Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Developmental polychlorinated biphenyl exposure influences adult zebra finch reproductive behaviour

  • Sara DeLeon ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    sd279@cornell.edu

    Affiliations Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York, United States of America, Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York, United States of America

  • Michael S. Webster,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York, United States of America, Department of Neurobiology and Behaviour, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York, United States of America

  • Timothy J. DeVoogd,

    Roles Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York, United States of America

  • André A. Dhondt

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York, United States of America, Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York, United States of America

Abstract

Polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs) are worldwide chemical pollutants that have been linked to disrupted reproduction and altered sexual behaviour in many organisms. However, the effect of developmental PCB-exposure on adult passerine reproductive behaviour remains unknown. A commercial PCB mixture (Aroclor 1242) or an estrogenic congener (PCB 52) were administered in sublethal amounts to nestling zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) in the laboratory to identify effects of developmental PCB-exposure on adult zebra finch reproductive parameters. Results indicate that although traditional measures of reproductive success are not altered by this PCB dosage, PCBs do alter sexual behaviours such as male song and nesting behaviour. Males treated with PCB 52 in the nest sang significantly fewer syllables than control males, while females treated with Aroclor 1242 in the nest showed the strongest song preferences. PCB treatment also caused an increase in the number of nesting attempts and abandoned nests in the Aroclor 1242 treatment relative to the PCB 52 treatment, and offspring with control fathers fledged significantly earlier than those with fathers treated with Aroclor 1242. Behavioural differences between males seem to best explain these reproductive effects, most notably aggression. These findings suggest that sublethal PCB-exposure during development can significantly alter key reproductive characteristics of adult zebra finches, likely reducing fitness in the wild.

Introduction

Polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs) are a class of syththetic chemicals that are ubiquitous in the environment. They were produced commercially for use in industrial products, such as dielectric, hydraulic and heat transfer fluids [1]. PCBs have a di-benzene backbone, one to ten degrees of chlorination and were manufactured in mixtures of structurally related compounds known as congeners [2]. Congener mixture production was banned in the United States in the late 1970s, however, the chemical mixtures are still present and identifiable in the environment [3]. There are 209 different PCB congeners with different modes of action and complex individual and interactive effects [4]. Exposure to high levels of most PCB congeners can be lethal and highly chlorinated PCBs are more toxic than PCBs with fewer chlorine substitutions [5]. Additionally, high-level exposure to PCBs with fewer chlorine substitutions causes endocrine disruption [6,7], while low-level PCB exposure has sublethal effects. Due to similarities between the chemical structure of PCBs and sex steroids, sublethal PCB-exposure may cause behavioural changes, especially affecting reproductive behaviours that are regulated by hormones [810].

While organisms in the wild can experience long-term or chronic PCB exposure [1113], captive studies show that the timing of PCB exposure is particulary influential. Organisms may be especially sensitive to PCB exposure during development (e.g. [14]), with lasting consequences into adulthood [15]. Indeed, the developmental stress hypothesis postulates that adult morphology and behaviour is of evolutionary significance because it indicates whether an individual received adequate nutrition during development [1618]. Decades of studies indicate that PCB exposure during development can cause long-lasting effects that are influenced by the degree and location of congener chlorination. For example, when pregnant female rats are treated with the weakly estrogenic PCB mixture Aroclor 1221, their offspring show decreased anxiety behaviour, while exploration increases in male, but not female, offspring [19].

In birds, PCB exposure is also correlated with disrupted reproductive behaviours. A field study of great black-backed gulls (Larus marinus) shows that females exposed to PCBs through their food source have higher PCB blood concentrations, correlating with a decline in egg laying [20]. Other field studies show that in female tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor), PCB exposure through food is linked with changes to plumage, clutch size, and nest building behaviour [2123]. In captivity, American kestrels (Falco sparverius) fed with PCB-contaminated food exhibit altered mating, nesting and incubation behaviour, as well as altered chick development [2426]. Additionally, feeding PCBs to adult female zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) affects incubation time, the number of nests built and the number of clutches laid [27].

Despite many studies investigating the consequences of PCB exposure, we know of no studies in passerines that isolate the effects of nestling PCB exposure to the reproductive characteristics and behaviours of the adult. Understanding exposure during the nestling stage is critical because in polluted areas it is likely adult passerines are feeding contaminated insects to their offspring [2830], exposing their offspring to potential long-term impacts of PCBs on development and adult behaviours. Subsequent effects are likely to affect populations well beyond locations of contamination because natal dispersal in males moves them from their nestling location [31].

This study makes an important contribution to the currently limited experimental evaluation of the effects of nestling PCB exposure on adult reproductive characteristics and behaviour in zebra finches. We focus on two types of PCBs, selected to mimic environmental exposure and estrogenic affects, and administer them at sublethal doses during the nestling stage to determine impacts on reproductive behaviour once individuals reach adulthood. We measure key reproductive endpoints that influence the success of breeding, including male song, female song preference, male reproductive behaviour, and male brain anatomy in key locations within the brain. These varied endpoints allow for a more comprehensive assessment of the consequences of developmental PCBs on reproductive characteristics in the adult bird. Furthermore, these results could help explain the correlations found between PCB load and behavioural variations in wild birds present in areas of PCB contamination [32].

Materials and methods

Experimental subjects

All zebra finches used in this study were reared and housed at Cornell University (Ithaca, New York USA) using protocols approved by the Cornell University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC Protocol 1988–0135). All subjects were fed Kaytee forti-finch diet (Kaytee Products, Inc., Chilton, Wisconsin USA), oyster shell, cuttlebone, and water ad libitum. Subjects were housed in rooms on a 14L: 10D photoperiod, with 40–50% relative humidity, and an average temperature of 22°C.

To obtain the subjects for this study, thirteen healthy zebra finch pairs (F0) with prior successful breeding experience were housed as pairs in 13 breeding cages (61.0cm x 35.6cm x 40.6cm) equipped with one plastic nest cavity and coconut fiber nesting material. F1 offspring were individually identified by daily marking each nestling with coloured Crayola non-toxic permanent markers until day 14 when they were uniquely colour banded for identification (Avinet, Inc. Dryden, New York USA). Individual males and females (F1) were identified based on genetic sex determination [33,34] (see Supporting information for details), and randomly assigned to a treatment. Sex was confirmed with adult plumage by day 60.

Experimental design

We randomly assigned nestlings (F1) to a PCB treatment, the commercial mixture Aroclor 1242 or the estrogenic congener PCB 52, or a control group (see Supporting information for further details). Nestlings assigned to the Aroclor 1242 or PCB 52 groups were administered a total of 165μL of PCB (AccuStandard.com) dissolved at 1-mg/mL in Canola oil, while control nestlings received 165μL of only Canola oil [35] (Fig 1). The treatments were administered orally across seven days during the most influential period for exogenous estrogen effects on sexual differentiation [36]:10μL on day 2, 15μL on day 3, 20μL on day 4, and 30μL on days 5–8. PCB dosage was determined based on the range of exposure nestlings are likely to encounter in areas of environmental PCB contamination [29,3740].

thumbnail
Fig 1. Experimental timeline of F1 male and female zebra finches.

Unit-less numbers on the timeline correspond to days.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230283.g001

Fledglings remained in their natal cage with their parents until post-hatch day 50, which is approximately the middle of their sensitive song-learning phase [41]. From hatching until day 50, F1 males were not in auditory isolation from their father or neighboring males. On post-hatch day 51 fledglings were placed in single-sex aviaries (91.4cm x 61.0cm x 122.0cm) in a room separate from their parents, but not in auditory isolation from other zebra finch adult males.

Male song recording

F1 males (Control: N = 17; Aroclor 1242 and PCB 52: N = 7 for each) were individually recorded for one hour in a soundproof room on day 120±1, when adult male zebra finch song is stable [42] (Fig 1). Males were placed in the soundproof room at least one hour prior to the recording to acclimate. A single, untreated, unrelated adult female from the colony was placed in a separate cage and used as a stimulus in all song recordings. Males were recorded for up to three days, as needed, to obtain at least 10 song recordings [43,44].

Spectrograms generated in RavenPro 1.4 (Bioacoustics Research Program, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York USA), were used to measure song motif length, peak frequency, syllable number (less complex songs having fewer syllables [44]), and song rate (syllable number/motif length). Sound Analysis Pro (SAP) was used to measure similarity, accuracy, and sequence within a male’s own song (see Supporting information for further details).

Male behavioural assay

When the F1 males were a minimum of 100 days post-hatch (Fig 1) they were tested for the effectiveness of their courtship and parental behaviors. Thirteen groups consisting of three zebra finches each were placed in experimental cages: 1) one F1 male from the control group, 2) one F1 male from either the Aroclor 1242 group (N = 7) or the PCB 52 group (N = 6), and 3) one inexperienced and unrelated female from the colony with no PCB-exposure (see Supporting information for further details). Additionally, prior to the grouping the colour bands placed on the F1 males as nestlings were removed. The males were given a single black leg band uniquely placed to identify each male and to prevent any preference for band colour from the female [45]. For simplicity, ‘PCB treatment’ terminology in the results refers to the experimental male in the cage.

Prior to the beginning of the behavioural assay, weight and tarsus length were measured in the females and F1 males. When possible, the Aroclor 1242- and the PCB 52- treatments contained male pairs that were matched by family (i.e. male siblings were preferentially paired), size (weight and tarsus length), and age.

Behavioural observations

Ten-minute observation periods of the triad of zebra finches in the male behavioural assay began on the second day of the experiment. The observation periods occurred on every second day in a randomized order, in the mornings, within the first three hours of lights on, when the birds were most active. Observations continued daily for 90 days or until 14 days after the last fledgling event of the first successful clutch, whichever first occurred. The first successful clutch was defined as the first clutch within the 90-day period that fledged.

Behavioural observations were grouped into three classifications: pre-laying (before the first egg was laid), laying (after the first egg was laid, but before the first egg hatched, which includes the incubation period), and post-hatch (after the first egg hatched). During the pre-laying and laying period the number of one-minute intervals in which courtship behaviours (male singing, allopreening, and time in the nest box with the female) were observed at least once were recorded. During the post-hatch period the number of one-minute intervals in which courtship and parenting behaviours (male singing, allopreening, time in the nest box with the female, food provision to hatchlings) were observed at least once were recorded. During the pre-laying and laying period the number of ten-minute observation periods in which aggressive behaviour (i.e. chasing another individual) was observed at least once were recorded. In addition, the nest boxes in each cage were monitored and nesting and reproductive characteristics (e.g. latency to nest building, number of nests built, latency to laying, numbers of eggs laid before a successful clutch, number of clutches, clutch size, mortality, fledgling age, fledgling size, etc.) were measured. Offspring from this behavioural assay with F1 fathers are defined as F2 offspring.

Genetic parentage

F2 hatchling parentage was genetically determined using microsatellites evaluated using pinfeathers and tissue (see Supporting information for details). Between two and five pinfeathers were collected from living F2 fledglings and tissue samples were taken from F2 nestlings that died in the nest. The quills of the feather samples were immediately submerged in buffer solution and stored at room temperature. Tissue samples were frozen at -20°C immediately after collection until analysis.

Female song preference trials

Proximity (or association) song preference trials for adult F1 female subjects (Control: N = 7; Aroclor 1242: N = 6; PCB 52: N = 7) began when females were approximately 365 days old and continued for about six months (see Fig 1). Song preference trials were performed following protocol by Lauay et al. [46] (see Supporting information for details), performed in the morning between 0700–1100, and the investigator was blind to the experimental treatment of the female during data collection.

Stimulus recordings.

Females were exposed to three types of song choice trials, where each trial consisted of two recorded males singing where the female could make a choice. The first trial type was a choice between archived recordings of songs from two untreated males, one raised with a tutor, or ‘social’ (N = 4) and one without a tutor, or ‘isolate’ (N = 4). Untreated female zebra finches had been previously shown to be able to distinguish between these two song treatments with this experimental set-up [46]. The second and third trial types were recordings from experimental F1 males, where the second trial type was a choice between the songs from control males (N = 4) and Aroclor 1242-treated males (N = 4), and the third between the songs from control males (N = 4) and PCB 52-treated males (N = 4). Each female underwent seven trials: one with the ‘social’ vs. ‘isolate’ song types and three trials each with the ‘Control’ vs. ‘Aroclor 1242’ song types and ‘Control’ vs. ‘PCB 52’ song types (see Supporting information for details). No female was presented with the song of a family member. If there were male siblings from different treatment groups, their songs were paired in the trials.

Male histology and dendritic spine quantification

We extracted the whole brain for each male between day 200 and 300 after males completed the behavioural assay (Fig 1). Birds were anesthetized with 0.1 mL Chloropent and transcardially perfused with 0.9% saline followed by 10% formalin. Immediately after extraction, the fresh brain was weighed (Control: N = 14; Aroclor 1242: N = 6; PCB 52: N = 6) and then immersed in Golgi-Cox solution [47] for approximately 6 weeks, with the solution changed after the first week, when the brain was weighed again. The tissue was dehydrated and embedded in celloiden, sectioned at 100μm in the coronal plane, reacted with ammonia, counter-stained with methylene blue and cresyl violet, and mounted on slides. Testes were also removed and weighed (Control: N = 14; Aroclor 1242: N = 7; PCB 52: N = 7).

Dendritic spines were quantified in the HVC, RA, and hippocampus. Two human judges blind to the experimental treatment correlated 0.99 for the hippocampus quantification, and a single observer, also blind to the experimental treatment, quantified all data from the HVC and RA. In all dendritic spine quantification, neurons from a brain region were randomly and blindly chosen by scanning the regions for fully stained neurons with dendrites parallel to the sectioning plane. Within each of the nuclei regions, the number of spines was counted on three dendrites using 945x magnification with oil. Three 11–12μm sections along each of the three dendrites were quantified: a proximal location (approximately 11–12μm from the soma), a medial location (approximately 22–24μm from the soma), and a distal location (the last 11–12μm, usually approximately 40μm from the soma). When possible, all three locations were measured from the same dendrite. When it was not possible to get all measurements from the same dendrite, additional measurements were taken from other dendrites that were parallel to the sectioning plane and fully stained to increase the sample size. Spines were counted on different regions of the dendrite to control for environmental and developmental variations in spine count and position [48,49]. All spines were counted, including those that looked like a swelling of the dendritic surface, and spines that were long, thin projections (see [50] for photographs of spine variation). Final sample size for the dendritic quantification was N = 11 (Control: N = 3; Aroclor 1242: N = 5; PCB 52: N = 3) for the HVC, N = 14 (Control: N = 6; Aroclor 1242: N = 6; PCB 52: N = 2) for the RA, and N = 16 (Control: N = 5; Aroclor 1242: N = 6; PCB 52: N = 5) for the hippocampus.

Statistical analysis

All variables were first tested for normality and homogeniety of variance with a Shapiro-Wilk test and Levene’s test, respectively. To satisfy parameters of normality specified variables were transformed with a Box Cox transformation. If the distribution of a variable deviated from normality despite transformation, analyses were performed with non-parametric statistical tests. All other analyses were performed with parametric statistics. A P-value of <0.05 was required for significance. All analyses were performed with JMP® 9 (SAS Campus Drive, Cary, North Carolina USA).

Song characteristics were analyzed with one-way ANOVAs (general linear models) with the experimental treatment as a fixed effect and the genetic family of the individual as a random effect.

Measurements prior to the start of the behavioural assay from F1 males and females, as well as F2 measurements, were analyzed with one-way ANOVAs (general linear models) with the experimental treatment as a fixed effect and the genetic family of the individual as a random effect. Measurements of reproductive success of the F1 males were analyzed with one-way ANOVAs (general linear models) with the genetic father as a fixed effect and the behavioural assay of the father as a random effect.

If courtship behaviours occurred at any time during the one-minute interval, the bird was scored as performing that behaviour during that minute, for a score of one. For aggressive behaviours, each ten-minute observation period was considered the unit of measurement. If an aggressive behaviour occurred at any time during the ten-minute observation period, the bird was scored as performing that behaviour during that observation period, for a score of one. The daily scores for courtship and aggressive behaviour were summed for each F1 male during each behavioural period (pre-laying, laying, post-hatch) and analysed as count data. Statistical differences were determined using a T-test or a Kruskal-Wallis test.

In cages where an adult male died or the behavioural assay was discontinued due to safety reasons, the behavioural stage during which the assay was stopped is excluded from the behavioural analysis, and only cage treatments with complete prior behavioural stages are included. Nesting and reproductive characteristics were analysed using a T-test or a Kruskal-Wallis test.

A mixed model was used to test for effects of treatment on female size (tarsus length and body weight at day 20 and day 120) and age at fledging, with treatment and mean tarsus length of the parents as fixed effects, and family and observer as a random effect.

Female location in the experimental set-up was recorded at the beginning of every 15-second interval, for a total of 33 measurements for each of the two 8-minute observation periods (track 1 and track 2) for a maximum possible count of 66 measurements. Females that did not move to an end region (S1 Fig) during the duration of the trial were removed from the analysis. The location of the female in the end regions (S1 Fig) was interpreted as a preference for song coming from the closest speaker and results are reported as the average number of counts that a female made a choice. Because sublethal levels of PCBs have shown to affect bird song [32], there were a priori reasons to expect differences between the groups. Therefore, paired T-tests were performed to analyse female preference for song.

Total brain mass and testes mass were analyzed with one-way ANOVAs where treatment was a fixed effect and age at perfusion was a random effect. Due to small sample sizes, the number of dendritic spines were analyzed with Student’s T-tests.

Results

Male song characteristics

Song motif length, peak frequency within the motif, and song rate did not differ among male zebra finches in the three treatment groups (Song motifs: F2,28 = 1.79, P = 0.19; Peak frequency: F2,28 = 0.69, P = 0.51; Song rate: F2,28 = 1.62, P = 0.22; Fig 2). However, the males in the three groups differed in the number of syllables within their song motifs (F2,28 = 4.08, P = 0.03; Fig 2c), where the control group sang significantly more syllables in their song motifs than individuals treated with PCB 52. No differences in any measurements of song consistency within an individuals’ own song were identified (Similarity: F2,28 = 2.54, P = 0.10; Accuracy: F2,28 = 2.71, P = 0.08; Sequence: Kruskal-Wallis: H3 = 0.29, P = 0.87).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Adult male song characteristics are influenced by PCB exposure in the nest.

The mean value of adult song characteristics (±SE) of developmentally treated Aroclor 1242, control, and PCB 52 male zebra finches. Significant differences are indicated with an asterisk (*).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230283.g002

Male behavioural assay

There were no differences between F1 male treatment groups in fledgling age, D20 body mass or tarsus length, D120 body mass or tarsus length, body mass at experiment start, or tarsus length at experiment start (S1 and S2 Tables). There was also no difference in body mass or tarsus length at experiment start between the adult females in the two types of treatment cages (S2 Table).

The assay was stopped in four cages due to extreme and continuous aggression between adult males. In one Aroclor 1242 treatment cage the assay was stopped (laying stage) due to the aggression from the control male towards the Aroclor 1242 male. The assay was also stopped in two separate PCB 52 treatment cages (pre-laying stage) due to the aggression from the control male towards the PCB 52 male. In one PCB 52 treatment cage the assay was stopped (post-fledgling stage) due to aggression from the PCB 52 male towards the control male. Additionally, the assay was stopped in one Aroclor 1242 treatment cage (post-hatch period) and in one PCB 52 treatment cage (laying stage) due to the death of the respective control males from unknown causes. However, there were no differences in the levels of measured aggressive behaviours in the pre-laying or laying period in either experimental treatment (S3 Table).

Reproductive behaviour.

With the exception of singing, reproductive behaviours did not differ between the control and treatment males, within the treatments or between the treatments (S4 Table) in the pre-laying, laying or post-hatch periods. During the pre-laying period, control males in the Aroclor 1242 treatment cages sang significantly more than control males in the PCB 52 treatment cages (Kruskal-Wallis: H2 = 4.40; P = 0.04), although the amount of total singing in the pre-laying period did not differ between treatment cages (Student’s T-test: t9 = -2.19, P = 0.06). During the laying period the amount of total singing in the Aroclor 1242 treatment cages was greater than the amount of total singing in the PCB 52 treatment cages (Student’s T-test: t7 = -3.60, P = 0.01).

Reproductive characteristics.

There is no evidence that the experimental treatment to the F1 nestling males compromised their fertility as adults, individuals from each treatment group sired genetic offspring. There was only one case of mixed paternity in the behavioural assay, where both the control male and the PCB 52- treated male in one cage each sired offspring. In all other cages only one male sired the entirety of the offspring from that cage.

The two cage treatments did not differ in the latency to nest building, laying, or hatching; number of eggs hatched; number of eggs per clutch; total number of eggs; or number of eggs abandoned (Table 1). There were no differences in any traditional measures of reproductive success between the F1 PCB-treated and control males (Table 1). The Aroclor 1242 cage treatment built significantly more nests before a successful clutch than the PCB 52 cage treatment (Table 1). Females in the Aroclor 1242 cages laid a significantly greater number of eggs before their first successful clutch than the females in the PCB 52 cages (Table 1). The PCB 52 cage treatment also had significantly earlier successful clutches than the Aroclor 1242 cage treatment (Table 1).

thumbnail
Table 1. Male behavioural assay nesting characteristics.

Significant differences are indicated with an asterisk (*) and darker shaded cells.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230283.t001

The F2 fledgling age was significantly different between the F1 genetic fathers (Table 2). A Tukey’s post hoc test shows that the F2 offspring in the control group fledged significanlty earlier than offspring with genetic fathers treated with Aroclor 1242, but not PCB 52, which had intermediate fledling age (Table 2). There were no significant differences between the number of F2 offspring hatched, fledged, and the mass or tarsus length in the F2 fledglings based on their genetic parentage (Table 2). There was also no difference between the number of male or female F2 offspring from the F1 genetic fathers (Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Male behavioural assay offspring (F2) characteristics.

Significant differences are indicated with an asterisk (*) and darker shaded cells.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230283.t002

Female song preference assay

All measurements of F1 female fledgling age and size (day 20 weight and tarsus length, day 120 weight and tarsus length) were not different between treatment groups (S1 Table). Out of the total 265 trials, females made a choice in 228 (86%). When the three female groups were presented with social male song in comparison to isolated male song, only females treated with Aroclor 1242 significantly preferred the social male song (Paired T-test: P = 0.004, Fig 3a). When the three female groups were presented with the control/Aroclor 1242 male song-choice they showed no preference for either song type (Paired T-test: P>0.05, Fig 3b). Similarly, the three female groups also showed no preference when presented with the control/PCB 52 male song-choice (Paired T-test: P>0.05, Fig 3c).

thumbnail
Fig 3. F1 female song preference assay.

The average number of counts that a female made a choice between a) social/isolate male songs, b) control/Aroclor 1242 male songs, and c) control/PCB 52 male songs. Bars are mean±SE; significant differences are indicated with an asterisk (*).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230283.g003

Male histology and dendritic spine quantification

There were no differences between the three treatment groups in total brain mass immediately after perfusion or after the brain was immersed in Golgi-cox solution for one week (S5 Table). Each male in this experiment had two testes and there was no difference between the three treatment groups in testes mass (S5 Table).

There was also no difference between treatment groups in the number of dendritic spines in the proximal, medial, or distal portions of the dendrite in the hippocampus (Student’s T-test: t13 = 2.16, P>0.05; Fig 4a), HVC (Student’s T-test: t8 = 2.31, P>0.05; Fig 4b), or RA (Student’s T-test: t11 = 2.20, P>0.05; Fig 4c).

thumbnail
Fig 4. F1 male brain dendritic spine quantification.

The mean value of adult brain dendritic spines in the a) hippocampus, b) HVC, and c) RA of F1 male zebra finches. Error bars indicate ±SE.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230283.g004

Discussion

Our results show that developmental exposure to sublethal PCB levels can negatively affect adult zebra finch reproductive characteristics and behaviour.

Male song

Male zebra finches treated with PCBs in the nest sang less complex adult songs. Nestlings exposed to the pure PCB congener, PCB 52, had fewer syllables in their adult songs compared to control birds, while nestlings exposed to the PCB mixture, Aroclor 1242, did not differ significantly in any adult song characteristic we measured. Despite all subjects also being involved in the male behavioural assay, it is unlikely that this assay, per se, affected these results. Male zebra finches have crystallized adult song at the time of recording [42]. Although the environmental enrichment of the behavioural assay could have caused neural changes after the recordings [51], any morphological consequences to the brain should have equally affected all treatment groups, not changing the pattern of the results.

Our results of decreased song complexity in PCB-treated males suggest potential negative implications for wild birds. Adult female zebra finches prefer males who sing with more complex songs [52,53] and in general, mate choice studies in birds show that females of some species prefer males that sing more complex songs [54]. Accordingly, PCB exposure to nestlings may have a negative impact on their attractiveness to potential adult partners. Furthermore, in the field, male nestlings hatched in hotspots of PCB contamination may sing compromised adult songs, thereby affecting their reproductive fitness.

A limited number of studies show consequences of environmental chemical pollution on bird song, yet with divergent results. Carolina wrens (Thryothorus ludovicianus), house wrens (Troglodytes aedon), song sparrows (Melospiza melodia), and great tits (Parus major) living in areas of heavy metal pollution sing with less note diversity, fewer songs during the dawn chorus, and shorter songs compared to individuals living in less polluted environments [55,56]. Similarily, black-capped chickadees sing less stereotyped songs and song sparrows sing slower trills in areas of higher PCB pollution [32]. In contrast, European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) treated with environmental estrogens sing more complex songs and female starlings preferred those songs over songs from male controls [57].

Although we found changes in adult song complexity when nestlings were treated with PCB 52, we do not see significant changes in adult song with Aroclor 1242 treatment. The different consequences of Aroclor 1242 and PCB 52 exposure is likely an effect of their different chemistry, and an effect of a mixture versus a pure PCB congener. Whereas Aroclor 1242 has an average of three chlorine atoms per molecule [58], PCB 52 is a molecule with four chlorine atoms. We suspect that the exposure to the higher chlorinated PCB 52 and the higher amount of a single non-natural estrogenic congener [59] caused mixed agonist/antagonist effects and interfered with the normal masculinization of the male song during development [10,60,61]. In contrast, the mixture of many different PCB congeners in Aroclor 1242 (many with lower chlorination) may have reduced the potency of the Aroclor 1242 treatment overall, resulting in no discernable song change in the adults.

Male behavioural assay

Although the consequences of adult PCB-exposure on the reproductive behaviours of female zebra finches have been examined [27], to our knowledge this is the first time the effects of neonatal PCB-exposure on male birds has been the subject of experimental investigation. In our male behavioural assay, the PCB 52 treatment resulted in a shorter latency to successful clutches in comparison to the Aroclor 1242 treatment. In contrast, there were significantly more nests built before a successful nest in the Aroclor 1242 cages than in the PCB 52 cages, and females in the Aroclor 1242 cages laid significantly more eggs before a successful clutch in the Aroclor 1242 cages compared to the PCB 52 cages.

Although there were no statistical differences in observed aggressive behaviour, we suspect that aggression plays a significant role in these results. In two out of seven PCB 52 cage treatments the experiments were stopped in the pre-laying period due to heavy aggression towards the PCB 52 male from the control male, in contrast to other instances of aggression during the post-hatch period that required stopping the experiment (in the Aroclor 1242 treatment: the control male towards the Aroclor 1242 male; in the PCB 52 treatment: PCB 52 male towards the control male). While the delay to a successful first clutch in the Aroclor 1242 treatment initially appears to indicate the Aroclor 1242 cage treatment was, overall, more disruptive than the PCB 52 cage treatment, the aggressive results indicate a more complicated story. Instead it seems more likely that the highly aggressive atmosphere in the PCB 52 cages, causing the assay to be discontinued, resulted in in an overall decrease in the average observed aggression in the PCB 52 cage treatment overall, thereby facilitating a shorter latency to successful clutches. Therefore, the delay in the first successful clutch in the Aroclor 1242 cage treatment, an event important in reproductive success in a number of species (for examples see [62] and [63]), appears mainly to be a result of an absence of a dominant male in the behavioural assay.

PCB exposure has been linked to aggressive behaviour in a number of organisms. In mice, aggression was inversely proportional to PCB treatment dosage [64], while a domestic rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) breeding colony that was continuously exposed to PCBs exhibited notable trauma from other exposed members of the colony [65]. Aggressive courtship interactions were observed in captive American kestrels [24], and in humans, aggression (violence) has also been linked to early PCB exposure. Early PCB exposure in humans has been shown to lower IQ, shorten attention span, and increase the frequency of antisocial behaviour, making these individuals more likely to commit a violent crime [66,67]. Within this context it is not surprising that we observed higher levels of aggressive behaviour in the male behavioural assay.

The amount of song in the pre-laying and laying period also differed between treatments in the male bahvioural assay. During the pre-laying period control males sang more in the Aroclor 1242 cage treatment than the control males in the PCB 52 cage treatment. There was also more total singing in the Aroclor 1242 cage treatment during the laying period than the PCB 52 cage treatment. These song results further suggest a lack of dominant male and more ongoing male-male competition in the Aroclor 1242 treatment than in the PCB 52 treatment.

There was no evidence that the F1 developmental treatment affected the adult reproductive success. Moreover, because the females in the male behavioural assay were untreated and the paternity data indicated that each male F1 treatment group sired offspring, there is no indication that these PCB-treatments caused infertility. Zebra finches are a monogamous passerine [68], but are known to engage in extra pair copulations resulting in extra pair paternity in both the wild and in captivity [68,69]. Our male behavioural assay set-up put two males in direct competition with each other, likely allowing ample opportunity for frequent copulations and female guarding [70,71], and therefore explaining the low levels of mixed paternity observed in our results. However, F2 young fledged significantly earlier if they had a control genetic father than if they had an Aroclor 1242 genetic father, with F2 young with PCB 52 genetic fathers having an intermediated fledgling age. A delayed fledgling age would likely cause decreased survival in the wild, reducing reproductive success [72,73].

Disrupted reproductive behaviours due to PCB exposure have been seen in other bird species, such as the previously mentioned captive American kestrels. When exposed to a mixture of Aroclor 1248, 1254, and 1260 as adults, the effects were primarily skewed towards disrupted male kestrel behaviour and caused by a delay in clutch initiation [25]. Similarly our Aroclor 1242 treatment shows a delay of the first successful clutch in comparison to the PCB 52 treatment and a later fledgling age of the offspring with Aroclor 1242 fathers, potentially mirroring the mechanisms affected in the kestrels. In contrast, the PCB 52 treatment shows a high level of aggression early in the assay. Therefore, environmental PCB-exposure to male passerines during development may be impacting their reproductive success in the wild through consequences such as the inability to secure a mate or increased aggression. Furthermore, this study shows that sublethal neonatal exposure of male birds to PCBs may not only affect their reproductive behaviour, but also may impact the breeding success of their un-exposed, female reproductive partners.

Female song preference

As song is vital to male zebra finch reproductive behaviour, from the females’ perspective, her mate choice decision is crucial to her reproductive success and is a strong selective force [74]. Only F1 females treated with Aroclor 1242 responded significantly to the song preference trials (Fig 3a), while control and PCB 52 treated females did not show a significant response to the trials (Fig 3). These results are difficult to interpret since the small sample of stimulus songs in these trials hinders the interpretation of the ‘attractiveness’ of the male song [75]. However, these female song preference trials do not examine which type of male song elicits a preference, but instead explores whether the expression of a preference differs between female groups. One possible explanation for the significant response from the Aroclor 1242 treated females is that the Aroclor 1242 congener mixture acts as an estrogen mimic to the F1 female zebra finches. In female zebra finches estrogen has been show to selectively heighten song responsiveness [76]. Other passerines, such as female sedge warblers (Acrocephalus schoenobaenus), that have been implanted with estradiol have also been shown to respond more strongly to male playback [77]. Indeed, chemicals such as PCBs are capable of disrupting aspects of mate choice, such as significantly altering the preference for a sex-specific signal (reviewed in [78]). In contrast, we suspect that in the other treatment groups male song playbacks with a stuffed male model and without the accompaniment of other cues from a live male [79] were not enough to mimic the complexity of signals needed for a clear female preference.

Male dendritic spine quantification

In zebra finches song is learned and produced by a well-defined set of brain nuclei that are regulated, in part, by sex steroids [80,81]. The pathway of song production consists of the HVC (proper name), which projects to the robust nucleus of the arcopallium (RA), which projects to the hypoglossal (nXII) nucleus that controls the muscles of the syrinx used for vocalizations [82]. The HVC contains both androgen and estrogen receptors, while the RA contains only androgen receptors [80,8386].

Although we found differences in adult male song characteristics, these differences were not reflected by changes to song nuclei dendritic spine count. We found no difference in the number of adult dendritic spines in the HVC or RA in either PCB 52- or Aroclor 1242- treated zebra finch nestlings. Studies on exposure to environmental chemical pollution on song nuclei in the passerine brain show that environmental estrogen increases the HVC volume in European starlings [57], while American robins (Turdus migratorius) from areas of DDT (1, 1, 1-trichloro-2, 2-di (4-chlorophenyl) ethane) pollution have decreased HVC and RA volumes [87]. When treated with the PCB mixture Aroclor 1248, adult female zebra finches have progeny with decreased RA volume, although there was no change to their HVC volume [88].

While we cannot conclusively say why there were no differences in the number of dendritic spines of the adult birds even though there were changes to the adult song, one possible explanation is that the time lapse between the dosage and brain analysis in our study allowed for a ‘recovery’ of the dendritic spines in our adult subjects. In the only other study we are aware of where the same bird was dosed with a pollutant and then sacrificed for song nuclei measurements in a controlled experiement, European starlings dosed as adults were then promptly sacrificed for song nuclei volume measurements after the dosage regime was finished [57]. In our experiment the perfusion and brain extraction occurred substansially after the PCB dosage regime (Fig 1). Neurogenesis is known to occur in adult passerine birds and can be influenced by circulating testosterone, singing behaviour and stress (reviewed in [89]). It is likely that all these conditions varied between the time of treatment and perfusion in our study, potentially masking any measurable neurological changes to the song nuclei.

It is important to note that although we saw no change in the number of dendritic spines in the HVC or RA, we did not measure HVC or RA volume. Although song nuclei volume is a common measurement used to determine pollutant effects on song nuclei in birds [52,73,74], due to the long time between dosage and brain analysis in our study, we did not expect to find volume changes in the HVC and RA. In the only other study we are aware of that looks at the effects of PCB exposure on zebra finch song nuclei volumes, adult laying females were exposed to Aroclor 1248 and the RA volume of day 50 progeny decreased, while there was no change to the HVC volume [88]. Furthermore, the number of dendritic spines has been successfully used to quantify neurological changes in PCB-exposed organisms [90,91]. To our knowledge, this is the first time dendritic spines have been used to investigate the effects of PCB exposure to bird song nuclei.

Additionally, we saw no changes to the number of dendritic spines in the hippocampus between our three treatment groups. Although developmental treatment with endocrine disrupting chemicals, including PCBs, have been shown to decrease spinal density in the hippocampus and alter adult behaviour in rats and mice [9294], we saw no such results. Once again, it is possible that the time lag between the neonatal dosage and adult dendritic spine count allowed for neurogenesis to occur and masked any measurable changes to the hippocampus dendritic spines [95].

Conclusions

PCB complexity as a chemical class necessitates that specific congeners, as well as the timing and quantity of the exposure, must all be considered when investigating potential developmental PCB-effects. The various reproductive endpoints measured in this study highlight that pure PCB congeners and PCB mixtures can have distinct consequences on biological systems. Additionally, it is evident that sublethal PCB-exposure for only a limited time during development influences both male and female adult behaviour in ways that can have significant consequences to their adult reproductive success.

In particular, we emphasize that in our treatment of sublethal PCB-levels to nestling passerines, we saw significant changes in the adult male song and reproductive behaviour, not in morphological data, brain anatomy, or traditional measure of reproductive succes. These results highlight the importance of including behavioural endpoints when investigating the effects of environmental chemical pollutants [96].

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Top view of the female song preference trial apparatus.

Solid lines indicate wire aviary cage boundaries, dotted line indicates a central perch, and dashed lines with circle endings indicate movable wire-mesh gates. Stuffed male models (M) were placed directly behind and below speakers (S) at each end of the apparatus. The female was initially placed in the center of the apparatus (darker shaded area), and the time the female spent near each speaker (lighter shaded area) was recorded.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230283.s001

(TIF)

S1 Table. F1 male and female fledging age and size.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230283.s002

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Male behavioural assay subject size.

F1 male and female size at start of the male behavioural assay.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230283.s003

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Male aggression in behavioural assay.

Aggression results in the pre-laying and laying period of the male behavioural assay.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230283.s004

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Male reproductive behaviour in behavioural asssy.

Pre-laying, laying and post-hatch reproductive behavioural results of the male behavioural assay. Significant differences are indicated with an asterisk (*) and darker shaded cells.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230283.s005

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

Many thanks to undergraduates C. Acosta, C. Arellano, A. Fombona, C. Iriarte, and E. Liput for help with zebra finch monitoring, recording, and histological preparations. SD acknowledges S. Bogdanowicz and N. Baran for help with the genetic sexing and microsatellites, O. Menyhart for help with the song analysis, and T. Van Deusen for help with the practical challenges of this project. Thanks also to E. Adkins-Regan, J. Bradbury, W. Hochachka, S. A. Campbell and R. Halitschke for early advice on the experiment, and for helpful comments and discussion about the analysis and manuscript.

References

  1. 1. Bonefeld-Jørgensen EC, Andersen HR, Rasmussen TH, Vinggaard AM. Effect of highly bioaccumulated polychlorinated biphenyl congeners on estrogen and androgen receptor activity. Toxicol. 2001;158: 141–153.
  2. 2. Frame GM. A collaborative study of 209 PCB congeners and 6 Aroclors on 20 different HRGC columns 2. Semi-quantitative Aroclor congener distributions. Fresen. J. Anal. Chem. 1997;357: 714–722.
  3. 3. Hu D, Lehmler HJ, Martinez A, Wang K, Hornbuckle KC. Atmospheric PCB congeners across Chicago. Atmos. Environ. 2010;44: 1550–1557.
  4. 4. Frye C, Bo E, Calamandrei G, Calzà L, Dessì-Fulgheri F, Fernández M, et al. Endocrine disrupters: A review of some sources, effects, and mechanisms of actions on behaviour and neuroendocrine systems. J. Neuroendocrinol. 2012;24: 144–159. pmid:21951193
  5. 5. Van den Berg M, Birnbaum LS, Denison M, De Vito M, Farland W, Feeley M, et al. The 2005 World Health Organization reevaluation of human and mammalian toxic equivalency factors for dioxins and dioxin-like compounds. Toxicol. Sci. 2006;93: 223–241. pmid:16829543
  6. 6. Crisp TM, Clegg ED, Cooper RL, Wood WP, Anderson DG, Baetcke KP, et al. Environmental endocrine disruption: An effects assessment and analysis. Environ. Health Perspect. 1998;106: 11–56. pmid:9539004
  7. 7. Tyler CR, Jobling S, Sumpter JP. Endocrine disruption in wildlife: A critical review of the evidence. Crit. Rev. Toxicol. 1998;28: 319–361. pmid:9711432
  8. 8. Bell MR. Endocrine-disrupting actions of PCBs on brain development and social and reproductive behaviors. Curr. Opin. Pharmacol. 2014;19: 134–144. pmid:25310366
  9. 9. Blocker TD, Ophir AG. Cryptic confounding compounds: A brief consideration of the influences of anthropogenic contaminants on courtship and mating behavior. Acta Ethol. 2013;16: 105–125.
  10. 10. Dickerson SM, Gore AC. Estrogenic environmental endocrine-disrupting chemical effects on reproductive neuroendocrine function and dysfunction across the life cycle. Rev. Endocr. Metab. Disord. 2007;8: 143–159. pmid:17674209
  11. 11. McCoy G, Finlay MF, Rhone A, James K, Cobb GP. Chronic polychlorinated biphenyls exposure on three generations of oldfield mice (Peromyscus polionotus): Effects on reproduction, growth, and body residues. Arch. Environ. Con. Tox. 1995;28(4): 431–435.
  12. 12. Tryphonas H, Luster MI, Schiffman G, Dawson L-L, Hodgen M, Germolec D, et al. Effect of chronic exposure of PCB (Aroclor 1254) on specific and nonspecific immune parameters in the rhesus (Macaca mulatta) monkey. Toxicol Sci. 1991;16(4): 773–786.
  13. 13. Eckbo N, Le Bohec C, Planas-Bielsa V, Warner NA, Schull Q, Herzke D, et al. Individual variability in contaminants and physiological status in a resident Arctic seabird species. Environ. Pollut. 2019;249: 191–199. pmid:30889502
  14. 14. Holladay SD, Smialowicz RJ. Development of the murine and human immune system: Differential effects of immunotoxicants depend on time of exposure. Environ. Health Perspect. 2000;108(3): 463–73.
  15. 15. Colborn T, Saal FSV, Soto AM. Developmental effects of endocrine-disrupting chemicals in wildlife and humans. Environ. Health Perspect. 1993;101: 378–384. pmid:8080506
  16. 16. MacDougall-Shackleton SA, Spencer KA. Developmental stress and birdsong: Current evidence and future directions. J. Ornithol. 2012;153(1): 105–117.
  17. 17. Peters S, Searcy WA, Nowicki S. Developmental stress, song-learning, and cognition. Integr. Comp. Biol. 2014;54(4): 555–567. pmid:24760795
  18. 18. Nowicki S, Searcy W, Peters S. Brain development, song learning and mate choice in birds: A review and experimental test of the "nutritional stress hypothesis". J. Comp. Physiol. A. 2002;188(11–12):1003–1014.
  19. 19. Gillette R, Reilly MP, Topper VY, Thompson LM, Crews D, Gore AC. Anxiety-like behaviors in adulthood are altered in male but not female rats exposed to low dosages of polychlorinated biphenyls in utero. Horm. Behav. 2017;87: 8–15. pmid:27794483
  20. 20. Helberg M, Bustnes JO, Erikstad KE, Kristiansen KO, Skaare JU. Relationships between reproductive performance and organochlorine contaminants in great black-backed gulls (Larus marinus). Environ. Pollut. 2005;134: 475–483. pmid:15620593
  21. 21. McCarty JP, Secord AL. Nest-building behavior in PCB-contaminated tree swallows. Auk. 1999;116: 55–63.
  22. 22. McCarty JP, Secord AL. Reproductive ecology of tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) with high levels of polychlorinated biphenyl contamination. Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 1999;18: 1433–1439.
  23. 23. McCarty JP, Secord AL. Possible effects of PCB contamination on female plumage color and reproductive success in Hudson River tree swallows. Auk. 2000;117: 987–995.
  24. 24. Fernie K, Bortolotti G, Smits J. Reproductive abnormalities, teratogenicity, and developmental problems in American kestrels (Falco sparverius) exposed to polychlorinated biphenyls. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health A. 2003;66: 2089–2103. pmid:14710594
  25. 25. Fisher SA, Bortolotti GR, Fernie KJ, Bird DM, Smits JE. Behavioral variation and its consequences during incubation for American kestrels exposed to polychlorinated biphenyls. Ecotoxicol. Environ. Saf. 2006;63: 226–235. pmid:16169079
  26. 26. Fisher SA, Bortolotti GR, Fernie KJ, Smits JE, Marchant TA, Drouillard KG, et al. Courtship behavior of captive American kestrels (Falco sparverius) exposed to polychlorinated biphenyls. Arch. Environ. Contam. Toxicol. 2001;41: 215–220. pmid:11462146
  27. 27. Hoogesteijn AL, DeVoogd TJ, Quimby FW, De Caprio T, Kollias GV. Reproductive impairment in zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata). Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 2005;24: 219–223. pmid:15683187
  28. 28. Dauwe T, Chu SG, Covaci A, Schepens P, Eens M. Great tit (Parus major) nestlings as biomonitors of organochlorine pollution. Arch. Environ. Contam. Toxicol. 2003;44: 89–96. pmid:12434223
  29. 29. Maul JD, Belden JB, Schwab BA, Whiles MR, Spears B, Farris JL, et al. Bioaccumulation and trophic transfer of polychlorinated biphenyls by aquatic and terrestrial insects to tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor). Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 2006;25: 1017–1025. pmid:16629140
  30. 30. Neigh AM, Zwiernik MJ, Bradley PW, Kay DP, Jones PD, Holem RR, et al. Accumulation of polychlorinated biphenyls from floodplain soils by passerine birds. Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 2006;25: 1503–1511. pmid:16764468
  31. 31. Clarke AL, Sæther BE, Røskaft E. Sex biases in avian dispersal: A reappraisal. Oikos. 1997;79: 429–438.
  32. 32. DeLeon S, Halitschke R, Hames RS, Kessler A, DeVoogd TJ, Dhondt AA. The effect of polychlorinated biphenyls on the song of two passerine species. PLoS ONE. 2013;8: e73471. pmid:24058475
  33. 33. Kahn NW, Quinn TW. Chromosome-specific intron size differences in the avian CHD gene provide an efficient method for sex identification in birds. Auk. 1998;115: 1074–1078.
  34. 34. Soderstrom K, Qin W, Leggett MH. A minimally invasive procedure for sexing young zebra finches. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2007;164: 116–119. pmid:17532050
  35. 35. Millam JR, Craig-Veit CB, Quaglino AE, Erichsen AL, Famula TR, Fry DM. Posthatch oral estrogen exposure impairs adult reproductive performance of zebra finch in a sex-specific manner. Horm. Behav. 2001;40: 542–549. pmid:11716584
  36. 36. Adkins-Regan E, Mansukhani V, Seiwert C, Thompson R. Sexual differentiation of brain and behavior in the zebra finch: Critical periods for effects of early estrogen treatment. J. Neurobiol. 1994;25: 865–877. pmid:8089662
  37. 37. Echols KR, Tillitt DE, Nichols JW, Secord AL, McCarty JP. Accumulation of PCB congeners in nestling tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) on the Hudson River, New York. Environ. Sci. Technol. 2004;38: 6240–6246. pmid:15597877
  38. 38. Hoogesteijn AL. Neurological development of the song control circuit in zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata): A new model for the evaluation of endocrine disruption by polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs). Ph.D. Thesis, Cornell University. 2003.
  39. 39. McCarty JP. The number of visits to the nest by parents Is an accurate measure of food delivered to nestlings in tree swallows. J. Field Ornithol. 2002;73: 9–14.
  40. 40. Secord AL, McCarty JP, Echols KR, Meadows JC, Gale RW, Tillitt DE. Polychlorinated biphenyls and 2,3,7,8-tetrachlorodibenzo-p-dioxin equivalents in tree swallows from the upper Hudson River, New York State, USA. Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 1999;18: 2519–2525.
  41. 41. Eales LA. Song learning in zebra finches: Some effects of song model availability on what is learnt and when. Animal Behav. 1985;33(4): 1293–1300.
  42. 42. Böhner J. Song learning in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata): Selectivity in the choice of a tutor and accuracy of song copies. Animal Behav. 1983;31: 231–237.
  43. 43. Airey DC, DeVoogd TJ. Greater song complexity is associated with augmented song system anatomy in zebra finches. Neuroreport. 2000;11: 2339–2344. pmid:10923697
  44. 44. Spencer KA, Buchanan KL, Goldsmith AR, Catchpole CK. Song as an honest signal of developmental stress in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). Horm. Behav. 2003;44: 132–139. pmid:13129485
  45. 45. Burley N, Krantzberg G, Radman P. Influence of colour-banding on the conspecific preferences of zebra finches. Animal Behav. 1982;30(2): 444–455.
  46. 46. Lauay C, Gerlach NM, Adkins-Regan E, DeVoogd TJ. Female zebra finches require early song exposure to prefer high-quality song as adults. Animal Behav. 2004;68: 1249–1255.
  47. 47. Glaser EM, Van der Loos H. Analysis of thick brain sections by obverse-Reverse computer microscopy: Application of a new, high clarity Golgi-Nissl stain. J. Neurosci. Methods. 1981;4: 117–125. pmid:6168870
  48. 48. Hill KM, DeVoogd TJ. Altered daylength affects dendritic structure in a song-related brain region in red-winged blackbirds. Behav. Neural. Biol. 1991;56: 240–250. pmid:1759944
  49. 49. Nixdorf-Bergweiler BE, Wallhäusser-Franke E, Devoogd TJ. Regressive development in neuronal structure during song learning in birds. J. Neurobiol. 1995;27: 204–215. pmid:7658201
  50. 50. Lauay C, Komorowski RW, Beaudin AE, DeVoogd TJ. Adult female and male zebra finches show distinct patterns of spine deficits in an auditory area and in the song system when reared without exposure to normal adult song. J. Comp. Neurol. 2005;487: 119–126. pmid:15880474
  51. 51. van Praag H, Kempermann G, Gage FH. Neural consequences of enviromental enrichment. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2000;1: 191–198. pmid:11257907
  52. 52. Clayton N, Prove E. Song discrimination in female zebra finches and bengalese finches. Animal Behav. 1989;38: 352–354.
  53. 53. Spencer KA, Wimpenny JH, Buchanan KL, Lovell PG, Goldsmith AR, Catchpole CK. Developmental stress affects the attractiveness of male song and female choice in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2005;58: 423–428.
  54. 54. Nowicki S, Searcy WA. Song function and the evolution of female preferences: Why birds sing, why brains matter. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2004;1016: 704–723. pmid:15313801
  55. 55. Gorissen L, Snoeijs T, Duyse EV, Eens M. Heavy metal pollution affects dawn singing behaviour in a small passerine bird. Oecologia. 2005;145: 504–509. pmid:16133189
  56. 56. Hallinger KK, Zabransky DJ, Kazmer KA, Cristol DA. Birdsong differs between mercury-polluted and reference sites. Auk. 2010;127: 156–161.
  57. 57. Markman S, Leitner S, Catchpole C, Barnsley S, Müller CT, Pascoe D, et al. Pollutants increase song complexity and the volume of the brain area HVC in a songbird. PLoS ONE. 2008;3: e1674. pmid:18301751
  58. 58. Erickson M, Kaley R II. Applications of polychlorinated biphenyls. Environ. Sci. Pollut. R. 2011;18: 135–151.
  59. 59. Jansen HT, Cooke PS, Porcelli J, Liu TC, Hansen LG. Estrogenic and antiestrogenic actions of PCBs in the female rat: In vitro and in vivo studies. Reproductive Toxicol. 1993;7: 237–248.
  60. 60. León-Olea M, Martyniuk CJ, Orlando EF, Ottinger MA, Rosenfeld CS, Wolstenholme JT, et al. Current concepts in neuroendocrine disruption. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 2014;203: 158–173. pmid:24530523
  61. 61. Waye A, Trudeau VL. Neuroendocrine disruption: More than hormones are upset. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health B. 2011;14: 270–291.
  62. 62. Ingold DJ. Delayed nesting decreases reproductive success in northern flickers: Implications for competition with european starlings. J. Field Ornithol. 1996;67: 321–326.
  63. 63. Siikamäki P. Limitation of reproductive success by food availability and breeding time in pied flycatchers. Ecology. 1998;79: 1789–1796.
  64. 64. Fanini D, Palumbo G, Giorgi R, Pantaleoni G. Behavioral effects of PCBs in mice. Behav Pharmacol. 1990;1(6): 505–510. pmid:11175436
  65. 65. Altman N, New A, McConnell E, Ferrell T. A spontaneous outbreak of polychlorinated biphenyl (PCB) toxicity in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta): Clinical observations. Lab. Anim. Sci. 1979;29(5): 661–665. pmid:117226
  66. 66. Carpenter DO, Nevin R. Environmental causes of violence. Physiol. Behav. 2010;99(2): 260–268. pmid:19758571
  67. 67. Hwang L. Environmental stressors and violence: Lead and polychlorinated biphenyls. Rev. Environ. Health. 2011;22(4): 313–328.
  68. 68. Birkhead TR, Pellatt J, Hunter F. Extra-pair copulation and sperm competition in the zebra finch. Nature. 1988;334(6177): 60–62. pmid:3386741
  69. 69. Birkhead T, Clarkson K, Zann R. Extra-pair courtship, copulation and mate guarding in wild zebra finches Taeniopygia guttata. Animal Behav. 1988;36(6): 1853–1855.
  70. 70. Birkhead T, Hunter F, Pellatt J. Sperm competition in the zebra finch, Taeniopygia guttata. Animal Behav. 1989;38(6): 935–950.
  71. 71. Birkhead T, Burke T, Zann R, Hunter F, Krupa A. Extra-pair paternity and intraspecific brood parasitism in wild zebra finches Taeniopygia guttata, revealed by DNA fingerprinting. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1990;27(5): 315–324.
  72. 72. Zann RA. Reproduction in a zebra finch colony in south-eastern Australia: The significance of monogamy, precocial breeding and multiple broods in a highly mobile species. Emu. 1994;94(4): 285–299.
  73. 73. Zann RA. The zebra finch: A synthesis of field and laboratory studies. New York: Oxford University Press; 1996.
  74. 74. Andersson MB. Sexual selection. New Jersey: Princeton University Press; 1994.
  75. 75. Kroodsma DE, Byers BE, Goodale E, Johnson S, Liu W-C. Pseudoreplication in playback experiments, revisited a decade later. Animal Behav. 2001;61: 1029–1033.
  76. 76. Vyas A, Harding C, Borg L, Bogdan D. Acoustic characteristics, early experience, and endocrine status interact to modulate female zebra finches’ behavioral responses to songs. Horm. Behav. 2009;55: 50–59. pmid:18804474
  77. 77. Catchpole CK, Dittami J, Leisler B. Differential responses to male song repertoires in female songbirds implanted with oestradiol. Nature. 1984;312: 563–564.
  78. 78. Gore AC, Holley AM, Crews D. Mate choice, sexual selection, and endocrine-disrupting chemicals. Horm. Behav. 2018;101: 3–12. pmid:28888817
  79. 79. Collins SA, Hubbard C, Houtman AM. Female mate choice in the zebra finch-The effect of male beak colour and male song. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1994;35: 21–25.
  80. 80. Ball GF, Riters LV, Balthazart J. Neuroendocrinology of song behavior and avian brain plasticity: Multiple sites of action of sex steroid hormones. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 2002;23: 137–178. pmid:11950243
  81. 81. Chen Z, Ye R, Goldman SA. Testosterone modulation of angiogenesis and neurogenesis in the adult songbird brain. Neuroscience. 2013;239: 139–148. pmid:23291451
  82. 82. DeVoogd TJ. Neural constraints on the complexity of avian song. Brain Behav. Evol. 2004;63: 221–232. pmid:15084815
  83. 83. Alward BA, Balthazart J, Ball GF. Differential effects of global versus local testosterone on singing behavior and its underlying neural substrate. PNAS. 2013;110: 19573–19578. pmid:24218603
  84. 84. Bernard DJ. Androgen receptor, estrogen receptor α, and estrogen receptor β show distinct patterns of expression in forebrain song control nuclei of European starlings. Endocrinology. 1999;140: 4633–4643. pmid:10499520
  85. 85. DeVoogd TJ, Lauay C. Emerging psychobiology of the avian song system. In: Blass EM, editor. Developmental Psychobiology. Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology. Boston: Springer; 2001. vol. 13.
  86. 86. Fusani L, Gahr M. Hormonal influence on song structure and organization: The role of estrogen. Neuroscience. 2006;138: 939–946. pmid:16310314
  87. 87. Iwaniuk AN, Koperski DT, Cheng KM, Elliott JE, Smith LK, Wilson LK, et al. The effects of environmental exposure to DDT on the brain of a songbird: Changes in structures associated with mating and song. Behav. Brain Res. 2006;173: 1–10. pmid:16828177
  88. 88. Hoogesteijn AL, Kollias GV, Quimby FW, De Caprio AP, Winkler DW, DeVoogd TJ. Development of a brain nucleus involved in song production in zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) is disrupted by Aroclor 1248. Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 2008;27: 2071–2075. pmid:18380522
  89. 89. Balthazart J, Ball GF. Endocrine and social regulation of adult neurogenesis in songbirds. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 2016;41: 3–22. pmid:26996818
  90. 90. Lein PJ, Yang D, Bachstetter AD, Tilson HA, Harry GJ, Mervis RF, et al. Ontogenetic alterations in molecular and structural correlates of dendritic growth after developmental exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls. Environ. Health Perspect. 2007;115(4): 556–563. pmid:17450224
  91. 91. Wayman GA, Yang D, Bose DD, Lesiak A, Ledoux V, Bruun D, et al. PCB-95 promotes dendritic growth via ryanodine receptor-dependent mechanisms. Environ. Health Perspect. 2012;120(7): 997–1002. pmid:22534141
  92. 92. Bowman RE, Luine V, Weinstein SD, Khandaker H, DeWolf S, Frankfurt M. Bisphenol-A exposure during adolescence leads to enduring alterations in cognition and dendritic spine density in adult male and female rats. Horm. Behav. 2015;69: 89–97. pmid:25554518
  93. 93. Fang Z, Zhu Q, Gu T, Shen X, Yang Y, Liang Y, et al. Anti-androgenic effects of bisphenol-A on spatial memory and synaptic plasticity of the hippocampus in mice. Horm. Behav. 2017;93: 151–158. pmid:28576649
  94. 94. Schantz S, Seo B, Wong P, Pessah IN. Long-term effects of developmental exposure to 2, 2’, 3, 5’, 6-pentachlorobiphenyl (PCB 95) on locomotor activity, spatial learning and memory and brain ryanodine binding. Neurotoxicology. 1997;18(2): 457–467. pmid:9291494
  95. 95. Sherry DF, MacDougall-Shackleton SA. Seasonal change in the avian hippocampus. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 2015;37: 158–167. pmid:25497862
  96. 96. Zala SM, Penn DJ. Abnormal behaviours induced by chemical pollution: A review of the evidence and new challenges. Animal Behav. 2004;68(4): 649–664.