Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Behavioural and psychiatric phenotypes in female carriers of genetic mutations associated with X-linked ichthyosis

  • Alice Cavenagh,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics and Division of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, School of Medicine, Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom

  • Sohini Chatterjee,

    Roles Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics and Division of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, School of Medicine, Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom

  • William Davies

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    daviesw4@cardiff.ac.uk

    Affiliations MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics and Division of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, School of Medicine, Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom, School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom, Neuroscience and Mental Health Research Institute, Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom

Abstract

X-linked ichthyosis (XLI) is a rare X-linked dermatological condition arising from deficiency for the enzyme steroid sulfatase (STS). STS is normally expressed in the brain, and males with XLI exhibit personality differences from males in the general population, and are at increased risk of developmental and mood disorders. As the STS gene escapes X-inactivation, female carriers of XLI-associated genetic mutations have reduced STS expression/activity relative to non-carrier females, and could manifest similar behavioural phenotypes to males with XLI. Additionally, as STS activity normally increases in female tissues towards late pregnancy and into the puerperium, carrier females could theoretically present with increased rates of postpartum psychopathology. Using a worldwide online survey comprising custom-designed demographic questionnaires and multiple validated psychological questionnaires, we collected detailed self-reported information on non-postpartum and postpartum behaviour in confirmed adult (>16yrs) female carriers of genetic mutations associated with XLI (n = 94) for statistical comparison to demographically-matched previously-published normative data from female controls (seven independent studies, 98≤n≤2562), adult males with XLI (n = 58), and to newly-obtained online survey data from a general population sample of mothers from the United Kingdom and United States of America (n = 263). The pattern of results in carrier females relative to controls was remarkably similar to that previously observed in males with XLI, with evidence for increased rates of developmental and mood disorders, and elevated levels of inattention, impulsivity, autism-related traits and general psychological distress. Carrier females exhibited a significantly elevated rate of postpartum mental health conditions (notably mild depression) relative to controls which could not be accounted for by social factors. Our data confirm the psychological profile associated with XLI-associated mutations, and suggest that female carriers may be at increased risk of psychopathology, including in the postpartum period. These findings are relevant to families affected by XLI, to clinicians involved in the care of these families, and to genetic counsellors.

Introduction

X-linked ichthyosis (XLI) is a dermatological condition characterised by large, dark brown scales occurring primarily on the extensor surfaces and trunk [1]. Most cases of XLI are caused by an X-linked genetic mutation resulting in deficiency for the enzyme steroid sulfatase (STS), which cleaves sulfate groups from a range of steroids; the skin phenotype results from accumulation of cholesterol sulfate in the stratum corneum [1]. 80–90% of XLI cases are caused by complete or partial deletion of the STS gene, with the remaining cases being attributed to point mutations within the gene. The typical deletion encompasses STS and a small number of adjacent genes (including members of the VCX family, PUDP, and PLPNA4) whilst larger deletions can encompass the KAL and NLGN4X genes [2]. Diagnosis of the condition is typically made on the basis of skin appearance and family history, with additional confirmatory biochemical and/or genetic analyses performed for some individuals [1]. As it arises from an X-linked mutation, XLI almost exclusively affects males: in >85% of cases of XLI, the mutation is thought to be inherited from a heterozygous carrier mother [3,4]. Prevalence rates for XLI based upon clinical samples range from 1 in 3000 to 1 in 6000 males [1], and for STS deficiency based upon prenatal screening are approximately 1 in 1500 males [5,6]. These findings suggest that, in many cases, STS deficiency does not result in clinically-identifiable disease. The inheritance data and prevalence rates above indicate that approximately 1 in 1200 females within the general population may be heterozygous carrier females, equivalent to ~30,000 individuals in the United Kingdom, and ~3.1million individuals worldwide.

STS is expressed in the developing [7] and adult [8] human brain, and males with XLI exhibit a personality and mental health profile distinct from that of males within the general population [2,4,9,10]; specifically, the former group exhibit higher rates of inattention, impulsivity (but not motor impulsivity), mood problems, disruptive behaviour and autism-related traits, and are more likely to be diagnosed with developmental conditions (including Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and autism) and mood disorders. Recently, a case report has identified a psychotic disorder resembling early-onset schizophrenia in a boy with XLI [11]. Importantly, these findings are unlikely to be confounded by ascertainment biases, in that high rates of psychopathology are seen in boys with XLI ascertained solely on the basis of maternal endocrine abnormalities during pregnancy [2], and a remarkably similar suite of behaviours to that reported in males with XLI is also seen Sts-deficient mouse models [1216].

The STS gene escapes X-inactivation [17], and carrier females heterozygous for XLI-associated genetic mutations exhibit reduced STS expression, and reduced protein levels/enzyme activity [3]. Consistent with this, carrier females can show comparable, though milder, phenotypes to males with XLI [1]. The behaviour and mental health of carrier females has not been systematically assessed, although one report has described paranoid schizophrenia in two female cases with deletions encompassing STS [18]. In female mammals, STS expression/activity increases during late pregnancy and into the puerperium in blood and brain tissue [19], whilst carrier mothers are at increased risk of experiencing stressful obstetric complications (specifically delayed or prolonged labour due to STS absence in the fetal placenta) relative to general population controls [1]. Hence, we have previously hypothesised that deficiency for the STS enzyme might result in an increased risk of postpartum psychopathology [19], notably for postpartum psychosis-related phenotypes [20].

Here, we aimed to determine whether behavioural measures and mental health risk (including in the postpartum period) differed between female carriers of XLI-associated genetic mutations and females from the general population, and whether the pattern of differences mirrored those seen in males with XLI. Using established and effective social media-focussed recruitment methods [9,21], we undertook an online survey of confirmed female carriers of XLI-associated mutations using well-validated and detailed self-report behavioural questionnaire measures. Data related to non-postpartum behaviours were compared to previously-published demographically-matched normative sample, and adult male XLI, data. Data relevant to the postpartum period were compared to normative data obtained from a demographically-matched, newly-recruited and online-surveyed general population sample of mothers.

Our a priori hypotheses were that, relative to females within the general population, carrier females (similar to males with XLI) would exhibit evidence for: impaired attention and a general increase in impulsivity (but normal, or reduced, motor impulsivity), hyperactivity, increased levels of depressive and anxiety-related phenotypes, increased behavioural inflexibility and social impairment, increased rates of psychotic experiences, and a greater likelihood of having been diagnosed with one or more developmental, mood, or postpartum psychiatric disorders.

We successfully recruited, and analysed online survey data from, a comparatively-large cohort of adult female carriers of XLI-associated genetic mutations from around the world, and identified a number of behavioural differences compared to normative female samples which were largely consistent with the aforementioned hypotheses.

Methods

Ethics

The studies were approved by Cardiff University School of Psychology Research Ethics Committee.

Survey structure for female carriers of XLI-associated genetic mutations

An online survey (Survey 1) was generated using Qualtrics software to be completed by confirmed adult (>16yrs) female carriers of genetic mutations associated with XLI. After an online consent form, the first part of the survey comprised demographic information including participant age, ethnic origin, country of residence, handedness, smoker status, and highest educational level attained. The second part of the survey asked about the basis on which carrier status was confirmed, and, if a genetic test was performed, the nature of the mutation. The third part of the survey asked about the participant’s sensory acuity (scored on a Likert scale from 1 = excellent to 5 = very poor for each sense), general personality type based upon the ‘Four Temperaments’ [22], and any previous diagnosis of developmental, psychiatric or neurodegenerative illness.

Participants were then asked to complete a series of five psychological questionnaires selected on the basis of previous data: i) the Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale version 1.1 (ASRS v1.1), a five-point response 18-item Likert-scale screening questionnaire assessing recent attentional (nine items) and hyperactive-impulsive (nine items) traits based on diagnostic criteria for ADHD and previously established for use in adults [23]; the presence of ≥4 symptoms (indexed by scores of ≥2 or ≥3 depending on the question) from Part A is consistent with, but not diagnostic for, a presentation of ADHD ii) the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11), a four-point response 30-item Likert-scale questionnaire assessing aspects of impulsivity [24] for which total scores, second-order factor scores (attentional, motor and planning impulsivity) and first-order factor scores (attention, cognitive instability, motor performance, perseverance, self-control and cognitive complexity) were calculated, iii) the adult Autism Quotient (AQ), an extensively-validated four-point response 50-item Likert-scale questionnaire assessing behavioural traits associated with autism spectrum disorders [25]; total and sub-scale scores (social skill, attention-switching, attention to detail, communication and imagination) were calculated, iv) the Kessler Psychological Distress Scale (K10) [26], a five-point 10-item Likert-scale questionnaire assessing recent depression or anxiety-related traits. K10 items were scored from 1–5 (total score range 10–50) with a score ≥20 being consistent with significant psychological distress and v) the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire, a 74-item ‘Yes/No’ screen designed to assess behavioural traits associated with schizotypal personality disorder in the general population, with items endorsed ‘Yes’ scoring one point [27]; total and sub-scale scores (items of reference, excessive social anxiety, odd beliefs or magical thinking, unusual perceptual experiences, odd or eccentric behaviour, no close friends, odd speech, constricted affect and suspiciousness) were calculated. Finally, participants completed a demographic questionnaire on their childbearing history (including any pregnancy-related medical complications or postpartum psychiatric diagnoses) and the Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences (CAPE-42) applied to the 6-week period following the birth of any of their children; the CAPE-42 is a 42-item four-point response scale designed to assess the mean frequency (1 = never to 4 = nearly always) and associated distress (1 = not distressed to 4 = very distressed) of positive, depressive and negative experiences related to psychosis within the general population [28]. The survey concluded with a short debrief describing the purpose of the study and its main hypotheses, links to relevant previous literature, and an explanation of mechanisms by which the study results would be disseminated.

Survey structure for adult mothers from the general population

A second online survey (Survey 2) was generated using Qualtrics software to be completed by adult mothers from the general populations of United Kingdom or United States of America to provide normative behavioural data relating to the postpartum period. After completing an online consent form, participants completed a general demographic questionnaire before completing a questionnaire on their childbearing history (including any pregnancy-related medical complications or postpartum psychiatric diagnoses) and the Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences (CAPE-42) applied to the 6-week period following the birth of any of their children. The survey concluded with a short debrief similar to that for Survey 1.

Participant recruitment and survey completion

We conservatively estimated, based upon our previous BIS-11, AQ and K10 data in adult males with XLI [9], that for two-tailed analyses with α = 0.05, a sample size of 85 female carriers would provide ~83% power to reliably detect an effect size half that observed in males. The URL for Survey 1 was advertised worldwide via ichthyosis support groups and their websites (in some cases after additional ethical and academic review), and through social media sites (including relevant Facebook pages, and Twitter). Participants completed the survey anonymously, and results were returned to the research team upon completion. Data were collected between 17th November 2017 and 7th June 2018. The URL for Survey 2 was advertised via the Qualtrics online participant panel [29] and respondents were reimbursed in accordance with standard Qualtrics procedures. Participants completed the survey anonymously, and results were returned to the research team upon completion. Data were collected between 8-9th January 2018.

Data analysis and statistics

Questionnaires were analysed as previously described in the literature. In the rare circumstance of participants not answering specific questions (which occurred <2.5% of the time), mean values for the group were imputed. Normality of data was assessed using Shapiro-Wilk test. Summary data are reported as percentages, mean values±standard deviation of the mean (if normally-distributed) or as median values with 95% confidence limits (if non-normally distributed); where non-normally distributed data are being directly compared to normally-distributed data, mean values±standard deviation of the mean are also presented for the former. Categorical data were analysed using Chi-Squared test with Yates’ correction or Fisher Exact Test. Testing to compare behavioural measures in the female carrier sample to the ‘average’ or mean score was done using a One Sample t-test/Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Test. Traits within the same set of individuals were compared using paired t-test or Related Samples Wilcoxon Signed Rank test. Data from female carriers of XLI-associated genetic mutations and previously-published normative control samples from seven independent studies were compared using Welch’s t-test with the degrees of freedom calculated using the Welch-Satterthwaite equation. P-values were calculated from the resultant t-statistic and degrees of freedom using online GraphPad software [30]. Data from female carriers of XLI-associated genetic mutations and adult males with XLI/general population mothers from our newly-recruited online sample were compared using categorical analyses and unpaired two-tailed t-test/Mann Whitney U-Test depending upon normality of the data. Summary descriptions of all control and comparison samples are available in S1 Table. P-values <0.05 were regarded as nominally-significant. Post hoc Principal Component Analysis (PCA) of the female carrier data for the 47 measures across ASRS, BIS-11, AQ, K10, SPQ and CAPE-42 questionnaires using SPSS 25 identified 8 distinct components explaining the majority (83%) of the total sample variance (Component 1 = 44%, Component 2 = 13%, Component 3 = 6% and Components 4–8 <5% each); thus only p-values<0.00625 (0.05/8) should be regarded as surviving conservative multiple testing correction.

Data availability

Raw survey data are available in S2 and S3 Tables.

Results

Recruitment and demographics of female carrier sample

A total of 104 females responded to Survey 1, 10 of whom were subsequently excluded from further analysis as their presentation was inconsistent with being a carrier for an XLI-associated mutation. The demographic features of the remaining 94 individuals, who we had high confidence were genuine carriers based upon their survey responses, are reported in Table 1. Although we did not genotype individuals within our female carrier sample, some participants reported their previously-defined genetic mutation; the ratio of carriers with atypical deletions (~2.5-5Mb) to carriers with typical deletions (~1.5–1.7Mb) or point mutations is consistent with previous literature [2,4].

thumbnail
Table 1. Demographic features of our sample of female carriers of genetic mutations associated with X-linked ichthyosis (XLI).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212330.t001

Sensory function in female carriers

Large deletions encompassing STS have been associated with anosmia in some cases [1], whilst STS is expressed in regions of the developing human tongue [7]; hence, we wanted to assess basic sensation in our female carrier cohort. Sensory function across all five domains was reported as being significantly better than average i.e. ‘3’ (p<0.001). Low numbers of individuals reported ‘very poor’ sensory function (vision: 2/94, hearing: 1/94, taste: 0/94, smell: 0/94 and touch: 0/94).

Personality types in female carriers

Within the female carrier group (n = 93 due to no response from one participant), participants self-identified most closely with the following personality attributes: ‘optimistic and social’ (30/93, 32%), ‘short-tempered and irritable’ (29/93, 31%), ‘analytical and quiet’ (31/93, 33.5%) and ‘relaxed and peaceful’ (3/93, 3.5%). Comparing these frequencies to large-scale worldwide gender-specific data from a commercial company assessing personality traits within the general population across >2million online tests (>1million individuals) [31] indicates that within our sample there is a substantially elevated proportion of individuals self-identifying as ‘short-tempered or irritable’ (31% vs. 17.5%), and a lower proportion self-identifying as ‘analytical and quiet’ (33.5% vs. 43.5%) and ‘relaxed and peaceful’ (3.5% vs. 7%); the proportion of individuals self-identifying as ‘optimistic and social’ was equivalent within our female carrier sample and the general population sample (32% vs. 32%).

Previous developmental, psychiatric or neurodegenerative diagnoses in the non-postpartum period in female carriers

A high proportion (39.5%) of our sample reported a previous non-postpartum diagnosis of a developmental, psychiatric or neurodegenerative condition; 29.8% reported a previous mood disorder diagnosis, 7.6% reported a previous developmental condition diagnosis, and 2.1% reported a previous mixed mood and developmental condition diagnosis (Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Developmental and mood disorders self-reported in our sample of female carriers of genetic mutations associated with X-linked ichthyosis (XLI).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212330.t002

Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS) scores in female carriers

A remarkably high proportion of carrier females (45/94, 48%) met the criteria for further investigations for possible ADHD i.e. ≥4 symptoms in Part A of the screening questionnaire. When compared to a mixed-gender adult (31.1±10.3yrs) general population sample from Sweden (n = 202, with no significant difference in scores between males and females)[32], our female carrier sample scored significantly, and substantially, more highly on both the Part A screener questions (12.2±4.8 vs. 8.9±3.5, t[141] = 5.96, p<0.0001) and on the total score measure (35.3±13.5 vs. 24.6±8.7, t[129] = 7.07, p<0.0001)(Cohen’s d = 0.79 and 0.94 respectively). Within the female carrier sample, the total inattentive symptom count was significantly higher than the total hyperactive-impulsive symptom count, and the total inattention scores were significantly higher than total hyperactive-impulsive scores (both p<0.001)(Table 3). The three carriers of large atypical deletions demonstrated marginally elevated numbers of total symptoms (8.7±5.7) but similar total scores (36.7±11.6) to the overall sample. The total ASRS symptoms and scores across the four individuals previously diagnosed with ADHD alone, or with ADHD and comorbid depression, were 14.5±4.0 and 54.5±12.1 respectively, consistent with the questionnaire being sensitive to ADHD-relevant behaviours.

thumbnail
Table 3. Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS) symptoms and scores in our sample of female carriers of genetic mutations associated with X-linked ichthyosis (XLI).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212330.t003

Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11) scores in female carriers compared to female controls

All BIS-11 measures from our female carrier group (n = 92) were compared to a normative sample of 1184 adult female college student and community-recruited participants screened for Axis I psychiatric disorders and positive drug, alcohol or pregnancy tests (mean age ~21.5yrs) designed to be broadly reflective of the healthy general population of the USA [33]; total BIS-11 scores within our sample were also compared to an adult female general population sample (n = 1503, mean age ~41.5yrs) from Canada screened for severe cognitive impairments [34]. The mean total score for our female carrier sample (65.8±12.4) was towards the high end of the usual range of impulsiveness (52–71) and was significantly, and substantially, higher than that of both the USA (62.1±10.6) and Canadian (55.9±9.6) general population samples (t[101] = 2.77, p<0.01, and t[97] = 7.50, p<0.0001, Cohen’s d = 0.32 and 0.89 respectively). Individuals carrying atypical large deletions performed similarly to the overall carrier sample on the total BIS-11 score measure (66.7±7.6). The total BIS-11 score across the four individuals previously diagnosed with ADHD alone, or ADHD with comorbid depression, was extremely high (85.0±8.5) consistent with the questionnaire being sensitive to clinically-relevant impulsive traits. In terms of second order BIS-11 factors, carrier females scored more highly than USA general population controls on attentional and non-planning impulsivity, but did not differ in terms of motor impulsivity; with respect to first order factors, female carriers scored more highly than general population controls on measures of attention, cognitive instability, perseverance, and self-control, but equivalently on a measure of cognitive complexity, and lower (though not significantly so) on a specific measure of motor impulsivity (Table 4).

thumbnail
Table 4. Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11) scores in our sample of female carriers of genetic mutations associated with X-linked ichthyosis (XLI) and a normative general population control sample.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212330.t004

Autism Quotient (AQ) scores in female carriers compared to female controls

All AQ measures from our female carrier group (n = 90) were compared to a normative sample of 98 unscreened adult female participants recruited from the UK general population (mean age ~37yrs)[25]; total AQ scores from our sample were also compared to a large adult female general population sample recruited online and screened for autism and other psychiatric disorders (n = 2562, aged 34.4±4.4yrs) [35]. The total score for our female carrier sample (21.0 (95%CI: 18.0–23.0); 21.6±8.8) was towards the high end of the usual female range for autism-related traits [25], and was significantly, and substantially, higher than that of both the smaller (15.4±5.7) and larger (17.1±7.6) normative general population samples (t[150] = 5.66, p<0.0001 and t[93] = 4.77, p<0.0001, Cohen’s d = 0.84 and 0.55 respectively). The three individuals carrying atypical large deletions scored more highly, on average, than the overall carrier sample on the total AQ score measure (30.0±11.5). The female carrier previously diagnosed with autism had a total score of 34 on the AQ, consistent with the questionnaire’s senstivity to autism-related traits. On four out of five AQ subscales, the female carrier sample scored significantly more highly than the normative control sample; however, on the ‘Attention to Detail’ subscale, there was no significant difference between the two groups’ scores (Table 5).

thumbnail
Table 5. Autism Quotient (AQ) scores in our sample of female carriers of genetic mutations associated with X-linked ichthyosis (XLI) and a normative general population control sample.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212330.t005

Kessler Psychological Distress Scale (K10) scores in female carriers compared to female controls

Data from the female carrier sample (n = 91) were compared to normative data from an unscreened community-recruited Australian adult female sample comprising 882 individuals (35–44yrs) [36]. Total scores in the female carrier sample were significantly, and substantially, larger than those seen in the normative sample (23.0 (95%CI: 20.0–25.0); 23.6±8.5 vs. 15.5±8.9 respectively, t[111] = 8.63, p<0.0001), Cohen’s d = 0.93) and were in the range consistent with the presence of a mild mental disorder (2024). Compared to the overall female carrier sample, the three female carriers of atypical deletions exhibited slightly lower than average total K10 scores (21.7±5.7). As expected, the 25 individuals with a previous diagnosis of depression alone, or depression with another comorbid psychiatric condition, had high K10 scores (29.9±8.5), consistent with a diagnosis of a moderate-severe mental disorder.

Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ) scores in female carriers compared to female controls

SPQ data from the female carrier sample (n = 88) were compared to normative data from an unscreened sample of adult (39.8±10.3yrs) females (n = 184) recruited from the community in Australia [37]. Female carriers exhibited significantly, and substantially, higher total SPQ scores than the normative sample (24.0 (95%CI: 20.0–28.5);25.5±15.2 vs.15.9±11.4 respectively, t[134] = 5.43, p<0.0001, Cohen’s d = 0.73). The total SPQ scores for the two female carriers with atypical deletions who completed this questionnaire were on the lower side of the average for the carrier group (16.5±6.4). On the majority of SPQ subscales, carrier females scored significantly more highly than females from the normative sample; however, on the ‘Unusual Perceptual Experiences’ measure the two groups performed equivalently, whilst the female carrier group scored nominally-significantly lower on the ‘Odd beliefs and magical thinking’ measures (Table 6).

thumbnail
Table 6. Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ) scores in our sample of female carriers of genetic mutations associated with X-linked ichthyosis (XLI) and a normative general population control sample.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212330.t006

Relationship between psychological questionnaire scores in female carrier sample

Within the female carrier sample, all total questionnaire scores (ASRS, BIS-11, AQ, K10 and SPQ) were highly significantly correlated with one another (correlation coefficients>0.48, p<0.001) apart from BIS-11 and AQ total scores which were moderately significantly correlated (Spearman’s rho = 0.22, p = 0.04).

Comparison of demographics and psychological questionnaire scores in female carrier sample and previous sample of adult males with XLI

We compared the demographics and total ASRS, BIS-11, AQ and K10 scores from our female carrier sample to a previously-ascertained adult male sample with XLI [9](Table 7). The two samples were well-matched for most demographic variables, although our female carrier sample was significantly more highly-educated. On average, total questionnaire scores were consistently lower in the female carrier than in the male XLI group, but they were only significantly lower in the case of the AQ and K10 measures (and only nominally so for the AQ) (Table 7).

thumbnail
Table 7. Demographics and total psychological questionnaire scores in our sample of female carriers of genetic mutations associated with X-linked ichthyosis (XLI) and adult males with XLI.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212330.t007

Comparison of demographics of female carrier mothers and general population sample of UK and US mothers

83 of our female carrier sample had given birth to at least one child, and we recruited 263 mothers from the general UK and USA populations as a control sample. The control sample, on average, were marginally younger at survey completion than the female carrier sample, had their first child at a slightly younger age, and had slightly fewer children overall; the two samples did not differ significantly on any other demographic measure we assessed (Table 8).

thumbnail
Table 8. Demographics of our sample of female carriers of genetic mutations associated with X-linked ichthyosis (XLI) and our adult female general population sample of mothers.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212330.t008

Pregnancy and postpartum-related medical complications and mental health conditions in female carriers compared to general population mothers

As expected, female carriers of mutations associated with XLI exhibited a significantly, and substantially (>threefold), higher rate of delayed or prolonged labour relative to females from the general population; the two groups did not differ with respect to any of the other pregnancy or childbirth-related medical conditions we examined (Table 9).

thumbnail
Table 9. Pregnancy and childbirth-related medical complications in our sample of female carriers of genetic mutations associated with X-linked ichthyosis (XLI) and our adult female general population sample of mothers; note that one individual could have experienced multiple complications.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212330.t009

Female carriers exhibited a significantly elevated (~1.5fold) burden of all aggregated postpartum mental health conditions, and ‘depressive’ postpartum mental health conditions, relative to general population controls (Table 10). Importantly, this finding was unlikely to be due to the former group being exposed to more adverse external social circumstances during pregnancy and postnatally; in fact there was a trend for the opposite to be the case (Table 10). For specific postpartum conditions, only the prevalence of ‘baby blues’ (mild depression not requiring further medical treatment) was significantly higher in the female carrier group (Table 10). Both females carrying large atypical deletions reported experiencing the ‘baby blues’ following one or more pregnancies. We tested whether there was any relationship between stressful childbirth i.e. delayed or prolonged labour and an increased rate of postpartum mental health conditions in our carrier sample: 26 female carriers experienced both delayed/prolonged labour and a postpartum mental health condition, 26 delayed/prolonged labour but no postpartum mental health condition, 12 no delayed/prolonged labour but a postpartum mental health condition, and 19 no delayed/prolonged labour and no postpartum mental health condition (χ2[1] = 0.59, p = 0.44). We also tested the postpartum-specificity of our findings by assessing whether carrier females who reported a postpartum mental health condition (n = 38) scored more highly than those who didn’t (n = 45) on the K10 ‘baseline’ (non-postpartum) measure of depressive and anxiety-related traits; whilst the former group scored more highly, on average, on the K10 (25.5±9.0 vs. 22 (95%CI: 19.0–24.0); 22.4±8.0), this was not significant (U = 672.5, p = 0.10). Female carriers who reported a postpartum mental health condition were not currently older, did not have their first child later, and did not have more children, than female carriers who did not report a postpartum mental health condition; age: 41.7±9.4yrs vs. 41.0 (95%CI: 35.0–46.0); 42.6±11.8yrs (U = 838.0, p = 0.88), age at first child: 27.9±5.0yrs vs. 26.9±5.7yrs (t[70] = 0.77, p = 0.44), and number of children: 2.0 (95%CI: 2.0–2.0) vs. 2.0 (95%CI: 2.0–2.0)(U = 855.0, p = 1.00) respectively.

thumbnail
Table 10. Postpartum mental health conditions in our sample of female carriers of genetic mutations associated with X-linked ichthyosis (XLI) and our adult female general population sample of mothers; note that one individual could have experienced multiple conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212330.t010

Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences (CAPE-42) scores related to the postpartum period in female carrier and general population mothers

We attempted to obtain a quantitative measure of positive, depressive and negative experiences related to psychosis in the postpartum period using the CAPE-42 [28] in our female carrier and general population samples. Female carriers reported significantly less frequent positive dimension psychosis-related experiences than females from the general population, but these experiences, when they occurred, caused equivalent amounts of distress in both groups (Table 11). Whilst the average frequency of depressive psychosis-related experiences (and the distress caused by these) was greater in the female carrier group, the two groups did not differ significantly; nor was there any significant group diffference with respect to the frequency of, or distress caused by, negative dimension psychosis-related experiences (Table 11). The two females carrying large atypical deletions scored similarly to the overall carrier sample (≤2 on all measures). Within the female carrier group, those reporting postpartum mental health conditions (n = 37) scored significantly higher on depressive (frequency and distress) and negative (frequency and distress) dimension experiences compared to those not reporting such conditions (n = 44): 2.45±0.69 vs. 2.09±0.63 (t[77] = 3.43, p = 0.001), 2.63±0.72 vs. 2.25±0.62 (t[77] = 3.54, p = 0.001), 2.14 (95%CI:1.79–2.57) vs. 1.79 (95%CI:1.57–2.14) (U = 504.5, p = 0.003), and 2.09±0.57 vs. 1.87±0.42 (t[77] = 2.34, p = 0.02) respectively; these two groups did not differ with respect to positive dimension experience frequency (1.20 (95%CI:1.10–1.25) vs. 1.25 (95%CI:1.20–1.30)(U = 721.5, p = 0.38)) or associated distress (2.00 (95%CI:1.77–2.11 vs. 1.60 (95%CI:1.25–2.00)(U = 366.5, p = 0.11)). Hence, the CAPE-42, as applied to the postpartum period, does appear to be sensitive to mental health symptoms.

thumbnail
Table 11. Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences-42 (CAPE-42) scores in our sample of female carriers of genetic mutations associated with X-linked ichthyosis (XLI) and our adult female general population sample of mothers.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212330.t011

Discussion

The pattern of non-postpartum psychiatric and behavioural traits demonstrated across multiple converging measures in our well-powered sample of carrier females bears remarkable similarities to that exhibited by adult males affected by XLI [9] i.e. a moderately increased prevalence of developmental/mood diagnoses (4.3% ADHD vs. ~2.5% in general adult female population [38], 1.1% autism vs. ~0.1% in general adult female population [39] and 27.6% depression vs. ~26.7% lifetime prevalence in adult female USA general population aged 35-49yrs [40]), together with generally elevated inattentive, irritable, impulsive, and depression, autism and anxiety-related traits relative to gender-matched control subjects, but with equivalent, or reduced, levels of motor impulsivity and ‘attention to detail’. The relatively high rate of developmental conditions within our female carrier sample is in line with data from the DECIPHER database [41] in which related phenotypes (e.g. developmental delay, language impairments, intellectual disability, autism and inattention) are frequently seen in female carriers with comparatively small genetic deletions (<2Mb) encompassing STS (S4 Table). The consistency of findings across female carriers and males with XLI (and, additionally, with rodent model work), indicates that our previously-reported observations regarding the neurobehavioural features associated with STS deficiency are robust and generalisable. Here, we further showed that female carriers generally exhibit higher levels of schizotypal personality-related traits than age and gender-matched controls, although with respect to ‘positive’ psychotic experience-related measures the former group scored lower or equivalently; schizotypal personality-related traits were not assayed in males with XLI. The observation that individuals carrying atypical deletions were largely similar to individuals carrying smaller mutations, apart from on the AQ where they scored slightly higher, is consistent with the idea that a gene absent in atypical but not atypical deletions (most likely NLGN4X) is relatively specifically involved in autism-related behavioural phenotypes [2]. Our observation that multiple questionnaire scores were strongly correlated with one another suggests one or more possibilities regarding the effects of STS deficiency: a) it has pleiotropic effects across multiple brain regions/psychological processes, the magnitude of which can vary between individuals, b) it impacts predominantly upon one core cognitive/psychological process common to multiple behavioural phenotypes e.g. attention, or c) it impacts upon one or more psychological processes in early development and subsequent adult behaviours are a response to the effects of this e.g. early high levels of autism-related traits may feasibly result in a later expression of depression and anxiety-related traits through a variety of mechanisms, including increased victimisation [42]. Future work involving detailed longitudinal characterisation of neuropsychological differences in carrier females and males with XLI should help to resolve which of these mechanisms is most plausible. It is also important to recognise that psychological traits in female carriers may conceivably be affected by both biological factors (STS deficiency and its physiological consequences) and environmental/social factors e.g. the stress of raising a child, or children, with a long-term medical condition (XLI).

In males with XLI, the STS enzyme is generally completely absent, whereas in carrier females it is expected to be haploinsufficient; hence, after allowing for normal gender differences in the expression of some psychological traits, we expected carrier females to demonstrate milder phenotypes. Whilst autism and depression/anxiety-related traits were significantly lower in carrier females than males with XLI, the two groups did not differ significantly with respect to ADHD-related traits or impulsivity (although average values were slightly lower in carrier females); these latter findings suggest that any true difference between carrier females and males with XLI (who are known to be at significantly elevated risk of ADHD) on ADHD-relevant measures is likely to be relatively small.

With respect to medical conditions associated with pregnancy and childbirth, we specifically observed the expected, and marked, elevation in prolonged/delayed labour in female carriers relative to females from the general population; this ‘control’ result provides confirmatory evidence that self-identified ‘carriers’ for XLI-associated genetic mutations are likely to be true carriers, and suggests that participants provided largely-accurate retrospective medical histories.

We successfully managed to recruit and phenotype a closely demographically-matched general population sample of mothers to compare with our female carrier sample. Importantly, self- and retrospectively-reported rates of postpartum psychiatric diagnoses in this normative sample were highly-consistent with previous epidemiological evidence from females within the general population [43] i.e. 14% vs. ~13% for postpartum depression, and 7% vs. ~10% for postpartum anxiety. Consistent with one of our original hypotheses, we identified a significantly, and substantially (~1.5-fold), higher burden of aggregated postpartum psychiatric conditions in our female carrier sample relative to the control sample; this group difference was primarily driven by a higher prevalence of depressive conditions (particularly in the mild category) and could not be readily explained by differential adverse social circumstances between the groups. We previously proposed that STS deficiency may predispose to postpartum psychosis [20]. Here, we found no evidence that carrier status confers very large increases in postpartum psychosis risk, but given both the low prevalence and the likely complex and polygenic nature of this condition, and our limited sample size, the possibility that STS deficiency moderately increases risk cannot be completely discounted. However, using the CAPE-42 measure applied to the postpartum period, we found no evidence for an increased rate of psychosis-related experiences across the female carrier group relative to the normative sample; indeed, consistent with the SPQ finding, the frequency of positive dimension experiences was actually lower in the carrier group.

The increased rate of depressive postpartum disorders in our female carrier sample could potentially have been due to the fact that this group was slightly older than the normative sample, had their children later (increased maternal age is a risk factor for postpartum mood conditions [44]), and had more children (hence, more opportunity for developing postpartum mental health problems); however, the size of the between-group differences on these demographic variables was small, and we found no evidence that female carriers reporting, or not reporting, postpartum mental health conditions differed with respect to any of these demographic measures. Our data suggest no clear relationship between delayed/prolonged labour and later postpartum psychiatric distress in female carriers, although given the relatively low power of our sample for this analysis a subtle relationship between these variables cannot be definitively ruled out; this would be interesting to explore further given comparatively high rates of caesarian births in female carriers (due to prolonged labour) and a putative relationship between caesarian birth and postpartum stress [45]. As female carriers with postpartum psychiatric diagnoses did not differ significantly from those with no postpartum psychiatric diagnosis on the K10 measure of recent depression and anxiety-related traits, we tentatively speculate that STS deficiency in late pregnancy and the postpartum period may exacerbate any non-postpartum psychiatric symptoms at these timepoints.

Our study has a number of limitations, notably: a) a lack of objective clinical and genetic confirmation, b) the possibility of response bias whereby more severely affected individuals are more motivated to participate in the study, and c) a reliance upon previously-published normative data and self, and retrospectively-reported, data. Additionally, as referred to above, the findings are simply descriptive and cannot shed light upon the mechanistic processes via which STS deficiency might ultimately impact upon physiology. We argue that these limitations do not significantly impact upon our main conclusions for the following reasons: a) participants were only included in the analyses if their carrier status was highly likely, and, consistent with this, our carrier sample exhibited expected physiological and behavioural features (i.e. high rates of delayed/prolonged labour and significant phenotypic overlap with males with XLI), b) there is no strong a priori reason to suspect response bias in that female carrier status has traditionally been considered to have little, or no, effect on phenotype, and c) carriers were explicitly compared to large control cohorts representative of the general population which were demographically very well (but not perfectly) matched, and for which data had been obtained in the same manner (i.e. by retrospective self-report on exactly the same measures).

Conclusions

The current data, together with previous work, help to confirm a psychological and psychiatric profile for individuals with STS deficiency; future basic science in males with XLI, in female carriers, and in rodent models will aim to identify and characterise the biological (neural and endocrine) substrates which mediate the behavioural and psychiatric phenotypes described above; these analyses could provide clinically-relevant information on possible new therapeutic targets, and insights of more general relevance to the pathophysiology of idiopathic developmental and mood disorders. Previous work assaying STS gene expression in developing human brain, and on the neurochemical and genetic sequelae of STS loss of function in rodents, has provided initial clues as to neural substrates requiring more focussed investigation: specifically, these studies have implicated corticostriatothalamic, hippocampal, and cerebellar (dys)function, changes in the cholinergic and serotonergic systems, and a suite of gene expression changes [7,13,14,19,46,47]. We have previously argued that acute inhibition of STS in the postpartum period in mice may elicit effects on maternal behaviour via increased brain expression of CCN protein family members [48]; the recent finding that CTGF (also known as CCN2) is a prodepressant [49] suggests that elevated expression of this protein could partially explain the excess of postpartum depressive phenotypes in our carrier female sample.

We hope that the present information will help to continue to raise awareness of behavioural/psychiatric features associated with XLI (and carrier status for associated genetic mutations) amongst: a) families affected by the condition, b) relevant medical professionals (General Practitioners, dermatologists, psychologists/psychiatrists and obstetricians), c) patient support charities and groups, and d) genetic counsellors, such that affected individuals have the best possible access to advice and multidisciplinary medical care should it be required. Finally, the current findings, and planned future work, may be useful for understanding the basis of neurobehavioural/cognitive abnormalities including elevated autism, ADHD and anxiety risk in females with Turner syndrome (X-monosomy) in which STS expression is haploinsufficient [5052].

Supporting information

S1 Table. Description of the various control samples compared to our newly-recruited female carrier sample.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212330.s001

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Demographic, psychiatric and behavioural data from female carrier sample.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212330.s002

(XLSX)

S3 Table. Demographic, psychiatric and behavioural data from control sample of mothers from the USA and UK.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212330.s003

(XLSX)

S4 Table. Developmental and behavioural phenotypes in female carriers of small (<2Mb) deletions spanning STS from the DECIPHER database.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212330.s004

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all of the respondents to our surveys, the administrators of the various Facebook sites we advertised on, staff at Qualtrics, and the following organisations who aided with advertisement of the study and recruitment of participants: Ichthyosis Support Group (UK), Foundation for Ichthyosis and Related Skin Types (FIRST) (USA), DermnetNew Zealand Trust and the British Skin Foundation.

References

  1. 1. Fernandes NF, Janniger CK and Schwartz RA (2010) X-linked ichthyosis: an oculocutaneous genodermatosis. J Am Acad Dermatol 62: 480–485. pmid:20080321
  2. 2. Kent L, Emerton J, Bhadravathi V, Weisblatt E, Pasco G, Willatt LR, et al. (2008) X-linked ichthyosis (steroid sulfatase deficiency) is associated with increased risk of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, autism and social communication deficits. J Med Genet 45: 519–524. pmid:18413370
  3. 3. Cuevas-Covarrubias SA, Valdes-Flores M, Orozco Orozco E, Diaz-Zagoya JC and Kofman-Alfaro SH (1999) Most "sporadic" cases of X-linked ichthyosis are not de novo mutations. Acta Derm Venereol 79: 143–144. pmid:10228635
  4. 4. Diociaiuti A, Angioni A, Pisaneschi E, Alesi V, Zambruno G, Novelli A, et al. (2018) X-linked ichthyosis: Clinical and molecular findings in 35 Italian patients. Exp Dermatol.
  5. 5. Hand JL, Runke CK and Hodge JC (2015) The phenotype spectrum of X-linked ichthyosis identified by chromosomal microarray. J Am Acad Dermatol 72: 617–627. pmid:25659225
  6. 6. Craig WY, Roberson M, Palomaki GE, Shackleton CH, Marcos J and Haddow JE (2010) Prevalence of steroid sulfatase deficiency in California according to race and ethnicity. Prenat Diagn 30: 893–898. pmid:20715120
  7. 7. Stergiakouli E, Langley K, Williams H, Walters J, Williams NM, Suren S, et al. (2011) Steroid sulfatase is a potential modifier of cognition in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Genes Brain Behav 10: 334–344. pmid:21255266
  8. 8. Perumal AS and Robins E (1973) Regional and subcellular distribution of aryl-and steroid sulfatases in brain. Brain Res 59: 349–358. pmid:4747761
  9. 9. Chatterjee S, Humby T and Davies W (2016) Behavioural and Psychiatric Phenotypes in Men and Boys with X-Linked Ichthyosis: Evidence from a Worldwide Online Survey. PLoS One 11: e0164417. pmid:27711218
  10. 10. Rodrigo-Nicolas B, Bueno-Martinez E, Martin-Santiago A, Canueto J, Vicente A, Torrelo A, et al. (2018) Evidence of the high prevalence of neurological disorders in nonsyndromic X-linked recessive ichthyosis: a retrospective case series. Br J Dermatol 179: 933–939. pmid:29901853
  11. 11. Malik A, Amer AB, Salama M, Haddad B, Alrifai MT, Balwi MA, et al. (2017) X-linked ichthyosis associated with psychosis and behavioral abnormalities: a case report. J Med Case Rep 11: 267. pmid:28934990
  12. 12. Davies W, Humby T, Trent S, Eddy JB, Ojarikre OA and Wilkinson LS (2014) Genetic and pharmacological modulation of the steroid sulfatase axis improves response control; comparison with drugs used in ADHD. Neuropsychopharmacology 39: 2622–2632. pmid:24842408
  13. 13. Trent S, Dean R, Veit B, Cassano T, Bedse G, Ojarikre OA, et al. (2013) Biological mechanisms associated with increased perseveration and hyperactivity in a genetic mouse model of neurodevelopmental disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38: 1370–1380. pmid:23276394
  14. 14. Trent S, Cassano T, Bedse G, Ojarikre OA, Humby T and Davies W (2012) Altered serotonergic function may partially account for behavioral endophenotypes in steroid sulfatase-deficient mice. Neuropsychopharmacology 37: 1267–1274. pmid:22189290
  15. 15. Trent S, Dennehy A, Richardson H, Ojarikre OA, Burgoyne PS, Humby T, et al. (2012) Steroid sulfatase-deficient mice exhibit endophenotypes relevant to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37: 221–229. pmid:21723668
  16. 16. Davies W, Humby T, Kong W, Otter T, Burgoyne PS and Wilkinson LS (2009) Converging pharmacological and genetic evidence indicates a role for steroid sulfatase in attention. Biol Psychiatry 66: 360–367. pmid:19251250
  17. 17. Carrel L and Willard HF (2005) X-inactivation profile reveals extensive variability in X-linked gene expression in females. Nature 434: 400–404. pmid:15772666
  18. 18. Milunsky J, Huang XL, Wyandt HE and Milunsky A (1999) Schizophrenia susceptibility gene locus at Xp22.3. Clin Genet 55: 455–460. pmid:10450863
  19. 19. Davies W (2018) The steroid sulfate axis and its relationship to maternal behavior and mental health. J Mol Endocrinol.
  20. 20. Davies W (2012) Does steroid sulfatase deficiency influence postpartum psychosis risk? Trends Mol Med 18: 256–262. pmid:22475435
  21. 21. Davies W (2016) Insights into rare diseases from social media surveys. Orphanet J Rare Dis 11: 151. pmid:27829465
  22. 22. Mendlowicz MV, Jean-Louis G, Gillin JC, Akiskal HS, Furlanetto LM, Rapaport MH, et al. (2000) Sociodemographic predictors of temperament and character. J Psychiatr Res 34: 221–226. pmid:10867117
  23. 23. Kessler RC, Adler L, Ames M, Demler O, Faraone S, Hiripi E, et al. (2005) The World Health Organization Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS): a short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychol Med 35: 245–256. pmid:15841682
  24. 24. Patton JH, Stanford MS and Barratt ES (1995) Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol 51: 768–774. pmid:8778124
  25. 25. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J and Clubley E (2001) The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord 31: 5–17. pmid:11439754
  26. 26. Kessler RC, Andrews G, Colpe LJ, Hiripi E, Mroczek DK, Normand SL, et al. (2002) Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychol Med 32: 959–976. pmid:12214795
  27. 27. Raine A (1991) The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophr Bull 17: 555–564. pmid:1805349
  28. 28. Konings M, Bak M, Hanssen M, van Os J and Krabbendam L (2006) Validity and reliability of the CAPE: a self-report instrument for the measurement of psychotic experiences in the general population. Acta Psychiatr Scand 114: 55–61. pmid:16774662
  29. 29. Unknown author. Qualtrics Online Sample [cited 11 July 2018]. Available: https://www.qualtrics.com/online-sample/
  30. 30. Unknown author. GraphPad QuickCalcs P-value Calculator [cited 11 July 2018]. Available: https://www.graphpad.com/quickcalcs/pvalue1.cfm
  31. 31. Unknown author. Frequency of personality types [cited 11 July 2018]. Available: http://www.mypersonality.info/personality-types/population-gender/
  32. 32. Edebol H, Helldin L and Norlander T (2013) Measuring adult Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder using the Quantified Behavior Test Plus. Psych J 2: 48–62. pmid:24294490
  33. 33. Stanford MS, Matthias CW, Dougherty DM, Lake SL, Anderson NE and Patton JH (2009) Fifty years of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale: An update and review. Personality and Individual Differences 47: 385–395.
  34. 34. Reyes Allyon A (2012) Impulsivity in men and women: A general population study in the Southwest of Montreal. Department of Psychiatry. McGill University.
  35. 35. Baron-Cohen S, Cassidy S, Auyeung B, Allison C, Achoukhi M, Robertson S, et al. (2014) Attenuation of typical sex differences in 800 adults with autism vs. 3,900 controls. PLoS One 9: e102251. pmid:25029203
  36. 36. Slade T, Grove R and Burgess P (2011) Kessler Psychological Distress Scale: normative data from the 2007 Australian National Survey of Mental Health and Wellbeing. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 45: 308–316. pmid:21332432
  37. 37. Badcock JC and Dragovic M (2006) Schizotypal personality in mature adults. Personality and Individual Differences 40: 77–85.
  38. 38. Simon V, Czobor P, Balint S, Meszaros A and Bitter I (2009) Prevalence and correlates of adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry 194: 204–211. pmid:19252145
  39. 39. Brugha TS, McManus S, Bankart J, Scott F, Purdon S, Smith J, et al. (2011) Epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders in adults in the community in England. Arch Gen Psychiatry 68: 459–465. pmid:21536975
  40. 40. Kessler RC, Birnbaum H, Bromet E, Hwang I, Sampson N and Shahly V (2010) Age differences in major depression: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R). Psychol Med 40: 225–237. pmid:19531277
  41. 41. Firth HV, Richards SM, Bevan AP, Clayton S, Corpas M, Rajan D, et al. (2009) DECIPHER: Database of Chromosomal Imbalance and Phenotype in Humans Using Ensembl Resources. Am J Hum Genet 84: 524–533. pmid:19344873
  42. 42. Rai D, Culpin I, Heuvelman H, Magnusson CMK, Carpenter P, Jones HJ, et al. (2018) Association of Autistic Traits With Depression From Childhood to Age 18 Years. JAMA Psychiatry 75:835–843. pmid:29898212
  43. 43. Meltzer-Brody S, Howard LM, Bergink V, Vigod S, Jones I, Munk-Olsen T, et al. (2018) Postpartum psychiatric disorders. Nat Rev Dis Primers 4: 18022. pmid:29695824
  44. 44. Muraca GM and Joseph KS (2014) The association between maternal age and depression. J Obstet Gynaecol Can 36: 803–810. pmid:25222359
  45. 45. Chen HH, Lai JC, Hwang SJ, Huang N, Chou YJ and Chien LY (2017) Understanding the relationship between cesarean birth and stress, anxiety, and depression after childbirth: A nationwide cohort study. Birth 44: 369–376. pmid:28594092
  46. 46. Trent S, Fry JP, Ojarikre OA and Davies W (2014) Altered brain gene expression but not steroid biochemistry in a genetic mouse model of neurodevelopmental disorder. Mol Autism 5: 21. pmid:24602487
  47. 47. Trent S and Davies W (2013) Cognitive, behavioural and psychiatric phenotypes associated with steroid sulfatase deficiency. World Journal of Translational Medicine 2: 1–12.
  48. 48. Humby T, Cross ES, Messer L, Guerrero S and Davies W (2016) A pharmacological mouse model suggests a novel risk pathway for postpartum psychosis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 74: 363–370. pmid:27728876
  49. 49. Turner CA, Sharma V, Hagenauer MH, Chaudhury S, O'Connor AM, Hebda-Bauer EK, et al. (2018) Connective Tissue Growth Factor Is a Novel Prodepressant. Biol Psychiatry 84:555–562. pmid:29861095
  50. 50. Hong DS and Reiss AL (2012) Cognition and behavior in Turner syndrome: a brief review. Pediatr Endocrinol Rev 9 Suppl 2: 710–712.
  51. 51. Davies W, Humby T, Isles AR, Burgoyne PS and Wilkinson LS (2007) X-monosomy effects on visuospatial attention in mice: a candidate gene and implications for Turner syndrome and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 61: 1351–1360. pmid:17161381
  52. 52. Zinn AR, Roeltgen D, Stefanatos G, Ramos P, Elder FF, Kushner H, et al. (2007) A Turner syndrome neurocognitive phenotype maps to Xp22.3. Behav Brain Funct 3: 24. pmid:17517138