig; in the model, maternal and paternal education levels were specified as predictors of family income and child behavioral problems. Prior to conducting the path analyses, correlational analyses were performed to measure associations between demographic variables and outcome variables; variables significantly correlated with total behavioral problems were entered into the predictive model as control variables (see S1 Table).

All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS Statistics 23.0 and AMOS version 23.0.

Results

Study population

Table 1 shows the demographic characteristics of children, familial SES at T1, and the prevalence of clinically significant behavioral problems, which was determined using the CBCL, at T2. At T1, the children’s mean age was 6.09 years (SD = 0.30), and the sex distribution was almost equal. Additionally, 86.4% lived in a nuclear family, 93.7% lived with both parents, 83.2% had one or more siblings, and the distribution of preschool institution attended (kindergarten or nursery school) was almost equal. Additionally, the mean ages of the mothers and fathers were 37.28 (SD = 4.62) and 39.29 (SD = 5.45) years, respectively. In the Japanese population, approximately 80% lived in a nuclear family, approximately 95% lived with both parents, and approximately 80% had one or more siblings [12]. Therefore, participants in the current study were roughly similar to the demographics of the Japanese population. Regarding SES, the median household income was between 5,000,000 and 5,999,999 JPY per year, and the ratio of families with an annual income of less than 3,000,000 JPY was 10.0%. Next, in relation to parents’ education status, the most common level of educational achievement for mothers was less than four years at college/university (40.7%), while the most common paternal educational achievement was having attended over four years of college/university (53.3%: undergraduate degree (four years); 45.9%, graduate degree (>four years); 7.4%). Meanwhile, the percentage of compulsory education (nine years) was 2.4% for mothers, while for fathers it was 4.7%; on the other hand, the proportion of having attended over four years at college/university for mothers was 32.1%, while for fathers it was 53.3%. In terms of Japanese SES, among a similar generation, the median household income was approximately 4.5 million JPY [12]. The proportion of having attended over four years of college/university for women was approximately 20% and for men was approximately 35% [49]. Therefore, SES of participants in the current study was relatively higher compared with that typical of the Japanese. Finally, regarding the CBCL results for clinically significant behavioral problems, the clinical cut-off was exceeded for internalized, externalized, and total behavior problems in 17.8%, 17.6%, and 18.8% of participants, respectively.

thumbnail
Table 1. Demographic characteristics and socioeconomic status of subjects at preschool, as well as CBCL behavioral problems in first grade (N = 1,712).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0197961.t001

Internalized behavioral problems

Associations of demographic characteristics and SES with clinically significant internalized behavioral problems are shown in Table 2. In our bivariate model, when children from families with an annual household income over 7,000,000 JPY were used as the reference category, we found that annual household income showed a significant association with clinically significant internalized behavioral problems (3–4,000,000: rate ratio (RR): 1.83, 95% confidence interval (CI): 1.35–2.50; less than 3,000,000: RR: 2.06, 95% CI: 1.41–3.01), and this association remained significant in the multivariate model, which was mutually adjusted. (In the multivariate model, as the demographic factors were not significantly associated with internalized behavioral problems in the bivariate analyses, we did not include demographic factors in the analyses. On the other hand, as all socioeconomic status factors were significantly associated with internalized behavioral problems in the bivariate analyses, we included all socioeconomic status factors in the multivariate analyses. More specifically, although not all indicators were significantly related to internalized behavioral problems in the bivariate analyses, all indicators of annual household income, maternal education level, and paternal education level were covaried in the multivariate analyses.) In other words, the rate ratio suggests that children from families with an annual household income of 3 to 4,000,000 JPY and less than 3,000,000 JPY were 1.66 times (95% CI: 1.20–2.30) and 1.91 times (95% CI: 1.24–2.94) more likely to show clinically significant internalized problems, respectively, independent of other SES indicators, compared to children from families with an annual household income over 7,000,000 JPY.

thumbnail
Table 2. Bivariate and multivariate analysis predicting CBCL clinically significant internalized behavioral problems (N = 1,712).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0197961.t002

When children of parents with over four years at college/university (≥ 16 years) were used as the reference category, although we found in the bivariate model that maternal education level showed a significant association with clinically significant internalized behavioral problems (upper secondary school (12 years): RR: 1.71, 95% CI: 1.26–2.31; compulsory education (nine years): RR: 2.17, 95% CI: 1.18–4.00), no significant association was found in the multivariate model. Similarly, although we found that paternal education level showed a significant association with clinically significant internalized behavioral problems in the bivariate model (compulsory education (nine years): RR: 1.75, 95% CI: 1.11–2.74), no significant association was found in the multivariate model.

In addition, to estimate how these variables related to each other, we conducted path analyses to estimate the direct and indirect paths between family income, maternal and paternal education levels, and internalized behavioral problems (see S2 Fig). The result of the analyses showed that maternal and paternal education levels were indirectly related to internalized behavioral problems through family income; on the other hand, maternal and paternal education levels were not directly related to internalized behavioral problems.

Externalized behavioral problems

The associations of demographics and SES with clinically significant externalized behavior problems are shown in Table 3. In our bivariate model, when children from families with an annual household income over 7,000,000 JPY were used as the reference category, we found that annual household income showed a significant association with clinically significant externalized behavioral problems (3–4,000,000: RR: 1.39, 95% CI: 1.02–1.91; less than 3,000,000: RR: 1.74, 95% CI: 1.19–2.54), and this association remained significant in the multivariate model. (In the multivariate model, as the demographic factors were not significantly associated with externalized behavioral problems in the bivariate analyses, we did not include demographic factors in the analyses. On the other hand, as all socioeconomic status factors were significantly associated with externalized behavioral problems in the bivariate analyses, we included all socioeconomic status factors in the multivariate analyses; more specifically, although not all indicators were significantly related to externalized behavioral problems in the bivariate analyses, all indicators of annual household income, maternal education level, and paternal education level were covaried in the multivariate analyses.) That is, the rate ratio suggests that children from families with an annual household income of less than 3,000,000 JPY were 1.54 times (95% CI: 1.02–2.42) more likely to show clinically significant externalized problems, independent of other SES indicators, compared to children from families with an annual household income of over 7,000,000 JPY.

thumbnail
Table 3. Bivariate and multivariate analysis predicting CBCL clinically significant externalized behavioral problems (N = 1,712).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0197961.t003

Similarly, in our bivariate model, when children from parents with over four years at college/university (≥ 16 years) were used as the reference category, we found that maternal education level showed a significant association with clinically significant externalized behavioral problems (upper secondary school (12 years): RR: 1.39, 95% CI: 1.02–1.89; compulsory education (nine years): RR: 2.68, 95% CI: 1.57–4.58), and this association remained significant in the multivariate model. In other words, the rate ratio suggests that children from families in which the maternal education level is compulsory education (nine years) were 2.08 times (95% CI: 1.14–3.79) more likely to show clinically significant externalized problems, independent of other SES indicators, compared to children from families with a maternal education level of over four years at college/university (≥ 16 years). On the other hand, although we found in the bivariate model that paternal education level shows a significant association with clinically significant externalized behavioral problems (compulsory education (nine years): RR: 1.68, 95% CI: 1.06–2.66), no significant association was found in the multivariate model.

In addition, to investigate how these variables related to each other, we performed path analyses, which were conducted to estimate direct and indirect paths between family incomes, maternal and paternal education levels, and externalized behavioral problems (see S3 Fig). The result of the analyses showed that paternal and maternal education level indirectly related to externalized behavioral problems through family income; at the same time, maternal education level directly related to externalized behavioral problems, which was mutually adjusted.

Total behavioral problems

Finally, associations of demographics and SES with clinically significant total behavioral problems are shown in Table 4. In our bivariate model, when children from families with an annual household income over 7,000,000 JPY were used as the reference category, we found that annual household income showed a significant association with clinically significant total behavioral problems (5–6,000,000: RR: 1.47, 95% CI: 1.08–1.99; 3–4,000,000: RR: 1.78, 95% CI: 1.31–2.41; less than 3,000,000: RR: 2.11, 95% CI: 1.46–3.04), and this association remained significant in the multivariate model. (In the multivariate model, as the demographic factors were not significantly associated with total behavioral problems in the bivariate analyses, we did not include demographic factors in the analyses. On the other hand, as all socioeconomic status factors were significantly associated with total behavioral problems in the bivariate analyses, we included all socioeconomic status factors in the multivariate analyses; more specifically, although not all indicators were significantly related to total behavioral problems in the bivariate analyses, all indicators of annual household income, maternal education level, and paternal education level were covaried in the multivariate analyses.) That is, the rate ratio suggested that children from families with an annual household income of 3–4,000,000 and less than 3,000,000 JPY are 1.55 times (95% CI: 1.12–2.13) and 1.72 times (95% CI: 1.13–2.63) more likely, respectively, to show clinically significant total behavioral problems, independent of other SES indicators, than children from families with an annual household income of over 7,000,000 JPY.

thumbnail
Table 4. Bivariate and multivariate analysis predicting CBCL clinically significant total behavioral problems (N = 1,712).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0197961.t004

Similarly, in our bivariate model, when children of parents with over four years at college/university (≥ 16 years) were used as the reference category, we found that maternal education level showed a significant association with clinically significant externalized behavioral problems (less than four years at college/university (13–15 years): 1.36, 95% CI: 1.02–1.89; upper secondary school (12 years): RR: 1.66, 95% CI: 1.23–2.25; compulsory education (nine years): RR: 3.17, 95% CI: 1.90–5.30), and this remained significant in the multivariate model; that is, the rate ratio suggests that children from families with a maternal education level of compulsory education (nine years) were 2.14 times (95% CI: 1.16–3.97) more likely to show clinically significant externalized problems, independent of other SES indicators, compared to children from families with a maternal education level of over four years at college/university (≥ 16 years). On the other hand, although we found in the bivariate model that paternal education level showed a significant association with clinically significant externalized behavioral problems (upper secondary school (12 years): RR: 1.40, 95% CI: 1.07–1.84; compulsory education (nine years): RR; 1.89, 95% CI: 1.23–2.91), no such significance was observed in the multivariate model.

In addition, to investigate how these variables related to each other, we conducted path analyses to estimate the direct and indirect paths between family income, maternal and paternal education levels, and total behavioral problems (see S4 Fig). The results of the analyses showed that paternal and maternal education level was indirectly related to total behavioral problems through family income; at the same time, maternal education level was directly related to total behavioral problems, which was mutually adjusted.

Discussion

In the present study, we found that lower SES at the preschool level negatively influenced behavioral problems upon entering school to a clinically significant degree, suggesting that children from families with lower SES have a higher risk of developing behavioral problems. Specifically, regarding family income, bivariate analyses revealed that lower family income significantly influenced all domains of clinical behavioral problems (i.e., internalized problems, externalized problems, and total behavioral problems). Furthermore, multivariate analyses also revealed that lower family income had a clinically significant influence within all of these domains of behavioral problems.

Similarly, regarding education level, bivariate analyses revealed that lower maternal and paternal education level significantly influence all domains of clinical behavioral problems (i.e., internalized problems, externalized problems, and total behavioral problems). Further multivariate analyses revealed that lower maternal education level negatively influenced externalized problems and total behavioral problems to a clinically significant degree while multivariate analyses revealed that paternal education level does not influence any behavioral problems to a clinically significant degree. These results are consistent with previous findings that lower familial socioeconomic circumstances during childhood influences the development of mental health problems later in life [14].

There are several potential mechanisms through which SES might influence the occurrence of behavioral problems. For example, many studies have suggested that social inequalities influence child development through not only the direct material path (which concerns an unequal distribution of material resources that can be used to support healthy child development [35,50]), which reflects material deprivation, but also through the indirect psychosocial path influenced by relative socioeconomic position [18,51,52]. That is, lower SES is likely to negatively affect developmental outcomes in children, as parents in such circumstances are unable to provide material resources necessary for healthy child development. On the other hand, lower SES is also likely to negatively affect developmental outcomes in children because these parents experience psychological distress; lower SES is associated with poor parental mental health, which in turn negatively influences parental functioning and parent–child interactions, predicting mental health problems in children [53].

In the current study, regarding family income, in the multivariate model, 3–4 million JPY was significantly related to internalized behavioral problems and total behavioral problems; in addition, <3 million JPY was significantly related to all of the domains of behavioral problems (i.e., internalized problems, externalized problems, and total behavioral problems). As mentioned earlier, in terms of Japanese SES, among similar generation families, the median household income was approximately 4.5 million JPY [12]. Children in families earning an income of <4 million JPY might be more strongly affected by relative poverty, than children of family’s earning more than the median household income.

Regarding parental educational level, in the multivariate model, maternal education level of compulsory education was significantly related to externalized problems and total behavioral problems. In Japan, the upper secondary school admission rate exceeds more than 95% [49]. Children of mothers completing only compulsory education (i.e., not completing more than upper secondary school) might be more strongly affected by educational inequality, than children of mothers completing more than upper secondary school. Therefore, social inequalities might influence child behavioral problems through not only the direct path but also through the indirect path influenced by relative socioeconomic position.

When considering the impact of family income on child outcomes, many parents in stressful economic situations are unable to provide the tangible or intangible resources necessary to support the successful development of their children [54,55]. One of the most critical pathways in this regard is the quality of the home environment. Lower SES is likely to create a family environment that is unsuitable for children’s healthy development, consequently affecting children’s developmental outcomes. Several studies have suggested that differences in the qualities of the home environments of higher- and lower-income children account for a substantial portion of the effect family income-to-needs has on the development of children [35,56,57].

Another important viewpoint is the investment perspective. Family income dictates the amount of time and money that families invest in materials and experiences that foster children’s development [50]. Families that are more financially secure are better able to invest time enriching their children, and they have more money to spend on helping their children experience greater opportunities to stimulate learning, on opportunities to encourage talent, and on improving the physical condition of their homes [58]. In other words, higher levels of income are likely to be associated with greater access to better home environments, such as reading materials and toys, cultural events and activities, and music and sports. On the other hand, more financially disadvantaged families are less able to invest time enriching their children, and they have less money to spend on cognitively stimulating materials and activities, high-quality child care, health care, and providing safe homes and neighborhoods [5961]. Thus, children from lower-income families tend to experience lower quality home environments than children from higher-income families.

Furthermore, economic hardship also causes practical difficulties for parents because it is related to elevations in parental psychological distress. As mentioned earlier, lower SES is associated with poor parental mental health [62]. Parents’ psychological distress resulting from the experience of financial insecurity has been associated with greater conflict among family members and also with disrupted child-rearing skills, such as less positive parenting practices and more negative parenting practices (i.e., the adoption of unsupportive, uninvolved, and inconsistent parenting styles, and harsh disciplinary strategies). These parenting practices, in turn, are related to a range of damaging outcomes for children’s well-being [6366]. SES could strongly influence the quality of communication and interactions between parents and children, and the higher quality of communications and interactions during early childhood have been positively associated with the later development of sociability and adaptability. On the other hand, the lower quality of communications and interactions during early childhood have been negatively associated with the later development of sociability and adaptability [53].

Further, the indirect psychosocial path involves the notion that not only being poor but also feeling of being poor in comparison to others (i.e., relative deprivation) elicits forms of psychological distress such as anxiety and psychological stress [67,68]. Relative deprivation is a social comparison theory that argues that individuals regularly compare themselves to those who are better off than they are [69]. Thus, it is likely that low SES partly influences child behavioral problems through upward comparisons of social class. The effects of stressors resulting from relative deprivation are particularly toxic for parents who are at the highest risk of experiencing socioeconomic hardship, as well as for those who are at an absolute economic disadvantage [70]. Such negative comparisons are likely to produce anxiety and psychological stress, which may negatively affect parent–parent relationships and parent-child interactions [71,72]. Thus, both absolute and relative detentions and economic disadvantages cause heightened economic stress, which in turn negatively affects parents’ parental psychological distress, which in turn negatively affects inter-familial conflict and parenting behavior, which in turn negatively affects children’s outcomes.

Considering the above situations, the findings of this study suggest that it is possible that family income influences all aspects of behavioral problems through several avenues, including the direct material path and the indirect psychosocial path.

When considering the impact of parental educational achievement on child outcomes in the current study, we also found that children of mothers who have lower educational achievement tend to have a higher risk of developing behavioral problems. This result is consistent with those of previous studies, which showed that mothers’ education is associated with externalized problems in offspring [7375]. There may be several reasons children with lower-educated mothers have higher behavioral problems, including, experiencing less favorable parenting styles, lower-quality environments in the home, and exposure to stressful events in their surroundings. Further, less educated mothers tend to rely more on negative parenting attitudes, including physical and authoritarian disciplinary tactics [7678]. It has been suggested that this is due to a lack of knowledge of means of developing a positive parental disposition toward children, of the counterproductive outcomes of severe disciplining measures, and of appropriate alternatives to physical and authoritarian disciplinary tactics [79]. In this study, although paternal educational level was significantly related to behavioral problems in the bivariate model, paternal educational level was not significantly related to behavioral problems in the multivariate model, which was mutually adjusted. Furthermore, path analyses also showed that paternal and maternal education levels were indirectly related to behavioral problems through family income. At the same time, maternal education level directly related to behavioral problems; on the other hand, paternal education level was not directly related to behavioral problems. One of the factors of the difference of effect level between paternal and maternal education levels may be the difference between the father and mother in spending time with their children. In this study, mean time spent talking or playing with children was 230.11 (SD = 146.45) minutes for mothers and 75.40 (SD = 77.25) minutes for fathers; mothers spent more time with their children than did fathers. The difference in the level of involvement may have led to the impact of maternal involvement on child outcomes.

In addition, we found maternal education to be an essential predictor of cognitively stimulating home environments, such as appropriate physical environments and learning experiences in the home, which influence children’s cognitive and behavioral outcomes [80,81]. Thus, the relationship between education and the qualities of home environments reveals the importance of disseminating information and knowledge concerning the development of positive parental dispositions toward children. Furthermore, there may be a specific possibility that Japanese women have higher relative deprivation in regard to education, which is in turn related to psychological distress.

While first-time upper secondary graduation rates in Japan are high for both men (96%) and women (98%), the share of female first-time graduates for all tertiary levels of education is the lowest in the OECD [26,27]. In addition, the gender gap in the Japanese labor market is among the largest in the OECD. The employment rate of tertiary-educated women between 25 and 64 years of age is 72% (below the OECD average of 80%), which is due to a significant proportion of women not being active in the labor market, while the employment rate of tertiary-educated men is 93% (above the OECD average of 88%). Even when they are employed, there are huge disparities between salaries for men and women with the same level of education. According to the Survey of Adult Skills, tertiary-educated men in Japan earn approximately 60% more than tertiary-educated women, the largest such gap in the OECD. Thus, Japanese women are likely to experience relative deprivation in regard to education and, thus, experience greater psychological stress, which in turn has a pathogenic influence on child-rearing skills. Thus, considering the above, the findings of this study also suggest that it is possible that maternal education level influences behavioral problems through several avenues, including the direct material path and the indirect psychosocial path.

Finally, regarding the impact of SES on child outcomes, there is another mechanism of the effects of low SES on behavioral problems in children: there is likely to be a role of genetic variation as a moderator. Socioeconomic circumstances are likely to influence child developmental outcomes through genetic factors. Children’s behavioral problems may be influenced by genetic risks, as well as their family’s environmental factors. A large body of evidence supports the conclusion that children’s behavioral problems are moderately heritable [8285]. Several studies have suggested the extent to which children’s mental health functioning is affected by family environmental factors depends on genetic and early temperamental characteristics; environments help determine how genes express themselves [8688]. Children with different genetic attributes will respond differently to the same environmental circumstances. In addition, epigenetic processes provide possibilities that low SES impacts health, including stress-related diseases, through environmental experiences that influence gene regulation [89,90]. Many studies suggest that exposure to adverse environments in early life has long-term consequences on later behavioral and neurobiological functioning including HPA axis reactivity to stress [91,92]. One possibility for the mechanism is that epigenetic alterations of the serotonin transporter gene expression account for the environmentally mediated effect of childhood victimization on HPA axis reactivity; childhood adversity experience may induce stable changes in HPA axis activity and increase vulnerability to psychopathology [9395]. Exposure to adversity caused by low SES is likely to influence methylation; stressful life events increase vulnerability to affective disorders later in life, possibly mediated by methylation of the serotonin transporter gene [96,97]. Indeed, adults from secure family environments related to SES in early life had lower levels of depressive symptomology, whereas those who experienced insecure environments had higher levels of depressive symptoms [98100]. Additionally, the impact of a stressful early family environment on depressive symptomatology has been found to be moderated by the serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism [98,99]. As mentioned earlier, children from low-SES families tend to be exposed to stressful life events through lower-quality home environments and lower quality of childcare (e.g., economic hardship and lower parents’ educational achievement). Thus, the influence of low SES on child behavior problems found in the present study could be influenced by genetic factors.

Limitations

There are several limitations to this study, and these must be addressed in future research. First, the CBCL was completed by caregivers only, which likely introduced reporting bias. In addition, behavioral problems in school were not included. Teachers’ reports are needed to evaluate this more accurately; further explorations should combine teacher and caregiver CBCL ratings. Second, there are likely to be other factors that were not accounted for in our model. As mentioned earlier, there is a potential role of genetic variation as a moderator of the effects of low SES on behavioral problems in children. Several studies have suggested the extent to which children’s mental health functioning is affected by family environmental factors depends on genetic and early temperamental characteristics [8688]. Future studies should investigate this possibility further to clarify more family environmental factors related to child mental health functioning, by using a genetically informative design (e.g., a twin or adoption study design), and estimating children’s early temperamental characteristics at baseline. Third, these findings may not be generalizable to all families, because there is a risk of attrition bias. The retention rate from T1 to T2 was 53.9%, and the returning participants in T2 tended to have relatively higher SES than the non-returning participants. There remains the possibility that our analysis could not fully evaluate the impact of SES on children outcomes, and our analysis may also underestimate the influence of SES. Future research would benefit from a study design that uses samples with higher retention rates (in particular, participants with lower SES). Finally, the sample was drawn from a limited geographical area in an urban metropolis in Japan. In this study, multivariate analyses revealed that lower maternal education level negatively influenced behavioral problems; on the other hand, paternal education level did not influence any behavioral problems. On the other hand, in research conducted in North America, lower paternal education level was found to be associated with increased mental health problems in the adult offspring of the fathers with lower education levels [99]. There is a possibility that the stronger impact of maternal education than paternal education on behavioral problems could be due to cultural effects of the status of mothers in Japan compared to western culture. Although Japanese and American women spend similar amounts of time on housework (excluding childcare), Japanese husbands spend less than a third of the time on household tasks than do American husbands [101]. Furthermore, similar discrepancies can be found with regard to child rearing. When comparing the average time spent per day with their young children, that of Japanese fathers is shorter than that of American and Swedish fathers; on the other hand, among these fathers, the Japanese work longer hours [102]. The difference in the level of involvement may have led to the influence on children outcomes. In addition, as mentioned earlier, the retention rate from the baseline survey to this survey was approximately 50%, and the returning participants tended to be relatively higher in SES than the non-returning participants. This indicates there is a risk of attrition bias. Therefore, there is the possibility that our analyses could not adequately evaluate the impact of SES on child outcomes, and our analyses may underestimate the influence of SES. Thus, the reproducibility of the current results should be confirmed using data from other regions in a variety of settings. Future research would benefit from samples with greater demographic and clinical diversity.

Conclusion

The main finding of our study is that, for children, their family income and parental education situation at the time they enroll in preschool influences their outcomes after they enter in elementary school. In particular, after controlling for other SES indicators, low income and low maternal educational achievement were found to be significantly associated with mental health problems. Understanding the mechanisms of these associations could contribute to improvements in interventions aimed at preventing child maladjustment. The life course approach suggests that early exposure to adverse social circumstances leads to poor mental health in the future [103,104]. Our results indicate that we should be sensitive to social inequalities in children’s mental health problems and developmental outcomes, and should strive to reduce such inequalities. Further, in the long-term, we should focus not only on providing economic support, but also on education, as providing equal access to suitable educational opportunities can positively impact the next generation, and is likely to have a more permanent impact on the child-rearing environment than a temporary increase in income. Prior intervention trials, which have examined whether providing only additional income to families to address child health inequalities, have mainly had null effects; on the other hand, there are reported to be effective interventions that provide support with educational content [105]. A prior study also suggested that SES strongly influenced the quality of communication and interactions between parents and children and that positive communications and interactions during early childhood were associated with the higher development of sociability and adaptability later [53]. To prevent maladjustment of children in low-SES families, educational interventions for parents may contribute to the social adaptation of the child. In addition, disadvantaged youths tend to have lower rates of enrollment in senior high schools and college, and higher rates of dropout [106]. Consequently, if more parents can become better educated through an improved social system, it might lead to better developmental outcomes for children. Thus, both social scientists and welfare policymakers should focus on addressing both economic and educational aspects.

Supporting information

S2 Fig. Statistically significant paths for internalized behavioral problems.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0197961.s002

(PDF)

S3 Fig. Statistically significant paths for externalized behavioral problems.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0197961.s003

(PDF)

S4 Fig. Statistically significant paths for total behavioral problems.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0197961.s004

(PDF)

S1 Table. Correlations among demographic characteristics, socioeconomic status, and behavioral problems.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0197961.s005

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge all the children, their parents, and preschool teachers who participated in this study. We are also grateful to two reviewers (Dr. Redford Williams and an anonymous reviewer) for their constructive comments and suggestions.

References

  1. 1. Currie J, Stabile M. Socioeconomic status and child health: why is the relationship stronger for older children? Am Econ Rev. 2003;93: 1813–1823. pmid:29058847
  2. 2. Velez CN, Johnson J, Cohen P. A longitudinal analysis of selected risk factors for childhood psychopathology. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1989;28: 861–864. doi: S0890-8567(09) 60209–4 pmid:2808256
  3. 3. van Oort F, van der Ende J, Wadsworth M, Verhulst F, Achenbach T. Cross-national comparison of the link between socioeconomic status and emotional and behavioral problems in youths. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2011;46: 167–172. pmid:20165830
  4. 4. Mendelson T, Kubzansky LD, Datta GD, Buka SL. Relation of female gender and low socioeconomic status to internalizing symptoms among adolescents: A case of double jeopardy? Soc Sci Med. 2008;66: 1284–1296. pmid:18248868
  5. 5. Ensminger M., Fotherill K. A decade of measuring SES: what it tells us and where to go from here. In: Bornstein M, Bradley R, editors. Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2003. pp. 13–28.
  6. 6. Ostberg V, Vågerö D. Socioeconomic differences in mortality among children. Do they persist into adulthood? Soc Sci Med. 1991;32: 403–410. pmid:2024155
  7. 7. Power C, Hypponen E, Smith GD. Socioeconomic position in childhood and early adult life and risk of mortality. Am J Public Health. 2005;9: 1396–1402.
  8. 8. Conger RD, Donnellan MB. An interactionist perspective on the socioeconomic context of human development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2007;58: 175–199. pmid:16903807
  9. 9. Adler NE, Snibbe AC. The role of psychosocial processes in explaining the gradient between socioeconomic status and health. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2003;12: 119–123.
  10. 10. McLoyd V, Shanahan M. Poverty, parenting, and children’s mental health. Am Sociol Rev. 1993;5: 351–366.
  11. 11. Looker ED. Accuracy of proxy reports of parental status characteristics. Sociol Educ. 1989;62: 257–276.
  12. 12. Ministry of Health, Labor, and Welfare. 2011. Summary Report of Comprehensive Survey of Living Conditions 2011. Available from www.mhlw.go.jp/english/database/db-hss/dl/report_gaikyo_2011.pdf. Cited 7 March 2018
  13. 13. OECD. Society at a Glance. 2016. Available from www.oecd-ilibrary.org/docserver/download/8116131e.pdf. Cited 7 March 2018
  14. 14. OECD. Growing unequal? Income distribution and poverty in OECD countries. Paris: OECD Publishing; 2008.
  15. 15. OECD. Doing Better for Children; 2009. Available from www.oecd.org/els/social/childwellbeing. Cited 7 March 2018
  16. 16. OECD. In It Together Why Less Inequality Benefits All. Paris: OECD Publishing; 2015.
  17. 17. Tachibanaki T. Confronting income inequality in Japan. Cambridge: MIT Press; 2005.
  18. 18. Adler NE, Snibbe AC. The role of psychosocial processes in explaining the gradient between socioeconomic status and health. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2003;12: 119–123.
  19. 19. Elgar FJ, De Clercq B, Schnohr CW, Bird P, Pickett KE, Torsheim T, et al. Absolute and relative family affluence and psychosomatic symptoms in adolescents. Soc Sci Med. 2013;91: 25–31. pmid:23849235
  20. 20. Reagan PB, Salsberry PJ, Olsen RJ. Does the measure of economic disadvantage matter? Exploring the effect of individual and relative deprivation on intrauterine growth restriction. Soc Sci Med. 2007;64: 2016–2029. pmid:17379372
  21. 21. Ensminger M, Fotherill K. A decade of measuring SES: what it tells us and where to go from here. In: Bornstein M, Bradley R, editors. Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2003. pp. 13–28.
  22. 22. von Rueden U, Gosch A, Rajmil L, Bisegger C, Ravens-Sieberer U. Socioeconomic determinants of health related quality of life in childhood and adolescence: results from a European study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2006;60: 130–135. pmid:16415261
  23. 23. Dearing E, McCartney K, Taylor BA. Change in family income-to-needs matters more for children with less. Child Dev. 2001;72: 1779–1793. pmid:11768145
  24. 24. Kohen DE, Brooks-Gunn J, Leventhal T, Hertzman C. Neighborhood income and physical and social disorder in Canada: associations with young children’s competencies. Child Dev. 2002;73: 1844–1860. pmid:12487498
  25. 25. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Shannon JD, Cabrera NJ, Lamb ME. Fathers and mothers at play with their 2- and 3-year-olds: contributions to language and cognitive development. Child Dev. 2004;75: 1806–1820. pmid:15566381
  26. 26. OECD. Education at a Glance. Paris: OECD Publishing; 2016.
  27. 27. OECD. Country note, Japan. Education at a Glance; 2016. Available from www.oecd-ilibrary.org/docserver/download/9616041ec065.pdf. Cited 7 March 2018
  28. 28. Duncan GJ, Brooks-Gunn J, Yeung WJ, Smith JR. How much does childhood poverty affect the life chances of children? Am Sociol Rev. 1998;63: 406–423.
  29. 29. Guo G. The timing of the influences of cumulative poverty on children’s cognitive ability and achievement. Soc Forces. 1998;77: 257–288.
  30. 30. NICHD. Duration and developmental timing of poverty and children’s cognitive and social development from birth through third grade. Child Dev. 2005;76: 795–810. pmid:16026497
  31. 31. Brunner E, Davey Smith G, Marmot M, Canner R, Beksinska M, O’Brien J. Childhood social circumstances and psychosocial and behavioural factors as determinants of plasma fibrinogen. Lancet. 1996;347: 1008–1013. pmid:8606563
  32. 32. Power C, Manor O. Explaining social class differences in psychosocial health among young adults: a longitudinal perspective. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 1992;27: 284–291. pmid:1492248
  33. 33. Rahkonen O, Lahelma E, Huuhka M. Past or present? Childhood living conditions and current socioeconomic status as determinants of adult health. Soc Sci Med. 1997;44: 327–336. pmid:9004368
  34. 34. Power C. Childhood adversity still matters for adult health outcomes. Lancet. 2002;360: 1610–1620.
  35. 35. Korenman S, Miller JE, Sjaastad JE. Long-term poverty and child development in the United States: results from the NLSY. Child Youth Serv Rev. 1995;17: 127–155.
  36. 36. Duncan GJ, Brooks-Gunn J, Klebanov PK. Economic deprivation and early childhood development. Child Dev. 1994;65: 296–318. pmid:7516849
  37. 37. Kessler RC, Amminger GP, Aguilar-Gaxiola S, Alonso J, Lee S, Ustun TB. Age of onset of mental disorders: a review of recent literature. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2007;20: 359–364. pmid:17551351
  38. 38. Rutter M, Sroufe LA. Developmental psychopathology: concepts and challenges. Dev Psychopathol. 2000;12: 265–296. pmid:11014739
  39. 39. Zigler EF, Muenchow S. Head Start: The inside story of America’s most successful educational experiment. New York: Basic Books; 1992.
  40. 40. Currie J, Thomas D. Does head start make a difference. Am Econ Rev. 1995;85.
  41. 41. Olds DL, Sadler L, Kitzman H. Programs for parents of infants and toddlers: Recent evidence from randomized trials. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007;48: 355–391. pmid:17355402
  42. 42. Achenbach TM. Manual for the Child Behaviour Check-List/4-18 and 1991 Profile. Burlington: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; 1991.
  43. 43. Itani T, Kanbayashi Y, Nakata Y, Kita M, Kuramoto H, Negishi T, et al. Standardization of the Japanese version of the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18. Psychiatr Neurol Pediatr Jpn. 2001;41: 243–252.
  44. 44. Kobayashi K, Endoh F, Ogino T, Oka M, Morooka T, Yoshinaga H, Ohtsuka Y. Questionnaire-based assessment of behavioral problems in Japanese children with epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 2013;27: 238–242. pmid:23466253
  45. 45. Drotar D, Stein RE, Perrin EC. Methodological issues in using the child behavior checklist and its related instruments in clinical psychology research. J Clin Child Psychol. 1995;24: 184–192.
  46. 46. Zhang J, Yu KF. What’s the relative risk? A method of correcting the odds ratio in cohort studies of common outcomes. JAMA. 1998;280: 1690–1691. pmid:9832001
  47. 47. Barros AJ, Hirakata VN. Alternatives for logistic regression in cross-sectional studies: an empirical comparison of models that directly estimate the prevalence ratio. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2003;3: 21. pmid:14567763
  48. 48. Petersen MR. A comparison of two methods for estimating prevalence ratios. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2008;8: 9. pmid:18307814
  49. 49. Population Census 2010. Statistics Bureau. Available from: http://www.stat.go.jp/english/data/kokusei/2010/summary.htm. Cited 09 February 2018
  50. 50. Becker G. A treatise on the family. Boston: Harvard University Press; 1991.
  51. 51. McLoyd V, Wilson L. Maternal behavior, social support, and economic conditions as predictors of distress in children. New Dir Child Dev. 1990;46: 49–69.
  52. 52. Wilkinson R, Marmot M. Psychosocial and material pathways in the relation between income and health: a response to Lynch. BMJ. 2001;322: 1233–1236. pmid:11358781
  53. 53. Hart B, Risley T. Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brooks; 1995.
  54. 54. Elder G, Conger K, Foster E, Ardelt M. Families under economic pressure. J Fam Issues. 1992;13: 5–37.
  55. 55. Meadows S, McLanahan S, Brooks-Gunn J. Parental depression and anxiety and early childhood behavior problems across family types. J Marriage Fam. 2007;69: 1162–1177.
  56. 56. Duncan GJ, Brooks-Gunn J. Family poverty, welfare reform, and child development. Child Dev. 2000;71: 188–196. pmid:10836573
  57. 57. Duncan GJ, Brooks-Gunn J, Klebanov PK. Economic deprivation and early-childhood development. Child Dev. 1994;65: 296–318. pmid:7516849
  58. 58. Garrett P, Ng’andu N, Ferron J. Poverty experience of young children and the quality of their home environments. Child Dev. 1994;65: 331–345. pmid:8013225
  59. 59. Magnuson KA, Votruba-Drzal E. Enduring influences of childhood poverty. In: Cancian M, Danziger S, editors. Changing poverty, changing policies. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 2009. pp. 153–179.
  60. 60. Duncan GJ, Brooks-Gunn J. Family poverty, welfare reform, and child development. Child Dev. 2000;71: 188–196. pmid:10836573
  61. 61. Yeung WJ, Linver MR, Brooks-Gunn J. How money matters for young children’s development: parental investment and family processes. Child Dev. 2002;73: 1861–1879. pmid:12487499
  62. 62. Petterson S, Albers AB. Effects of poverty and maternal depression and early childhood development. Child Dev. 2001;72: 1794–1813. pmid:11768146
  63. 63. Conger RD, Conger KJ, Elder GH, Lorenz FO, Simons RL, Whitbeck LB. A family process model of economic hardship and adjustment of early adolescent boys. Child Dev. 1992;63: 526–541. pmid:1600820
  64. 64. Gershoff ET. Corporal punishment by parents and associated child behaviours and experiences: a meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychol Bull. 2002;128: 539–579. pmid:12081081
  65. 65. McLoyd VC. The impact of economic hardship on black families and children: Psychological distress, parenting, and socioemotional development. Child Dev. 1990;61: 311–346. pmid:2188806
  66. 66. Evans GW. The environment of childhood poverty. Am Psychol. 2004;59: 77–92. pmid:14992634
  67. 67. Wilkinson R, Marmot M. Psychosocial and material pathways in the relation between income and health: a response to Lynch. BMJ. 2001;322: 1233–1236. pmid:11358781
  68. 68. Conger RD, Conger KJ, Elder G. Family economic hardship and adolescent academic performance: Mediating and moderating processes. In: Duncan GJ, Brooks-Gunn J, editors. Consequences of growing up poor. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 1997. pp. 288–310.
  69. 69. Runciman WG. Relative deprivation and social justice. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul; 1966.
  70. 70. Deater-Deckard K, Chen N, Wang Z, Bell MA. Socioeconomic risk moderates the link between household chaos and maternal executive function. J Fam Psychol. 2012;26: 391–399. pmid:22563703
  71. 71. Wilkinson R. Health inequalities: relative or absolute material standards? BMJ. 1997;314: 591–595. pmid:9055723
  72. 72. Eibner C, Evans W. Relative deprivation, poor health habits deprivation, poor health habits, and mortality. J Hum Resour. 2005;40: 591–620.
  73. 73. Huisman M, Araya R, Lawlor DA, Ormel J, Verhulst FC, Oldehinkel AJ. Cognitive ability, parental socioeconomic position and internalising and externalising problems in adolescence: findings from two European cohort studies. Eur J Epidemiol. 2010;25(8): 569–580. pmid:20535529
  74. 74. Miech RA, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Wright BRE, Silva PA. Low socioeconomic status and mental disorders: a longitudinal study of selection and causation during young adulthood. Am J Sociol. 1999;104: 1096–1131.
  75. 75. Fergusson DM, Woodward LJ. Maternal age and educational and psychosocial outcomes in early adulthood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1999;43: 479–489.
  76. 76. Frias-Armenta M., McCloskey LA. Determinants of harsh parenting in Mexico. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1998;26: 129–139. pmid:9634135
  77. 77. Jackson S, Thompson RA, Christiansen EH, Colman RA, Wyatt J, Buckendahl CW, et al. Predicting abuse-prone parental attitudes and discipline practices in a nationally representative sample. Child Abuse Negl. 1999;23: 15–29. pmid:10075190
  78. 78. Kelley ML, Power TG, Wimbush DD. Determinants of disciplinary practices in low-income black mothers. Child Dev. 1992;63: 573–582. pmid:1600823
  79. 79. Dietz TL. Disciplining children: Characteristics associated with the use of corporal punishment. Child Abuse Negl. 2000;24: 1529–1542. pmid:11197032
  80. 80. Klebanov PK, Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan GJ. Does neighborhood and family poverty affect mothers’ parenting, mental health, and social support? J Marriage Fam. 1994;56: 441–455.
  81. 81. Davis-Kean PE. The influence of parent education and family income on child achievement: the indirect role of parental expectations and the home environment. J Fam Psychol. 2005;19: 294. pmid:15982107
  82. 82. Rhee SH, Waldman ID. Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior: a meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychol Bull. 2002;128: 490–529. pmid:12002699
  83. 83. Bouchard TJ. Genetic influence on human psychological traits: a survey. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2004;13: 148–151.
  84. 84. DiLalla LF, Gottesman II. Biological and genetic contributors to violence—Widom’s untold tale. Psychol Bull. 1991;109: 125–129. pmid:2006224
  85. 85. Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Rutter M. Strategy for investigating interactions between measured genes and measured environments. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62: 473–481. pmid:15867100
  86. 86. Boyce WT, Ellis BJ. Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary-developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Dev Psychopathol. 2005;17: 271–301. pmid:16761546
  87. 87. Pluess M, Belsky J. Differential susceptibility to parenting and quality child care. Dev Psychol. 2010;46: 379–390. pmid:20210497
  88. 88. Plomin R, Crabbe J. Dna. Psychol Bull. 2000;126: 806–828. pmid:11107877
  89. 89. Miller GE, Chen E, Fok AK, Walker H, Lim A, Nicholls EF, et al. Low early-life social class leaves a biological residue manifested by decreased glucocorticoid and increased proinflammatory signaling. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106: 14716–14721. pmid:19617551
  90. 90. Umer M, Herceg Z. Deciphering the epigenetic code: an overview of DNA methylation analysis methods. Antioxid Redox Signal. 2013;18: 1972–1986. pmid:23121567
  91. 91. Levine S. Developmental determinants of sensitivity and resistance to stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005;30: 939–946. pmid:15958281
  92. 92. Meaney MJ, Szyf M. Environmental programming of stress responses through DNA methylation: life at the interface between a dynamic environment and a fixed genome. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2005;7: 103–123. pmid:16262207
  93. 93. Susman EJ. Psychobiology of persistent antisocial behavior: stress, early vulnerabilities and the attenuation hypothesis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30: 376–389. pmid:16239030
  94. 94. Meaney MJ. Epigenetics and the biological definition of gene × environment interactions. Child Dev. 2010; 81:41–79. pmid:20331654
  95. 95. van der Knaap LJ, Riese H, Hudziak JJ, Verbiest MM, Verhulst FC, Oldehinkel AJ, et al. Adverse life events and allele-specific methylation of the serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4) in adolescents: the TRAILS study. Psychosom Med. 2015;77: 246–550. pmid:25849128
  96. 96. Borghol N, Suderman M, McArdle W, Racine A, Hallett M, Pembrey M, et al. Associations with early-life socioeconomic position in adult DNA methylation. Int J Epidemiol. 2011; 41:62–74. pmid:22422449
  97. 97. Tehranifar P, Wu H, Fan X, Flom JD, Ferris JS, Cho YH, et al. Early life socioeconomic factors and genomic DNA methylation in midlife. Epigenetics. 2013; 8: 23–27. pmid:23196856
  98. 98. Taylor SE, Way BM, Welch WT, Hillmert CJ, Lehman BJ, Eisenberger NI. Early family environment, current adversity, the serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism, and depressive symptomatology. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60: 671–676. pmid:16934775
  99. 99. Brummett BH, Boyle SH, Siegler IC, Kuhn CM, Ashley-Koch A, Jonassaint CR, et al. Effects of environmental stress and gender on associations among symptoms of depression and the serotonin transporter gene linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR). Behav Genet. 2008;38: 34–43. pmid:17955359
  100. 100. Puig J, Englund MM, Simpson JA, Collins WA. Predicting adult physical illness from infant attachment: a prospective longitudinal study. Health Psychol. 2013;32: 409–417. pmid:22823067
  101. 101. Tsuya NO, Bumpass LL. Gender and housework. In: Tsuya NO, Bumpas LL, editors. Marriage, work & family life in comparative perspective: Japan, South Korea & the United States. Honolulu: University of Hawai’i Press; 2004. pp. 115–133.
  102. 102. National Women’s Education Center of Japan (NWEC). International Comparative Research on ‘Home Education’ 2005: survey on children and the family life. Saitama: National Woman’s Education Center; 2007.
  103. 103. Ben-Shlomo Y, Kuh D. A life course approach to chronic disease epidemiology: conceptual models, empirical challenges and interdisciplinary perspectives. Int J Epidemiol. 2002;31: 285–293. pmid:11980781
  104. 104. Najman JM, Hayatbakhsh MR, Clavarino A, Bor W, O’Callaghan MJ, Williams GM. Family poverty over the early life course and recurrent adolescent and young adult anxiety and depression: a longitudinal study. Am J Public Health. 2010;100: 1719–1723. pmid:20634459
  105. 105. Bennett J, Tayler CP. Starting Strong II: Early Childhood Education and Care. OECD; 2006.
  106. 106. Kearney MS, Levine PB. Income inequality, social mobility, and the decision to drop out of high school. NBER Working Paper 20195. Cambridge: National Bureau of Economic Research; 2014.