Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Serotonin improves behavioral contrast sensitivity of freely moving rats

  • Akinori Y. Sato,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Laboratory of Brain Information Science in Sports, Graduate School of Frontier Biosciences, Osaka University, Toyonaka, Osaka, Japan

  • Keisuke Tsunoda,

    Roles Methodology, Software

    Affiliation Laboratory of Brain Information Science in Sports, Graduate School of Frontier Biosciences, Osaka University, Toyonaka, Osaka, Japan

  • Ryo Mizuyama,

    Roles Methodology, Software

    Affiliation Laboratory of Brain Information Science in Sports, Graduate School of Frontier Biosciences, Osaka University, Toyonaka, Osaka, Japan

  • Satoshi Shimegi

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    shimegi@celas.osaka-u.ac.jp

    Affiliations Laboratory of Brain Information Science in Sports, Graduate School of Frontier Biosciences, Osaka University, Toyonaka, Osaka, Japan, Laboratory of Brain Information Science in Sports, Center for Education in Liberal Arts and Sciences, Osaka University, Toyonaka, Osaka, Japan, Laboratory of Brain Information Science in Sports, Graduate School of Medicine, Osaka University, Toyonaka, Osaka, Japan

Abstract

Serotonin (5-HT) is a neuromodulator secreted from serotonergic neurons located in the pons and upper brain stem in a behavioral context-dependent manner. The serotonergic axon terminals innervate almost the whole brain, causing modulatory actions on various brain functions including vision. Our previous study demonstrated the visual responses of neurons in the primary visual cortex (V1) of anesthetized monkeys were modulated by the activation of 5-HT receptors depending on the response magnitude, in which 5-HT2A receptor-selective agonists enhanced weak visual responses but not strong responses. This observation suggests that the activation of serotonergic receptors modulates neuronal visual information processing to improve the behavioral detectability of a stimulus. However, it remains unknown if 5-HT improves visual detectability at the behavioral level. To investigate this point, visual detectability was measured as contrast sensitivity (CS) in freely moving rats using a two-alternative forced-choice visual detection task (2AFC-VDT) combined with the staircase method. The grating contrast was decreased or increased step by step after a correct choice (hit) or incorrect choice (miss), respectively. CS was evaluated as an inverse value of the visual contrast threshold. The effect of the intraperitoneal administration of fluoxetine (FLX, 5 mg/kg), a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor, on CS was tested. The CS of rats was significantly higher in FLX than control conditions, and the drug effect showed specificity for the spatial frequency (SF) of a grating stimulus, in which CS improvement was observed at optimal SF but not non-optimal high SF. Thus, we conclude that endogenously-secreted serotonin in the brain improves visual detectability, which may be mediated by vision-related neurons acquiring SF information of the visual stimulus.

Introduction

Information processing in the brain dynamically changes depending on the arousal state and behavioral context [1,2]. The dynamics are mediated by neuromodulators, including 5-hydroxytryptamine (5-HT, or serotonin) depending on behavioral contexts.

Serotonin is secreted from afferent terminals originating from neurons in the dorsal and median raphe nuclei, which act on subcortical and cortical areas [3,4] to regulate various brain functions [57]. For example, serotonergic neurons regularly fire at low frequencies (0.2–4 Hz) during awakening [8,9] to maintain the arousal state by acting on the hypothalamus [10,11] and cerebral cortex [12,13]. Serotonergic neurons project to the cerebral cortex including visual areas [1416] and are known to modulate neuronal visual responses via various kinds of 5-HT receptor subtypes [1720]. For example, Waterhouse et al. (1990) reported that iontophoretically-administered serotonin decreases the signal-to-noise ratio of the neuronal activities in the primary visual cortex (V1) of anesthetized rats [17]. On the other hand, serotonin receptor-selective agonists have bi-directional effects of facilitation and inhibition on the visual response of monkey V1 neurons depending on the response magnitude, an effect considered to optimize visual information processing [18,20].

However, since all previous studies on serotonergic visual modulation have focused on neuronal activities, it still remains unclear whether and how endogenous serotonin modulates visual perception. To examine this point, here we measured contrast sensitivity (CS) as an index of behavioral visual detectability of freely moving rats using a two-alternative forced choice-visual detection task (2AFC-VDT) combined with the staircase method, and tested the effect of fluoxetine (FLX), a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor, on CS.

Materials and methods

Ethical approval

All experimental protocols were approved by the Research Ethics Committee of Osaka University (Permit Number: 28-074-000). All procedures were carried out in compliance with the policies and regulations of the guidelines approved by the Animal Care Committee of the Osaka University Medical School and National Institutes of Health guidelines for the care of experimental animals.

Animals

Long-Evans rats (male, 200–350 g; Japan SLC Inc., Shizuoka, Japan) were used in this study. The rats were kept on a 12/12-hour light-dark cycle (lights on at 6:00 AM), and all experiments including the 2AFC-VDT and training to learn the task were performed only during the light period. Each week, five days and the remaining two days were assigned to the training/test period and non-task period, respectively. Rats obtained water only by performing the 2AFC-VDT during the training/test period, but were able to access water without any restriction during the non-task period. Signs of possible dehydration were monitored (reduced skin tension, sunken eyes, etc.), but none were observed in this study.

2AFC-VDT

An experimental box was used to measure CS. The box was partitioned into three areas by translucent walls, and a liquid crystal display monitor (mean luminance: 30 cd/m2, maximum luminance: 60 cd/m2, refresh rate: 60 Hz, screen resolution: 1920×1080 pixels, screen size: 50.9×28.6 cm) was attached to the front side (Fig 1A and 1B). Three task-related levers were equipped at the center of each area. The trial was initiated by the rat pulling the central lever. In response, a visual stimulus was presented on either the left or right side of the monitor. The stimulus was a circular patch of drifting sinusoidal grating with horizontal orientation, temporal frequency (TF) 4 Hz, and diameter 20 degrees in visual angle. When the rat pulled the lever on the side where the visual stimulus was presented (correct choice), 4–5 μL of water was given from the tip of the lever as a reward. On the other hand, when the rat pulled the opposite lever (incorrect choice), no reward and an audible sound (200–500 Hz) as an error signal were given. The visual stimulus presentation continued until the rat pulled the choice lever, and the stimulus location was pseudo-randomly changed from trial to trial. The rat’s behavior during the task was monitored and recorded by a web camera. Details of the training protocol of the 2AFC-VDT have been described in our previous study [21]. All systems were controlled by programs written in MATLAB (Mathworks, Natick, MA) with extensions from the Psychophysics Toolbox [22,23].

thumbnail
Fig 1. Schema of the CS measurement using 2AFC-VDT combined with the staircase method.

(A) Schema and (B) photo of the experimental box. (C) Schema of the 2AFC-VDT. A rat pulls up the central lever to begin the task, by which a grating patch is presented on the right or left side of the display. The rat is rewarded with water by pulling the lever corresponding to the stimulus location. (D) The contrast changes in one session if the rat pulls the correct lever in all trials. (E) Typical result from one session in the 2AFC-VDT combined with the staircase method. In this session, the rat was able to detect 27.3% contrast (horizontal line, Cthreshold = 27.3%), therefore CS was assessed as 3.66.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230367.g001

Animal age at the beginning of the task training ranged from 70 to 100 days old, and the task training was mostly completed within 2 weeks. Experiments for the CS measurements were conducted over the subsequent 2–3 months.

CS measurements

To measure CS, 2AFC-VDT combined with the staircase method was conducted [2427]. In the staircase method, the contrast of the grating stimulus was changed according to the rat’s choice in the prior trial. Upon pulling the correct choice-lever (hit) or incorrect one (miss), the grating contrast was decreased or increased, respectively. In one session of the CS measurement, the contrast changed at increments of 10%, 4%, 1%, or 0.2% for high (50–100%), middle (10–50%), low (4–10%), or very low (1–4%) contrast ranges, respectively (Fig 1D). The session was finished when the average hit rate in the last 10 trials reached 50% (chance level) (Fig 1E), and the average stimulus contrast in the last 3 trials of the session was regarded as the contrast threshold (Cthreshold) of the rat. CS was calculated as CS = 100/Cthreshold [28].

One session was repeated for 1 hour, and the geometric average of the CS obtained from all sessions was determined as the CS of the rat. CS depends on the spatial frequency (SF) of the stimulus, with the highest CS occurring at optimal SF in rats as well as in humans [24]. Therefore, there is a possibility that the effect of serotonin on CS is SF-dependent. To confirm the possibility, we conducted CS measurements at optimal SF (0.1 cycle/degree (cpd)) and non-optimal high SF (0.5 cpd).

Open field test

The open field test was used to evaluate the locomotor activity of freely moving rats. In this test, a 60-cm diameter circular container was used as the open field. The movement of the rat was monitored by a web camera from above for 5 min. The total moving distance in the 5 min was calculated by using a custom-made tracking system and evaluated as the locomotor activity.

Water intake measurement

To examine the motivation for drinking water, water intake was measured. Rats were put in their home cage and allowed to access the water bottle for 5 min. The weight of the bottle was measured before and after the 5 min period, and the difference was evaluated as the water intake of the rat.

Drugs

Fluoxetine hydrochloride (FLX, a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor dissolved in 0.9% saline; 5 mg/kg i.p.; Sigma Aldrich, MO) was injected intraperitoneally 30 min before the 2AFC-VDT, open field test and water intake measurement. The dose of the drug was determined on the basis of a previous study [29].

Statistical analyses

The normally distributed data obtained from the 2AFC-VDT were analyzed using a two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) for repeated measures. The factors were drug (control, FLX) and SF (0.1, 0.5 cpd). When the ANOVA showed a significant interaction between drug and SF, post hoc analysis using the Holm–Bonferroni method for multiple comparisons was performed. Statistical analyses of other data obtained from the open field test and water intake measurement were carried out using the paired t-test. These statistic tests were performed using MATLAB and R version 3.5.2 (The R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria). Significance criterion (alpha) was 0.05 (two- tailed). Data in the text and figures are mean ± SEM.

Results

To investigate the modulatory effects of serotonin on behavioral visual function, we measured CS in freely moving rats using the 2AFC-VDT and tested the effects of intraperitoneally- administered FLX.

Fig 2 shows typical results of the CS measurement. In one session, as the trial number progressed, the stimulus contrast decreased step-by-step and reached a certain level (Cthreshold) for any SF or drug condition (Fig 2A and 2B). However, the drug effect on Cthreshold differed between the two SF conditions (0.1 and 0.5 cpd). Under optimal SF condition (0.1 cpd), FLX decreased Cthreshold from 12.5% to 7.0% and increased CS from 8.0 to 14.4 (Fig 2A). On the other hand, the CS did not differ between the two drug conditions at non-optimal high SF (0.5 cpd), in which the Cthreshold values were 40.6% and 41.9% for control and FLX conditions, respectively, and the corresponding CS values were 2.5 and 2.4 (Fig 2B). The averaged results over all sessions of the same rat showed similar results (Fig 2C and 2D). The contrast values of the stimulus for all 60 min sessions were averaged over all trial numbers, and the averaged data were plotted as average transition curves (Fig 2C and 2D). Again, FLX lowered Cthreshold at optimal SF (control: 11.5%, FLX: 6.3%), but not at non-optimal high SF (control: 44.2%, FLX: 38%). CS at optimal and non-optimal high SF were 8.7 and 2.3, respectively, for the control condition, and 16.0 and 2.6 for the FLX condition.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Effects of FLX on CS in the 2AFC-VDT combined with the staircase method.

(A)–(B) Typical data from one session of the 2AFC-VDT at two different SFs: 0.1 cpd (A) and 0.5 cpd (B) for one rat (black circles, control; white squares, FLX). Solid and dotted horizontal lines indicate the Cthreshold of the control and FLX conditions, respectively. (C)–(D) Average contrast changes over all sessions for the same rat as (A)–(B). Black circles and white squares indicate the mean contrast value calculated for each trial number. Horizontal lines indicate the geometric mean of Cthreshold (solid line, control; dotted line, FLX). Error bars are SEM.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230367.g002

The results of the population analysis of the CS for all rats (n = 7) reveal the effects of FLX on CS are SF-dependent (Fig 3). A two-way ANOVA for repeated measures was used for the data analysis, and statistical significance was observed for the treatments (F(1, 24) = 14.13, p = 0.004), SFs (F(1, 24) = 20.18, p = 0.009) and the interaction (F(1, 24) = 6.439, p = 0.044). The post hoc analysis using the Holm–Bonferroni’s multiple comparison test showed that FLX improved CS significantly only at optimal SF (SF 0.1 cpd, p = 0.022), but not at non-optimal high SF (SF 0.5 cpd, p = 1). These results suggest that increasing the serotonin concentration in the brain improves behavioral sensitivity to stimulus contrast in an SF-dependent manner.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Population data of FLX effects on CS.

The population data of CS (n = 7) obtained from control and FLX conditions (black bar, control; white bar, FLX). CS was significantly increased by FLX at 0.1 cpd (*p = 0.022, Holm–Bonferroni’s multiple comparison test), but not at 0.5 cpd. Error bars are SEM. The vertical axis is shown in logarithmic scale.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230367.g003

Serotonin in the brain elevates the motivation for drinking water and changes the activity level of the animal [30,31]. Changes in drinking motivation and activity level could affect performance in the 2AFC-VDT to improve CS. To investigate this point, water intake and activity level for 5 min were measured after the administration of saline or FLX (Fig 4), but we found no difference between the two conditions (water intake: control, 5.1 ± 0.7 g, FLX, 4.5 ± 0.7 g, p = 0.369; average moving distance: control, 24.0 ± 1.25 m, FLX, 22.3 ± 1.75 m, p = 0.570, paired t-test).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Effect of FLX on water intake and activity level.

(A) Water intake for 5 min under control and FLX conditions (black bar, control; white bar, FLX). No significant difference was observed (p = 0.369, paired t-test). (B) Total moving distance of rats in an open field arena for 5 min was also unaffected by FLX administration (p = 0.570, paired t-test).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230367.g004

Discussion

In the present study, we examined the effects of FLX, a serotonin-selective reuptake inhibitor, on the CS of freely moving rats, finding that CS was enhanced only at optimal SF. This finding suggests that increasing serotonin in the brain improves behavioral visual detectability in an SF-dependent manner.

The SF dependency of the FLX effect suggests that the candidate target area of FLX is the visual areas, which represent the SF of a visual stimulus, for example, area V1 [32]. Our previous neuropharmacological studies [18,33] demonstrated that serotonin affects the contrast-response function of neurons in monkey V1, where the 5-HT receptor subtypes, 1B and 2A are present exclusively among neocortical areas. Interestingly, the iontophoretic administration of DOI, a 2A receptor agonist, directly to the recorded neuron caused bi-directional modulation on the contrast-response function, in which weak responses at low contrast were enhanced but strong responses at high contrast were suppressed [18,33]. DOI’s effect at low contrast is consistent with our finding that FLX enhances CS, that is, reduces the Cthreshold at which rats become unable to detect a grating stimulus. The activation of 5-HT 2A receptor has been reported to cause bi-directional modulation in rat V1 neurons depending on the magnitude of the visual responses, in which weak visual responses were facilitated but strong ones suppressed [20]. Thus, 5-HT 2A receptors seem to play an important role in the enhancement of weak visual signals, potentially improving the behavioral CS observed in the present study. On the other hand, 1B receptor is reported to be located on thalamocortical axons projecting to area V1 of rats [34], and its activation reduces spontaneous activity, i.e., noise [33], which would enhance the signal-to-noise ratio responsible for behavioral CS. Further study is required to elucidate whether and how 5-HT receptors in area V1 contribute to CS improvement.

Serotonin is released from serotonergic neurons in the dorsal and median raphe nuclei. The neurons of both regions project their axons to the occipital cortex, but density of the projection is histologically known to be higher in dorsal raphe nucleus than median raphe nucleus [3537]. Therefore, comparing to the median raphe nucleus, the dorsal raphe nucleus seems to contribute more to the modulatory effect of serotonin in the visual cortex.

In the present study, FLX was intraperitoneally administered to minimize invasiveness, but this strategy has the disadvantage of wide-ranging effects across the brain areas where serotonergic neurons project to. To determine if brain areas other than visual areas and peripheral nervous system were influenced by this administration, we examined water intake and activity levels. Serotonin has been known to induce intake behavior toward food and water via the accumbens nucleus [31]. However, we found FLX did not change water consumption volume. Additionally, we found FLX did not change activity level.

In addition, it has been reported that FLX not only inhibits serotonin transporters but also acts as a 5-HT 2B receptor selective agonist [38]. Therefore, FLX’s modulatory effect on CS may be mediated through activation of 5-HT 2B receptors as well as increment of serotonin concentration. In cerebral cortex of adult rat brain, the expression of the 5-HT 2B receptor mRNA is not observed [3941]. On the other hand, the immunoreactivity of 5-HT 2B receptors protein was reported to be positive in frontal cortex but not in other cortical areas [42]. Therefore, it is considered that the contribution of activation of 5-HT 2B receptor to the CS improvement by FLX administration is small in this study.

In conclusion, serotonin endogenously released from the dorsal and median raphe nuclei during the 2AFC-VDT can improve CS depending on the SF of the visual stimulus, in which visual detectability at the most sensitive SF (optimal SF) which is considered to carrier the most valuable and available visual information for behavior, is significantly enhanced.

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Hiromichi Sato for discussions and comments, and thank Dr. Peter Karagiannis for improving the English of the manuscript. A.Y.S. and S.S. designed the research, A.Y.S. collected the data and performed the analyses, A.Y.S., K.T., and R.M. provided technical assistance for the task training and CS measurement, and A.Y.S. and S.S. wrote the paper.

References

  1. 1. Cooper JR, Bloom FE, Roth RH. The biochemical basis of neuropharmacology. Oxford University Press; 2003.
  2. 2. Harris KD, Thiele A. Cortical state and attention. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12: 509–523. pmid:21829219
  3. 3. O’Hearn E, Molliver ME. Organization of raphe-cortical projections in rat: A quantitative retrograde study. Brain Res Bull. 1984;13: 709–726. pmid:6099744
  4. 4. Monti JM. The role of dorsal raphe nucleus serotonergic and non-serotonergic neurons, and of their receptors, in regulating waking and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep. Sleep Med Rev. 2010;14: 319–327. pmid:20153670
  5. 5. Monti JM. Serotonin control of sleep-wake behavior. Sleep Med Rev. 2011;15: 269–281. pmid:21459634
  6. 6. Prouty EW, Chandler DJ, Waterhouse BD. Neurochemical differences between target-specific populations of rat dorsal raphe projection neurons. Brain Res. 2017;1675: 28–40. pmid:28867482
  7. 7. Miyazaki KW, Miyazaki K, Doya K. Activation of dorsal raphe serotonin neurons is necessary for waiting for delayed rewards. J Neurosci. 2012;32: 10451–7. pmid:22855794
  8. 8. Montagne-Clavel J, Oliveras J-L, Martin G. Single-unit recordings at dorsal raphe nucleus in the awake-anesthetized rat: spontaneous activity and responses to cutaneous innocuous and noxious stimulations. Pain. 1995;60: 303–310. pmid:7596626
  9. 9. Evrard A, Laporte AM, Chastanet M, Hen R, Hamon M, Adrien J. 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B receptors control the firing of serotoninergic neurons in the dorsal raphe nucleus of the mouse: studies in 5-HT1B knock-out mice. Eur J Neurosci. 1999;11: 3823–3831. pmid:10583471
  10. 10. Portas CM, Bjorvatn B, Ursin R. Serotonin and the sleep/wake cycle: special emphasis on microdialysis studies. Prog Neurobiol. 2000;60: 13–35. pmid:10622375
  11. 11. Miyamoto H, Nakamaru-Ogiso E, Hamada K, Hensch TK. Serotonergic Integration of Circadian Clock and Ultradian Sleep-Wake Cycles. J Neurosci. 2012;32: 14794–14803. pmid:23077063
  12. 12. Puig MV, Gulledge AT. Serotonin and prefrontal cortex function: neurons, networks, and circuits. Mol Neurobiol. 2011;44: 449–64. pmid:22076606
  13. 13. Celada P, Puig MV, Artigas F. Serotonin modulation of cortical neurons and networks. Front Integr Neurosci. 2013;7: 25. pmid:23626526
  14. 14. Koh T, Nakazawa M, Kani K, Maeda T. Investigations of origins of serotonergic projection to developing rat visual cortex: A combined retrograde tracing and immunohistochemical study. Brain Res Bull. 1991;27: 675–684. pmid:1721860
  15. 15. Dori I, Dinopoulos A, Blue ME, Parnavelas JG. Regional Differences in the Ontogeny of the Serotonergic Projection to the Cerebral Cortex. Exp Neurol. 1996;138: 1–14. pmid:8593886
  16. 16. Janusonis S, Fite KV, Bengston L. Subdivisions of the dorsal raphe nucleus projecting to the lateral geniculate nucleus and primary visual cortex in the Mongolian gerbil. Neuroreport. 2003;14: 459–62. pmid:12634503
  17. 17. Waterhouse BD, Ausim Azizi S, Burne RA, Woodward DJ. Modulation of rat cortical area 17 neuronal responses to moving visual stimuli during norepinephrine and serotonin microiontophoresis. Brain Res. 1990;514: 276–292. pmid:2357542
  18. 18. Watakabe A, Komatsu Y, Sadakane O, Shimegi S, Takahata T, Higo N, et al. Enriched Expression of Serotonin 1B and 2A Receptor Genes in Macaque Visual Cortex and their Bidirectional Modulatory Effects on Neuronal Responses. Cereb Cortex August. 2009;19: 1915–1928. pmid:19056862
  19. 19. Seillier L, Lorenz C, Kawaguchi K, Ott T, Nieder A, Pourriahi P, et al. Serotonin Decreases the Gain of Visual Responses in Awake Macaque V1. J Neurosci. 2017;37: 11390–11405. pmid:29042433
  20. 20. Michaiel AM, Parker PRL, Niell CM. A Hallucinogenic Serotonin-2A Receptor Agonist Reduces Visual Response Gain and Alters Temporal Dynamics in Mouse V1. Cell Rep. 2019;26: 3475–3483.e4. pmid:30917304
  21. 21. Soma S, Suematsu N, Shimegi S. Efficient training protocol for rapid learning of the two-alternative forced-choice visual stimulus detection task. Physiol Rep. 2014;2: 1–11. pmid:24994895
  22. 22. Brainard DH. The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spat Vis. 1997;10: 433–6. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9176952.
  23. 23. Pelli DG. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis. 1997;10: 437–42. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9176953.
  24. 24. Soma S, Suematsu N, Shimegi S. Cholinesterase inhibitor, donepezil, improves visual contrast detectability in freely behaving rats. Behav Brain Res. 2013;256: 362–367. pmid:23994545
  25. 25. Mizuyama R, Soma S, Suemastu N, Shimegi S. Noradrenaline Improves Behavioral Contrast Sensitivity via the β-Adrenergic Receptor. Malmierca MS, editor. PLoS One. 2016;11: e0168455. pmid:27992510
  26. 26. Soma S, Suematsu N, Yoshida J, Ríos A, Shimegi S. Discretion for behavioral selection affects development of habit formation after extended training in rats. Behav Processes. 2018;157: 291–300. pmid:30366108
  27. 27. Tsunoda K, Sato A, Kurata R, Mizuyama R, Shimegi S. Caffeine improves contrast sensitivity of freely moving rats. Physiol Behav. 2019;199: 111–117. pmid:30445067
  28. 28. Histed MH, Carvalho LA, Maunsell JHR. Psychophysical measurement of contrast sensitivity in the behaving mouse. J Neurophysiol. 2012;107: 758–765. pmid:22049334
  29. 29. Hueletl-Soto ME, Carro-Juárez M, Rodríguez-Manzo G. Fluoxetine Chronic Treatment Inhibits Male Rat Sexual Behavior by Affecting Both Copulatory Behavior and the Genital Motor Pattern of Ejaculation. J Sex Med. 2012;9: 1015–1026. pmid:21676185
  30. 30. Köhler C, Lorens SA. Open field activity and avoidance behavior following serotonin depletion: A comparison of the effects of parachlorophenylalanine and electrolytic midbrain raphe lesions. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1978;8: 223–233. pmid:148656
  31. 31. Rowland NE, Caputo FA, Fregly MJ. Water intake induced in rats by serotonin and 5-hydroxytryptophan: Different mechanisms? Brain Res Bull. 1987;18: 501–508. pmid:3300863
  32. 32. Campbell FW, Cooper GF, Enroth-Cugell C. The spatial selectivity of the visual cells of the cat. J Physiol. 1969;203: 223–235. pmid:5821877
  33. 33. Shimegi S, Kimura A, Sato A, Aoyama C, Mizuyama R, Tsunoda K, et al. Cholinergic and serotonergic modulation of visual information processing in monkey V1. J Physiol Paris. 2016;110: 44–51. pmid:27619519
  34. 34. Bennett-Clarke CA, Leslie MJ, Chiaia NL, Rhoades RW. Serotonin 1B receptors in the developing somatosensory and visual cortices are located on thalamocortical axons. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1993;90: 153–7. pmid:8419917
  35. 35. Waterhouse BD, Mihailoff GA, Baack JC, Woodward DJ. Topographical distribution of dorsal and median raphe neurons projecting to motor, sensorimotor, and visual cortical areas in the rat. J Comp Neurol. 1986;249: 460–476. pmid:3528236
  36. 36. Vertes RP. A PHA-L analysis of ascending projections of the dorsal raphe nucleus in the rat. J Comp Neurol. 1991;313: 643–668. pmid:1783685
  37. 37. Vertes RP, Fortin WJ, Crane AM. Projections of the median raphe nucleus in the rat. J Comp Neurol. 1999;407: 555–582. pmid:10235645
  38. 38. Peng L, Gu L, Li B, Hertz L. Fluoxetine and all other SSRIs are 5-HT2B Agonists—Importance for their Therapeutic Effects. Curr Neuropharmacol. 2014;12: 365–379. pmid:25342944
  39. 39. Kursar JD, Nelson DL, Wainscott DB, Baez M. Molecular Cloning, Functional Expression, and mRNA Tissue Distribution of the Human 5-Hydroxytryptamine2B Receptor. 1994.
  40. 40. Pompeiano M, Palacios JM, Mengod G. Distribution of the serotonin 5-HT2 receptor family mRNAs: comparison between 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptors. Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 1994;23: 163–78. pmid:8028479
  41. 41. Osredkar D, Kržan M. Expression of serotonin receptor subtypes in rat brain and astrocyte cell cultures: an ageand tissue-dependent process. Period Biol. 2009;111: 129–135. Available: https://hrcak.srce.hr/36034
  42. 42. Duxon MS, Flanigan TP, Reavley AC, Baxter GS, Blackburn TP, Fone KCF. Evidence for expression of the 5-hydroxytryptamine-2B receptor protein in the rat central nervous system. Neuroscience. 1997;76: 323–329. pmid:9015317