Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Power to the voice hearer — The German version of the voice power differential scale

  • A. Gmeiner ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    andrea.gmeiner@meduniwien.ac.at

    Affiliation Division of Social Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

  • A. Gaglia,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Division of Social Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria, Division of Psychology, Bangor University Wales, Bangor, United Kingdom

  • S. Habicher,

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation

    Affiliation Division of Social Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

  • T. Rumpold,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology

    Affiliation Division of Social Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

  • S. Süßenbacher,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Division of Social Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

  • B. Schrank,

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology

    Affiliation Department of Adult Psychiatry, Karl Landsteiner University for Health Sciences, University Clinic Tulln, Vienna, Austria

  • M. Amering

    Roles Conceptualization, Supervision

    Affiliation Division of Social Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

Abstract

Voice power is an important concept in daily life of voice hearers and in the support and therapy for voice hearers who seek help. Therefore, the ability to examine voice power differentials between a voice and a voice hearer is essential. The present study aimed to collect data on voice power differentials and to further validate the Voice Power Differential Scale (VPD). 105 participants aged ≥ 18 with an ICD10 F2-diagnosis that included hearing voices were included in this study. Internal consistency was good (alpha = 0.792), as well as test-retest-reliability (r = 0.855) and correlations with other constructs were generally as expected. The VPD questionnaire results correlated negatively with the Beliefs About Voices Questionnaire-Revised’s (BAVQ-R) items of Benevolence and Engagement-emotion. It correlated positively with Omnipotence and Resistance-emotion, as well as with Negative Content on the Psychotic Symptoms Rating-Scale (PSYRATS). Unexpectedly, no correlations were found with overall severity and command hallucinations. The Voice Power Differential Scale is an important tool for assessing and formulating a voice hearer’s experience when they seek treatment or support for their verbal auditory hallucinations. The results of this study enrich the on-going discussion about the importance of voice power for voice hearers.

Introduction

The experience of hearing voices is varied. Hearing voices can occur in the general population [13], with many people reporting that voice hearing has been a meaningful human experience [4, 5]. Additionally, hearing voices is common among people with a diagnosis of a schizophrenia spectrum disorder with approximately 70% of people with this diagnosis experiencing this phenomenon[6]. In the context of schizophrenia, voices are labeled as verbal auditory hallucinations (VAH) and are understood to be amongst the most treatment resistant symptoms [7], while they are often stated within treatment resistant schizophrenia [8, 9]. A promising area of development over the last years for voice hearers who are distressed by their experiences is in the field of cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT), e.g. Paulik et al. [10]. Through the application of the cognitive model to verbal auditory hallucinations [11] power differentials have emerged as important concepts in the voice–voice hearer relationship [12] and the perceived power of voices and subjective control over voice hearers’ experiences is one of the key processes targeted by psychological interventions [13].

Higher perceived voice power or voice omnipotence has been shown to be associated with the amount and intensity of voice related distress [1416] as well as with depression, suicidal thoughts and higher social distress [14, 17]. Voice power was found to be one of the best predictors of harmful compliance among people diagnosed with schizophrenia, schizo-affective, mood-disorders [18] and a forensic sample with mixed diagnoses [19]. Voice hearers who experience their voice as dominant, malevolent and frowning suffer from higher burden and have been shown to be at greater risk of engaging in aggressive and self-harming behaviours [2022].

While power relationships play a central role in different instruments that assess VAHs, such as the item Controllability of the Psychotic Symptoms Rating Scale [23, 24] and the item Omnipotence in the Beliefs About Voices Questionnaire [25], the Voice Power Differential Scale (VPD) [26] focuses exclusively on the power relationship. The VPD was originally validated with good psychometric properties in a sample of 59 people diagnosed with schizophrenic psychoses. It has since been used in studies on CBT [14, 2729] and in an evaluation of AVATAR-therapy [30] in patients with a diagnosis of schizophrenia spectrum disorders as well as mixed-diagnosis samples, and in a sample diagnosed with Borderline Personality Disorder [31].

The study described here aimed to further validate the VPD through a German language version tested on a sizeable sample of people with schizophrenia spectrum disorders.

Materials and methods

Procedure

Our working group translated the original English version of the VPD to German, which was then translated back into English by a bilingual native-speaker. The German version and its back translation were then reviewed and approved by the author of the original version [32].

Study inclusion criteria were: males or females ≥ 18 years old, who heard voices in the 4 weeks prior to recruitment, who also met criteria for an ICD-10 F2-diagnosis [33] and were willing to consent to participation. Exclusion criteria were: an insufficient knowledge of the German language and people who were in hospital as involuntary patients.

The Ethics Committee of the Medical University Vienna provided approval for the study (EC-number: 1342/2013). Study participants were recruited from the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy at the Medical University of Vienna in-patient and day-clinics, the University Clinic for Psychiatry and Psychotherapy Tulln’s in-patient department, and the Social Psychiatric Center of the Caritas Vienna’s day-clinic. Potential study participants were informed about the study by their clinical teams who subsequently referred interested people to the research-team. Potential participants were then provided with the details and requirements of the study and those who wished to participate in the study provided written informed consent in accordance with the guidelines of the Ethics Committee of the Medical University of Vienna. A member of the research team (all with a medical background) then met with a potential study participant to take informed consent and to distribute the questionnaires which were self-report. A researcher was present while participants completed the questionnaires to answer any questions. Depending on the preference of the participant, the researcher met with the participant at the clinic or at the participant’s home.

Instruments

Socio-demographic and clinical variables.

Demographic and clinical variables including age, sex, family status, social network, education, job status, living arrangements, time elapsed since first diagnosis, age at first in-patient admission, number of in-patient stays and duration of voice hearing experience were gathered using a self-report questionnaire.

Voice power differential—Scale (VPD).

The VPD [26], comprised of a 5-point-Likert-scale measures the power differential between voice and voice hearer on 7 dimensions: power, strength, confidence, respect, ability to inflict harm, superiority and knowledge. The greater the score, the greater the perceived power differential, e.g. “I am much stronger than my voice” [1]–“My voice is much stronger than me” [5]. Participants who heard more than one voice were asked to complete the questionnaire with respect to their experience of the voice that they perceived to be most dominant. The scale showed good internal reliability (Cronbach's α = 0.85) with high test-re-test reliability (r = 0.82) in the original study that established the measure’s validity [26].

The Beliefs About Voices Questionnaire–Revised (BAVQ-R).

The BAVQ-R [25], often used in CBT, is an instrument that assesses beliefs about voices as well as behavioural and emotional responses to them on 5 subscales: malevolence, benevolence, omnipotence, resistance and engagement. The BAVQ-R consists of 35 items that are self-rated on a 4-point-Likert-scale (0 = disagree, 1 = don’t know, 2 = rather agree, 3 = agree), again this scale was completed by the participants for their dominant voice. The German-language version [17] showed high internal consistency for the subscales malevolence (a = 0.83), benevolence (a = 0.91), resistance (a = 0.85), engagement (a = 0.87) but a low internal consistency in the subscale omnipotence (a = 0.62). Test-Retest-Reliability was satisfactory.

Psychotic symptoms rating scale–auditory hallucinations (PSYRATS-AH).

PSYRATS-AH [23, 24, 34], also often used in CBT, is a reliable and valid semi-structured interview that inquires about the participants’ experience of their voices overall. It consists of eleven items covering the following dimensions: frequency, duration, location, loudness, beliefs of origin of voices, amount of negative content, amount and intensity of distress, life-disruption, and controllability of the voices. Data are assessed on a 5-point ordinal scale (0–4).

Additional items.

These participants of this group of people under medical treatment were also asked if they ever self-harmed, harmed anyone else, or attempted suicide in the past. Additionally, they were asked if the voices ever commanded them to harm themselves or someone else.

Clinical global impression–schizophrenia scale (CGI-SCH).

The CGI [35] was developed by Busner & Targum and adapted for schizophrenia (CGI-SCH) by Haro et al [36]. Used in routine clinical practice as well as research, the brief, valid and reliable instrument, assesses the symptom severity of the following five items on a seven-point-Likert-scale: positive symptoms (CGI-pos), negative symptoms (CGI-neg), depressive symptoms (CGI-dep), cognitive symptoms (CGI-cog) and overall severity (CGI-total).

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were calculated with “Statistical Package for Social Sciences”(SPSS Version 24, SPSS GmbH, IBM AG, USA). Descriptive statistics are presented in absolute numbers and sample percentages. All analyses were performed using two-tailed tests with alpha = 0.05. In order to analyse reliability, Cronbach’s alpha was calculated with values higher than 0.7 were considered acceptable. The test-retest-reliability was calculated via Pearson correlations, again values above 0.7 were considered acceptable. Construct validity was calculated via Pearson-correlations with PSYRATS-AH, BAVQ-R and CGI-SCH. A factor analysis of the VPD was not conducted in this study due to the small number of items.

Results and discussion

Recruitment

A total of 130 eligible participants were referred to the research team of which 105 provided informed consent and completed the questionnaires. A sample size of 105 is in compliance with the conventional rule for validation studies [37]. In order to assess the test-retest-reliability of the VPD, 19 participants were recruited to repeat the VPD after 7–15 days (mean: 9d ± 2.2).

Study sample

Socio-demographic and clinical data are shown in Table 1. 56.2% of the participants were male, with a median age of 33.0 years (range: 19.0–86.0) and a median duration of voice hearing experience of 10.5 years (range: 0.1–45.0). 79% of the participants were single, 42.9% were out of the workforce as either retired or in receipt of permanent disability benefits, and only 3.8% were employed. 32.4% had a sufficient social network (self-defined by each participant), 49.5% lived alone in their own household. These socio-demographic data are typical for a sample with diagnoses of schizophrenia spectrum disorders [38].

Reliability

Internal consistency.

Internal consistency was good at 𝜶 = 0.792.

Test-retest-reliability.

To examine test-retest-reliability of the VPD, 19 participants completed the VPD-scale again after 7–15 days (SD ± 2.24). Good test-retest-reliability with a Pearson’s correlation coefficient of 0.855 (p ≤ 0.001) was shown.

Construct validity

With regards to construct validity, the following expected correlations were shown: there were negative correlations between VPD overall and the BAVQ-R subscales Benevolence (r = -0.268, p = 0.009) and Engagement-Emotion (r = -0.294, p = 0.004), and positive correlations between VPD overall and the BAVQ-R subscales Omnipotence (r = 0.485, p ≤ 0.001), Resistance-Emotion (r = 0.295, p = 0.004). The VPD item “pure power” correlated negatively with the subscale Engagement–Emotion (r = -0.093, p = 0.019) and positively with the subscales Malevolence (r = 0.237, p = 0.018), Omnipotence (r = 0.504, p ≤ 0.001) and Resistance-Emotion (r = 0.028, p = 0.002). The VPD item strength correlated negatively with Benevolence (r = -0.235, p = 0.018), Engagement-Emotion (r = -0.233, p = 0.019) and positively with Omnipotence (r = 0.439, p ≤ 0.001) and Resistance-Emotion (r = 0.277, p = 0.005). Confidence correlated positively with Omnipotence (r = 0.360, p ≤ 0.001) and Resistance-Emotion (r = 0.203, p = 0.044). Ability to inflict harm correlated negatively with Benevolence (r = -0.318, p = 0.002), Engagement-Emotion (r = -0.397, p ≤ 0.001) and Engagement-Behaviour (r = -0.205, p = 0.046) and positively with Omnipotence (r = 0.289, p = 0.005). Superiority correlated negatively with Benevolence (r = -0.216, p = 0.032) and Engagement-Emotion (r = -0.245, p = 0.014), and positively with Omnipotence (r = 0.303, p = 0.003). Knowledge correlated negatively with Resistance-Behaviour (r = -0.212, p = 0.036) and positively with Omnipotence (r = 0.228, p = 0.025). Details about correlations between BAVQ-R and VPD are shown in Table 2. Finally, calculations based on the 4 BAVQ-R factors recently identified by Strauss et al. [39] showed the following results: The factor Persecutory Beliefs correlated positively with Power (r = 0.313, p = 0.002) and Strength (r = 0.228, p = 0.024). The factor Benevolence correlated negatively with Strength (r = -0.249 p = 0.012), Ability to inflict harm (r = -0.339, p = 0.001) and Superiority (r = -0.226, p = 0.025). The factor Resistance did not show any significant correlations. The factor Engagement correlated negatively with Strength (r = -0.244, p = 0.014), Ability to inflict harm (r = -0.342, p = 0.001) and Superiority (r = -0.277, p = 0.006).

Respect correlated negatively with the CGI item Negative Symptoms and positively with the item Depressive Symptoms (r = 0.214, p = 0.037). No correlations were shown with regards to overall severity. Details about correlations between CGI and VPD are shown in Table 3.

43.8% perceived voices that commanded self-harm, 24.8% perceived voices that commanded to harm others. 30.5% had ever harmed themselves, 23.8% had attempted suicide. These demographics are in line with those reported for clinical voice hearers in the meta-analysis by Baumeister et al. [2]. Self-harming or harming behaviour and VPD were not shown to be correlated.

The PSYRATS-item Duration of voices correlated positively with power (r = 0.238, p = 0.017) and strength (r = 0.304, p = 0.002). Loudness of the voice correlated negatively with the VPD item Ability to inflict harm (r = -0.207, p = 0.043). Negative content correlated positively with VPD overall (r = 0.250, p = 0.014), Strength (r = 0.217, p = 0.029) and Superiority (r = 0.266, p = 0.007). Calculations based on the 4 factors identified by Woodward et al. [40] showed the following results: The factor Distress correlated positively with Power (r = 0.220, p = 0.029), Strength (r = 0.258, p = 0.010) and Superiority (r = 0.288, p = 0.004). The factor Frequency correlated positively with Power (r = 0.212, p = 0.035) and Strength (r = 0.271, p = 0.006). The Attribution factor did not show any significant correlations. The factor Loudness correlated negatively with Ability to inflict harm (r = -0.207, p = 0.043).

Descriptive statistics of the VPD

The VPD yields a score of between 7–35 with higher scores indicating greater power for the voice relative to the voice hearer [41]. The mean of the VPD score for these study participants was 23.3, which was relatively high. The means of each dimension, with the exception of the dimension “Strength” which was 3.0, were above 3, indicating that voice power was rather high. About 90% of the participants completed the VPD-scale fully and within 2 minutes. Details are shown in Table 4.

105 persons with a diagnosis of a schizophrenia spectrum disorders and voice hearing experience participated in this largest study on the validity of the Voice Power Differential Scale [26] to date. The VPD was fast and easy to complete and had a 90% total completion rate.

The descriptive statistics of the VPD-scale (overall-score) showed a mean of 23.3. In the domains “power”, “confidence”, “respect”, “ability to inflict harm”, “superiority”, and “knowledge”, the voice was experienced to be more powerful than the voice hearer whereas the item “strength” showed an equal power balance. This is consistent with the original study in which 59 people diagnosed with schizophrenic psychoses were included [26] and two further studies [15, 27], in which the means of the total-scale were even higher. These results suggest voice hearers tend to experience their voices as more potent than themselves, thus voice hearers are likely to experience themselves as overpowered, bullied, or scared. Regardless of whether the main focus is on the concepts of voice content [42], contribution to goal interference and facilitation [43], or specialised and non-specialised therapies for alleviating the effects of voices [44], voice power is an essential construct.

The Internal Consistency (Cronbach’s Alpha: 0.792) was good, but lower than in the original study [26] and two further studies assessing reliability [27, 45]. Test-Retest-Reliability was good and comparable to the original study [26]. Therefore, it can be assumed that the power differential between voice hearer and voice is stable over a couple of weeks and that the results do not depend on daily conditions.

In order to determine the construct validity, correlations between the power of the voice, beliefs about voices (BAVQ-R), voice topography (PSYRATS-AH), and severity of positive, negative, depressive, and cognitive symptoms (CGI-SCH) were calculated. The original validation study [26] found a correlation between voice power and depressive symptoms, measured on the Beck Depression Inventory. The current study demonstrated a positive correlation between depressive symptoms and voice power subscale of respect. In this study, the loudness and duration of voice were related to voice power. In the original study, loudness was found to be related as well. However, loudness is not a clearly defined construct and it would benefit from additional research to further investigate its importance. In addition, this study found negative content and voice power to be correlated. Thus, the relationship between voice power and negative content might additionally be an important aspect for research, particularly because at present there are conflicting ideas on how talking treatments for voice hearers, ranging from CBTp, compassion focused therapy, trauma focused therapy or more recently AVATAR-therapy could most effectively intervene in the voice hearers’ experience [30, 42, 46]. Interestingly in this study harming, self-harming behaviour and command hallucinations showed no correlations with voice power. These findings differed from the results of the COMMAND Trial [28], where voice power was found to be one of the best predictors of harmful compliance and a mediator of change. Nevertheless, it is important to note that the sample in this study differed from that of the sample in the COMMAND TRIAL in which participants had F2 or F3 diagnoses along with the experience of command voices. Furthermore, no relationship between voice power and overall severity (CGI-SCH) was found.

The main limitation of this study was that it focused on a subsection of the voice hearing population, namely those with an ICD 10 F2 diagnosis who were seeking treatment at the time of the study. Therefore, the findings of the study cannot be generalised to other voice hearing populations, for example healthy voice hearers or people with mood disorders or Post Traumatic Stress Disorders, etc. In addition, it offers no insight into the experiences of healthy voice hearers and indeed we did not investigate how many of our study participants identify as healthy voice hearers when they are not in crisis.

Conclusions

A person’s perception of the power of the voice they hear is important and can be measured easily, validly, reliably, and efficiently with the Voice Power Differential Scale.

Acknowledgments

We want to thank everyone who participated in the study. Furthermore, we would like to thank Ingrid Sibitz for initiating the present project.

References

  1. 1. Linscott R, Van Os J. An updated and conservative systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological evidence on psychotic experiences in children and adults: on the pathway from proneness to persistence to dimensional expression across mental disorders. Psychological medicine. 2013;43(6):1133–49. pmid:22850401
  2. 2. Baumeister D, Sedgwick O, Howes O, Peters E. Auditory verbal hallucinations and continuum models of psychosis: A systematic review of the healthy voice-hearer literature. Clinical psychology review. 2017;51:125–41. pmid:27866082
  3. 3. Garrison JR, Fernyhough C, McCarthy-Jones S, Simons JS, Sommer IEC. Paracingulate Sulcus Morphology and Hallucinations in Clinical and Nonclinical Groups. Schizophr Bull. 2019;45(4):733–41. pmid:30380115
  4. 4. Romme MA, Escher AD. Hearing voices. Schizophr Bull. 1989;15(2):209–16. pmid:2749184
  5. 5. Corstens D, Longden E, McCarthy-Jones S, Waddingham R, Thomas N. Emerging perspectives from the hearing voices movement: implications for research and practice. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40 Suppl 4:S285–94.
  6. 6. Landmark J, Merskey H, Cernovsky Z, Helmes E. The positive triad of schizophrenic symptoms. Its statistical properties and its relationship to 13 traditional diagnostic systems. The British journal of psychiatry : the journal of mental science. 1990;156:388–94.
  7. 7. Sommer IE, Slotema CW, Daskalakis ZJ, Derks EM, Blom JD, van der Gaag M. The treatment of hallucinations in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38(4):704–14. pmid:22368234
  8. 8. Dold M, Leucht S. Pharmacotherapy of treatment-resistant schizophrenia: a clinical perspective. Evid Based Ment Health. 2014;17(2):33–7. pmid:24713315
  9. 9. Murray RM, Quattrone D, Natesan S, van Os J, Nordentoft M, Howes O, et al. Should psychiatrists be more cautious about the long-term prophylactic use of antipsychotics? The British journal of psychiatry : the journal of mental science. 2016;209(5):361–5.
  10. 10. Paulik G, Hayward M, Jones AM, Badcock JC. Evaluating the "C" and "B" in brief cognitive behaviour therapy for distressing voices in routine clinical practice in an uncontrolled study. Clinical psychology & psychotherapy. 2019.
  11. 11. Chadwick P, Birchwood M. The omnipotence of voices. A cognitive approach to auditory hallucinations. The British journal of psychiatry : the journal of mental science. 1994;164(2):190–201.
  12. 12. Birchwood M, Gilbert P, Gilbert J, Trower P, Meaden A, Hay J, et al. Interpersonal and role-related schema influence the relationship with the dominant ‘voice’in schizophrenia: a comparison of three models. Psychological medicine. 2004;34(8):1571–80. pmid:15724887
  13. 13. Thomas N, Hayward M, Peters E, van der Gaag M, Bentall RP, Jenner J, et al. Psychological therapies for auditory hallucinations (voices): current status and key directions for future research. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40 Suppl 4:S202–12.
  14. 14. Birchwood M, Gilbert P, Gilbert J, Trower P, Meaden A, Hay J, et al. Interpersonal and role-related schema influence the relationship with the dominant 'voice' in schizophrenia: a comparison of three models. Psychological medicine. 2004;34(8):1571–80. pmid:15724887
  15. 15. Trower P, Birchwood M, Meaden A, Byrne S, Nelson A, Ross K. Cognitive therapy for command hallucinations: randomised controlled trial. The British journal of psychiatry : the journal of mental science. 2004;184:312–20.
  16. 16. Peters E, Williams S, Cooke M, Kuipers E. It's not what you hear, it's the way you think about it: appraisals as determinants of affect and behaviour in voice hearers. Psychological medicine. 2012;42(7):1507–14. pmid:22115329
  17. 17. Gmeiner A, Aslan J, Gaglia A, Rumpold T, Schrank B, Süßenbacher S, et al. Beliefs and distress related to hearing voices: the German version of the Beliefs About Voices Questionnaire-Revised (BAVQ-R). Neuropsychiatrie: Klinik, Diagnostik, Therapie und Rehabilitation: Organ der Gesellschaft Osterreichischer Nervenarzte und Psychiater. 2018;32(4):214–21.
  18. 18. Birchwood M, Dunn G, Meaden A, Tarrier N, Lewis S, Wykes T, et al. The COMMAND trial of cognitive therapy to prevent harmful compliance with command hallucinations: predictors of outcome and mediators of change. Psychological medicine. 2018;48(12):1966–74. pmid:29202885
  19. 19. Reynolds N, Scragg P. Compliance with command hallucinations: the role of power in relation to the voice, and social rank in relation to the voice and others. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology. 2010;21(1):121–38.
  20. 20. Mawson A, Cohen K, Berry K. Reviewing evidence for the cognitive model of auditory hallucinations: The relationship between cognitive voice appraisals and distress during psychosis. Clinical Psychology Review. 2010;30(2):248–58. pmid:20071062
  21. 21. Berman BA, Duffy K, Serper MR. Beliefs about voices and aggressive behavior in inpatients in an acute psychiatric setting. The Journal of clinical psychiatry. 2010;71(4):497–501. pmid:19925747
  22. 22. Simms J, McCormack V, Anderson R, Mulholland C. Correlates of self‐harm behaviour in acutely ill patients with schizophrenia. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, research and practice. 2007;80(1):39–49.
  23. 23. Haddock G, McCarron J, Tarrier N, Faragher E. Scales to measure dimensions of hallucinations and delusions: the psychotic symptom rating scales (PSYRATS). Psychological medicine. 1999;29(4):879–89. pmid:10473315
  24. 24. Kronmüller K-T, von Bock A, Grupe S, Büche L, Gentner NC, Rückl S, et al. Psychometric evaluation of the psychotic symptom rating scales. Comprehensive psychiatry. 2011;52(1):102–8. pmid:21220071
  25. 25. Chadwick P, Lees S, Birchwood M. The revised beliefs about voices questionnaire (BAVQ–R). The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2000;177(3):229–32.
  26. 26. Birchwood M, Meaden A, Trower P, Gilbert P, Plaistow J. The power and omnipotence of voices: Subordination and entrapment by voices and significant others. Psychological medicine. 2000;30(2):337–44. pmid:10824654
  27. 27. Gilbert P, Birchwood M, Gilbert J, Trower P, Hay J, Murray B, et al. An exploration of evolved mental mechanisms for dominant and subordinate behaviour in relation to auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia and critical thoughts in depression. Psychological medicine. 2001;31(6):1117–27. pmid:11513379
  28. 28. Birchwood M, Dunn G, Meaden A, Tarrier N, Lewis S, Wykes T, et al. The COMMAND trial of cognitive therapy to prevent harmful compliance with command hallucinations: predictors of outcome and mediators of change. Psychological medicine. 2018;48(12):1966–74. pmid:29202885
  29. 29. Hazell CM, Strauss C, Cavanagh K, Hayward M. Barriers to disseminating brief CBT for voices from a lived experience and clinician perspective. PloS one. 2017;12(6):e0178715. pmid:28575094
  30. 30. Craig TK, Rus-Calafell M, Ward T, Leff JP, Huckvale M, Howarth E, et al. AVATAR therapy for auditory verbal hallucinations in people with psychosis: a single-blind, randomised controlled trial. The lancet Psychiatry. 2018;5(1):31–40. pmid:29175276
  31. 31. Slotema CW, Blom JD, Deen M, Niemantsverdriet MBA, van der Gaag M, Hoek HW, et al. Negative Beliefs about Voices in Patients with Borderline Personality Disorder Are Associated with Distress: A Plea for Cognitive-Behavioural Therapy? Psychopathology. 2017;50(4):255–61. pmid:28738347
  32. 32. Sartorius N, Janca A. Psychiatric assessment instruments developed by the World Health Organization. Social psychiatry and psychiatric epidemiology. 1996;31(2):55–69. pmid:8881086
  33. 33. Simms G. The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines: World Health Organization; 1992.
  34. 34. Schneider SD, Jelinek L, Lincoln TM, Moritz S. What happened to the voices? A fine-grained analysis of how hallucinations and delusions change under psychiatric treatment. Psychiatry research. 2011;188(1):13–7. pmid:21215461
  35. 35. Busner J, Targum SD. The clinical global impressions scale: applying a research tool in clinical practice. Psychiatry (Edgmont (Pa : Township)). 2007;4(7):28–37.
  36. 36. Haro JM, Kamath SA, Ochoa S, Novick D, Rele K, Fargas A, et al. The Clinical Global Impression-Schizophrenia scale: a simple instrument to measure the diversity of symptoms present in schizophrenia. Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica Supplementum. 2003(416):16–23. pmid:12755850
  37. 37. Boateng GO, Neilands TB, Frongillo EA, Melgar-Quinonez HR, Young SL. Best Practices for Developing and Validating Scales for Health, Social, and Behavioral Research: A Primer. Front Public Health. 2018;6:149. pmid:29942800
  38. 38. Sibitz I, Friedrich ME, Unger A, Bachmann A, Benesch T, Amering M. [Internalized Stigma of Schizophrenia: Validation of the German Version of the Internalized Stigma of Mental Illness-Scale (ISMI)]. Psychiatrische Praxis. 2013;40(2):83–91. pmid:23354628
  39. 39. Strauss C, Hugdahl K, Waters F, Hayward M, Bless JJ, Falkenberg LE, et al. The Beliefs about Voices Questionnaire—Revised: A factor structure from 450 participants. Psychiatry research. 2018;259:95–103. pmid:29035759
  40. 40. Woodward TS, Jung K, Hwang H, Yin J, Taylor L, Menon M, et al. Symptom dimensions of the psychotic symptom rating scales in psychosis: a multisite study. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40 Suppl 4:S265–74.
  41. 41. Birchwood M, Michail M, Meaden A, Tarrier N, Lewis S, Wykes T, et al. Cognitive behaviour therapy to prevent harmful compliance with command hallucinations (COMMAND): a randomised controlled trial. The lancet Psychiatry. 2014;1(1):23–33. pmid:26360400
  42. 42. Laroi F, Thomas N, Aleman A, Fernyhough C, Wilkinson S, Deamer F, et al. The ice in voices: Understanding negative content in auditory-verbal hallucinations. Clinical psychology review. 2019;67:1–10. pmid:30553563
  43. 43. Varese F, Mansell W, Tai SJ. What is distressing about auditory verbal hallucinations? The contribution of goal interference and goal facilitation. Psychology and psychotherapy. 2017;90(4):720–34. pmid:28726350
  44. 44. Kennedy L, Xyrichis A. Cognitive Behavioral Therapy Compared with Non-specialized Therapy for Alleviating the Effect of Auditory Hallucinations in People with Reoccurring Schizophrenia: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Community mental health journal. 2017;53(2):127–33. pmid:27295054
  45. 45. Connor C, Birchwood M. Through the looking glass: self-reassuring meta-cognitive capacity and its relationship with the thematic content of voices. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2013;7:213. pmid:23734118
  46. 46. Lincoln TM, Peters E. A systematic review and discussion of symptom specific cognitive behavioural approaches to delusions and hallucinations. Schizophrenia research. 2019;203:66–79. pmid:29352708