Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Training of paced breathing at 0.1 Hz improves CO2 homeostasis and relaxation during a paced breathing task

Abstract

Volitional control of breathing often leads to excessive ventilation (hyperventilation) among untrained individuals, which disrupts CO2 homeostasis and may elicit a set of undesirable symptoms. The present study investigated whether seven days of training without any anti-hyperventilation instructions improves CO2 homeostasis during paced breathing at a frequency of 0.1 Hz (6 breaths/minute). Furthermore, the present study investigated the effects of training on breathing-related changes in affective state to examine the hypothesis that training improves the influence of slow paced breathing on affect. A total of 16 participants performed ten minutes of paced breathing every day for seven days. Partial pressure of end-tidal CO2 (PetCO2), symptoms of hyperventilation, affective state (before and after breathing), and pleasantness of the task were measured on the first, fourth, and seventh days of training. Results showed that the drop in PetCO2 significantly decreased with training and none of the participants experienced a drop in PetCO2 below 30 mmHg by day seven of training (except one participant who already had PetCO2 below 30 mmHg during baseline), in comparison to 37.5% of participants on the first day. Paced breathing produced hyperventilation symptoms of mild intensity which did not decrease with training. This suggests that some participants still experienced a drop of PetCO2 that was deep enough to produce noticeable symptoms. Affective state was shifted towards calmness and relaxation during the second and third laboratory measurements, but not during the first measurement. Additionally, the breathing task was perceived as more pleasant during subsequent laboratory measurements. The obtained results showed that training paced breathing at 0.1 Hz led to decrease in hyperventilation. Furthermore, the present study suggests that training paced breathing is necessary to make the task more pleasant and relaxing.

Introduction

In recent decades, slow paced breathing (most often around the 0.1 Hz frequency) has been widely studied as a method for improving cardiovascular health and decreasing affective arousal. Breathing at frequencies around 0.1 Hz is used in biofeedback-based methods aimed at increasing heart rate variability [1, 2, 3]. Research has shown promising results regarding the effectiveness of heart rate variability biofeedback in treating depression [4, 5], anxiety [6], asthma [7], fibromyalgia [8], posttraumatic stress disorder [9, 10], as well as other conditions. Breathing at lower frequencies has also been successfully tested as a method to improve baroreflex function in the complementary treatment of cardiovascular diseases [11, 12, 13]. Furthermore, research suggests that breathing at 0.1 Hz decreases pain, which makes it a promising complementary method for the treatment of chronic pain [14, 15, 16]. It can also improve sleep quality [17, 18] and decrease affective arousal [19], so it is a widely-used method of emotion regulation, that has recently been implemented in multiple mobile applications [20, 21].

Spontaneous breathing is primarily controlled in an automatic manner by respiratory networks in the medulla oblongata and pons on the basis of information from CO2, O2, and pH chemoreceptors and respiratory mechanoreceptors [22] with descending influences related to emotional and cognitive processes [23]. During paced breathing, volitional breathing shifts the balance in neural control of breathing toward higher brain centers instead of the control mechanism in the brainstem [24, 25]. Furthermore, paced breathing prevents the use of compensatory mechanisms such as apnea. As a result, some untrained participants tend to hyperventilate during slow paced breathing [26, 27, 28]. Hyperventilation leads to a decrease in arterial pressure of CO2, and when this decrease is large enough it causes several undesirable physiological and psychological changes, such as increase in heart rate, paresthesia and tetany, dizziness, lightheadedness, and increased emotional arousal [29, 30, 31, 32]. For this reason, hyperventilation is considered undesirable during paced breathing exercises.

The effects of paced breathing training on hyperventilation during paced breathing remains largely unknown. One study on nine weeks of slow paced breathing training for hot flashes reported adverse effects such as dizziness, palpitations, and tingling in extremities, which are typical symptoms of hyperventilation [33]. This study suggests sustained hyperventilation during paced breathing training. Some studies on paced breathing training implemented various methods to counteract hyperventilation and successfully taught participants to avoid hyperventilation One study on heart rate variability suggests that daily paced breathing training removes hyperventilation [2]. In this study, participants stopped hyperventilating after three days of training. However, capnography was used during training sessions and researchers instructed participants to breathe shallower whenever the partial pressure of end-tidal CO2 (PetCO2) dropped too much. Furthermore, multiple studies on the use of breathing training in panic disorder and asthma have shown that capnometry biofeedback can be successfully used to decrease hyperventilation during paced breathing [34, 35, 36, 37]. However, often neither capnography measurement nor an anti-hyperventilation instruction is used in paced breathing training [17, 33, 38]. Furthermore, paced breathing is widely used in mobile applications [21, 22], where the issue of potential hyperventilation is frequently overlooked. Therefore, it is important to investigate the effects of training on CO2 homeostasis during paced breathing tasks without any anti-hyperventilation instruction. It is possible that the mere training of paced breathing tasks improves CO2 homeostasis during paced breathing.

Change in affective state is another effect of paced breathing which can be modified by training. More specifically, the effects of slow paced breathing on affective state may improve with training. Previous studies that investigated the influence of paced breathing at frequencies around 0.1 Hz on affect among untrained participants provided mixed results: some reported relaxing and anti-anxiety effects [39, 40, 41, 42, 43] and others did not [44, 45, 46, 27, 28]. At the same time, a lot of studies suggest that regular practice of paced breathing at frequencies around 0.1 Hz improves emotional functioning, for example by reducing anxiety (for meta-analysis see: [6]). This inconsistency suggests that paced breathing tasks may require training to produce relaxing effects.

The present study aimed to investigate how daily practice of breathing at 0.1 Hz for seven days influences arterial CO2 homeostasis, hyperventilation symptoms, breathing-related changes in affective state, and the pleasantness of the task. No instructions aimed at influencing the depth of breathing were used in this study. The primary question was whether training decreases the tendency to hyperventilate. The secondary question was whether training slow paced breathing improves its effects on affective state. It was expected that, after training, hyperventilation and symptoms of hyperventilation would decrease and paced breathing would be perceived as more pleasurable and would decrease affective arousal more effectively than during the first performance of the task.

Method

Participants

A total of 16 participants were recruited via social media (8 men and 8 women). Participants were 19 to 29 years old (M = 22.37, SD = 3.32). Exclusion criteria based on self-report were chronic respiratory diseases (e.g. asthma), other chronic diseases (e.g. diabetes), cardiovascular diseases, psychiatric and neurological conditions, and any condition that may affect the ability to breathe through one’s nose (e.g. allergy). Participants were debriefed about their health by the experimenter upon arrival at the laboratory. Furthermore, only participants who had not participated in any form of breathing training, such as yoga or singing classes with breathing exercises, were recruited. Participants were asked to refrain from drinking beverages with caffeine and smoking tobacco for four hours before laboratory measurements. Participants were paid the equivalent of 23 euros for participating in the study. Approval from the local ethics committee was obtained (the ethical committee of the University of Warsaw).

Apparatus and materials

Measurement of affective state was conducted with the use of the Polish translation of the Two-Dimensional Mood Scale [47]. This questionnaire is based on the two dimensional model of affect [48, 49]. Four measures of affective state were computed: arousal and valence, and vitality and stability. The latter two are the arousal and valence axes rotated by 45°. The rotated axes are also known as pleasant and unpleasant arousal, and because these terms are more common they will be used in the rest of the text [50, 51]. Symptoms of hyperventilation were measured on a 0–6 Likert scale which ranged from 0 (not experiencing the symptom at all) to 6 (experiencing the symptom with maximal imaginable intensity). Symptoms of hyperventilation were drawn from the Nijmegen Questionnaire [52] and a study which measured hyperventilation symptoms [29]. The following hyperventilation symptoms were examined: dizziness, tingling and pricking, numbness, headache, and increased muscle tension in hands and feet. Means of pre- and post-task symptoms were computed for further analysis. Furthermore, participants were asked to rate how pleasant they found the task on a scale which ranged from 0–6 (from extremely unpleasant to extremely pleasant).

Respiratory rate and PetCO2 were measured with a Capnocheck Sleep Capnograph 9004 (BCI). Before the experiment, the capnograph was calibrated with 5% CO2 calibration gas. Data were transferred to a PC computer and the mean value of PetCO2 and respiratory rate were computed in MATLAB (Release 7.1, TheMathWorks, Natick, MA).

Procedure

The study lasted seven days. Participants performed a ten-minute-long paced breathing task every day. Laboratory measurements of PetCO2 were conducted on the first, fourth, and seventh days of practise. On the other days, participants used an online procedure. They logged-in using individual codes to access the experimental procedure. To ensure that the participants performed the task at home, the experimenter had access to the system and could examine who had logged in and when. Four participants skipped one day of training.

Laboratory measurements were conducted in a soundproof room with stable temperature. The experimenter sat in a separate room and monitored the experimental procedure and capnogram. The experimental procedure was automated and ran on OpenSesame 3.0.7 software [53]. The experiment begin with a six-minute resting period during which participants were asked to sit comfortably with opened eyes. The first minute was an adaptation period and the next five minutes were the baseline period which was used in analysis. Then, participants answered questions about current affective state (Two-Dimensional Mood Scale) and symptoms of hyperventilation. Next, participants breathed for ten minutes at a frequency of 0.1 Hz. Breathing was paced by an acoustic signal with different pitches for inhalation and exhalation and changing volume (it was loudest in the middle of each breathing phase). Furthermore, the words “inhale” and “exhale” were displayed on the screen. Inhalation lasted four seconds and exhalation lasted six seconds, a ratio that resembles the spontaneous ratio during breathing at 0.1 Hz when only the length of the whole breathing cycle is paced [54]. After the breathing task, participants once more answered the questions about affective state as well as questions about symptoms of hyperventilation; they also rated how pleasant they found the task. Home practice was done using the same audiovisual pacer. During home practice, participants were asked to sit down comfortably with opened eyes.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted in IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows (version 22.0, IBM Corp., Armonk, NY) with significance set at p < 0.05. ANOVAs for repeated measures were conducted to examine the effects of day of laboratory measurement (first x second x third laboratory session) and phase of the experiment (baseline x breathing at 0.1 Hz) on respiratory rate, PetCO2, hyperventilation symptoms, and scores on four dimensions of affective state. A repeated measures ANOVA was conducted to examine the effects of day of laboratory measurement (first x second x third laboratory session) on the pleasantness of the task. When the sphericity assumption was violated, Greenhouse-Geisser correction was used. Post-hoc analyses were conducted with Bonferroni correction. When the interactions in the ANOVAs were significant, paired sample t-tests were computed for differences between baseline and measurement during the breathing task for PetCO2 and post-task measurements of self-reported variables. Linear regression was performed with changes in symptoms of hyperventilation between baseline and paced breathing as a dependent variable and PetCO2 during paced breathing as predictor. Confidence intervals for the plots were calculated with the use of the data normalization procedure proposed by Morey [55].

Results

Participants decreased respiratory rate between baseline and the paced breathing task, F(1, 15) = 162.87, p < .001, η2 = .92. PetCO2 also dropped between baseline and paced breathing, F(1, 15) = 14.61, p < .01, η2 = .49. Furthermore, PetCO2 increased between laboratory measurements, F(2, 30) = 4.83, p < .05, η2 = .24, and there was an interaction between phase of the experiment (baseline x breathing at 0.1 Hz) and laboratory measurement (first x second x third laboratory session), F(2, 30) = 9.22, p < .001, η2 = .38. Post-hoc analysis showed that the drop in PetCO2 between baseline and paced breathing was significant during the first, p < .001, and second laboratory measurements, p < .01, but not during the third laboratory measurement, p = .17. Changes in PetCO2 between baseline and the breathing task for all measurements are presented in Fig 1. Paired sample t-tests showed that drop of PetCO2 during first and second laboratory measurements did not differ significantly, t(15) = 1.63, p = .13, but PetCO2 dropped less during the third measurement than during the first one, t(15) = 4.63, p < .001, and the second one, t(15) = 2.51, p < .05. Thus, results indicate that hyperventilation decreased with training. Data on how many participants experienced a drop in PetCO2 below 30 mmHg are presented in Table 1. Analysis of symptoms of hyperventilation showed that they increased between the baseline measurement and measurement after paced breathing, F(1, 15) = 5.35, p < .05, η2 = .26. However, the intensity of symptoms did not change significantly between laboratory measurements. Changes in symptoms of hyperventilation are presented in Fig 2. Linear regression showed that PetCO2 during the paced breathing task did not significantly predict symptoms intensity during the first laboratory measurement, F(1, 14) = 3.07, p = .10, but PetCO2 predicted hyperventilation symptoms during the second, F(1, 14) = 6.30, p < .05, and third measurements, F(1, 14) = 11.78, p < .01 with a R2 of .31 and .46. The perceived pleasantness of the task increased with training, F(2, 30) = 5.22, p < .05, η2 = .26. Changes in perceived pleasantness of paced breathing are presented in Fig 3. Post-hoc analysis showed that there was a significant difference between pleasantness during the first and third laboratory measurements, p < .01.

thumbnail
Table 1. Percent of participants with PetCO2 below 30 mmHg during baseline and paced breathing for the three days of laboratory measurements.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218550.t001

thumbnail
Fig 1. Changes in drop in PetCO2 between the three laboratory measurements with 95% confidence intervals.

Changes computed by subtraction of PetCO2 during paced breathing from PetCO2 during baseline. Note. * p < .05 *** p < .001; the significance of the difference between the laboratory measurements is indicated.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218550.g001

thumbnail
Fig 2. Changes in the intensity of symptoms of hyperventilation between the three laboratory measurements with 95% confidence intervals.

Changes computed by subtraction of symptom intensity before paced breathing from symptoms experienced during paced breathing.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218550.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Changes in the perceived pleasantness of the paced breathing task between the three laboratory measurements with 95% confidence intervals.

Note. ** p < .01; the significance of the difference between the laboratory measurements is indicated.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218550.g003

The effects of the paced breathing task and the seven days of training on affect were investigated using repeated measures ANOVA. General arousal decreased after the paced breathing task in comparison to baseline measurement, F(1, 15) = 32.5, p < .001, η2 = .69, but there were no significant changes between laboratory measurements. Valence did not change significantly during the task and between laboratory measurements. Pleasant arousal decreased between baseline and measurement after paced breathing, F(1, 15) = 20.16, p < .001, η2 = .57, but the observed decrease did not change significantly with training. Unpleasant arousal did not change significantly between baseline and the paced breathing task, but this effect was close to significant, F(1, 15) = 4.27, p = .056, η2 = .22. Unpleasant arousal decreased more with training, F(2, 30) = 4.27, p < .05, η2 = .43. Post-hoc comparison showed that the decrease in unpleasant arousal was non-significant during the first laboratory measurement, p = .63, but unpleasant arousal decreased during the second, p < .05, and third, p < .05, laboratory measurements. Thus, the results indicate that training of paced breathing makes it more effective in reducing unpleasant arousal. Changes in the effects of paced breathing on unpleasant arousal between laboratory measurements are presented in Fig 4. Comparisons of changes between baseline and post-task unpleasant arousal scores using paired sample t-tests showed that decreases in unpleasant arousal increased between first and second laboratory measurements, t(15) = -2.45, p < .05, as well as between first and third laboratory measurements, t(15) = -3.02, p < .01. The mean values for the physiological and self-reported variables are presented in Table 2.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Changes in decrease in unpleasant arousal between the three laboratory measurements with 95% confidence intervals.

Changes computed by subtraction of unpleasant arousal after paced breathing from unpleasant arousal during baseline. Note. *** p < .001; the significance of the difference between the laboratory measurements is indicated.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218550.g004

thumbnail
Table 2. Means and standard deviations of the physiological and self-reported measures for the different moments of the experiment (baseline and paced breathing) and three laboratory measurements.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218550.t002

Discussion

The present study investigated the effects of seven days of training paced breathing at 0.1 Hz on adequacy of ventilation during paced breathing and on symptoms of hyperventilation. Furthermore, the present study examined the influence of paced breathing training at 0.1 Hz on breathing-related changes in affective state and perceived pleasantness of the task.

Results showed that the drop in PetCO2 during paced breathing at 0.1 Hz was significantly reduced after seven days of training, which indicates improved respiratory homeostasis. A previous study by Vaschillo, Vaschillo, and Lehrer [2] reported cessation of hyperventilation after three days of training, but an anti-hyperventilation instruction was used in this study and the experimenter monitored PetCO2 to inform participants when their PetCO2 dropped too much. In contrast, the present study used no instruction aimed at altering the depth of breathing. There was a relatively large variation in changes in hyperventilation. For example, the participant who had the largest drop in PetCO2 during the first laboratory session (a drop of 7.99 mmHg) still experienced decreases during the second (6.05 mmHg) and third laboratory measurements (7.80 mmHg). However, by the end of the training, the PetCO2 of all participants stopped dropping below 30 mmHg, a commonly-used threshold for hyperventilation (except one participant who already had PetCO2 below 30 mmHg during baseline; for data on how many participants experienced a drop in PetCO2 below 30 mmHg, see Table 1). Thus, the obtained results indicate that training paced breathing at 0.1 Hz significantly reduces the tendency to hyperventilate even when no hyperventilation instruction is used. This finding is important for the use of paced breathing tasks in applied psychophysiology, especially for situations when it is employed without the guidance of a qualified breathing coach, for example in mobile apps which use slow paced breathing as an affect regulation method.

The decrease of hyperventilation observed in the present study may be an effect of increased automatization of the paced breathing task. As a consequence of practise, motor tasks become more automated, which involves changes in neural control and decreased attentional resources required to perform the task [56]. Gradual automatization of paced breathing tasks during training has been shown by Gallego and Perruchet [57]. In their study, eight days of training of paced breathing gradually decreased the attentional resources used to perform paced breathing. Presumably, partial automatization of paced breathing during training alters neural control of breathing, allowing an increase in the role of the CO2 homeostatic mechanism in the control of breathing. The decrease of hyperventilation could be also the result of automatic optimization of breathing aimed at reducing respiratory work. Hyperventilation requires increased tidal volume, which already must be augmented during slow breathing. Therefore, hyperventilation is costly in terms of the elastic work of breathing [58]. It is thought that the minimization of respiratory work is one of the main factors regulating breathing [59, 60]. Therefore, training should lead to gradual optimization of breathing, which results in decreased hyperventilation.

Participants reported a small increase in symptoms of hyperventilation during breathing at 0.1 Hz. In line with expectations, the results suggest that the intensity of symptoms was related to magnitude of hyperventilation. However, despite the decrease in the drop of PetCO2, the intensity of symptoms did not decrease significantly with training. This suggests that even after training some participants experienced large enough disruption of arterial CO2 to produce noticeable symptoms. Despite the fact that reported hyperventilation symptoms can be considered minor in most participants, two participants reported increases of symptoms by 2.8 and 3.4 on a 7 point scale while experiencing a drop of PetCO2 by 4.86 and 5.53 mmHg, respectively; several participants experienced a comparable drop of PetCO2 with increases in symptom intensity smaller than 1. This result suggests the existence of significant individual differences in sensitivity to physiological changes produced by the disruption of arterial CO2 homeostasis. Individual differences in symptom intensity during voluntary hyperventilation have previously been shown to be related to trait anxiety and anxiety sensitivity [61]. If such psychological traits affect the experience of symptoms during slow paced breathing, hyperventilation may be of particular importance among the population with higher levels of anxiety.

The effects of paced breathing tasks on affective state changed with training. It was hypothesised that training paced breathing is necessary to produce consistent anti-arousal changes in affective state. The results support this hypothesis because the effects of paced breathing on unpleasant arousal appeared during the second and third laboratory measurements and not during the first. Unpleasant arousal is a dimension of affective state that extends from states of high arousal and negative valence (e.g. irritation and nervousness) to states of low arousal and positive valence (e.g. relaxation and calmness [47]). The task was also perceived as more pleasurable with training. Thus, the present study suggests that training increases the relaxing effects of paced breathing at 0.1 Hz and makes it more pleasurable. Therefore, the lack of effects of paced breathing on affect reported by some previous studies could be caused by the participants’ lack of experience with paced breathing [44, 45, 46, 27, 28].

Multiple mechanisms may be responsible for the improvement of the affective effects of paced breathing with training. Studies have shown that slow paced breathing increases breathing discomfort (dyspnea; [62, 63, 64]). Mitigation of dyspnea is considered one of the factors determining breathing behavior [65]. Training may allow the optimization of breathing mechanics to decrease dyspnea. The observed increase in pleasantness of the task suggests a decrease of breathing discomfort with training. Changes in affective state may also be a result of the partial automatization of the task. The shift from controlled processing to automatic processes during motor skill acquisition is typically associated with reduced mental effort required to perform a task [66]. This may improve the relaxing properties of slow paced breathing.

The results of the present study has consequences for the generalization of the results of previous studies and for future research on the effects of slow paced breathing. Previous research conducted on untrained individuals presumably failed to show some effects of paced breathing exercises that are experienced by those who perform such exercises regularly. Therefore, future research which aims to examine the effects of slow paced breathing should employ training.

The present study also has consequences for the use of paced breathing as an applied method. Some methods that use slow paced breathing employ anti-hyperventilation instructions [2, 35]. However, the issue of hyperventilation is often overlooked and no anti-hyperventilation instructions are used [17, 33, 38]. Furthermore, paced breathing is currently widely implemented in many mobile applications [20, 21]. For this reason, it is certainly widely used without the assistance of a breathing coach, which increases the risk of hyperventilation. The present study suggests that there exists a relatively large variation in changes in hyperventilation during paced breathing training. Because of individuals who still experience significant drops of PetCO2 after the training, methods to avoid hyperventilation—such as anti-hyperventilation instructions [28] or, when possible, capnometer biofeedback [36]—should be routinely included as part of paced breathing training and implemented in mobile applications. The present and previous studies suggest that symptoms of hyperventilation can be considered a mild adverse effect during slow paced breathing training. Therefore, effective avoidance of hyperventilation may increase tolerability and adherence to breathing training.

The issue of hyperventilation during paced breathing is of particular importance among chronic hyperventilators. Around 6–10% of the population tend to hyperventilate chronically [67]. This subpopulation can be particularly prone to look for some form of breathing training because of hyperventilation-related dyspnea. Due to low resting arterial CO2 level, paced breathing without paying attention to hyperventilation may lead to deep hyperventilation with severe symptoms. For the safety of paced breathing training it is important to investigate the effects of paced breathing on hyperventilation in this group.

To sum up, the present study showed that during breathing at 0.1 Hz, even without any anti-hyperventilation instruction, the drop in PetCO2 decreases with training. However, there was relatively high variability, which indicates that even after training some participants experience a significant drop in PetCO2. Paced breathing produced mostly minor increases in hyperventilation symptoms, however some subjects experienced symptoms of medium intensity. Symptom intensity did not change as a result of training. Furthermore, these results indicate that the affective effects of paced breathing improve with training, making paced breathing more relaxing. Unpleasant arousal did not change during the first performance of paced breathing, but it begin to decrease during the second measurement (third day of training). Thus, the present results suggest that both CO2 homeostasis and the affective effects of paced breathing improve with training.

References

  1. 1. Lehrer PM, Vaschillo E, Vaschillo B. Resonant frequency biofeedback training to increase cardiac variability: Rationale and manual for training. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback; 2000;25(3):177–91. pmid:10999236
  2. 2. Vaschillo EG, Vaschillo B, Lehrer PM. Characteristics of Resonance in Heart Rate Variability Stimulated by Biofeedback. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback. Springer Nature; 2006 Jun;31(2):129–42. pmid:16838124
  3. 3. Wheat AL, Larkin KT. Biofeedback of Heart Rate Variability and Related Physiology: A Critical Review. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback; 2010 May 5;35(3):229–42. pmid:20443135
  4. 4. Karavidas MK, Lehrer PM, Vaschillo E, Vaschillo B, Marin H, Buyske S, et al. Preliminary Results of an Open Label Study of Heart Rate Variability Biofeedback for the Treatment of Major Depression. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback; 2007 Mar 1;32(1):19–30. pmid:17333315
  5. 5. Siepmann M, Aykac V, Unterdörfer J, Petrowski K, Mueck-Weymann M. A Pilot Study on the Effects of Heart Rate Variability Biofeedback in Patients with Depression and in Healthy Subjects. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback; 2008 Sep 19;33(4):195–201. pmid:18807175
  6. 6. Goessl VC, Curtiss JE, Hofmann SG. The effect of heart rate variability biofeedback training on stress and anxiety: a meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine [Internet]. Cambridge University Press (CUP); 2017 May 8;47(15):2578–86. pmid:28478782
  7. 7. Lehrer PM, Vaschillo E, Vaschillo B, Lu S-E, Scardella A, Siddique M, et al. Biofeedback Treatment for Asthma. Chest; 2004 Aug;126(2):352–61. pmid:15302717
  8. 8. Hassett AL, Radvanski DC, Vaschillo EG, Vaschillo B, Sigal LH, Karavidas MK, et al. A Pilot Study of the Efficacy of Heart Rate Variability (HRV) Biofeedback in Patients with Fibromyalgia. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback; 2007 Jan 12;32(1):1–10. pmid:17219062
  9. 9. Tan G, Dao TK, Farmer L, Sutherland RJ, Gevirtz R. Heart Rate Variability (HRV) and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD): A Pilot Study. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback; 2010 Aug 3;36(1):27–35. pmid:20680439
  10. 10. Zucker TL, Samuelson KW, Muench F, Greenberg MA, Gevirtz RN. The Effects of Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia Biofeedback on Heart Rate Variability and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Symptoms: A Pilot Study. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback; 2009 Apr 25;34(2):135–43. pmid:19396540
  11. 11. Chen S, Sun P, Wang S, Lin G, Wang T. Effects of heart rate variability biofeedback on cardiovascular responses and autonomic sympathovagal modulation following stressor tasks in prehypertensives. Journal of Human Hypertension. 2015 Apr 30;30(2):105–11. pmid:25924910
  12. 12. Lin G, Xiang Q, Fu X, Wang S, Wang S, Chen S, et al. Heart Rate Variability Biofeedback Decreases Blood Pressure in Prehypertensive Subjects by Improving Autonomic Function and Baroreflex. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine; 2012 Feb;18(2):143–52. pmid:22339103
  13. 13. Zou Y, Zhao X, Hou Y-Y, Liu T, Wu Q, Huang Y-H, et al. Meta-Analysis of Effects of Voluntary Slow Breathing Exercises for Control of Heart Rate and Blood Pressure in Patients With Cardiovascular Diseases. The American Journal of Cardiology; 2017 Jul;120(1):148–53. pmid:28502461
  14. 14. Chalaye P, Goffaux P, Lafrenaye S, Marchand S. Respiratory Effects on Experimental Heat Pain and Cardiac Activity. Pain Medicine; 2009 Nov;10(8):1334–40. pmid:19671085
  15. 15. Jafari H, Courtois I, Van den Bergh O, Vlaeyen JWS, Van Diest I. Pain and respiration. PAIN; 2017 Jun;158(6):995–1006. pmid:28240995
  16. 16. Zautra AJ, Fasman R, Davis MC, Craig AD (Bud). The effects of slow breathing on affective responses to pain stimuli: An experimental study. Pain; 2010 Apr;149(1):12–8. pmid:20079569
  17. 17. Laborde S, Hosang T, Mosley E, Dosseville F. Influence of a 30-Day Slow-Paced Breathing Intervention Compared to Social Media Use on Subjective Sleep Quality and Cardiac Vagal Activity. Journal of Clinical Medicine; 2019 Feb 6;8(2):193. pmid:30736268
  18. 18. Tsai HJ, Kuo TBJ, Lee G-S, Yang CCH. Efficacy of paced breathing for insomnia: Enhances vagal activity and improves sleep quality. Psychophysiology; 2014 Sep 19;52(3):388–96. pmid:25234581
  19. 19. Zaccaro A, Piarulli A, Laurino M, Garbella E, Menicucci D, Neri B, et al. How Breath-Control Can Change Your Life: A Systematic Review on Psycho-Physiological Correlates of Slow Breathing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience; 2018 Sep 7;12. pmid:30245619
  20. 20. Christmann CA, Hoffmann A, Bleser G. Stress Management Apps With Regard to Emotion-Focused Coping and Behavior Change Techniques: A Content Analysis. JMIR mHealth and uHealth; 2017 Feb 23;5(2):e22. pmid:28232299
  21. 21. Coulon SM, Monroe CM, West DS. A Systematic, Multi-domain Review of Mobile Smartphone Apps for Evidence-Based Stress Management. American Journal of Preventive Medicine; 2016 Jul;51(1):95–105. pmid:26993534
  22. 22. Del Negro CA, Funk GD, Feldman JL. Breathing matters. Nature Reviews Neuroscience; 2018 May 8;19(6):351–67. pmid:29740175
  23. 23. Homma I, Masaoka Y. Breathing rhythms and emotions. Experimental Physiology; 2008 Aug 14;93(9):1011–21. pmid:18487316
  24. 24. Colebatch JG, Adams L, Murphy K, Martin AJ, Lammertsma AA, Tochon-Danguy HJ, et al. Regional cerebral blood flow during volitional breathing in man. The Journal of Physiology; 1991 Nov 1;443(1):91–103. pmid:1822545
  25. 25. McKay LC, Evans KC, Frackowiak RSJ, Corfield DR. Neural correlates of voluntary breathing in humans. Journal of Applied Physiology; 2003 Sep;95(3):1170–8. pmid:12754178
  26. 26. Anderson DE, McNeely JD, Windham BG. Device-guided slow-breathing effects on end-tidal CO2and heart-rate variability. Psychology, Health & Medicine; 2009 Dec;14(6):667–79.
  27. 27. Szulczewski MT, Rynkiewicz A. The effects of breathing at a frequency of 0.1 Hz on affective state, the cardiovascular system, and adequacy of ventilation. Psychophysiology; 2018 Jul 16;55(12):e13221. pmid:30010195
  28. 28. Szulczewski MT, An Anti-hyperventilation Instruction Decreases the Drop in End-tidal CO2 and Symptoms of Hyperventilation During Breathing at 0.1 Hz. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback; Forthcoming 2019. pmid:31065914
  29. 29. Carter MM, Suchday S, Gore KL. The utility of the ASI factors in predicting response to voluntary hyperventilation among nonclinical participants. Journal of Anxiety Disorders; 2001 May;15(3):217–30. pmid:11442140
  30. 30. Hornsveld H, Garssen B, Dop MF, van Spiegel P. Symptom reporting during voluntary hyperventilation and mental load: Implications for diagnosing hyperventilation syndrome. Journal of Psychosomatic Research; 1990 Jan;34(6):687–97. pmid:2290141
  31. 31. Thyer BA, Papsdorf JD, Wright P. Physiological and psychological effects of acute intentional hyperventilation. Behaviour Research and Therapy; 1984;22(5):587–90. pmid:6508709
  32. 32. Van de Borne P, Mezzetti S, Montano N, Narkiewicz K, Degaute JP, Somers VK. Hyperventilation alters arterial baroreflex control of heart rate and muscle sympathetic nerve activity. American Journal of Physiology-Heart and Circulatory Physiology; 2000 Aug;279(2):H536–H541. pmid:10924051
  33. 33. Sood R, Sood A, Wolf SL, Linquist BM, Liu H, Sloan JA, et al. Paced breathing compared with usual breathing for hot flashes. Menopause: The Journal of The North American Menopause Society; 2012 Sep;1. pmid:22990758
  34. 34. Kim S, Wollburg E, Roth WT. Opposing Breathing Therapies for Panic Disorder. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry; 2012 Jul 15;73(07):931–9. pmid:22901344
  35. 35. Meuret AE, Ritz T, Wilhelm FH, Roth WT, Rosenfield D. Hypoventilation Therapy Alleviates Panic by Repeated Induction of Dyspnea. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging. Elsevier BV; 2018 Jun;3(6):539–45. pmid:29573981
  36. 36. Meuret AE, Wilhelm FH, Ritz T, Roth WT. Feedback of end-tidal pCO2 as a therapeutic approach for panic disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research; 2008 Jun;42(7):560–8. pmid:17681544
  37. 37. Ritz T, Rosenfield D, Steele AM, Millard MW, Meuret AE. Controlling Asthma by Training of Capnometry-Assisted Hypoventilation (CATCH) vs Slow Breathing. Chest; 2014 Nov;146(5):1237–47.: pmid:25122497
  38. 38. Laborde S, Allen MS, Göhring N, Dosseville F. The effect of slow-paced breathing on stress management in adolescents with intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. Wiley; 2016 Dec 9;61(6):560–7. pmid:27933677
  39. 39. Prinsloo GE, Derman WE, Lambert MI, Rauch HGL. The effect of a single episode of short duration heart rate variability biofeedback on measures of anxiety and relaxation states. International Journal of Stress Management; 2013;20(4):391–411.
  40. 40. Sherlin L, Gevirtz R, Wyckoff S, Muench F. Effects of respiratory sinus arrhythmia biofeedback versus passive biofeedback control. International Journal of Stress Management; 2009;16(3):233–48.
  41. 41. Wells R, Outhred T, Heathers JAJ, Quintana DS, Kemp AH. Matter Over Mind: A Randomised-Controlled Trial of Single-Session Biofeedback Training on Performance Anxiety and Heart Rate Variability in Musicians. PLoS ONE; 2012 Oct 4;7(10):e46597. pmid:23056361
  42. 42. Lin IM, Tai LY, Fan SY. Breathing at a rate of 5.5breaths per minute with equal inhalation-to-exhalation ratio increases heart rate variability. International Journal of Psychophysiology; 2014 Mar;91(3):206–11. pmid:24380741
  43. 43. Van Diest I, Verstappen K, Aubert AE, Widjaja D, Vansteenwegen D, Vlemincx E. Inhalation/Exhalation Ratio Modulates the Effect of Slow Breathing on Heart Rate Variability and Relaxation. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback; 2014 Aug 26;39(3–4):171–80. pmid:25156003
  44. 44. Cappo BM, Holmes DS. The utility of prolonged respiratory exhalation for reducing physiological and psychological arousal in non-threatening and threatening situations. Journal of Psychosomatic Research; 1984 Jan;28(4):265–73. pmid:6481661
  45. 45. Stark R, Schienle A, Walter B, Vaitl D. Effects of paced respiration on heart period and heart period variability. Psychophysiology; 2000 May;37(3):302–9. pmid:10860408
  46. 46. Steffen PR, Austin T, DeBarros A, Brown T. The Impact of Resonance Frequency Breathing on Measures of Heart Rate Variability, Blood Pressure, and Mood. Frontiers in Public Health; 2017 Aug 25;5. pmid:28890890
  47. 47. Sakairi Y, Nakatsuka K, Shimizu T. Development of the Two-Dimensional Mood Scale for self-monitoring and self-regulation of momentary mood states. Japanese Psychological Research; 2013 Jun 5;n/a–n/a.
  48. 48. Barrett LF, Mesquita B, Ochsner KN, Gross JJ. The Experience of Emotion. Annual Review of Psychology; 2007 Jan;58(1):373–403. pmid:17002554
  49. 49. Russell JA. Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review; 2003;110(1):145–72. pmid:12529060
  50. 50. Yik M, Russell JA, Steiger JH. A 12-point circumplex structure of core affect. Emotion; 2011;11(4):705–31. pmid:21707162
  51. 51. Yik MS, Russell JA, Barrett LF. Structure of self-reported current affect: Integration and beyond. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology; 1999;77(3):600–19.
  52. 52. Van Dixhoorn J, Duivenvoorden HJ. Efficacy of Nijmegen questionnaire in recognition of the hyperventilation syndrome. Journal of Psychosomatic Research; 1985 Jan;29(2):199–206. pmid:4009520
  53. 53. Mathôt S, Schreij D, Theeuwes J. OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behavior Research Methods; 2011 Nov 16;44(2):314–24. pmid:22083660
  54. 54. Wang Y-P, Kuo TBJ, Lai C-T, Chu J-W, Yang CCH. Effects of respiratory time ratio on heart rate variability and spontaneous baroreflex sensitivity. Journal of Applied Physiology; 2013 Dec;115(11):1648–55. pmid:24092689
  55. 55. Morey RD. Confidence Intervals from Normalized Data: A correction to Cousineau (2005). Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology; 2008 Sep 1;4(2):61–4.
  56. 56. Doyon J, Bellec P, Amsel R, Penhune V, Monchi O, Carrier J, et al. Contributions of the basal ganglia and functionally related brain structures to motor learning. Behavioural Brain Research; 2009 Apr;199(1):61–75. pmid:19061920
  57. 57. Gallego J, Perruchet P. Effect of practice on the voluntary control of a learned breathing pattern. Physiology & Behavior; 1991 Feb;49(2):315–9.
  58. 58. Otis AB. The Work of Breathing. Physiological Reviews; 1954 Jul;34(3):449–58. pmid:13185751
  59. 59. Mortola JP. How to breathe? Respiratory mechanics and breathing pattern. Respiratory Physiology & Neurobiology; 2019 Mar;261:48–54. pmid:30605732
  60. 60. Poon CS. Introduction: Optimization Hypothesis in the Control of Breathing. Control of Breathing and Its Modeling Perspective; 1992;371–84.
  61. 61. Rapee RM, Medoro L. Fear of physical sensations and trait anxiety as mediators of the response to hyperventilation in nonclinical subjects. Journal of Abnormal Psychology; 1994;103(4):693–9. pmid:7822570
  62. 62. Allen B, Friedman BH. Positive emotion reduces dyspnea during slow paced breathing. Psychophysiology; 2012 Jan 31;49(5):690–6. pmid:22292794
  63. 63. Chonan T, Mulholland MB, Altose MD, Cherniack NS. Effects of changes in level and pattern of breathing on the sensation of dyspnea. Journal of Applied Physiology [Internet]. American Physiological Society; 1990 Oct;69(4):1290–5. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jappl.1990.69.4.1290 pmid:2262446
  64. 64. Denot-Ledunois S, Vardon G, Gallego J. 26. Effects of voluntary changes in breathing frequency on respiratory comfort. Biological Psychology; 1996 Jan;43(3):257.
  65. 65. Oku Y, Saidel GM, Altose MD, Cherniack NS. Perceptual contributions to optimization of breathing. Annals of Biomedical Engineering; 1993 Sep;21(5):509–15. pmid:8239091
  66. 66. Shenhav A, Musslick S, Lieder F, Kool W, Griffiths TL, Cohen JD, et al. Toward a Rational and Mechanistic Account of Mental Effort. Annual Review of Neuroscience; 2017 Jul 25;40(1):99–124. pmid:28375769
  67. 67. Boulding R, Stacey R, Niven R, Fowler SJ. Dysfunctional breathing: a review of the literature and proposal for classification. European Respiratory Review; 2016 Aug 31;25(141):287–94. pmid:27581828