Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Pupil response to noxious corneal stimulation

  • Emmanuel B. Alabi ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Emmanuel B. Alabi, Trefford L. Simpson

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    ebalabi@uwaterloo.ca

    Affiliation University of Waterloo, School of Optometry and Vision Science, Waterloo, ON, Canada

  • Trefford L. Simpson

    Contributed equally to this work with: Emmanuel B. Alabi, Trefford L. Simpson

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation University of Waterloo, School of Optometry and Vision Science, Waterloo, ON, Canada

Abstract

Purpose

Ocular somatosensory-autonomic reflexes play critical roles in maintaining homeostasis of the eye. The purpose of this study was to investigate the pupil response to nociceptive corneal stimuli.

Methods

A Waterloo-Belmonte pneumatic esthesiometer was used to determine detection thresholds and randomly deliver mechanical and chemical stimuli from levels of detection threshold to twice the threshold in 50% steps to the central cornea of 15 healthy subjects. For each stimulus, imaging of the stimulated/unstimulated eye was performed using two modified/calibrated Logitech c920 digital cameras for 4 seconds each, pre/post stimulus capture. The data were processed with a custom segmentation algorithm to help identify the pupils and pupil diameter was measured using ImageJ software. Pupil dilation response differences between the ipsi- and contralateral eye was analyzed using dependent t-tests. The effect of stimulus intensity, modality and sex of subjects were analyzed using repeated measures.

Results

In mechanical and chemical stimulation experiments, there was no difference in pupil responses between the stimulated eye and the unstimulated eye, (all dependent T-test p > 0.05). On average, pupil diameter increased from baseline as the corneal stimulus intensity increased. This happened regardless of whether mechanical or chemical stimulation occurred (ANOVA p < 0.05). At 200% threshold, pupil diameter was greater than at all stimulus intensities (Tukey HSD, all p < 0.05). Based on stimulus intensity, females had greater pupil diameters than males at levels of 150% threshold and 200% threshold (ANOVA p < 0.05, all Tukey HSD p < 0.05).

Conclusion

This study serves as a basis for the characterization of the local stimulus-response neural circuitry relating nociceptive stimuli to autonomic responses and in combination with our work on completely separate autonomic circuits of bulbar conjunctival vessel dilation and reflex tearing suggests that the monotonic measurements of redness, tearing and pupils provide accurate, separable responses that reflect painful stimulus intensity.

Introduction

The International Association for the Study of Pain defined pain as, “an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage, or described in terms of such damage” [1]. The word “pain” has also been used to describe the experiences associated with discomfort and other unpleasant feelings [2,3]. Pain is subjective and by definition it is experienced only when an individual is in a conscious state, yet the perception and modulation of pain induces brain activity that is driven by autonomic processes that operate below the level of consciousness [46]. Previous reports have proposed that autonomic nervous system (ANS) responses have a strong relationship to pain perception and as such, may be possible alternatives for the measurement of pain [7,8].

Within the eye, the ANS controls (among others) two antagonistic iris muscles, the sphincter and dilator pupillae to change pupil size. The sphincter pupillae is innervated by parasympathetic fibers and constricts the pupil, and conversely, the dilator pupillae is innervated by sympathetic fibers and dilates the pupil [9]. Accommodation, luminance, attention, and alertness (among others) cause fluctuations in pupil size [1012]. The relationship between pupil size changes and pain perception has been looked into quantitatively by various researchers and has been termed pupillary reflex dilation [13], pupil dilation response [14], phasic pupil dilation [14,15], reflex pupillary dilation [16], and ciliospinal reflex [17]. Chapman et al. [14] delivered intra-cutaneous noxious fingertip stimulation to 20 subjects at four different intensities and observed a pupillary dilation response. The pupillary dilation response began 0.3 s after delivery of the stimulus and peaked at 1.25s. The researchers concluded that there was a consistent pupillary dilation response to painful systemic stimulation in a dose-response manner.

The pain experience for men and women appear to be different [18,19]. Ellermeier and Westphal [20] suggested females had greater pupil dilation responses than males when tonic pressure was applied to the fingers of subjects, and in the same study, females reported greater pain than males while experiencing the same amounts of noxious stimulation.

On the ocular surface, corneal sensitivity has been shown to vary with age [2123], time of day [24], and menstruation [25,26] (among other factors). However, there is limited research on the effect of gender on corneal sensitivity [23,27]. Acosta et al. [23] reported that in comparison to men of similar age, premenopausal women had lower thresholds to both mechanical and chemical corneal stimulation but, there was no difference between the overall corneal sensitivity of males and females.

Corneal nociceptors receive their innervation from the trigeminal ganglion, via the nasociliary branch of the ophthalmic division of the trigeminal nerve [28,29] and respond to chemical, mechanical and thermal stimulation [3037]. These neurons have been shown to respond to pneumatic stimulation delivered using the esthesiometer used in the experiment we report: Corneal mechanical response is to ocular surface temperature flow, chemical response is to the protons dissolved in the tears when the stimulus column has additional CO2, and cooling is to room temperature flow [30,35,36].

The autonomic responses to painful ocular stimuli are relatively poorly understood. We have shown that tearing occurs after painful corneal stimulation [38], in a threshold-scaled, dose-response manner and, similarly, bulbar conjunctival vaso-dilation (hyperemia) [39] also occurs in a dose-dependent manner. Understanding these local neural circuits are important in-and-of-themselves, but since ocular inflammatory responses are tied to local pain and redness responses, understanding these mechanisms is important in order to systematically and fully characterize the inflammatory response in humans, in for example, in dry eye [40]. In addition, since pupillary responses themselves occur after painful systemic stimulation, we explored how the pupil changed with these local (corneal) stimuli in order to begin to determine if this autonomic system varied similarly to tearing and redness responses.

The purpose of this exploratory study, then, was to determine whether a pupil response exists for nociceptive corneal mechanical and chemical stimuli, and if so, whether the pupil response is intensity specific. In addition, we were interested in exploring whether the stimulation modality, ipsi- and contralateral effects and whether there were differences in the response between sexes.

Methods

Subjects

Ethics clearance (ORE # 19252) was obtained from the Office of Research Ethics at the University of Waterloo before the study began. Eligible subjects signed an informed consent document before enrolment in the study.

15 healthy subjects participated in this study. All subjects underwent a comprehensive ocular examination (with 3 months prior to the study), including an ocular surface work up, where a licensed clinician concluded that they were free from any disease or disorder of the ocular system and appendages. There were 8 male and 7 female volunteers ranging in age from 19 to 34. Subjects on any topical or systemic medication were excluded from the study.

Waterloo computer-controlled Belmonte esthesiometer

The computer-controlled Belmonte esthesiometer has been described before [31,35,41]. Our modified device used for the delivery of mechanical and chemical stimuli to the ocular surface consists of a control box that electronically regulates the mixture of air and carbon dioxide (CO2). The flow rates of air and concentration of CO2 are separately controlled by two digital flow controllers. Within the nozzle assembly is a thermostat to control temperature. A calibrated video camera was used to ensure that the stimulus was orthogonal to, and the nozzle tip was 5mm from the ocular surface.

Nociceptive stimuli

Mechanical stimuli consisted of a series of air pulses with varying flow rates from 0 to 200 ml/min and chemical stimulation was delivered by increasing the concentration of CO2 in the air. An ascending methods of limits [42] was used to determine mechanical and chemical detection thresholds of the cornea.

The mechanical threshold, which is the lowest air flow rate (with CO2 set at 0%) that the subject could detect, was first determined. The flow-rate steps were set at 10 mL/min, and the mechanical threshold was the average of three readings when the subject first reported the stimulus. For determining the chemical threshold, the flow rate of air was set at half the initially determined mechanical threshold, and CO2 was added to the air in increments of 5% CO2. The chemical threshold was the average of three first reports of stimulus detection.

Stimulus delivery

The subjects wore in-ear headphones with white noise playing in the background. The stimulus was presented at the corneal apex of the left eye while subjects viewed a fixation target that was 3 meters away. The tip of the esthesiometer was rotated to ensure the stimulus was delivered perpendicular to the corneal surface during stimulus delivery. The temperature of the air was set to 50°C, this decreased to 33.4°C at the ocular surface at room temperature of 23°C. This was calibrated using a custom electronic thermometer positioned 5 mm from the probe tip (which corresponds to the position of the ocular surface in the experiments). The duration of the stimulus was 2 seconds and it was delivered to the ocular surface immediately after a blink. The subject blinked freely between trials. The next stimulus was triggered after the sensation caused by the last stimulus had disappeared completely.

Once the mechanical and chemical thresholds were determined, a sham (no stimulus was delivered but the participant thought a stimulus was being delivered) and three stimuli were then delivered to the subject in random order, in both the mechanical and chemical stimulation experiments–stimuli at 0%, threshold (sham), stimulus at 150% threshold, and stimulus at 200% threshold. The pupil size prior to stimulus delivery (baseline) and after stimulus delivery were compared.

Data processing and pupil size measurements

Imaging of the stimulated and unstimulated eye was performed using two modified and calibrated Logitech c920 digital cameras (Logitech c920; Logitech International S.A., Newark, CA), for 4 seconds before (pre-stimulus capture) and 4 seconds after the delivery of the stimulus (post-stimulus capture). The data were processed with a custom segmentation algorithm to help identify the pupils. We then measured the pupil diameter (average of horizontal and vertical measures) using ImageJ software (NIH, Bethesda, MD), for the pre/post capture periods.

The percent (%) change in pupil size was calculated in 2 ways. First, the average pupil size for the 4 seconds after “stimulation” in the baseline (no stimulus) trial was used as the reference and then, the maximum pupil size from the fitted function was used. “% change maximum” being (100*(baseline–max) / baseline) was calculated. Second, % change in average pupil diameter after no stimulation was calculated using the mean of baseline and no stimulus average as the baseline, and pupil diameter after 2x threshold stimulus intensity. “% change average” was (100*(mean pupil diameter after stimulation—mean of baseline)/ mean of baseline).

Analyses

Preliminary results showed large pupil change effect sizes (of approximately 1.5) and sample size calculations showed approximately 6 participants were necessary to test the (paired) mean difference before and after corneal stimulation [43]. However, in order to stratify by the sex predictor variable, we chose a 15-participant sample size with approximately equal males and female subject numbers.

Initial exploratory examination of pupillary dilation after stimulation was done using generalized additive models (GAMs), fitting smooth functions (spline-based) to pupil size against time, with stimulus intensity, stimulus type (mechanical or chemical) and sex as predictor variables. This was done using the MGCV package in R [44,45]. Non-linear mixed effect models were fit to pupil size against time functions, using the SAEMIX package in R[44], and non-linear regression was done in SPSS for Windows, Version 16.0 (Chicago, SPSS Inc.). Differences in mean pupil diameter using stimulus modality, sex and stimulus intensity predictors were analyzed using repeated measures (RM)-ANOVA and Tukey’s Honestly Significant Difference (HSD) tests for post hoc analysis. Pupil size differences between the ipsi- and contralateral eye was analyzed using dependent t-tests. An alpha value of 0.05 or less was assumed to be significant.

Results

A GAM with a 5 knot spline to smooth the pupil diameter and time function (separately for each stimulus intensity), stimulus type, and sex predictors accounted for approximately 60% of the deviance (R2 = 0.6), with significant smooth terms (all p<0.001), and significant main effects of stimulus type, intensity and sex (all p<0.01). This general smoothing function fit to the data, consisting of an initial pupil dilation and an approximate return to near baseline diameter 8 seconds after stimulation is shown in Fig 1.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Normalized smooth pupil dilation function (“Effect”, ordinate, normalized mm) versus time (seconds) derived from a generalized additive model with time, sex, and stimulus intensity and type predictor variables.

The grey region around the solid line is the approximate 95% confidence interval.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0227771.g001

A similar pupil dilation and constriction function, was fit to the data using nonlinear mixed modeling.

After initial exploratory analysis, it was apparent that the confidence intervals of the estimates C commonly included zero, so we fitted a 2-parameter model without C.

An example of the mixed model results on randomly selected participants for different stimulus intensities is in Fig 2. The left panel is using data from baseline stimulation and the right panel from twice-threshold intensities. Illustrating that the stimulus does have an effect on the pupil, the difference in the ‘size” component (B) was statistically significant comparing baseline to twice-threshold stimuli (paired t(df = 29) -41.702, p< 0.001). Figs 3 and 4 show the comparisons of these fits for male and female participants.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Example hierarchical fits of pupil size versus time for 4 (randomly selected) subjects.

Left panel, 4 baseline stimuli and right panel same 4 subjects’ stimuli at twice threshold. Each time point has mechanical and chemical stimulus measurements.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0227771.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Group pre- and post-stimulus pupil diameter for mechanical corneal stimulation in male and female subjects.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0227771.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. Group pre- and post-stimulus pupil diameter for chemical corneal stimulation in male and female subjects.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0227771.g004

Mean pupil diameters

In order to examine integrated (“area under curve”) performance we also looked at the average effects over the 8 second post-stimulus interval. A summary of the mean pupil diameters for males and females across the different stimulus intensities and modalities can be found in Table 1 below. Pupil diameters at baseline (before each measurement session), 0% threshold (the catch trials when the esthesiometer intensity setting was zero) and with mechanical and chemical stimulation of differing intensities stratified by modality are shown in Fig 5.

thumbnail
Fig 5.

Mean pupil diameter across different stimulus intensities for mechanical (blue)/chemical (red) corneal stimulation experiments (upper panel) and for male (blue) and female (red) subjects (lower panel). Error bars denote 95% confidence interval.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0227771.g005

thumbnail
Table 1. Mean ±(SD) pupil size between males and females for corneal stimulation experiments.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0227771.t001

On average, pupil diameter increased from baseline as the corneal apical stimulus intensity increased. This happened regardless of whether mechanical or chemical stimulation occurred (ANOVA F(4,224) = 356.6, p < 0.05). At 200% threshold, average pupil diameter was greater than at all other stimulus intensities (Tukey HSD, all p < 0.05).

Effects of stimulus modality and stimulus intensity on pupil diameter

There was no difference in average pupil size between chemical and mechanical stimulation based on stimulus intensity (ANOVA F(4,224) = 0.1, p > 0.05).

Relationship between ipsi- and contralateral eye

With mechanical and chemical stimulation of the cornea (Fig 6), there was no difference in pupil responses between the ipsilateral eye (stimulated eye [left eye]) and the contralateral (unstimulated) eye (paired t-test t(14) = 0.6, and t(14) = 0.8, mechanical and chemical respectively, both p>0.05).

thumbnail
Fig 6.

Box-plot of mean pupil diameter in the ipsilateral (stimulated) and the contralateral (unstimulated) eye after corneal mechanical (upper panel) and chemical (lower panel) stimulation. Error bars denote 95% confidence interval.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0227771.g006

Effects of sex and stimulus intensity on pupil diameter

There was a difference in pupil diameter between male and female subjects based on stimulus intensity (ANOVA F(4,224) = 5.9, p < 0.05). Females had greater pupil diameters than males at 150% and 200% of threshold (Tukey HSD p < 0.05).

Discussion

This is the first study to demonstrate the effect of systematic mechanical and chemical noxious ocular surface stimulation on pupil responses. Suprathreshold stimulation of the cornea appears to evoke a 2-to-3 second dose-response-like pupil diameter increase and a near-return to pre-stimulation diameter after 4 seconds.

The dilator and sphincter pupillae muscles of the iris are innervated by sympathetic and parasympathetic neurons respectively. Together, these smooth muscles work antagonistically to control pupil size [9]. The Edinger-Westphal nucleus, located in the midbrain, controls circular fibers within the sphincter pupillae to cause constriction of the pupils, mediating the pupillary light reflex[46]; it is however not involved in dilation. The hypothalamus controls radial muscles in the dilator pupillae to cause pupillary dilation [9]. The hypothalamus is also directly activated by ocular surface pain via the trigeminal pathway [28,29], therefore the pupil dilation response to nociceptive corneal stimuli observed in this study may support the idea that a neural connection exists within the hypothalamus linking dilation response and corneal nociception.

There have been reports of similar increase in pupil size in response to noxious stimulation. Chapman et al. [14] reported that pupil diameter increased when the intensity of painful fingertip stimulation was increased and proposed that this pupil size change was a complex defensive response to nociception mediated within the brain, and thus a good indicator of central processing of painful stimuli. Larson et al. [47] observed the effect of painful stimulation on physiological outcomes including pupil size, heart rate and arterial blood pressure in anesthetized subjects. They observed greater pupil sizes in subjects as putative painful stimulation increased and concluded that in comparison to heart rate and arterial blood flow, pupil responses provide greater sensitivity as a measurement of noxious stimulation. Oka et al. [15] studied the pupil dilation response to nociceptive stimuli and concluded that the increased pupil response to increasing noxious sensory input was not an artifact of cognitive effort, independent of painful experience, as some researchers had earlier suggested [48], but rather existed as part of a higher order defense response.

In our experiment reported here, we assessed the effect of the sex of subjects on the pupil dilation response to nociceptive corneal stimuli. From our results, there seems to be a difference in male and female pupil responses to threshold-scaled and therefore approximately equivalent noxious ocular surface stimulation: In females, pupil dilation was greater to suprathreshold stimuli than it was in males. Fillingim and Maixner [18] reviewed experiments conducted by others on gender differences in responses to noxious experimental stimuli using a ‘box-score’ or vote counting method and concluded that “females exhibit greater sensitivity to noxious stimulation than males”. Population based research by Unruh [19] shows that in comparison to men, there is a greater prevalence of back pain, arthritis and headaches among women. These findings may possibly be due to women having a tendency to honestly report pain (both acute and chronic) more often than men [49]. Hypotheses about sex differences include sex-role expectations [50], hormones [51,52], differences in skin thickness and body size [53] and sensory differences between men and women [54]. Ellermeier and Westphal [20] reported females had greater pupil dilation responses than males when high tonic pressure was applied to the fingers of subjects, and in that same study, females reported greater pain than males when the same amount of pressure was applied to the subjects’ fingers. Since it is not possible for one to voluntarily control his/her pupil response to noxious stimulation, the sex differences found in our study point to affective or sensory components of pain as opposed to subject bias in response [50] or attitudinal factors [55].

In this experiment, noxious stimulation of one eye caused pupil dilation in both eyes, much as withdrawal of light causes direct and consensual mydriasis. Similarly, Beradelli et al. [56] reported that ipsilateral electrical stimulation of the supraorbital branch of the trigeminal nerve caused a bilateral ocular reflex (blinking). The balance between the sympathetic and the parasympathetic system input determines the pupillary response as increased sympathetic innervation will cause a resultant pupillary dilation while decreased innervation will result in pupillary constriction. A possible explanation for the pupil dilation response may be linked to the fight or flight response–a physiological activation of the sympathetic nervous system that occurs when a harmful event (in this case noxious corneal stimulation) is perceived. The fight or flight response is characterized by the release of different hormones, and, for example, the adrenal medulla is known to secrete epinephrine and norepinephrine, the latter being the same neurotransmitter that modulates the iris dilator responsible for pupil dilation [57].

Situ and Simpson [38] reported comparable results to those of this study when they investigated the interaction between noxious corneal stimulation and tear secretion; mechanical and chemical corneal stimulation evoked increased reflex tearing. In addition, Situ and Simpson [38] reported that mechanical corneal stimulation produced the most reflex tearing. In a recent study by Alabi and Simpson [39], conjunctival redness increased in a dose dependent way to noxious corneal stimulation. The authors went on to further show that chemical stimulation of the cornea had a greater effect on conjunctival redness than mechanical corneal stimulation. However, in the current study, we have shown that the pupil response to corneal stimulation was the same regardless of whether a chemical or mechanical stimulus is applied. This leads to the speculation that the reflex arcs producing local changes in tear formation and conjunctival vessel dilation in response to painful corneal stimulation do not behave in the same way to that producing pupil dilation. The pupils respond to both mechanical and chemical stimulation in a similar manner therefore it can be assumed that the afferent parts of these reflex arcs read the information from mechanical and chemical nociceptors in an analogous way.

Increased tear secretion, conjunctival vessel dilation and more recently, the pupil dilation in response to corneal nociception shown in this study, result from different motor effects in different tissue. Because of this, and because each response to pain is monotonically related to pain (at least when scaled to detection thresholds), stimulating the cornea systematically, might be useful in assessing pain (and perhaps even consciousness), objectively and with greater accuracy by assessing multiple ocular responses. Treister et al. [58] suggested that a combination of several autonomic measures provided more accurate information than each single measures, and so a combination of these physiological responses to pain could provide better characterization of the pain process.

Our pupil dilations in response to painful suprathreshold corneal stimuli were comparable to some recent results reported in the literature although details are difficult to match since factors such as subjective stimulus intensity (that we did not measure) or fixation target luminance were not uniform across studies, so numerical comparisons are somewhat fuzzy. Nevertheless, there are similarities: For example, our pupil dilations were maximum approximately 2 seconds after stimulation and this is very similar to that reported by Hofle et al. [59], who used pressure pain on fingers as stimuli. In a different study, Hofle et al. [60] showed that the small pupil dilation effect occurred much sooner than ours when participants just viewed a painful stimulus compared to an innocuous one. On the other hand, our pupil dilation maxima occurred somewhat sooner than those reported by Eisenach et al. [61], perhaps not surprisingly, since they used thermal stimulation of the forearm or calf. The % maximum change was and % average change of pupil diameters in our study (see methods) were 79.7% and 57.4 respectively, generally quite high, compared to other reports, but again, similar concerns about different methods make direct comparisons problematic. For example, Oka et al. [62], and Hofle et al. [59], report pupil dilation in response to pain of less than 1 mm while we have average change approaching 3 mm. Scaled differently, Eisenach et al. [61], show around 15% change whereas our stimulation produced between 60%– 80% dilation. Although the timing and amount of dilation differ, these reports and ours show the same thing—dilation of the pupil after painful stimulation. On the other hand, the differences might reflect varying methods but also might point to the neural circuits driving the ocular response differing from those responsible for those driven by painful stimulation of the limbs.

Some experimental limitations of this study include a restriction to the stimulus intensity range, a limit to the amount of time selected to observe the pupil response and not observing several autonomic responses simultaneously. The highest stimulus intensity used was twice the threshold. The potential to go beyond twice threshold for both mechanical and chemical corneal stimulation would be beneficial because information regarding what happens to the pupil response at higher ocular surface stimulation intensities remains unknown. In assessing the pupil response to noxious stimulation, we limited the measurements to a time frame of 4 seconds. This method was chosen because prior studies involving painful stimulation and pupil responses [14,15,20] identified the greatest pupil response within the first three seconds of the post stimulus period; however, the potential to observe the pupil response over a longer period of time would provide important information regarding the complete nature of the pupillary response to noxious stimulation.

Conclusion

In summary, this study provides some evidence that noxious mechanical and chemical stimulation of the central cornea evokes a dose dependent autonomic pupil dilation response. There seems to be a sex difference in the pupil dilation response, with women having a greater response than men when experiencing threshold-scaled equivalent amounts of noxious stimulation. This study serves as a basis for the characterization of the local stimulus-response neural circuitry relating nociceptive stimuli to autonomic responses and in combination with our work on completely separate autonomic circuits of bulbar conjunctival vessel dilation and reflex tearing suggests that the monotonic measurements of redness, tearing and pupils provide accurate, separable responses that reflect painful stimulus intensity.

References

  1. 1. Merskey H, Bogduk N (1994) Classification of chronic pain, IASP Task Force on Taxonomy. Seattle, WA: International Association for the Study of Pain Press.
  2. 2. Rosenthal P, Borsook D (2012) The corneal pain system. Part I: the missing piece of the dry eye puzzle. The Ocular Surface 10: 2–14. pmid:22330055
  3. 3. Jensen MP, Karoly P, Harris P (1991) Assessing the affective component of chronic pain: development of the Pain Discomfort Scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 35: 149–154. pmid:2046049
  4. 4. Casey KL, Minoshima S, Berger KL, Koeppe RA, Morrow TJ, Frey KA. (1994) Positron emission tomographic analysis of cerebral structures activated specifically by repetitive noxious heat stimuli. Journal of Neurophysiology 71: 802–807. pmid:8176441
  5. 5. Chen AC (1993) Human brain measures of clinical pain: a review I. Topographic mappings. Pain 54: 115–132. pmid:8233525
  6. 6. Coghill RC, Talbot JD, Evans AC, Meyer E, Gjedde A, Bushnell MC, et al. (1994) Distributed processing of pain and vibration by the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience 14: 4095–4108. pmid:8027764
  7. 7. Donaldson GW, Chapman CR, Nakamura Y, Bradshaw DH, Jacobson RC, Chapman CN. (2003) Pain and the defense response: structural equation modeling reveals a coordinated psychophysiological response to increasing painful stimulation. Pain 102: 97–108. pmid:12620601
  8. 8. Colloca L, Benedetti F, Pollo A (2006) Repeatability of autonomic responses to pain anticipation and pain stimulation. European Journal of Pain 10: 659–659. pmid:16337150
  9. 9. Beatty J, Lucero-Wagoner B (2000) The pupillary system. Handbook of Psychophysiology 2: 142–162.
  10. 10. Wierda SM, van Rijn H, Taatgen NA, Martens S (2012) Pupil dilation deconvolution reveals the dynamics of attention at high temporal resolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109: 8456–8460.
  11. 11. Hoeks B, Levelt WJ (1993) Pupillary dilation as a measure of attention: A quantitative system analysis. Behavior Research Methods 25: 16–26.
  12. 12. Iriki A, Tanaka M, Iwamura Y (1996) Attention-induced neuronal activity in the monkey somatosensory cortex revealed by pupillometrics. Neuroscience Research 25: 173–181. pmid:8829154
  13. 13. Yang LL, Niemann CU, Larson MD (2003) Mechanism of pupillary reflex dilation in awake volunteers and in organ donors. Anesthesiology: The Journal of the American Society of Anesthesiologists 99: 1281–1286.
  14. 14. Chapman CR, Oka S, Bradshaw DH, Jacobson RC, Donaldson GW (1999) Phasic pupil dilation response to noxious stimulation in normal volunteers: relationship to brain evoked potentials and pain report. Psychophysiology 36: 44–52. pmid:10098379
  15. 15. Oka S, Chapman CR, Jacobson RC (2000) Phasic pupil dilation response to noxious stimulation: Effects of conduction distance, sex, and age. Journal of Psychophysiology 14: 97.
  16. 16. Loewenfeld IE (1958) Mechanisms of reflex dilatation of the pupil. Documenta Ophthalmologica 12: 185–448. pmid:13609524
  17. 17. Reeves AG, Posner JB (1969) The ciliospinal response in man. Neurology 19: 1145–1145. pmid:5389693
  18. 18. Fillingim RB, Maixner W. Gender differences in the responses to noxious stimuli; 1995. Elsevier. pp. 209–221.
  19. 19. Unruh AM (1996) Gender variations in clinical pain experience. Pain 65: 123–167. pmid:8826503
  20. 20. Ellermeier W, Westphal W (1995) Gender differences in pain ratings and pupil reactions to painful pressure stimuli. Pain 61: 435–439. pmid:7478686
  21. 21. Lawrenson J, Ruskell G (1993) Investigation of limbal touch sensitivity using a Cochet-Bonnet aesthesiometer. British Journal of Ophthalmology 77: 339–343. pmid:8318479
  22. 22. Mollodot M, Owens H (1984) The influence of age on the fragility of the cornea. Acta Ophthalmologica 62: 819–824. pmid:6507068
  23. 23. Acosta MC, Alfaro ML, Borrás F, Belmonte C, Gallar J (2006) Influence of age, gender and iris color on mechanical and chemical sensitivity of the cornea and conjunctiva. Experimental Eye Research 83: 932–938. pmid:16784741
  24. 24. Millodot M (1972) Diurnal variation of corneal sensitivity. The British Journal of Ophthalmology 56: 844. pmid:4647135
  25. 25. Riss B, Binder S, Riss P, Kemeter P (1982) Corneal sensitivity during the menstrual cycle. The British Journal of Ophthalmology 66: 123–126. pmid:7059554
  26. 26. Millodot M, Lamont A (1974) Influence of menstruation on corneal sensitivity. The British Journal of Ophthalmology 58: 752. pmid:4433487
  27. 27. Millodot M (1975) Do blue-eyed people have more sensitive corneas than brown-eyed people? Nature 255: 151–152. pmid:1168859
  28. 28. Marfurt C, Kingsley R, Echtenkamp S (1989) Sensory and sympathetic innervation of the mammalian cornea. A retrograde tracing study. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science 30: 461–472.
  29. 29. Van der Werf F, Baljet B, Prins M, Ruskell G, Otto J (1996) Innervation of the palpebral conjunctiva and the superior tarsal muscle in the cynomolgous monkey: a retrograde fluorescent tracing study. Journal of Anatomy 189: 285. pmid:8886950
  30. 30. Vega JA, Simpson TL, Fonn D (1999) A noncontact pneumatic esthesiometer for measurement of ocular sensitivity: a preliminary report. Cornea 18: 675–681. pmid:10571298
  31. 31. Feng Y, Simpson TL (2003) Nociceptive sensation and sensitivity evoked from human cornea and conjunctiva stimulated by CO2. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science 44: 529–532.
  32. 32. Chen J, Feng Y, Simpson TL (2010) Human corneal adaptation to mechanical, cooling, and chemical stimuli. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science 51: 876–881.
  33. 33. Rao SBS, Simpson TL (2014) Measurement of Difference Thresholds on the Ocular Surface Difference Thresholds on the Ocular Surface. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science 55: 1095–1100.
  34. 34. Chen X, Gallar J, Pozo MA, Baeza M, Belmonte C (1995) CO2 stimulation of the cornea: a comparison between human sensation and nerve activity in polymodal nociceptive afferents of the cat. European Journal of Neuroscience 7: 1154–1163. pmid:7582088
  35. 35. Belmonte C, Acosta MC, Schmelz M, Gallar J (1999) Measurement of corneal sensitivity to mechanical and chemical stimulation with a CO2 esthesiometer. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science 40: 513–519.
  36. 36. Acosta MC, Tan ME, Belmonte C, Gallar J (2001) Sensations evoked by selective mechanical, chemical, and thermal stimulation of the conjunctiva and cornea. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science 42: 2063–2067.
  37. 37. Belmonte C, Aracil A, Acosta MC, Luna C, Gallar J (2004) Nerves and sensations from the eye surface. The Ocular Surface 2: 248–253. pmid:17216099
  38. 38. Situ P, Simpson TL (2010) Interaction of corneal nociceptive stimulation and lacrimal secretion. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science 51: 5640–5645.
  39. 39. Alabi EB, Simpson TL (2019) Conjunctival Redness Response to Corneal Stimulation. Optometry and Vision Science 96: 507–512. pmid:31274739
  40. 40. Stern ME, Gao J, Siemasko KF, Beuerman RW, Pflugfelder SC (2004) The role of the lacrimal functional unit in the pathophysiology of dry eye. Experimental Eye Research 78: 409–416. pmid:15106920
  41. 41. Situ P, Simpson T, Fonn D (2007) Eccentric variation of corneal sensitivity to pneumatic stimulation at different temperatures and with CO 2. Experimental Eye Research 85: 400–405. pmid:17662716
  42. 42. Gescheider GA (1997) Psychophysical measurement of thresholds: differential sensitivity. Psychophysics: the fundamentals: 1–15.
  43. 43. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A (2007) G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods 39: 175–191. pmid:17695343
  44. 44. Team RC (2013) R: A language and environment for statistical computing.
  45. 45. Pedersen EJ, Miller DL, Simpson GL, Ross N (2019) Hierarchical generalized additive models in ecology: an introduction with MGCV. PeerJ 7: e6876. pmid:31179172
  46. 46. Kardon R (1995) Pupillary light reflex. Current opinion in ophthalmology 6: 20–26. pmid:10160414
  47. 47. Larson MD, Sessler DI, Washington DE, Merrifield BR, Hynson JA, McGuire J. (1993) Pupillary response to noxious stimulation during isoflurane and propofol anesthesia. Anesthesia & Analgesia 76: 1072–1078.
  48. 48. Beatty J (1982) Phasic not tonic pupillary responses vary with auditory vigilance performance. Psychophysiology 19: 167–172. pmid:7071295
  49. 49. Bingefors K, Isacson D (2004) Epidemiology, co‐morbidity, and impact on health‐related quality of life of self‐reported headache and musculoskeletal pain—a gender perspective. European Journal of Pain 8: 435–450. pmid:15324775
  50. 50. Levine FM, De Simone LL (1991) The effects of experimenter gender on pain report in male and female subjects. Pain 44: 69–72. pmid:2038491
  51. 51. Velle W (1987) Sex differences in sensory functions. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine 30: 490–522. pmid:3452194
  52. 52. Craft RM, Mogil JS, Aloisi AM (2004) Sex differences in pain and analgesia: the role of gonadal hormones. European Journal of Pain 8: 397–411. pmid:15324772
  53. 53. Lautenbacher S, Strian F (1991) Sex differences in pain and thermal sensitivity: the role of body size. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 50: 179–183.
  54. 54. Feine JS, Bushnell MC, Miron D, Duncan GH (1991) Sex differences in the perception of noxious heat stimuli. Pain 44: 255–262. pmid:2052394
  55. 55. Otto MW, Dougher MJ (1985) Sex differences and personality factors in responsivity to pain. Perceptual and Motor Skills 61: 383–390. pmid:4069906
  56. 56. Berardelli A, Rothwell J, Day B, Marsden C (1985) Pathophysiology of blepharospasm and oromandibular dystonia. Brain 108: 593–608. pmid:4041776
  57. 57. Jansen AS, Van Nguyen X, Karpitskiy V, Mettenleiter TC, Loewy AD (1995) Central command neurons of the sympathetic nervous system: basis of the fight-or-flight response. Science 270: 644–646. pmid:7570024
  58. 58. Treister R, Kliger M, Zuckerman G, Aryeh IG, Eisenberg E (2012) Differentiating between heat pain intensities: the combined effect of multiple autonomic parameters. Pain 153: 1807–1814. pmid:22647429
  59. 59. Hofle M, Hauck M, Engel AK, Senkowski D (2012) Viewing a needle pricking a hand that you perceive as yours enhances unpleasantness of pain. Pain 153:1074–1081. pmid:22520059
  60. 60. Hofle M, Kenntner-Mabiala R, Pauli P, Alpers GW (2008) You can see pain in the eye: pupillometry as an index of pain intensity under different luminance conditions. International Journal of Psychophysiology 70:171–175. pmid:18644409
  61. 61. Eisenach JC, Curry R, Aschenbrenner CA, Coghill RC, Houle TT(2017) Pupil responses and pain ratings to heat stimuli: Reliability and effects of expectations and a conditioning pain stimulus. Jouranl of Neuroscience Methods 1:279:52–59.
  62. 62. Oka S, Chapman CR, Kim B, Nakajima I, Shimizu O, Oi Y (2007) Pupil dilation response to noxious stimulation: effect of varying nitrous oxide concentration. Clinical Neurophysiology 118:2016–2024. pmid:17646133