Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

The relationship between executive functions and fluid intelligence in multiple sclerosis

  • Belén Goitia,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Institute of Translational and Cognitive Neuroscience (INCyT), INECO Foundation, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, Laboratory of Neuroscience, Torcuato Di Tella University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina

  • Diana Bruno,

    Roles Data curation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Institute of Translational and Cognitive Neuroscience (INCyT), INECO Foundation, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina

  • Sofía Abrevaya,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Institute of Translational and Cognitive Neuroscience (INCyT), INECO Foundation, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina

  • Lucas Sedeño,

    Roles Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Institute of Translational and Cognitive Neuroscience (INCyT), INECO Foundation, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina

  • Agustín Ibáñez,

    Roles Funding acquisition, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Institute of Translational and Cognitive Neuroscience (INCyT), INECO Foundation, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina, Center for Social and Cognitive Neuroscience (CSCN), School of Psychology, Universidad Adolfo Ibáñez, Santiago, Chile, Universidad Autónoma del Caribe, Barranquilla, Colombia, Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Australian Research Council (ACR), Sydney, Australia

  • Facundo Manes,

    Roles Project administration, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Institute of Translational and Cognitive Neuroscience (INCyT), INECO Foundation, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina

  • Mariano Sigman,

    Roles Conceptualization, Project administration, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Laboratory of Neuroscience, Torcuato Di Tella University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina

  • Vladimiro Sinay,

    Roles Data curation, Resources, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Institute of Translational and Cognitive Neuroscience (INCyT), INECO Foundation, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina

  • Teresa Torralva,

    Roles Data curation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Institute of Translational and Cognitive Neuroscience (INCyT), INECO Foundation, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina

  • John Duncan,

    Roles Conceptualization, Resources, Software, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Medical Research Council (MRC) Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, United Kingdom, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom

  • María Roca

    Roles Conceptualization, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    mroca@ineco.org.ar

    Affiliations Institute of Translational and Cognitive Neuroscience (INCyT), INECO Foundation, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina

Abstract

Background & objective

Deficits in cognitive functions dependent upon the integrity of the prefrontal cortex have been described in Multiple Sclerosis (MS). In a series of studies we have shown that fluid intelligence (g) is a substantial contributor to frontal deficits and that, for some classical “executive” tasks, frontal deficits were entirely explained by g. However, for another group of frontal tasks deficits remained once g was introduced as a covariate. This second set of tests included multitasking and theory of mind tasks. In the present study, we aimed at determining the role of fluid intelligence in frontal deficits seen in patients with MS.

Methods

A group of patients with Relapsing Remitting MS (n = 36) and a group of control subjects (n = 42) were assessed with a battery of classical executive tests (which included the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Verbal Fluency, and Trail Making Test B), a multitasking test, a theory of mind test and a fluid intelligence test.

Results

MS patients showed significant deficits in the fluid intelligence task. We found differences between patients and control subjects in all tests except for the multitasking test. The differences in the classical executive tests became non-significant once fluid intelligence was introduced as a covariate, but differences in theory of mind remained.

Conclusions

The present results suggest that fluid intelligence can be affected in MS and that this impairment can play a role in the executive deficits described in MS.

Introduction

Multiple Sclerosis (MS) is a neurological disease characterized by demyelination and axonal loss, which results in disruption of neuronal communication in the Central Nervous System. Of the four MS subtypes defined by their clinical course, Relapsing Remitting MS (RRMS) is the most common, with 85% of MS patients falling into this category [1].

Even though a few decades ago cognitive deficits were considered uncommon among MS patients, it is now estimated that 43–65% of MS patients suffer from cognitive impairment [25]. Deficits have been reported in complex attention, efficiency of information processing, executive functioning, processing speed, and long-term memory and are known to impact daily living abilities [6]. Such deficits have been related to disruptions in white matter tracts, particularly in frontal-subcortical networks [712], but also to depressive symptoms [13,14]⁠ or high fatigue levels [13,15]⁠, though some studies argue against the latter relationship [1618].

A wide variety of cerebral structures and mechanisms have been linked to MS cognitive deficits. Reports include cortical volume loss [19,20], reduced cerebral blood flow in the frontal lobes [21], volume loss in deep grey matter structures, particularly the thalamus, hippocampus and putamen [2227], and focal white matter and grey matter damage [27,28]⁠.

Executive impairment in MS has been related to damage in fronto-subcortical tracts. In clinical neuropsychological studies, the prefrontal cortex (PFC) is believed to support “executive functions”. Executive functions are understood as processes that organize behaviour executing cognitive control of lower order functions. Within executive functions, many processes can be identified, such as planning, flexibility, switching and inhibition. Both in clinical and in experimental fields multiple tests have been used to measure each function, including the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), Verbal Fluency and the Trail Making Test B as the most widely used in clinical and experimental neuropsychology. Besides these classical executive tests, damage to PFC has also been associated with deficits in social cognition and multitasking.

Parallel to specific frontal functions supposedly associated with specific PFC regions, experimental neuroscience has linked the frontal lobe to the concept of “general intelligence” or “g”, introduced by Spearman (1904, 1927) to account for universal positive correlations between different cognitive tests. More generally, factor g has been linked to a multiple-demand (MD) [29, 30⁠] system—including regions of the lateral frontal surface, the middle frontal gyrus, the premotor cortex, the anterior insula, and the dorsomedial frontal cortex, accompanied by a further region in lateral parietal cortex [31]⁠—that is active during many different kinds of cognitive tasks.

The importance of the frontal lobe in fluid intelligence made us question the nature of the relationship between g and the above frontal tasks. If g is positively correlated with all tasks, then g deficits in frontal patients may explain bad performance in frontal tasks. We wanted to know how well deficits in tasks associated with PFC were explained by a fluid intelligence loss. In this regard, an early paper from our group [32]⁠ showed that fluid intelligence is a substantial contributor to cognitive deficits in frontal patients; however, this relationship is not simple. While for classical executive tasks once fluid intelligence is partialled out, no deficits remained, for a second set of tasks—mainly tasks of multitasking and social cognition—deficits remained. We showed similar results in many conditions, such as frontal lobe lesions [32]⁠, Parkinson’s disease [33], Frontotemporal Dementia [34]⁠, Schizophrenia [35]⁠, and Bipolar Disorder [36].

Our results have had strong implications for the use and interpretation of widely used tests such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Verbal Fluency and Trail Making Test B. According to our results, deficits measured in such tests were not reflecting problems specific to their particular cognitive content—such as flexibility, energisation or switching—but instead they might be reflecting a much broader cognitive loss. On the other hand, deficits in social cognition and multitasking seemed not to be explained in this way.

Our aim in this paper is to test whether frontal deficits measured by frontal tests in MS patients can be accounted for by a deficit in g. Similar to our previous work, we introduce g as a covariate to see to what extent frontal deficits remain. If the same pattern that we have found in the past is repeated, then we expect that impairments in classical executive tests will be completely explained by reduced g, while deficits in other tests—here including multitasking and social cognition—will remain. In this paper we also searched for a link between MS cognitive deficits and the MD system [37]⁠⁠ asking if volume loss in the MD system, as measured with MRI, correlates with the general intelligence factor in this population.

Methods

Participants

Permission for the study was obtained from the local research ethics committee (INECO Foundation) and all participants gave their signed informed consent prior to inclusion, according to the Declaration of Helsinki of 1975, as revised in 2008. Thirty-six patients diagnosed with Relapsing Remitting MS (30 women, 6 men), fulfilling Poser and McDonald criteria [38,39]⁠ and referred to our MS clinic for routine follow-up, underwent neuropsychological and MRI evaluation for the present study. All had mild clinical disability [Expanded Disability Status Scale (EDSS) <2], without visual deficit or upper limb impairment potentially affecting neuropsychological test performance or history of alcohol or drug abuse, major psychiatric disorder, head trauma or other neurological disorder or systemic illness. All tests were performed at least 90 days after the most recent relapse episode, and with all patients off steroid treatment for at least three months. Mean age was 39.2 ± 10.2 years (range 21–64 years) and mean disease duration 9.3 ± 7.3 years (range 1–35 years). Physical disability was assessed using EDSS [40]⁠ and MS Functional Composite (MSFC) score [41]⁠. Forty-two subjects matched for age, gender (29 women, 13 men) and educational level recruited from a local volunteer group served as controls. Participants were included in the control group if they reported no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders, including traumatic brain injury or substance abuse. To control for mood symptoms and fatigue in the patients with MS, we assessed them with the Beck Depression Inventory [42] and the Modified Fatigue Impact Scale [43]⁠.

All participants in the study (patients and controls) were examined to ensure they had no comorbidity with other psychiatric or neurological disorders.

Neuropsychological assessment

To estimate pre-morbid intelligence we used the Word Accentuation Test-Buenos Aires edition [44]. This test, similar to the National Adult Reading Test [45]⁠, measures the ability to read 51 irregularly stressed Spanish words.

Fluid intelligence (g).

Matrix Reasoning is a subtest of WAIS-III [46]⁠⁠ that gives a measure of fluid reasoning. In this test the subject is presented with an incomplete pattern and given 5 options for completing it properly. Each correct answer gives a score of 1 point (0 for incorrect answers). The test is interrupted after 4 consecutive incorrect answers. The maximum gross score is 26 points. We have taken this as our measure of g.

Classical executive tests

Wisconsin card sorting test (WCST) [47].

For the WCST we used Nelson’s modified version of the standard procedure. Cards varying on three basic features—colour, shape and number of items—must be sorted according to each feature in turn. The participant’s first sorting choice becomes the correct feature, and once a criterion of six consecutive correct sorts is achieved, the subject is told that the rules have changed, and cards must be sorted according to a new feature. After all three features have been used as sorting criteria, subjects must cycle through them again in the same order as they did before. Each time the feature is changed, the next must be discovered by trial and error. Score was total number of categories achieved. Data were available for 35 patients.

Verbal fluency [48].

In verbal fluency tasks, the subject generates as many items as possible from a given category. We used the standard Argentinian phonemic version, asking subjects to generate words beginning with the letter P in a one-minute block. Score was the total number of correct words generated. Data were available for 36 patients.

Trail making test B (TMTB) [49].

In this test the subject is required to draw lines sequentially connecting 13 numbers and 12 letters distributed on a sheet of paper. Letters and numbers are encircled and must be connected alternately (e.g., 1, A, 2, B, 3, C, etc.). Score was the total time (s) required to complete the task, given a negative sign so that higher scores meant better performance. Data were available for 35 patients.

Multitasking and social cognition tests

Hotel task [50,51].

The hotel task, originally designed by Manly in 2002, has been used as an ecological assessment tool to evaluate goal management and multitasking abilities in different neurological and psychiatric conditions. The test requires planning, problem solving abilities, prospective memory, organizing and monitoring behavior. The task comprised five primary activities related to running a hotel (compiling bills, sorting coins for a charity collection, looking up telephone numbers, sorting conference labels, proofreading). The materials needed to perform these activities were arranged on a desk, along with a clock that could be consulted by removing and then replacing a cover. Subjects were told to try at least some of all five activities during a 15 min period, so that, at the end of this period, they would be able to give an estimate of how long each task would take to complete. It was explained that time was not enough to actually complete the tasks; the goal instead was to ensure that every task was sampled. Subjects were also asked to remember to open and close the hotel garage doors at specified times (open at 6 min, close at 12 min), using an electronic button. Of the several scores possible for this task, we used time allocation: for each primary task we assumed an optimal allocation of 3 min, and measured the summed total deviation (in seconds) from this optimum. Total deviation was given a negative sign so that higher scores meant better performance. Data were available for 31 patients.

Faux pas [52].

In each trial of this test, the subject was read a short, one paragraph story. To reduce working memory load, a written version of the story was also placed at all times in front of the subject and available to re-read as many times as needed. In 10 stories there was a faux pas, involving one person unintentionally saying something hurtful or insulting to another. In the remaining 10 stories there were no faux pas. After each story, the subject was asked whether something inappropriate was said and if so, why it was inappropriate. If the answer was incorrect, an additional memory question was asked to check that basic facts of the story were retained; if they were not, the story was re-examined and all questions repeated. The score was 1 point for each faux pas correctly identified, or non-faux pas correctly rejected.

Statistical analysis of neuropsychological data

All statistical analyses regarding neuropsychological results were performed with IBM SPSS® Statistics 20. Groups were compared through Student’s t-tests for the following variables: age, education years, WAT-BA, WCST, Verbal Fluency, TMTB, Hotel task, Faux Pas. Groups were compared again, this time taking Matrix Reasoning as a covariate through an ANCOVA, for the following variables: WCST, Verbal Fluency, TMTB, Hotel task, Faux Pas.

Image acquisition

We obtained MRI recordings from 28 multiple sclerosis patients and 29 controls. Subjects were scanned in a 1.5 T Philips Intera scanner with a standard head coil. We used a T1-weighted spin echo sequence that covered the whole brain (matrix size = 256 × 240 × 120, 1 mm isotropic; TR = 7489 ms; TE = 3420 ms; flip angle = 8°).

Grey-matter analysis

A grey-matter analysis was performed to establish the participants’ grey matter volume. Data were preprocessed on the DARTEL Toolbox following validated procedures [5357]⁠ and using Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM12) (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm12/). Images were segmented into grey matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid volumes. Next, images were smoothed with a 12 mm full-width half-maximum kernel as proposed in other reports [53,58]⁠ and normalized to MNI space. To test whether the performances on the Matrix Reasoning test (g) and the Faux Pas test (the latter being expected not to be related to g) were associated with the MD system, we restricted our analysis using a mask of the main areas of this network (http://imaging.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/imaging/MDsystem) [59]⁠, to extract the grey matter volume for each participant. The preprocessed images were used to extract the grey matter volume (in ml) of this mask for each participant with a toolbox that runs in the MATLAB environment (The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, Massachusetts; Ged Ridgway, http://www.cs.ucl.ac.uk/staff/g.ridgway/vbm/get_totals.m), and has been used in previous studies [6064]⁠. Once the values were obtained, we performed non-parametric Spearman correlation tests between either Matrix Reasoning Raw scores and Faux Pas and the MD grey matter volume with STATISTICA version 10 (StatSoft, Inc., 2011, www.statsoft.com.). Considering the size of our experimental samples (<30), we first applied a previously used strategy [6569]⁠ of combining samples to add greater variance, thereby increasing the statistical power of the study to detect associations. This approach provides knowledge regarding a general association between brain markers and each cognitive task. Complementary to this analysis, non-parametric Spearman correlation tests were also calculated for each separate group to evaluate the critical brain areas for each group. In addition, we selected two control networks, the somatosensory and default mode network (based on the AAL atlas [70], as done in previous research [71], to evaluate the specificity of association with the MD system. Bonferroni correction was applied to control for the multiple comparison problem for each group and index associations (p-value corrected < .01).

Results

Clinical and demographic data for all participants are shown in Table 1. Neuropsychological results are shown in Table 2. Patients and controls were compared for WCST, Verbal Fluency and TMTB using two-tailed t-tests. The MS group showed statistically significant impairments on all three tests from the classical executive battery, as had been expected: WCST, t(75) = −2.8, p = 0.007; Verbal Fluency, t(75) = −2.1, p = 0.041; TMTB, t(75) = 4.13, p<0.001. The MS group was also significantly impaired in the Faux Pas task (t(76) = −5.2, p<0.001). Unexpectedly, no differences were found between groups in the Hotel task (t(70) = 1.5, p = 0.148).

thumbnail
Table 1. Clinical and demographic data for MS patients and controls.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0231868.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Patient and control scores, average within-group correlation with g, and significance of group differences for each task.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0231868.t002

All three classical executive tests showed correlation with Matrix Reasoning. The average within-group correlations with Matrix Reasoning, after combining data from patients and controls, were r = 0.322 for WCST, r = 0.228 for Verbal Fluency, and r = 0.575 for TMTB (Table 2). Higher Matrix Reasoning values were associated with better performance in all three executive tasks, as shown in the scatter plots (Fig 1); beyond this linear regression, there was no apparent additional group effect. For a better assessment, the two groups were again compared, but Matrix Reasoning was taken as a covariate (ANCOVA). After this, significant differences between patients and controls in all three classical executive tasks were no longer found: for WCST, F = 1.784, p = 0.186; for Verbal Fluency, F = 0.767, p = 0.384; and for TMTB, F = 3.593, p = 0.062 (Table 2). This suggests that, for the classical executive tasks, frontal deficits were largely explained by fluid intelligence. On the contrary, for the Faux Pas task significant group differences remained after including Matrix Reasoning as a covariate (ANCOVA): F = 18.300, p<0.001 (Table 2).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Performance in classical executive tests.

Scatter plots relating performance in (A) the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), (B) Verbal Fluency, (C) Trail Making Test part B (TMTB), (D) Faux Pas, and (E) Hotel task to Matrix Reasoning for patients with multiples sclerosis (circles) and controls (squares). Regression lines (grey for multiple sclerosis and black for controls) reflect the correlation values for each group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0231868.g001

Since it has been suggested that fatigue and depression in MS patients might explain their cognitive deficits [13,72], we also ran ANCOVAs taking either fatigue or depression as the covariate instead of Matrix Reasoning. In the case of fatigue, the significant differences between groups for all tasks remained, while in the case of depression significant differences between groups remained for all tests except for Verbal fluency (Table 2).

Grey matter analysis

When patients and controls were combined, we found a significant correlation between the MD system grey matter volume and Matrix Reasoning Raw scores. Non-significant correlations were found between MD system grey matter volume and the Faux Pas total score. When correlations were calculated separately for the control and the patient groups, the correlation between MD grey matter and Matrix Reasoning Raw scores was significant only in the control group (Table 3). In the patient group no significant correlations were found in either of the comparisons. The DMN mask showed the same results; however, the somatosensory mask only presented significant correlation in the analysis combining control and patient’s samples. Drawing strong conclusions is hard, however,as grey matter volumes in the 3 networks were themselves strongly correlated (Table 4). This means we had little power to separate their effects.

thumbnail
Table 3. Correlation between grey matter and test scores.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0231868.t003

thumbnail
Table 4. Correlation between grey matter volume of each mask.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0231868.t004

Discussion

Many frontal deficits have been described as a part of the cognitive profile in patients with MS. Impaired performance has been described in classic executive tests and in tests of multitasking and social cognition. Since the frontal lobe has also been related to fluid intelligence, we asked how far frontal deficits can be explained by a general fluid intelligence loss. We found that for the classical executive tests included in this investigation (WCST, Verbal Fluency and TMTB) the differences between MS patients and controls became non-significant once fluid intelligence was introduced as a covariate. On the contrary, differences on the Faux Pas test, which gives a measure of social cognition (Theory of Mind), remained significant after adjustment for fluid intelligence.

As stated above, the results described in this paper are consistent with our previous research. Previously, we have analyzed a variety of psychological and neurological populations, including patients with frontal lobe lesions [32⁠], Parkinson’s disease [33⁠]⁠, Frontotemporal Dementia [34⁠]⁠, Schizophrenia [35⁠]⁠, and Bipolar Disorder [36⁠]⁠. We have found that, in general, deficits in classical executive tasks can largely be explained by a loss in g, but deficits in multitasking and social cognition cannot. Our results in the MS population are compatible with our previous research. Our results are also in line with a recent study showing that, in MS patients, impairment in Theory of Mind (measured both with the “mind in the eyes” and a video test) is independent from impairment in executive functions [73⁠]⁠⁠.

Surprisingly, contrary to what was expected due to the common complaints of MS patients regarding multitasking and previous research [35], in the present study no deficits were found in the Hotel task of multitasking. There is a possibility that this is due to the rather small sample size of our study. Our results are also in line with a recent study that showed through cluster analysis that in MS patients impairment in Theory of Mind (measured both with the “mind in the eyes” and a video test) is independent from impairment in executive functions [73]⁠⁠.

Regarding grey matter volumes, we searched for a link between MS cognitive deficits and the MD system [37⁠]⁠⁠. When taking the control group alone, we see that Matrix Reasoning correlates with MD and DMN grey matter volume, while Faux Pas scores do not. The lack of specificity in the association between Matrix Reasoning and grey matter networks could be related to the moderate sample size of our study. Given that grey matter volumes of each network were highly correlated with one another, very large samples would be needed to have any real power to separate the specific contribution of each network. Nevertheless, the significant association between MD grey matter and the Matrix Reasoning score, together with the robust absence of association between Faux Pax and Matrix Reasoning scores, supports our previous findings regarding fluid intelligence and social cognition tests. Future and larger studies should be performed to corroborate this preliminary evidence.

Though the frontal lobes contribute to multiple cognitive functions, separating those functions has remained difficult to achieve. Taking into account the data from this paper together with our previous results on other neurological and psychiatric conditions, there seems to be a parcellation of cognitive functions based on the role of fluid intelligence. Here, as in our previous research, cognitive deficits in the set of classical executive tests were largely explained by a loss in fluid intelligence (no statistical deficit remained after the effects of fluid intelligence were partialled out). In contrast, for the social cognition test, deficits could not to be explained by a loss in fluid intelligence, since they remained after taking fluid intelligence as a covariate. Thus, we propose that deficits in classical executive tests might be explained by damage to the distributed frontoparietal MD system, likely including its white matter connections [74], although from our results we cannot rule out a possible relation to DMN or the somatosensory network. On the other hand, coherently with previous literature, deficits in social cognition may be related to impaired function in the anterior prefrontal cortex, outside the MD system (e.g. [7578]).

Given the wide range of data published on the effect of fatigue in the neuropsychological evaluation of MS patients, we asked whether fatigue, instead of fluid intelligence, could lead to poor performance in cognitive tasks. Our results show that fatigue levels could not explain the impairments observed, while fluid intelligence losses could. This is in agreement with several studies [1618,73,79,80] that reported no effect of fatigue on cognitive tasks. Regarding depression, it could only explain part of the impairment, specifically in the verbal fluency task, while the statistical differences for the other tests remained, supporting thus the idea that fluid intelligence, rather than depression, is strongly linked to the performance in classical excecutive tests.

Our data could have interesting implications for using and interpreting the performance of MS patients on classical executive tests, such as WCST, Verbal Fluency, and TMTB. Deficits detected by these tests may not be related to their cognitive content in particular, but rather to a general cognitive loss. For a comprehensive cognitive assessment, MS patients should go through fluid intelligence tests and, separately, through social cognition tests. In our view, this approach would allow healthcare providers to find potential deficits inside and outside the MD system, providing them with a more complete picture of each patient’s cognitive disabilities. It should be noted that, since the present study only dealt with RRMS, the suggested approach cannot be extrapolated to other clinical forms of MS (Primary Progressive MS, Secondary Progressive MS, and Clinically-isolated Syndromes) until future studies indicate whether similar conclusions also apply to those patient populations.

Various limitations can be pointed out for the present study. Evidently the small sample size has limited our ability to draw strong conclusions in our imaging data. Further studies need to pursue the investigation of MS cognitive deficits and fluid intelligence using imaging studies with larger sample sizes. Additionally, the fact that only patients with RRMS were included in the present study, make our findings nor generalizable to populations with other forms of the disease. Finally, although three of the most classical executive tests have been investigated in the present study, the relationship between fluid intelligence and other executive tests should be further studied, in this and in other forms of multiple sclerosis.

References

  1. 1. Thompson AJ, Baranzini SE, Geurts J, Hemmer B, Ciccarelli O. Multiple sclerosis. The Lancet. 2018.
  2. 2. Peyser JM, Edwards KR, Poser CM, Filskov SB. Cognitive function in patients with multiple sclerosis. Arch Neurol [Internet]. 1980 Sep;37(9):577–9. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7417060 pmid:7417060
  3. 3. Heaton RK, Nelson LM, Thompson DS, Burks JS, Franklin GM. Neuropsychological findings in relapsing-remitting and chronic-progressive multiple sclerosis. J Consult Clin Psychol [Internet]. 1985 Feb;53(1):103–10. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3980815 pmid:3980815
  4. 4. McIntosh-Michaelis SA, Roberts MH, Wilkinson SM, Diamond ID, McLellan DL, Martin JP, et al. The prevalence of cognitive impairment in a community survey of multiple sclerosis. Br J Clin Psychol [Internet]. 1991 Nov;30 (Pt 4):333–48. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1777755
  5. 5. Rao SM, Leo GJ, Bernardin L, Unverzagt F. Cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. I. Frequency, patterns, and prediction. Neurology [Internet]. 1991 May;41(5):685–91. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2027484 pmid:2027484
  6. 6. Chiaravalloti ND, DeLuca J. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Lancet Neurol [Internet]. 2008 Dec;7(12):1139–51. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19007738 pmid:19007738
  7. 7. Stuss DT, Gow CA, Hetherington CR. “No longer Gage”: frontal lobe dysfunction and emotional changes. J Consult Clin Psychol [Internet]. 1992 Jun;60(3):349–59. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1619089 pmid:1619089
  8. 8. Swirsky-Sacchetti T, Mitchell DR, Seward J, Gonzales C, Lublin F, Knobler R, et al. Neuropsychological and structural brain lesions in multiple sclerosis: a regional analysis. Neurology [Internet]. 1992 Jul;42(7):1291–5. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1620336 pmid:1620336
  9. 9. Foong J, Rozewicz L, Quaghebeur G, Davie CA, Kartsounis LD, Thompson AJ, et al. Executive function in multiple sclerosis. The role of frontal lobe pathology. Brain [Internet]. 1997 Jan;120 (Pt 1:15–26. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9055794
  10. 10. Arnett PA, Rao SM, Bernardin L, Grafman J, Yetkin FZ, Lobeck L. Relationship between frontal lobe lesions and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in patients with multiple sclerosis. Neurology [Internet]. 1994 Mar;44(3 Pt 1):420–5. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8145908 pmid:8145908
  11. 11. Filley CM. White matter and behavioral neurology. Ann N Y Acad Sci [Internet]. 2005 Dec;1064:162–83. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16394155 pmid:16394155
  12. 12. Roca M, Torralva T, Meli F, Fiol M, Calcagno M, Carpintiero S, et al. Cognitive deficits in multiple sclerosis correlate with changes in fronto-subcortical tracts. Mult Scler J [Internet]. 2008 Apr;14(3):364–9. Available from: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/1352458507084270
  13. 13. Heesen C, Schulz KH, Fiehler J, Von der Mark U, Otte C, Jung R, et al. Correlates of cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. Brain Behav Immun [Internet]. 2010;24(7):1148–55. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2010.05.006 pmid:20621641
  14. 14. Kern KC, Gold SM, Lee B, Montag M, Horsfall J, O’Connor MF, et al. Thalamic-hippocampal-prefrontal disruption in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. NeuroImage Clin [Internet]. 2015;8:440–7. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2014.12.015 pmid:26106524
  15. 15. Krupp LB, Elkins LE. Fatigue and declines in cognitive functioning in multiple sclerosis. Neurology [Internet]. 2000 Oct 10;55(7):934–9. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11061247 pmid:11061247
  16. 16. Paul RH, Beatty WW, Schneider R, Blanco CR, Hames KA. Cognitive and physical fatigue in multiple sclerosis: relations between self-report and objective performance. Appl Neuropsychol [Internet]. 1998;5(3):143–8. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16318452 pmid:16318452
  17. 17. Parmenter BA, Denney DR, Lynch SG. The cognitive performance of patients with multiple sclerosis during periods of high and low fatigue. Mult Scler [Internet]. 2003 Mar;9(2):111–8. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12708805 pmid:12708805
  18. 18. Bailey A, Channon S, Beaumont JG. The relationship between subjective fatigue and cognitive fatigue in advanced multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler [Internet]. 2007 Jan;13(1):73–80. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17294614 pmid:17294614
  19. 19. Calabrese M, De Stefano N, Atzori M, Bernardi V, Mattisi I, Barachino L, et al. Detection of cortical inflammatory lesions by double inversion recovery magnetic resonance imaging in patients with multiple sclerosis. Arch Neurol [Internet]. 2007 Oct;64(10):1416–22. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17923625 pmid:17923625
  20. 20. Calabrese M, Rocca MA, Atzori M, Mattisi I, Favaretto A, Perini P, et al. A 3-year magnetic resonance imaging study of cortical lesions in relapse-onset multiple sclerosis. Ann Neurol [Internet]. 2010 Mar;67(3):376–83. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20373349 pmid:20373349
  21. 21. Hojjat S-P, Cantrell CG, Vitorino R, Feinstein A, Shirzadi Z, MacIntosh BJ, et al. Regional reduction in cortical blood flow among cognitively impaired adults with relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis patients. Mult Scler [Internet]. 2016;1–8. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26754799
  22. 22. Houtchens MK, Benedict RHB, Killiany R, Sharma J, Jaisani Z, Singh B, et al. Thalamic atrophy and cognition in multiple sclerosis. Neurology [Internet]. 2007 Sep 18;69(12):1213–23. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17875909 pmid:17875909
  23. 23. Calabrese M, Rinaldi F, Grossi P, Mattisi I, Bernardi V, Favaretto A, et al. Basal ganglia and frontal/parietal cortical atrophy is associated with fatigue in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler [Internet]. 2010 Oct;16(10):1220–8. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20670981 pmid:20670981
  24. 24. Minagar A, Barnett MH, Benedict RHB, Pelletier D, Pirko I, Sahraian MA, et al. The thalamus and multiple sclerosis: modern views on pathologic, imaging, and clinical aspects. Neurology [Internet]. 2013 Jan 8;80(2):210–9. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23296131 pmid:23296131
  25. 25. Debernard L, Melzer TR, Alla S, Eagle J, Van Stockum S, Graham C, et al. Deep grey matter MRI abnormalities and cognitive function in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Psychiatry Res—Neuroimaging [Internet]. 2015;234(3):352–61. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.10.004
  26. 26. Schoonheim MM, Hulst HE, Brandt RB, Strik M, Wink AM, Uitdehaag BMJ, et al. Thalamus structure and function determine severity of cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Neurology [Internet]. 2015 Feb 24;84(8):776–83. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25616483 pmid:25616483
  27. 27. Damjanovic D, Valsasina P, Rocca MA, Stromillo ML, Gallo A, Enzinger C, et al. Hippocampal and Deep Gray Matter Nuclei Atrophy Is Relevant for Explaining Cognitive Impairment in MS: A Multicenter Study. Am J Neuroradiol [Internet]. 2016 Sep 29;1–7. Available from: http://www.ajnr.org/cgi/doi/10.3174/ajnr.A4952
  28. 28. Rossi F, Giorgio A, Battaglini M, Stromillo ML, Portaccio E, Goretti B, et al. Relevance of brain lesion location to cognition in relapsing multiple sclerosis. PLoS One [Internet]. 2012;7(11):e44826. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23144775 pmid:23144775
  29. 29. Spearman C. “General Intelligence,” Objectively Determined and Measured. Am J Psychol [Internet]. 1904;15(2):201–92. Available from: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.2307/1412107
  30. 30. Duncan J, Seitz RJ, Kolodny J, Bor D, Herzog H, Ahmed A, et al. A Neural Basis for General Intelligence. Science (80-) [Internet]. 2000 Jul 21 [cited 2014 Jun 5];289(5478):457–60. Available from: http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/doi/10.1126/science.289.5478.457
  31. 31. Duncan J. The structure of cognition: attentional episodes in mind and brain. Neuron [Internet]. 2013 Oct 2 [cited 2014 May 24];80(1):35–50. Available from: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3791406&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract pmid:24094101
  32. 32. Roca M, Parr A, Thompson R, Woolgar A, Torralva T, Antoun N, et al. Executive function and fluid intelligence after frontal lobe lesions. Brain [Internet]. 2010 Jan [cited 2014 Jun 4];133(Pt 1):234–47. Available from: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2801324&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract pmid:19903732
  33. 33. Roca M, Manes F, Chade a, Gleichgerrcht E, Gershanik O, Arévalo GG, et al. The relationship between executive functions and fluid intelligence in Parkinson’s disease. Psychol Med [Internet]. 2012 Nov [cited 2014 Jun 3];42(11):2445–52. Available from: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3466050&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract pmid:22440401
  34. 34. Roca M, Manes F, Gleichgerrcht E, Watson P, Ibáñez A, Thompson R, et al. Intelligence and executive functions in frontotemporal dementia. Neuropsychologia [Internet]. 2013 Mar [cited 2014 Jun 18];51(4):725–30. Available from: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3610016&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract pmid:23347963
  35. 35. Roca M, Manes F, Cetkovich M, Bruno D, Ibáñez A, Torralva T, et al. The relationship between executive functions and fluid intelligence in schizophrenia. Front Behav Neurosci [Internet]. 2014 Jan [cited 2014 Jun 18];8(February):46. Available from: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3932409&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract
  36. 36. Goitia B, Manes F, Torralva T, Sigman M, Duncan J, Cetkovich M, et al. The relationship between executive functions and fluid intelligence in euthymic Bipolar Disorder patients. Psychiatry Res. 2017;257.
  37. 37. Woolgar A, Parr A, Cusack R, Thompson R, Nimmo-Smith I, Torralva T, et al. Fluid intelligence loss linked to restricted regions of damage within frontal and parietal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107(33):14899–902. pmid:20679241
  38. 38. Poser CM, Paty DW, Scheinberg L, McDonald WI, Davis FA, Ebers GC, et al. New diagnostic criteria for multiple sclerosis: Guidelines for research protocols. Ann Neurol. 1983;
  39. 39. Thompson AJ, Banwell BL, Barkhof F, Carroll WM, Coetzee T, Comi G, et al. Diagnosis of multiple sclerosis: 2017 revisions of the McDonald criteria. The Lancet Neurology. 2018.
  40. 40. Kurtzke JF. Rating neurologic impairment in multiple sclerosis: an expanded disability status scale (EDSS). Neurology [Internet]. 1983 Nov;33(11):1444–52. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6685237 pmid:6685237
  41. 41. Cutter GR, Baier ML, Rudick RA, Cookfair DL, Fischer JS, Petkau J, et al. Development of a multiple sclerosis functional composite as a clinical trial outcome measure. Brain [Internet]. 1999 May;122 (Pt 5:871–82. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10355672
  42. 42. Beck AT, Steer RA, Ball R, Ranieri W. Comparison of Beck Depression Inventories -IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients. J Pers Assess [Internet]. 1996 Dec;67(3):588–97. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8991972 pmid:8991972
  43. 43. Kos D, Kerckhofs E, Carrea I, Verza R, Ramos M, Jansa J. Evaluation of the Modified Fatigue Impact Scale in four different European countries. Mult Scler [Internet]. 2005 Feb;11(1):76–80. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15732270 pmid:15732270
  44. 44. Burin DI, Jorge RE, Arizaga RA, Paulsen JS. Estimation of premorbid intelligence: the word accentuation test—Buenos Aires version. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol [Internet]. 2000 Oct [cited 2014 Jun 20];22(5):677–85. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11094402 pmid:11094402
  45. 45. Nelson H, Willison J. The Revised National Adult Reading Test—Test manual. Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson; 1991.
  46. 46. Wechsler D. WAIS-III. Escala de inteligencia para adultos de Wechsler. Buenos Aires. 2002.
  47. 47. Nelson HE. A modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects. Cortex [Internet]. 1976 Dec [cited 2014 Jun 25];12(4):313–24. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1009768 pmid:1009768
  48. 48. Benton A, Hamsher K, Sivan A. Multilingual aphasia examination: manual of instructions. Washington: AJA; 1994.
  49. 49. Partington JE, Leiter GR. Partington’s Pathways Test. Psychol Serv Cebter J. 1949;1:11–20.
  50. 50. Manly T, Hawkins K, Evans J, Woldt K, Robertson IH. Rehabilitation of executive function: facilitation of effective goal management on complex tasks using periodic auditory alerts. Neuropsychologia [Internet]. 2002 Jan [cited 2014 Jun 25];40(3):271–81. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11684160 pmid:11684160
  51. 51. Torralva T, Roca M, Gleichgerrcht E, Bekinschtein T, Manes F. A neuropsychological battery to detect specific executive and social cognitive impairments in early frontotemporal dementia. Brain [Internet]. 2009 May [cited 2014 May 29];132(5):1299–309. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19336463
  52. 52. Stone VE, Baron-Cohen S, Knight RT. Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. J Cogn Neurosci [Internet]. 1998 Sep [cited 2014 Jun 12];10(5):640–56. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9802997 pmid:9802997
  53. 53. Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Voxel-based morphometry—the methods. Neuroimage [Internet]. 2000 Jun;11(6 Pt 1):805–21. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10860804 pmid:10860804
  54. 54. Couto B, Manes F, Montañés P, Matallana D, Reyes P, Velasquez M, et al. Structural neuroimaging of social cognition in progressive non-fluent aphasia and behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia. Front Hum Neurosci [Internet]. 2013;7:467. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23966929 pmid:23966929
  55. 55. García-Cordero I, Sedeño L, Fraiman D, Craiem D, de la Fuente LA, Salamone P, et al. Stroke and Neurodegeneration Induce Different Connectivity Aberrations in the Insula. Stroke [Internet]. 2015 Sep;46(9):2673–7. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26185182 pmid:26185182
  56. 56. García AM, Abrevaya S, Kozono G, Cordero IG, Córdoba M, Kauffman MA, et al. The cerebellum and embodied semantics: evidence from a case of genetic ataxia due to STUB1 mutations. J Med Genet [Internet]. 2017 Feb;54(2):114–24. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27811304 pmid:27811304
  57. 57. Abrevaya S, Sedeño L, Fitipaldi S, Pineda D, Lopera F, Buritica O, et al. The Road Less Traveled: Alternative Pathways for Action-Verb Processing in Parkinson’s Disease. J Alzheimers Dis [Internet]. 2017;55(4):1429–35. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27834777 pmid:27834777
  58. 58. Good CD, Johnsrude IS, Ashburner J, Henson RN, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage [Internet]. 2001 Jul;14(1 Pt 1):21–36. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11525331 pmid:11525331
  59. 59. Fedorenko E, Duncan J, Kanwisher N. Broad domain generality in focal regions of frontal and parietal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A [Internet]. 2013 Oct 8 [cited 2014 Jun 7];110(41):16616–21. Available from: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3799302&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract pmid:24062451
  60. 60. Wolk DA, Price JC, Saxton JA, Snitz BE, James JA, Lopez OL, et al. Amyloid imaging in mild cognitive impairment subtypes. Ann Neurol [Internet]. 2009 May;65(5):557–68. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19475670 pmid:19475670
  61. 61. Ansell EB, Rando K, Tuit K, Guarnaccia J, Sinha R. Cumulative adversity and smaller gray matter volume in medial prefrontal, anterior cingulate, and insula regions. Biol Psychiatry [Internet]. 2012 Jul 1;72(1):57–64. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22218286 pmid:22218286
  62. 62. Rando K, Tuit K, Hannestad J, Guarnaccia J, Sinha R. Sex differences in decreased limbic and cortical grey matter volume in cocaine dependence: a voxel-based morphometric study. Addict Biol [Internet]. 2013 Jan;18(1):147–60. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23167305 pmid:23167305
  63. 63. Bang L, Rø Ø, Endestad T. Normal gray matter volumes in women recovered from anorexia nervosa: a voxel-based morphometry study. BMC Psychiatry [Internet]. 2016 May 13;16:144. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27177782 pmid:27177782
  64. 64. Heinen R, Bouvy WH, Mendrik AM, Viergever MA, Biessels GJ, de Bresser J. Robustness of Automated Methods for Brain Volume Measurements across Different MRI Field Strengths. PLoS One [Internet]. 2016;11(10):e0165719. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27798694 pmid:27798694
  65. 65. Irish M, Piguet O, Hodges JR, Hornberger M. Common and unique gray matter correlates of episodic memory dysfunction in frontotemporal dementia and alzheimer’s disease. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;
  66. 66. O’Callaghan C, Bertoux M, Irish M, Shine JM, Wong S, Spiliopoulos L, et al. Fair play: Social norm compliance failures in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia. Brain. 2016;
  67. 67. Melloni M, Billeke P, Baez S, Hesse E, De La Fuente L, Forno G, et al. Your perspective and my benefit: multiple lesion models of self-other integration strategies during social bargaining. Brain. 2016;
  68. 68. García-Cordero I, Sedeño L, de la Fuente L, Slachevsky A, Forno G, Klein F, et al. Feeling, learning from and being aware of inner states: Interoceptive dimensions in neurodegeneration and stroke. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2016;
  69. 69. Sollberger M, Stanley CM, Wilson SM, Gyurak A, Beckman V, Growdon M, et al. Neural basis of interpersonal traits in neurodegenerative diseases. Neuropsychologia. 2009;
  70. 70. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage [Internet]. 2002 Jan;15(1):273–89. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11771995 pmid:11771995
  71. 71. Sedeño L, Couto B, García-Cordero I, Melloni M, Baez S, Morales Sepúlveda JP, et al. Brain Network Organization and Social Executive Performance in Frontotemporal Dementia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc [Internet]. 2016 Feb;22(2):250–62. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26888621 pmid:26888621
  72. 72. Denney DR, Lynch SG, Parmenter B a, Horne N. Cognitive impairment in relapsing and primary progressive multiple sclerosis: mostly a matter of speed. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10(7):948–56. pmid:15803558
  73. 73. Batista S, Freitas S, Afonso A, Macário C, Sousa L, Cunha L, et al. Theory of Mind and Executive Functions are Dissociated in Multiple Sclerosis. Arch Clin Neuropsychol [Internet]. 2018 Aug 1;33(5):541–51. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29088343 pmid:29088343
  74. 74. Gläscher J, Rudrauf D, Colom R, Paul LK, Tranel D, Damasio H, et al. Distributed neural system for general intelligence revealed by lesion mapping. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A [Internet]. 2010 Mar 9;107(10):4705–9. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20176936 pmid:20176936
  75. 75. Gilbert SJ, Spengler S, Simons JS, Steele JD, Lawrie SM, Frith CD, et al. Functional specialization within rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10): a meta-analysis. J Cogn Neurosci [Internet]. 2006;18(6):932–48. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16839301 pmid:16839301
  76. 76. Gilbert SJ, Williamson IDM, Dumontheil I, Simons JS, Frith CD, Burgess PW. Distinct regions of medial rostral prefrontal cortex supporting social and nonsocial functions. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci [Internet]. 2007 Sep;2(3):217–26. Available from: https://academic.oup.com/scan/article-lookup/doi/10.1093/scan/nsm014 pmid:18985143
  77. 77. Sommer M, Döhnel K, Sodian B, Meinhardt J, Thoermer C, Hajak G. Neural correlates of true and false belief reasoning. Neuroimage [Internet]. 2007 Apr;35(3):1378–84. Available from: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1053811907000468 pmid:17376703
  78. 78. Roca M, Torralva T, Gleichgerrcht E, Woolgar A, Thompson R, Duncan J, et al. The role of Area 10 (BA10) in human multitasking and in social cognition: a lesion study. Neuropsychologia [Internet]. 2011 Nov [cited 2014 Jun 21];49(13):3525–31. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21930138 pmid:21930138
  79. 79. Nunnari D, De Cola MC, D’Aleo G, Rifici C, Russo M, Sessa E, et al. Impact of depression, fatigue, and global measure of cortical volume on cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Biomed Res Int. 2015;2015:11–3.
  80. 80. Matias-Guiu JA, Cortés-Martínez A, Valles-Salgado M, Oreja-Guevara C, Pytel V, Montero P, et al. Functional Components of Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis: A Cross-Sectional Investigation. Front Neurol [Internet]. 2017;8:643. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29234305 pmid:29234305