Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Adults’ visual recognition of actions simulations by finger gestures (ASFGs) produced by sighted and blind individuals

  • Dannyelle Valente ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    dannyelle.valente@univ-lyon2.fr

    Affiliations Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland, Department of Developmental Psychology, University of Lumière Lyon, Bron, France

  • Amaya Palama,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland

  • Jennifer Malsert,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland

  • Guillemette Bolens,

    Roles Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Faculty of Humanities, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland

  • Edouard Gentaz

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland

Abstract

The present study examines the visual recognition of action simulations by finger gestures (ASFGs) produced by sighted and blind individuals. In ASFGs, fingers simulate legs to represent actions such as jumping, spinning, climbing, etc. The question is to determine whether the common motor experience of one’s own body is sufficient to produce adequate ASFGs or whether the possibility to see gestures from others are also necessary to do it. Three experiments were carried out to address this question. Experiment 1 examined in 74 sighted adults the recognition of 18 types of ASFGs produced by 20 blindfolded sighted adults. Results showed that rates of correct recognition were globally very high, but varied with the type of ASFG. Experiment 2 studied in 91 other sighted adults the recognition of ASFGs produced by 10 early blind and 7 late blind adults. Results also showed a high level of recognition with a similar order of recognizability by type of ASFG. However, ASFGs produced by early blind individuals were more poorly recognized than those produced by late blind individuals. In order to match data of recognition obtained with the form that gestures are produced by individuals, two independant judges evaluated prototypical and atypical attributes of ASFG produced by blindfolded sighted, early blind and late blind individuals in Experiment 3. Results revealed the occurrence of more atypical attributes in ASFG produced by blind individuals: their ASFGs transpose more body movements from a character-viewpoint in less agreement with visual rules. The practical interest of the study relates to the relevance of including ASFGs as a new exploratory procedure in tactile devices which are more apt to convey action concepts to blind users/readers.

Introduction

Gestures are movements made by the hand or body, meant to express or clarify spoken content [13]. They can be codified into an autonomous language, as is the case with sign languages used by deaf communities [4]. Various studies have been conducted to describe and classify the diversity of gestures in sighted communities [1, 3, 5, 6]. In Why We Gesture, McNeill [2] posits that we gesture to orchestrate speech. Some gestures are used to emphasize prosody and match the rhythm of speech (beat gestures) while others represent attributes of concrete objects, spatial relationships and actions (representational gestures) [5]. Representational gestures are used, for example, to describe the width of a container, the size of a person or to simulate the gait of an animal.

In a perspective of embodied cognition, Hostetter and Alibali [5] support the idea that gestural activity emerges from the activation of visual and motor imagery. The degree to which each type of imagery is involved is related to the point of view used by the speaker when he or she gestures. Two points of view are principally identified, character-viewpoint and observer- viewpoint [1]. The occurrence of these points of view varies according to the profile of subjects and their capacity to think through imagery. A character-viewpoint is produced as though the speaker’s body is the body of the character [5], for example when a speaker mimicking the character’s manual actions (throwing, pounding, etc.). The speaker might also use his/her hand or fingers to show a non-manual action, for example when he/she moves his/her hand up and down to simulate the legs of a cat [1]. Observer-viewpoint gestures depict the motion involved in a scene observed at a distance, for example when a speaker simulates with the hand a car that climbs a sloping street. Gestures produced from a character-viewpoint result more from motor imagery than observer-viewpoint gestures, which principally result from visual imagery.

The present study examined a special category of representational gestures that simulates leg movements using the index and middle fingers. Assessing the production of action simulations by finger gestures (ASFGs) in the absence of sight has a practical interest. We studied the relevance of including ASFGs to illustrate the contents of tactile books for the blind. To explore the illustration, children’s fingers act as the two legs of a character. In our design idea, illustrations use unfolding pop-up mechanisms to produce a miniature 3D scene, upon which the two fingers/legs perform various actions: jumping on a trampoline, climbing stairs, etc. In most cases, tactile books for the blind contain 2D tactile illustrations that are more or less directly transferred from visual illustrations, formed by using raised lines or areas of low relief textures. Several studies have shown that these can be hard for children and adults with visual impairments to understand [712]. Linked with embodied views of perception and concepts [1315] we hypothesized that incorporating body experiences into the book could be a promising way to better adapt illustrative contents for blind readers. Embodied approaches to cognition argue that our sensorimotor experiences with real objects contribute to perceptual and conceptual processes. For example, the concept stairs, rather than being an abstract and arbitrary representation of its components, is composed of the simulation of our real experience of climbing and going down stairs [13]. In the same way, studies have shown that reactivation of motor components involved in interactions with objects (enactment effect) can facilitate learning and memorization of concepts [16, 17]. These components can also help in processing contents in the context of blindness. Basing illustrations on sensorimotor rather than visual components also has the advantage of being based on experiences that are possibly the same for sighted and blind children [18, 19].

In order to validate this idea, it is first necessary to know whether a simulation process that transposes the experiences of the legs by imitating their actions with the fingers is meaningful for blind as well for sighted individuals.

Regarding more generally the occurrence of gestures in congenitally blind persons, studies showed that they produced gestures when they spoke, even though they had never seen gestures and had no experience with their communicative function [2022]. Results suggested that gestures may serve a cognitive function for the speakers themselves, beyond their communicative value. However, the gestural activity of the blind or individuals with visual impairment seems to differ from that of sighted. They use more adaptors (continuous body touching with no manifest relation with their speech) than illustrator or representational gestures linked to the content of the conversation [2325].

The use of two fingers to simulate the legs of a character, a special category of gestures that will be examined in our study, is part of some games in infancy (nursery rhymes, finger soccer, etc). “Action Simulations by Finger Gestures–ASFG” are also included in the sign language of deaf people to express concepts like climbing stairs, sitting or jumping (Spreadhesign dictionary, European Center of Deaf Language). However, no studies have examined whether individuals spontaneously produce similar patterns of ASFGs. If so, similar prototypical patterns of ASFGs would be observed in blindfolded sighted. Therefore, the comparison between early blind, late blind and blindfolded sighted could allow us to evaluate the role of visual experience in the production of ASFGs. If visual representations and visual experience play a role, similar prototypical patterns of ASFGs would be observed only in late blind and blindfolded sighted. If visual representations and visual experience do not play a role, similar prototypical patterns of ASFGs would be observed in early blind, late blind and in blindfolded sighted.

Because in ASFGs the two fingers are used as a replacement of the legs, we can expect a strong involvement of motor imagery in the process. But individuals also execute gestures with a communicative purpose. Thus, visual experiences could be necessary to convey norms of gestural expressivity learned by face-to-face interactions. These norms can relate to an Observer-viewpoint of the gesture [5]. Because early blind individuals cannot, from birth or shortly thereafter, see others’ gestures, they cannot learn these norms. Thus, if ASFG produced by early blind individuals are recognized in the same way as ASFG produced by late blind and sighted individuals, this would provide compelling evidence of a stronger involvement of motor imagery than visual imagery in this type of gesture.

Three experiments were carried out to examine the occurrence of “Action Simulations by Finger Gestures–ASFG” in blind and sighted adults and their visual recognition by adults. The aim of Experiment 1 was to examine, in 74 sighted adults, the visual recognition of 18 types of ASFGs produced by 20 blindfolded sighted adults. Experiment 2 investigated the role of visual experience in the production of the types of ASFGs and their visual recognition. Thus, it examined in 100 other sighted adults visual recognition of the same 18 types of ASFGs presented in Experiment 1, but this time produced by 10 early blind and 7 late blind adults. In Experiment 3, two independent judges analysed prototypical and atypical attributes of ASFG produced by blindfolded sighted, early blind and late blind individuals.

Experiment 1: Visual recognition of ASFG produced by blindfolded sighted adults

In Experiment 1, ASFGs produced by 20 blindfolded sighted adults were videotaped and presented to 74 sighted judges in an online visual recognition task. Because these gestures are based on the same perceptual experience for sighted individuals who produce these stimuli and the sighted individuals who should recognize them, we hypothesized that ASFGs produced by blindfolded sighted participants will obtain very high rates of recognition. However, some errors of interpretation can be expected among actions which have a similar appearance, such as Squatting and Sitting or Skating and Skiing.

Method

Participants

A total of 80 sighted adults (48 women and 32 men, age range 20–70 years) took part in a online visual recognition task consisting of videos of ASFGs produced by blindfolded sighted. Because the task design used an online survey (produced with Quatrics platform) for the final sample, we took into account only those participants who gave a response for more than 90% of the videos. Considering also the hypothesis that subjects can complete the survey without performing the task seriously or attentively, we excluded participants for whom the mean of recognition was less than two standard deviations (2 SD) away from the global mean of recognition. Thus, the final sample was composed of 74 sighted adults participants (44 women and 30 men, age range 20–70 years). All participants are francophone although their nationalities vary (70% french, 22% swiss and 8% other nationalities). French is the native language for 91% of them. Participants’ responses were collected anonymously.

Stimuli: Production of 18 ASFGs by blindfolded sighted adults

Twenty blindfolded sighted adults (16 women and 4 men; mean age = 27.5 years, SD = 3.48, range = 22–36 years) were asked to produce ASFGs of 18 actions. They were recruited at the University of Geneva. The study was conducted with the written consent of each participant, in accordance with Declaration of Helsinki and controlled and authorized by the Swiss Ethics Committee.

Blindfolded sighted adults were videotaped individually with a Sony HDR–CX 220 camera facing them. They were asked to simulate 18 actions, verbally announced by the experimenter, using the index and middle fingers of their right hand. Between actions, participants were to go back to the resting position (hands placed flat on the table). This procedure limited the influence of the final spatial positioning of an action on the next one. The action presentation order was randomized across subjects. No constraint in terms of space was imposed. Subjects were free to raise and move their hand in all directions and to use the left hand as a support for the gesture if necessary. This study respects ethical principles for research involving human subjects (World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki) and was approved by the Swiss Ethics Committee on reseach involving humans. The individuals described in this manuscript have given written informed consent (as outlined in the PLOS consent form) to publish these case details.

Table 1 presents actions used in the preparation of stimuli. The list encompasses the broadest range of actions that it is possible to simulate with finger gestures. Conjointly with designers at Les Doigts Qui Rêvent, a French publishing house specialized in the production of tactile books for blind children, we selected ASFGs which would be technically possible to include in a future tactile book.

With CyberLink Power Editor 14, videos of 3 seconds were prepared for each ASFG (Fig 1); each of the 18 actions was produced by 20 encoders (i.e 360 videos at total). All 360 videos were included in the visual recognition task.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Examples of stimuli.

ASFGs of Playing on a swing and Climbing stairs produced by blindfolded sighted participants. Videos of ASFGs are available at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=zr2Rr28vzoE and https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=Uzzly9EUZcA.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214371.g001

Experimental condition of the visual recognition task

The task of visually recognizing the AFSGs (360 stimuli) was conducted using an online survey organized in four blocks. A total of 20 different sighted participants viewed each block. The online survey procedure was chosen to obtain the most varied profiles of decoders, totally naïve regarding the study’s hypothesis. Content was divided in 4 blocks no longer than 30 minutes each to minimize the risk the survey would be dropped, often important in online survey platforms. Each block contained a total of 90 videos, including sequences of each of the 18 actions in equal number (18 actions x 5 encoders).

We instructed blindfolded sighted participants to watch each video and click the action that they thought the person had simulated with their fingers. The response measure was a multiple-choice method with 18 choices (e.g. 18 actions included in the study). Presentation of videos was randomized across decoders.

Data analysis of visual recognition task

The percentages of correct recognition as well as the percentages of other meanings attributed to the action were measured. Among the 360 ASFGs, a total of 20 were excluded from subsequent analysis because they had a mean recognition rate less or more than two standard deviations (2 SD) away from the mean recognition rate per action. To maximize approximation to normality for generalized linear model analysis, arcsin transformation was performed on rates of correct recognition [26] (data in S1 Dataset). Results from both statistical analysis (with ratios data and using arcsin transformation) will be presented.

Preliminary main effects ANOVA showed no significant effect of gender (Ratio: F(1, 68) = .86, p = .35, ηp2 = .01/Arcsin transformation: F(1, 68) = .67, p = .41, ηp2 = .01) on rates of recognition of sighted participants (data in S1 Dataset). Consenquently, data were further collapsed across this factor.

In order to control block effect, an 18 (each action) x 4 (survey response blocks) factorial ANOVA was performed on the mean of action recognition with the actions as within-subjects factor and blocks as between-subjects factors. Block conditions were significant, but only using arcsin transformations (Ratio: F(3, 268) = 2.30, p = .07, ηp2 = .02/Arcsin transformation: F(3,268) = 4.23, p = .005, ηp2 = .04). However the interaction between actions and blocks conditions was not significant in both cases (Ratio: F(51, 268) = .57, p = .99, ηp2 = .09/Arcsin transformation: F(51, 268) = .66, p = .95, ηp2 = .11). Given the fact that the rate of recognition of all actions is the same for all blocks, results were further collapsed across blocks.

A one-way ANOVA was performed on the mean of action recognition (ratios data and after arcsin transformation) with the actions as within-subjects factor. A cross-tabulation was also performed to examine the relationship between the original intended meaning of actions (i.e., ASFG produced) and the meaning attributed to the ASFG by participants (i.e., ASFG chosen from the list of possible answers). The significance threshold was .05; effect sizes are given in partial eta-squared η for ANOVAs.

Results and discussion

To facilitate readability, only means of ratios data will be presented in this section. The overall results with ratios data and arcsin transformation are available in S1 Appendix. On average, action simulations produced by blindfolded sighted were very well recognized (M = 80.5%, SD = 13.8%). An 18 (each action) one-way ANOVA by means of recognition showed a significant effect for action condition (Ratio: F(17, 322) = 14.16, p < .001, ηp2 = .42/Arcsin transformation: F(17, 322) = 20.33, p < .001, ηp2 = .51). Fig 2 contains percentages of recognition for each action. We observed that mean rates of recognition were very high for most of these: 9 simulated action simulations were recognized at a rate between 80–100% (grey bars), 7 between 60–80% (dark blue bars) and only 2 between 60–40% (light blue bars).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Rates of visual recognition (N = 74) for ASFGs produced by blindfolded sighted individuals (N = 20).

Colors differenciate rates between 80–100% (grey), between 60–80% (dark blue) and between 60–40% (light blue). The vertical bars represent positive standard deviations. Results with arcsin transformation is available in S1 Appendix.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214371.g002

A cross-tabulation (Table 2) showed relationships between the intended meanings and attributed meanings for the 9 actions recognized at less than 80%. We observed that among the 12 incorrect meanings attributed at a level which was above the level of chance (in grey), the vast majority are in the same category as the intended meaning of that ASFG. For instance, in the category To turn, the incorrect meaning Turning on a merry-go-round is attributed at a level of 11,4% to Turning in place. Inversely, an incorrect meaning Turning in place is also attributed at a level of 21,4% to Turning on a merry-go-round. Similar results were founded in other actions within the same category, such as Skating and Skiing or Playing leapfrog and Jumping off a Step.

thumbnail
Table 2. Cross-tabulation of ASFG produced by blindfolded sighted individuals (Columns) and the meaning attributed by participants (Rows).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214371.t002

In accordance with our hypothesis, rates of correct recognition were globally high for ASFGs produced by blindfolded sighted. This result suggests a prototypical effect: gesture patterns for each action are both similar enough as a whole yet enough distinct from others to enable participants to arrive at a correct response in a consensual and unambiguous way. Results also showed that recognition rates varied with the type of ASFG. Differences in recognizability were related to differences in the degree of similarity between actions included in the list. As expected, confusions in recognition involved ASFGs which were similar because they are in the same type of action (Turn, Slide or Jump). Logically, well-recognized ASFGs possessed a distinctive attribute that differentiated them from all others: the Balancing movement in Playing on a swing or the Back out movement in Walking backwards, for example.

Experiment 2: Visual recognition of ASFGs produced by late and early blind adults

Experiment 2 investigated the role of visual experience in the production of the types of ASFG and their visual recognition by sighted adults. Thus, its aim was to examine in a different set of 100 sighted adults visual recognition of 18 ASFGs produced by 10 early blind and 7 late blind adults. Experiment 2 would determine if blind participants produced the same patterns of gesture, and if these were as well-recognized as those produced by sighted individuals.

Considering that ASFGs are based on real motor experiences of the legs that are common between the two groups, blind individuals who produced stimuli and sighted individuals who recognized them, our first hypothesis is that ASFGs produced by late and early blind would also obtain good rates of recognition.

However, even if a motor component is expected, the ASFG also possesses a representational purpose (to illustrate an action to someone). According to studies which have shown the rare occurrence of representational gestures in congenitally blind individuals [2325] we expected that sighted participants would have some difficulty in recognizing ASFGs produced by early blind participants, more so than those produced by late blind participants with previous visual experience.

Method

Participants

One hundred sighted subjects (80 women and 20 men, age range = 20–70 years) who had not participated in Experiment 1 responded to new blocks of online surveys presenting videos of gestures produced by late blind and early-blind. Following the same selection procedure used in Experiment 1, our final sample was composed of 91 sighted adult participants (74 women and 17 men, age range 20–70 years). All participants are French speakers, although their nationalities vary (73% french, 20% Swiss and 8% other nationalities). French is the native language for 91% of them. Participants’ responses were collected anonymously.

Stimuli: Production of 18 ASFGs by blind adults

Ten early blind and 7 late blind adults participated at this stage (see Table 3, Characteristics of blind adults). The group of early blind subjects is composed of 9 women and 1 man, 4 Swiss and 6 French, aged between 28 and 70 (M = 43.5 years, SD = 12.7). Six participants were congenitally blind since birth and 4 became blind before the age of 3. The group of late blind subjects was composed of 1 woman and 6 men, 3 Swiss and 4 French, aged 27 to 69 (M = 47.7, SD = 13.7). Late blind participants became blind at ages ranging from 7 to 37. French participants were recruited with the help of the publishing house “Les Doigts Qui Rêvent” and Swiss participants with the help of “l’Association Suisse pour le Bien des Aveugles”. The study was conducted with the written consent of each participant. This research respects ethical principles for research involving human subjects (World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki) and was approved by the Swiss Ethic Committee on reseach involving humains. The individuals described in this manuscript have given written informed consent (as outlined in the PLOS consent form) to publish these case details.

The procedure for production of ASFGs by blind adults was identical to that of blindfolded sighted participants. One early blind participant produced no ASFG for Playing Leapfrog and Turning on a merry-go-round. One late blind produced no ASFG for Climbing. They stated they did not know how to simulate these actions with two fingers. Other participants produced all of the 18 action simulations requested. In total, 303 videos (ASFGs made by 17 encoders for 18 actions, minus 3 missing simulations mentioned) were included in the recognition task.

Experimental condition of the visual recognitiontask

Anticipating that ASFGs produced by blind participants would be less representative than those made by sighted and that the visual recognition task would probably require more decision time, we reduced the number of videos to be viewed in each block in Experiment 2. Contents were divided into 5 blocks; each block contained an equal number of videos per action. Each survey block was viewed by 20 different sighted individuals. Profiles of early and late blind were included in the blocks in a randomized manner.

Data analysis of visual recognition task

ASFGs with a mean rate of recognition less or more than two standard deviations (2 SD) from the global mean recognition rate by action for their profile group (late blind and early blind) were excluded. As in Experiment 1, arcsin transformation was performed on rates of correct recognition (data in S2 Dataset) and results from both statistical analysis (with ratios data and using arcsin transformation) will be presented.

Preliminary analysis of variance (ANOVAs) was performed to control for gender on rates of recognition of sighted participants in the online survey. Once again, results showed no significant effect on the rates of recognition (Ratio: F(1,85) = .03, p = .85, ηp2 = <. 001/Arcsin transformation: F(1,85) = .01, p = .92, ηp2 < .001) and data were further collapsed across these factor.

An 18 (each action) x 5 (response blocks) factorial ANOVA was performed on the mean of action recognition (ratios data and after arcsin transformation) with the actions as within-subjects factors and blocks as between-subjects factors. The block condition was significant (Ratio: F (4, 199) = 3.88, p < .001, ηp2 = .07/Arcsin transformation: F (4, 199) = 3.45, p = .005, ηp2 = .07), but the interaction between actions and blocks conditions was not significant (Ratio: F(68, 199) = 0.67, p = .97, ηp2 = .18/Arcsin transformation = F(68, 199) = .85, p = .76, ηp2 = .22). Given the fact that the rate of recognition of all actions is the same for all blocks, results were further collapsed across blocks.

Results and discussion

Factorial ANOVA on mean of action recognition by groups showed that the group condition was significant (Ratio: F(1, 253) = 24.75, p < .001, ηp2 = .09/Arcsin transformation: F(1, 253) = 28, p < .001, ηp2 = .10). Means of recognition with ratios data and arcsin transformation are availables in S1 Appendix. On average, action simulations produced by late blind individuals were better recognized (M = 77.0%, SD = 16.6%) than those produced by early blind individuals (M = 64.5%, SD = 18.5%). Figs 3 and 4 present the order of recognizability of ASFGs produced by late blind and early blind respectively. Factorial ANOVA also showed that the action condition was significant (Ratio: F(17, 253) = 9.58, p < .001, ηp2 = .40/Arcsin transformation: F(17, 253) = 13, p < .001, ηp2 = .46) but interaction between action and group conditions was not significant (Ratio: F(17, 253) = 1.24, p = .23, ηp2 = .08/Arcsin transformation: F(17, 253) = 1.18, p = .27, ηp2 = .07). Similar differences in recognizability by action was found in both groups. However, a greater number of ASFGs produced by early blind were situated in the set of recognizability of 60–40% or less.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Rates of visual recognition (N = 92) for ASFGs produced by late blind individuals (N = 7).

Bar color differenciates rates between 80–100% (grey), between 60–80% (dark blue) and between 60–40% (light blue). The vertical bars represent positive standard deviations. Results with arcsin transformation are available in S1 Appendix.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214371.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. Rates of recognition (N = 92) for ASFGs produced by early blind individuals (N = 10).

Bar color differenciates rates between 80–100% (grey), between 60–80% (dark blue) and between 60–40% (light blue). The vertical bars represent positive standard deviations. Results with arcsin transformation are availables in S1 Appendix.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214371.g004

Tables 4 and 5 present cross-tabulation between the intended meaning and meaning attributed to ASFGs produced by late blind and early blind respectively. Among the 11 actions produced by the group of late blind with a recognition rate below 80% and the 14 actions produced by early blind with a recognition rate below 80%, we observe 18 and 21 occurrences, respectively, of incorrect meaning (in grey) attributed at a level above the level of chance. Observe that certain identity confusions of ASFGs were specific to the group of early blind: Sliding on a toboggan was confused with Skiing (27%), and vice versa (17%), Going down stairs was confused with Climbing stairs (13%) and Jumping off a step confused with Going down stairs (11,3%). Note also that it is only in the case of early blind that an incorrect meaning was attributed more often than the correct intended meaning: Climbing is identified as Climbing at 36.9% and as Climbing stairs at 38.5% and Squatting is identified as Squatting at 41% and as Sitting at 49.7%.

thumbnail
Table 4. Cross-tabulation of ASFG produced by late blind (columns) and the meaning attributed by participants (rows).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214371.t004

thumbnail
Table 5. Cross-tabulation of ASFG produced by early blind (columns) and the meaning attribuated by participants (rows).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214371.t005

Results and discussion

The results observed in Experiments 1 and 2 are globally similar: a very high level of visual recognition and a very similar order of recognizability among the 18 actions. In fact, the recognition rates obtained by ASFGs produced by early blind individuals are quite good (64.5%) though less accurate than those of late blind individuals, which are excellent (77.0%) and very close to results obtained by ASFGs produced by sighted individuals (81.5%). Results from cross-tabulations between the intended meaning of the ASFG and the meaning attributed by participants in the recognition task revealed that some incorrect meanings were specific to the ASFG produced by the early blind group.

Experiment 3: Additional evaluation by two independent judges of prototypical and atypical attributes of ASFGs produced by blindfolded sighted, late blind and early blind individuals

In order to match data of production with data of recognition obtained in Experiment 3 two independent judges were asked to evaluate and classify each ASFG as prototypical or atypical in regard to their similarity with a prototypical exemplar. This procedure of codage is based on Rosh and Mervis's theory of family resemblances [27]. In sum, members of a category come to be viewed as typical exemples in proportion to how many attributes they have in common with other members. A prototypical member is one who has the most attributes in common with other members of the category and the fewest attributes in common with other categories. In a connected field, a study about the recognition and production of tactile geometric shapes showed that prototypical effects depend on visual experience, given that no occurrence of this effect was found in congenitally blind adults in a tactile task [28]. In Experiment 3, we expected the ASFGs produced by three groups (and more particularly the ASFGs produced by blindfolded sighted individuals) would be evaluated as globally protypical. Following results obtained in Experiments 1 and 2, we also expected that ASFGs judged as more protoypical would be those that possesses a distinctive attribute that differentiated them from all others (e.g. Playing on a swing or Walking backwards). However, some evaluations of atypical attributes of ASFG produced by blind individuals can be expected among specific actions in which ambiguous meaning was attributed in Experiment 2 (e.g. Sliding on a toboggan or Going down stairs). To test these hypotheses, each judge was asked to evaluate each of 663 videos of ASFGs produced by three groups. For ASFGs judged as atypical, judges were asked to describe attributes that cause problems.

Method

Participants

Two research assistants, Swiss University of Geneva Master's students in Psychology aged 23 and 25, were recruited to evaluate all ASFGs produced by blindfolded sighted, late blind and early blind individuals in Experiments 1 and 2. They were unaware of the purpose of the experiment and the experimental conditions.

Experimental condition

In total, each judge looked at 663 actions (e.g. 37 ASFG x18 actions = 666 minus 3 missing ASFGs in the group of blind individuals as indicated in Experiment 2). First, they were instructed to look at all the videos of ASFGs for each action set: 37 ASFGs in total by action set with 20 produced by sighted, 10 by early blind and 7 by late blind participants in each action set). They were not preliminary informed about the visual status of ASFG encoders. Then, they were asked to classify each ASFG as typical or atypical in regard to their similarity with a prototypical exemplar. For ASFGs judged as atypical, judges were asked to write attributes that cause problems (data in S3 Dataset). Global percentage agreement between the two judges was 79%.

Results and discussion

Table 6 presents prototypical and atypical ASFGs produced by blindfolded sighted, early blind and late blind individuals. Highlighted areas (grey) present a description of protopypical ASFG and how many times ASFGs produced by each group (columns) were considered by judges as prototypical. White lines presented how many times judges considered ASFG as atypical with examples of atypical attributes listed.

thumbnail
Table 6. Prototypical and atypical ASFGs produced by blindfolded sighted, early blind and late blind individuals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214371.t006

As expected, the first result, the 18 ASFGs that obtained a high level of recognition in Experiments 1 and 2 by action, has been judged as a prototype (Jumping on one leg, Playing on a swing, Walking backward). 68%, 39% and 44% ASFGs produced by blindfolded sighted, early blind and late blind individuals, respectively were coded as prototypical by judges and 14%, 37% and 29% were coded as atypical. The remainer of the ASFGs produced did not obtain an agreement from judges (e.g. 18% for blindfolded sighted, 23% for early blind and 26% for late blind). Overall, these results indicated that ASFGs produced by blindfolded sighted individuals were more prototypical than those produced by early and late blind individuals. Here the prototypical affect is influenced by the visual status of ASFG producers.

It is interesting to observe that in actions such as Playing on a swing, Pedaling and Skating, only the early blind group produced the gesture with two hands or two arms. We argue that here, they produce a gesture rather from a character-viewpoint than from an observer- viewpoint. Concerning the simulation of the action Sliding on a toboggan, even if early blind, late blind and sighted individuals produced the same finger gesture pattern (i.e. fingers that slide), 6 to 10 of early blind did not raise their hand. Their two fingers remained on a surface simulating only the motor experience of the body which slides in contact with the toboggan's surface. It is important to note that this simulation is perfectly relevant to a tactilo-kinesthetic experience. When we slide on a toboggan, our body stays in contact with the toboggan throughout the entire slide. The same behavior was identified in the actions Going down stairs and Climbing Stairs, also presented in ASFG produced by late blind individuals in this case. Some atypical attributes of ASFG produced by early blind and, to lesser extent, late blind individuals seem to be more centred in the tactile-kinesthesic or haptic experience of the body.

General discussion

Despite some atypical attributes found to a greater degree in ASFGs produced by early blind individuals, all three groups obtained good rates of recognition in Experiments 1 and 2. In Experiment 3, the percentage of ASFGs coded as atypical by both judges is 68% for blindfolded sighted producers, 39% for early blind producers and 44% for late blind producers. Qualitative and descriptive analysis of atypical attributes showed that ASFGs produced by early blind individuals convey more a character-viewpoint and less the visual norms of gestural communication. These results could explain why ASFGs produced by early blind participants were less recognized by sighted adults in Experiment 2 and also why they were more coded as atypical by two independent judges in Experiment 3. The results obtained corroborate those of other studies showing that representational components are rare in the gestural activity of individuals who have never seen or became blind quite early [2325]. This representational component resulting from observer-viewpoint, concerns the visual appearance of the action seen at a distance and/or the visual appearance of the gestures (as will be seen by others). These gestural visual patterns are influenced by visual experience. This explains why sighted participants sometimes have more difficulty recognizing ASFGs produced by blind participants, even though gestural motor based patterns are very similar between groups. This idea is schematized in Fig 5.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Action simulations by finger gestures in blind and sighted individuals: Motor and visually based patterns.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214371.g005

Due to the nature of the gestures studied (simulation of the legs with fingers), a substantial part of character-viewpoint gestures [5] resulting from motor based patterns was expected. High rates of recognition for ASFGs produced by all three groups suggested that gestural motor based patterns of ASFGs are very similar among early blind, late blind and sighted individuals (i.e., motor experience of the feet which go up in Climbing Stairs). Differences in recognition rates obtained in Experiments 1 and 2 seems to be related to the visual and representational component of gesture.

To conclude, the results of Experiments 1 and 2 may have important practical implications. Despite some differences related to the visual rules of gesturing, the ASFGs examined seem to be a meaningful simulation procedure for both blind and sighted individuals. The next step of the research project is to design 3D illustrations engaging the common ASFGs observed. More recognized ASFGs produced by all three groups will be selected and prototypes will test the haptic recognition of these devices by blind and sighted children [29]. A feasibility study is already underway with our editorial partnership (Les Doigts Qui Rêvent, www.ldqr.org) to evaluate the possibilities of including ASFGs in tactile devices such as children's books but also in mobility aids. We are also currently exploring the field of new technologies to develop and test new multimodal interfaces to associate ASFGs with sound feedback in 3D scenarios. The advantage of 3D scenarios that engage ASFGs over traditional 3D representations (toys or miniature objects already commonly used in this field of intervention) is the reactivation of motor components from real interactions with objects (enactment effect). These 3D illustrations involving ASFGs seem to open a promising design path in the field of visual disability. Exploring new relationships between representational devices and common sensorimotor experiences between individuals helps develop the design of new communication interfaces that are straightfowardly understood by a larger number of subjects, regardless of their sensory abilities.

Supporting information

S3 Dataset. Codage by two independent judges experiment 3.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214371.s003

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the participants who took part in the study, Lola Chennaz and Chloé Ruche, master students of University of Geneva for their participation in Experiment 3 and Suzette Wright, Susan Campbell and Guillemette Bolens who provided English editing.

References

  1. 1. Hand McNeill D. and Mind: What Gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1992.
  2. 2. McNeill D. Why we gesture: The surprising role of hand movements in communication. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2016.
  3. 3. Kendon A. Gesture: Visible action as utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2004.
  4. 4. Cuxac C. La langue des signes française (LSF); les voies de l'iconicité. langues Fd, editor. Paris: Ophrys; 2000.
  5. 5. Hostetter AB, Alibali MW. Visible embodiment: Gestures as simulated action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2008;15(3):495–514. WOS:000257217800002.
  6. 6. Bolens G. The Style of Gestures: Embodiment and Cognition in Literary Narrative. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press; 2012.
  7. 7. Heller MA, Gentaz E. Psychology of touch and blindness. New York: Psychology Press; 2014.
  8. 8. Theurel A, Witt A, Claudet P, Hatwell Y, Gentaz E. Tactile Picture Recognition by Early Blind Children: The Effect of Illustration Technique. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied. 2013;19(3):233–40. WOS:000324793600005. pmid:24059824
  9. 9. Lederman SJ, Klatzky RL, Chataway C, Summers CD. Visual mediation and the haptic recognition of two-dimensional pictures of common objects. Perception & Psychophysics. 1990;47(1):54–64. WOS:A1990DF59100006.
  10. 10. Thompson LJ, Chronicle EP, Collins AF. Enhancing 2-D tactile picture design from knowledge of 3-D haptic object recognition. European Psychologist. 2006;11(2):110–8. WOS:000239233700004.
  11. 11. Heller MA. Picture and pattern perception in the sighted and the blind—The advantage of the late blind. Perception. 1989;18(3):379–89. WOS:A1989AQ18300009. pmid:2798020
  12. 12. Klatzky RL, Loomis JM, Lederman SJ, Wake H, Fujita N. Haptic identification of objects and their depictions. Perception & Psychophysics. 1993;54(2):170–8. WOS:A1993LM88700004.
  13. 13. Wilson M. Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2002;9(4):625–36. WOS:000181104600001.
  14. 14. Barsalou L. Grounded Cognition. Annual Review of Psychology. 2008;59(1):617–45.
  15. 15. Barsalou L. Grounded Cognition: Past, Present, and Future. Topics in Cognitive Science. 2010;2(4):716–24. WOS:000283870400009. pmid:25164052
  16. 16. Hainselin M, Quinette P, Eustache F. Qu'est-ce que la mémoire de l'action? Revue théorique et perspectives. Revue de neuropsychologie. 2013;5(2):129–34.
  17. 17. Madan CR, Singhal A. Using actions to enhance memory: effects of enactment, gestures, and exercise on human memory. Frontiers in Psychology. 2012;3. WOS:000208864000215. pmid:23293612
  18. 18. Bara F, Gentaz E, Valente D. The Effect of Illustrations on Reading Process of Tactile Books: An Exploratory Study. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness. in press.
  19. 19. Valente D, Gentaz E. La reconnaissance des illustrations tactiles dans les livres tactiles par l'enfant aveugle. ANAE. in press.
  20. 20. Iverson JM, Tencer HL, Lany J, Goldin-Meadow S. The relation between gesture and speech in congenitally blind and sighted language-learners. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 2000;24(2):105–30. WOS:000087871200004.
  21. 21. Iverson JM, GoldinMeadow S. What's communication got to do with it? Gesture in children blind from birth. Developmental Psychology. 1997;33(3):453–67. WOS:A1997WX88400007. pmid:9149924
  22. 22. Iverson JM, Goldin-Meadow S. The resilience of gesture in talk: gesture in blind speakers and listeners. Developmental Science. 2001;4(4):416–22. WOS:000171830600005.
  23. 23. Frame MJ. The Relationship Between Visual Impairment and Gestures. Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness. 2000;94(3):155–72.
  24. 24. Sharkey WF, Asamoto P, Tokunaga C, Haraguchi G, McFaddon-Robar T. Hand Gestures of Visually Impaired and Sighted Interactants. Journal of Visual Impairement & Blindness. 2000;94(9):549–64.
  25. 25. Blass T, Freedman N, Steingart I. Body movement and verbal encoding in the congenitally blind. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 1974;39(1):279–93. pmid:4412230
  26. 26. Howell DC. Statistical methods for psychology. Belmont: Cengage Learning; 2010.
  27. 27. Rosch E, Mervis CB. Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology. 1975;7(4):573–605.
  28. 28. Theurel A, Frileux S, Hatwell Y, Gentaz E. The haptic recognition of geometrical shapes in congenitally blind and blindfolded adolescents: is there a haptic prototype effect? PLoS One. 2012;7(6):e40251. Epub 2012/06/28. pmid:22761961; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3386238.
  29. 29. Valente D, Palama A, Gentaz E. Action simulations by fingers gestures: a new way to illustrate tactile books for blind and sighted children. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. submitted.