Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Visual preference for social vs. non-social images in young children with autism spectrum disorders. An eye tracking study

  • Julia Vacas ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Julia Vacas, Adoración Antolí, Araceli Sánchez-Raya, Carolina Pérez-Dueñas, Fátima Cuadrado

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Resources, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    l72varuj@uco.es

    Affiliations Department of Psychology, University of Cordoba, Cordoba, Andalusia, Spain, Maimonides Biomedical Research Institute of Cordoba (IMIBIC), Cordoba, Andalusia, Spain, Reina Sofía University Hospital of Cordoba, Cordoba, Andalusia, Spain

  • Adoración Antolí ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Julia Vacas, Adoración Antolí, Araceli Sánchez-Raya, Carolina Pérez-Dueñas, Fátima Cuadrado

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Psychology, University of Cordoba, Cordoba, Andalusia, Spain, Maimonides Biomedical Research Institute of Cordoba (IMIBIC), Cordoba, Andalusia, Spain, Reina Sofía University Hospital of Cordoba, Cordoba, Andalusia, Spain, Early Childhood Care Centre, University of Cordoba, Cordoba, Andalusia, Spain

  • Araceli Sánchez-Raya ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Julia Vacas, Adoración Antolí, Araceli Sánchez-Raya, Carolina Pérez-Dueñas, Fátima Cuadrado

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Psychology, University of Cordoba, Cordoba, Andalusia, Spain, Maimonides Biomedical Research Institute of Cordoba (IMIBIC), Cordoba, Andalusia, Spain, Reina Sofía University Hospital of Cordoba, Cordoba, Andalusia, Spain, Early Childhood Care Centre, University of Cordoba, Cordoba, Andalusia, Spain

  • Carolina Pérez-Dueñas ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Julia Vacas, Adoración Antolí, Araceli Sánchez-Raya, Carolina Pérez-Dueñas, Fátima Cuadrado

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Psychology, University of Cordoba, Cordoba, Andalusia, Spain, Maimonides Biomedical Research Institute of Cordoba (IMIBIC), Cordoba, Andalusia, Spain, Reina Sofía University Hospital of Cordoba, Cordoba, Andalusia, Spain

  • Fátima Cuadrado

    Contributed equally to this work with: Julia Vacas, Adoración Antolí, Araceli Sánchez-Raya, Carolina Pérez-Dueñas, Fátima Cuadrado

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Psychology, University of Cordoba, Cordoba, Andalusia, Spain, Maimonides Biomedical Research Institute of Cordoba (IMIBIC), Cordoba, Andalusia, Spain, Reina Sofía University Hospital of Cordoba, Cordoba, Andalusia, Spain

Abstract

Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) are associated to social attention (SA) impairments. A gaze bias to non-social objects over faces has been proposed as an early marker of ASD. This bias may be related to the concomitant circumscribed interests (CI), which question the role of competing objects in this atypical visual behavior. The aim of this study was to compare visual attention patterns to social and non-social images in young children with ASD and matched typical controls (N = 36; age range 41–73 months) assessing the role of emotion in facial stimuli and the type of competing object. A paired preference task was designed pairing happy, angry, and neutral faces with two types of objects (related or not related to autism CI). Eye tracking data were collected, and three indexes were considered as dependent variables: prioritization (attentional orientation), preference, and duration (sustained attention). Results showed that both groups had similar visual pattern to faces (prioritization, more attention and longer visits to faces paired with objects non-related to their CI); however, the ASD group attended to faces significantly less than controls. Children with ASD showed an emotional bias (late orientation to angry faces and typical preference for happy faces). Finally, objects related to their CI captured attention in both groups, significantly reducing SA in children with ASD. Atypical SA is present in young children with ASD regardless the competing non-social object. Identifying strengths and difficulties in SA in this population may have substantial repercussion for early diagnosis, intervention, and ultimately prognosis.

Introduction

Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) comprise a set of neurodevelopmental and pervasive conditions in which social-emotional and communication impairments along with restricted and repetitive behaviors are core symptoms [14]. The wide heterogeneity of the autism spectrum hampers early diagnosis, which is considered an important predictor of future outcomes and prognosis as it leads to the early intervention and the deployment of appropriate supports [2, 510]. In this sense, research is claiming for the study of new markers to improve the early identification of this disorder. The study of social attention (SA, the allocation of visual attention to social stimuli or socially relevant content of scenes instead of non-social elements) has received increasing attention of research on ASD in the last decades. Likewise, new approaches and methodologies such as eye tracking have become widespread due to their significant contribution to the developmental understanding of SA.

Eye tracking methodology allows to analyse individuals’ gaze behavior by tracing and monitoring their eye movements during image visualisation. This methodology draws on corneal reflection technology, so it yields reliable attentional markers in a non-invasive way [1113]. This asset alongside its high sensitivity to detect subtle biases in visual emotion processing and disentangle the core mechanisms of facial emotional expression decoding have increased the use of eye tracking in research on ASD [1417].

Most studies on SA in children with ASD with eye tracking have reported reduced attention to social images or the social content of visual scenes in favour of an attentional bias to non-social stimuli [15, 1824]. This atypical attention to social events has been found in toddlers with ASD from the first year of life and is usually accompanied by difficulties in attention disengagement [15, 25] and lack of arousal modulation towards others’ emotions [24]. Moreover, visual preference for geometric patterns instead of social images has been suggested as a potential marker for early diagnosis of ASD [2629]. The scanning pattern (also known as gaze or looking pattern, the sequence of looking shifts during images visualization) has also been studied in children with ASD. Findings in this area yielded a dispersed scanning pattern among individuals with ASD when viewing social images, that is, they displayed scattered eye movements across the scene when it involved a social situation [30]. In the study on SA, face deserves special attention as it is an important gateway to perceive, recognize, and understand others’ inner emotional states [31, 32]. Likewise, attention to faces at early ages is a natural mechanism which leads to brain specialization and, consequently, to social expertise [3235]. Children with ASD have showed reduced attention to faces [24, 3638]. However, increased attentional orientation to emotional faces rather than to neutral ones was found in this population (although to a lesser extent than the controls). This may suggest that children with ASD, similar to their typically developing (TD) peers, have emotional sensitivity [37, 39]. Moreover, some studies have reported a bias toward positive emotions and better recognition rates of these emotions in children with ASD, as well as in TD children [4042]. Familiarity has also been yielded as a potential trigger of pupil reactivity and visual attention to faces in children with ASD [39]. These results suggest that attention to faces is not completely impaired in children with ASD, as it can be modulating considering some variables. Regarding the attention to the core facial features, specific results related to the eyes and the mouth are still inconsistent when considering studies independently; however, sound current reviews agree that children with ASD show atypical face processing, which means that they scan faces differently than TD children [16, 43, 44]. This atypicality has been found in toddlers with ASD as young as two years [45], which have led some researchers to suggest that these children may have reduced their social learning input from early ages, which impact on their brain specialization and, consequently, on their social development at later ages [25, 32, 35, 4346].

The role of competing objects in SA

The saliency of competing non-social objects plays a crucial role when assessing SA in children with ASD [25, 46, 47]. It has been suggested that the restricted and recurring bias to non-social elements reported in this population may be related to the same cognitive mechanisms involved in the repetitive behaviors associated to this disorder, mainly the circumscribed interests (CI, [4648]). Despite the relevance of the topic, few studies have addressed the role of competing objects and their interaction with social stimuli in SA in children with ASD. A restricted looking behaviour toward CI-related objects (CIO, not to non-related ones or to social stimuli) has been found in young children [46] and in school-aged children with ASD [25]. This behavior was described as circumscribed, perseverative, and detail oriented in both studies.

Additionally, Sasson and Touchstone assessed SA in young children with ASD (age range 24–62 months) who performed a paired preference task where faces with different emotions (happiness, sadness, fear, anger, neutral) were paired with CIO and non-CI-related objects (non-CIO) [47]. No difference between emotions was found in this study but their findings revealed that children with ASD showed the same attentional bias towards faces as their TD peers when the competing non-social objects were unrelated to their CI (non-CIO), thus only CIOs were able to capture their attention more than faces.

Given that the presence of CI is a hallmark of ASD and that this restricted looking behaviour seems to be also specific of the autistic phenotype, the effect of the saliency of competing non-social objects on SA may help identify children with ASD at earlier ages.

Aims and hypotheses

Taken together, previous results showed the high potential of studies on SA in this population as they have yielded some specific markers of the autism spectrum. Thus, the particular role of CIO in SA in children with ASD may also contribute to early diagnosis. Following this rationale and the approach applied in [47], the aim of this study was to compare young children with ASD and their TD peers in terms of SA assessing the role of facial emotions and competing objects in both groups. As in [47], we designed an eye tracking paired preference task where a social stimulus (happy, angry, or neutral face) and a non-social one (CIO or non-CIO) competed for attracting participants’ SA. This experiment was based on the approach applied in [47] in terms of the application of the paired preference paradigm and the eye tracking dependent variables definition; however, both studies differs in aspects of design such as the number of emotions considered ([47] comprised happiness, sadness, anger, fear, and neutral, while this study included happiness, anger, and neutral), the manipulation of emotion intensity (in [47] researchers manipulated that variable, while in this experiment intensity was not considered), and the number of trials per condition ([47] included 1trial, while we repeated each condition 3 times). Based on previous studies, we hypothesized that young children with ASD would show significantly reduced attention to faces compared with objects, which would differentiate them from the TD group (H1). We included faces with different emotional expressions (happiness, anger, and neutral) to see the effect of emotionality in SA. Thus, we expected that both groups would pay more attention to emotional faces, particularly to happy faces (H2), due to the reported emotional sensitivity [37, 39] and bias toward positive emotions in children with ASD [4042]. This would imply that positive emotions had an attracting effect which could be relevant to consider for clinical practice. We also predicted that young children with ASD would pay atypically less attention to faces when they competed with CIO, but typical attention when they competed with non-CIO (H3). Finally, we expected to find a significant bias toward CIO with respect to non-CIO in both groups (H4).

Method

Participants

Thirty-eight pre-schoolers: 19 with ASD (18 boys, 1 girl; Mage = 55.89 months, SDage = 9.40, range = 44–72 months) and 19 TD (18 boys, 1 girl; Mage = 53.53 months, SDage = 9.28, range = 41–72 months) participated in this study. This sample size could detect between-groups differences with 85% of power and an effect size of 0.5, according to a sensitivity analysis in GPower 3.1.9.7 [49, 50]. The clinical group was recruited from centres of early childhood intervention in the province of Córdoba. For the clinical group, inclusion criteria comprised 1) the attendance to a centre of early childhood intervention, 2) having received a thorough assessment by a licensed experienced clinician who had determined the presence of ASD according to the DSM-5 criteria and following the protocol of Infant Mental Health program at a community mental health service, and 3) the absence of any mental or medical condition.

Children of the TD group were recruited from the preschool classes at a public school in Córdoba. Inclusion criteria for this group encompassed not having a history of developmental disorder either now or in the past, as well as the gender and chronological age matching with the clinical group. As in [47], we matched groups on chronological age instead of on developmental age due to the low demand of the task applied, as passive-observational tasks do not require high-level cognitive abilities. We also matched groups on gender to control its potential effect on CI [47, 5153].

Stimuli

Following the approach of [47], the eye tracking paired preference task consisted of pairing social and non-social images to assess visual attention patterns for faces and objects. We paired faces displaying three different emotions (happiness, anger, and neutral) with two types of objects (CIO and non-CIO), which yielded a total of six experimental conditions which were repeated six times using different facial identities (a total of 36 trials). The gender of the faces and the location in the screen were counterbalanced to avoid potential effects of both variables. Thus, 36 pictures from 12 different identities (the same identities were taken for the three emotions, but these were not repeated for the same emotion) were taken from the Amsterdam Dynamic Facial Expression Set (ADFES; [54]). Faces were paired with 36 images of objects (18 CIO and 18 non-CIO, see Fig 1A and 1B). Some of these images were of our own creation by taking pictures of ordinary toys and puzzles used in the Early Childhood Intervention Centre associated to the University of Córdoba, while others were taken from the Pixabay website and free of copyright under the Creative Commons CC0 license. Inclusion criteria for CIO were based on previous studies on CI-related topics and objects in ASD [25, 47, 55]. Thus, CIO belonged to the categories of toys, puzzles, means of transport, animals, and blocks, while non-CIO were plants, furniture, musical instruments, tools, school material, and clothes.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Examples of stimuli and heatmaps in ASD and TD groups.

(A) Example stimulus pairing a happy face and a CIO. (B) Example stimulus pairing a non-CIO and an angry face. (C and D). Heatmaps of the ASD group performance. (E and F) Heatmaps of the TD group performance.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252795.g001

Pairs were designed using power point to readjust images size (to 15 x 15 cm approximately), control the screen location, and remove pictures background to avoid contrast with the black background. The background removal forced us to rescale all pictures so that pairs looked similar in size. Final slides were resized to 1500 x 843 pixels to be correctly displayed in the laptop used to assess children and presentation order was counterbalanced.

Procedure

This study had the approval of the Research Ethics Committee of Córdoba. We contacted with potential candidate centres of early childhood intervention and those interested in participating distributed the project information and the written informed consent between families. Once we collected signed written informed consent from parents or guardians of children, we scheduled the assessment sessions according to families’ and centres’ availability. The assessment sessions took place in children’s natural environment using a quiet room without distractions. In most cases, children were alone with the researcher during the assessment; however, there were some cases in which any parent was present, either for child requirement or for their own request. Assessment sessions were carried out in one single session in all cases, although the option of dividing the evaluation in two sessions had also been offered. The entire process lasted 45 minutes approximately. First, children performed the paired preference task, which lasted 3.6 minutes. Eye tracking data were recorded using Tobii X2-30 (Tobii Technology AB, Stockholm, Sweden) which tracks eye movements at a sampling rate of 30 Hz with a spatial accuracy of 36°. Children were seated at a deemed distance of 60 cm from a 15’screen of a laptop. They were given no other instruction but to look at the screen. The task started with a nine-points calibration with an animated stimulus as target. Calibration process was repeated in those cases where a child failed to complete it. After calibration, the task consisted of visualizing the set of 36 slides showing one face and one object. Each slide was displayed for 5 s. Prior to the presentation of each slide, children viewed an animated fixation point (a cartoon) for 1 s. to drive their attention to the centre of the screen. After the eye tracking task, we assessed receptive vocabulary, affect recognition, and Theory of Mind (ToM) as described in the next section.

Measures

Along with the eye tracking task, receptive vocabulary, affect recognition, and Theory of Mind (ToM) abilities were assessed. using the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Third Edition (PPTV-III, [56]) and the two subtests comprised in the social perception domain of the Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment (NEPSY-II, [57]) to analyse potential correlations among these domains and visual attention patterns in the sample. These scales were chosen due to their wide usability for clinical and research purposes, and the fact that both yield typification data with special groups (including ASD).

PPTV-III is a screening test which assesses vocabulary comprehension in individuals from 2.6 to 90 years. Subjects are presented with an array of four pictures and are asked to choose which one fits to a given word. Individuals are allowed to point the finger at their answer to reduce expressive language demands.

NEPSY-II is a neuropsychological assessment battery which comprises 32 tests to assess six domains (attention and executive functioning, language, memory and learning, sensorimotor, social perception, and visuospatial processing) in children and adolescents from 3 to 16 years. In the present study, only social perception domain was assessed. This domain encompasses affect recognition and ToM. Some participants of the ASD group were not able to perform either any of the two tasks or only the ToM part because of their age or the high linguistic demand required to pass these tasks.

Following the approach of [47], we defined three indexed as dependent variables: 1) Prioritization, time to first fixation to faces to assess attentional orientation, 2) Preference, the proportion of total fixation duration on each facial AOI to compare the distribution of visual attention between faces regarding the competing object, and 3) Duration, mean time per visit to faces to describe the gaze maintenance (sustained attention) in facial stimuli. Statistical tests were performed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences, 25 (SPSS, 25, IBM, Armonk, NY, United States of America).

Data analysis

To analyse visual attention pattern to faces in both groups, we conducted separate repeated measures ANOVAs on each dependent variable (Prioritization, Preference, and Duration), with Group (ASD, control) as the between-group factor and Type of Object (CIO, non-CIO) as the within-group factor. Likewise, to examine visual attention pattern to objects in each group, we again performed separate repeated measures ANOVAs on each dependent variable (Prioritization, Preference, and Duration), with Group (ASD, control) as the between-group factor and Type of Object (CIO, non-CIO) as the within-group factor. As groups significantly differed in receptive vocabulary (see Table 1), PPVT-II standard scores were tentatively included as covariates, but preliminary analyses yielded no effect, therefore this measure was dropped from the final analyses. Effect sizes were also calculated for repeated measures ANOVAs (partial eta-squared, ηp2) considering small (< .01), medium (< .06), or large (< .14) effects. Finally, correlation analyses were carried out to check potential relationships between visual attention pattern and PPVT-III or NEPSY-II scores in any group. Thus, Pearson correlation coefficient was used for PPVT-III scores, while Spearman correlation coefficient was used for NEPSY-II scores as this measure did not fulfil requirements for performing parametric tests.

thumbnail
Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of variables of the study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252795.t001

Results

Descriptive characteristics of variables of the study

All participants assessed were included in the statistical analysis as the eye tracker apparatus collected more than 50% of their fixations (which is a standard cut-point applied in many studies [26, 28, 29, 36, 58]), thus all the 38 children were suitable for the analysis. Sample characteristics are summarized in Table 1. No significant differences were found between groups in age or gender (p > .05). However, ASD and TD groups differed in terms of vocabulary comprehension (PPVT-II standard scores), emotion recognition abilities (NEPSY-II Total Score Affect Recognition) and ToM (Nepsy-II Verbal ToM and Total Score ToM).

Social attention

Prioritization.

No effect for group × type of object interaction was found in any emotion. A main effect for group was only found in angry faces (F(1, 37) = 4.54, p < .04, ηp2 = .11), indicating that children with ASD looked at angry faces significantly later than TD children (see Fig 2B). A main effect for type of object was significant in all emotions independently: happiness (F(1, 37) = 5.17, p < .03, ηp2 = .13), anger (F(1, 37) = 10.45, p < .00, ηp2 = .23), neutral (F(1, 37) = 6.47, p < .02, ηp2 = .15), and in total faces (F(1, 37) = 22.38, p < .00, ηp2 = .38), implying that all participants looked at faces paired with non-CIOs earlier than those paired with CIOs (see Figs 2A and 2B and 3A).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Visual attention patterns in happy and angry faces.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252795.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Visual attention patterns in total faces and objects.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252795.g003

Preference.

No group × type of object interaction effect was yielded in any particular emotion or total faces, but a main effect for group was found in angry (F(1, 37) = 7.40, p < .01, ηp2 = .17), neutral (F(1, 37) = 14.78, p < .00, ηp2 = .29), and total faces (F(1, 37) = 10.29, p < .00, ηp2 = .22), indicating that children with ASD attended to angry and neutral faces substantially less than their TD peers (see Figs 1D, 1F, 2D and 3C). A main effect for type of object also emerged in angry (F(1, 37) = 5.97, p < .02, ηp2 = .14), neutral (F(1, 37) = 16.83, p < .00, ηp2 = .32), and total faces (F(1, 37) = 31.57, p < .00, ηp2 = .47), implying that both groups paid significantly more attention to angry and neutral faces paired with non-CIOs than those paired with CIOs. Any effect was found in happy faces, which suggests that both groups looked at these faces the same amount of time regardless the type of object (see Figs 1C, 1E and 2C).

Duration.

No group × type of object interaction effect was highlighted in any specific emotion or total faces, but a main effect for group was found in all emotions: happy (F(1, 37) = 13.66, p < .00, ηp2 = .28), angry (F(1, 37) = 14.41, p < .00, ηp2 = .29), neutral (F(1, 37) = 11.80, p < .00, ηp2 = .25), and total faces (F(1, 37) = 24.08, p < .00, ηp2 = .40), indicating that children with ASD made significantly shorter visits to all type of faces than their TD peers. A main effect for type of object was also found in neutral (F(1, 37) = 4.62, p < .04, ηp2 = .11), and total faces (F(1, 37) = 6.59, p < .02, ηp2 = .16). This implies that the lack of facial emotion drove both groups to pay more attention to faces paired with non-CIOs than those paired with CIOs; otherwise, when faces displayed any positive or negative emotion, the type of object had no impact in their sustained attention (see Figs 2E, 2F and 3E).

Object attention

Prioritization.

Significant effects were found for group × type of object interaction (F(1, 37) = 12.85, p < .00, ηp2 = .26), group (F(1, 37) = 6.26, p < .02, ηp2 = .15), and type of object (F(1, 37) = 5.08, p < .03, ηp2 = .12), indicating that, relative to TD, children with ASD were significantly faster at focusing on non-CIOs (see Fig 3B). These effects were replicated only when objects were paired with neutral faces, but not when these were paired with happy or angry faces, implying that both groups showed similar orientation to objects when these competed with emotional faces.

Preference.

No group × type of object interaction was found in this index, but a main effect emerged for group (F(1, 37) = 10.29, p < .00, ηp2 = .22), indicating that children with ASD looked at objects substantially longer than their counterparts (see Fig 3D). An effect for type of object was also found (F(1, 37) = 31.57, p < .00, ηp2 = .47), implying that both groups spent more time viewing CIOs than non-CIOs. When considering emotions, these effects were replicated only for happy and neutral faces, but these were marginal for angry faces, which suggests that children with ASD showed typical preference for objects when they competed with angry faces.

Duration.

No significant effect was found at any level in this index, indicating that children with ASD as well as TD made similar visits to objects, regardless its typology (see Fig 3F).

Correlation analysis

To explore whether the visual attention pattern found in each group was related to their performance in the PPVT-III and the NEPSY-II, we conducted correlation analyses. No significant correlation between PPVT-III and any dependent variable was found in any group. Only Spearman correlation coefficient yielded significant values (see Table 2) for Affect Recognition Total Raw Score and prioritization of happy faces (rs(7) = .775, p = .04) in the ASD group. For the TD group, a significant correlation was found in Affect Recognition Total Raw Score and TFF to CIO (rs(19) = .588, p = .01). This data indicates that: 1) the higher Affect Recognition scores children with ASD have, the later they look at happy faces; 2) the higher Affect Recognition scores TD children have, the later they focus on CIO.

thumbnail
Table 2. Spearman correlation between dependent variables and NEPSY-II indexes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252795.t002

Discussion

This study aimed at comparing SA in young children with ASD and TD peers analysing the effect of competing facial emotions and type of objects in their visual attention pattern to faces and objects. Taken together, our results suggest that children with ASD display a visual attention pattern typical in terms of direction (early orientation, longer looking time, and more detailed exploration of faces paired with non-CIOs than those paired with CIOs, and visual preference for CIOs over non-CIOs), but quantitatively atypical. Thus, relative to TD children, children with ASD showed a lack of attention to faces and an excessive visual preference for objects in general.

Our results revealed that children with ASD looked at faces later, during less time and they made shorter visits than their TD peers. In light of these results, our first hypothesis was confirmed as children with ASD differed from TD children in reduced attention to faces. This statement aligns with many studies reporting reduced attention to social images in favour of non-social ones [15, 1824] and, particularly, decreased attention to faces [24, 3638].

We also predicted different effects of competing facial emotions in children’s visual behavior (H2). These differences were found in prioritization and preference indexes during the analysis of the visual attention pattern to faces, confirming emotional sensitivity in children with ASD. Thus, our ASD group looked at angry faces later than the control one, indicating late orientation to angry faces compared to controls. Moreover, happy faces were the only ones in which children with ASD displayed a typical visual preference behavior, which implies that happiness was relatively unimpaired in our ASD sample. Likewise, the effect of the type of object in sustained attention to faces in children with ASD was only significant when objects competed with neutral faces, which suggests that the absence of emotion increases atypical visual behavior in this population. This emotional sensitivity was again replicated in the analysis of visual attention pattern to objects, as children with ASD only showed atypical visual orientation to objects when they competed with neutral faces, otherwise there were no differences between groups, type of object, or interaction. These assumptions are in line with those studies reporting emotional sensitivity [37, 39] and highlighting that negative emotions are compromised in this population, while positive emotions are relatively intact [4042, 59, 60]. This finding is worthy to be considered when designing and developing interventions. Hence, clinicians may avoid negative emotions (which drive to attention disengagement) and foster positive ones (which are attention-getters); this may help involve the child within the activity, increasing the effectiveness and potential benefits of interventions. Emotional sensitivity was not reported in [47], this difference between studies could be due to the fact that we included more essays per condition, which may have contributed to uncover atypicalities in visual behavior to different emotional faces in the ASD group.

Regarding our H3, we expected to find a differential role of the type of object in young children with ASD, which would drive them to display less attention to faces competing with CIO, but typical attention when they competed with non-CIO (H3). This hypothesis was refused in this study as children with ASD paid less attention to faces in both conditions. Moreover, TD children seemed to identify quickly the type of object they were looking at, as they focused on CIOs significantly earlier than on non-CIOs; however, children with ASD did not make this distinction, instead they fixated on both type of objects equally fast. Therefore, the type of object was not what made the greatest impact on SA in children with ASD, but the competing facial emotion was. This disagreement with conclusions from [47] may rely on the age of participants in both studies (being ours older) or the fact that our participants had been receiving early intervention for a deemed mean time of 30 months (SD = 8 months), which implies a previous work on their potential social-emotional deficits. In this sense, future studies should address the effect of early diagnosis and intervention in SA in children with ASD.

We also found a powerful effect of CIOs over non-CIOs in both groups, this effect was found in the three indexes when analysing visual attention pattern to faces, but only in prioritization and preference when considering the visual attention pattern to objects. The lack of differences in duration index during the analysis of visual attention pattern to objects could be partially explained by the fact that the ASD group was more heterogeneous in terms of duration and number of visits to both type of objects. This shows the wide heterogeneity of CI among children with ASD due to their idiosyncratic feature. Due to the close relationship between the duration index and the visual processing style, we may say that children with ASD made less detailed processing of faces compared to their TD peers, but not a different visual processing of objects. Still, these data confirm our H4 suggesting that CIOs disrupt social attention in children with ASD as well as TD. This result is in line with a large body of literature on the catching role of CIOs children with ASD and TD [25, 46, 47, 5153, 55]. Nevertheless, given the fact that children with ASD have difficulties engaging with faces, we think that clinicians should be cautious including CIOs in interventions as they can help engage the child in an activity, but they can also distract him/her from its real purpose.

On the other hand, no significant correlation between vocabulary comprehension or ToM and visual attention pattern was highlighted in any group. Only affect recognition was significantly related to prioritization of happy faces and CIO in the ASD group and TD group, respectively. This indicates that the more proficiency in affect recognition, the later children with ASD look at happy faces and TD children look at CIO. This outcome is reasonable in the case of TD children as affect recognition is closely related to social attention [31, 32, 61], thus more proficiency in affect recognition could imply diminishing attention to non-social stimuli in favour of social ones. However, the pattern found in ASD is surprising and cannot be explained based on existing literature; hence it could stem from the small sample of children with ASD to whom NEPSY-II could be applied.

Finally, we consider some limitations of this study. First of all, the sample size was relatively small, although it is in line with that of related studies [22, 30, 37, 38, 45, 47]; studies with larger samples may help to stress differences between groups and remark patterns within-group. Secondly, we controlled for receptive language, affect recognition, and ToM abilities, but not for expressive language, which would have been interesting given its key role in this population [62]. Moreover, the small number of children with ASD who could not be assessed with the social-emotions subscale of the NEPSY-II may have distorted results on correlations between variables, as some studies have highlighted the close relationship between affect recognition and SA [31, 32, 61] as well as facial emotion recognition and ToM [63, 64] in this population. Alongside these limitations, it is important to be cautious when interpreting results of SA in children with ASD as some variables may impact on them. Among these variables, research has highlighted the following: 1) the stimulus type, being interactive and naturalistic stimuli those which better find differences between clinical and non-clinical groups [18, 65]; 2) the social content of scenes, that is, the more people involved in the scene or the more scene complexity, the less SA deployed by the ASD population [20, 21, 58]; 3) the presence of speech during social interactions, which has been related to slower latencies and less social orientation to faces in children with ASD compared to TD [66]; and 4) the language proficiency, with typical SA in children with ASD and normal language but reduced SA in those with language delayed [62]. For these reasons, futures studies must include larger sample sizes, a more thorough assessment of language profile of the clinical sample, as well as affect recognition and ToM measures, and more comparison groups to contribute to differential diagnosis.

Conclusions

Children with ASD have demonstrated SA atypicalities which may impair their relationship with the social environment and, consequently, their social expertise from early ages. Current eye tracking research on SA has been aimed at identifying these atypicalities in young children, which has substantial repercussion in clinical practice. In this sense, the finding of specific visual attentional markers such as the less detailed processing of faces and the bias to non-social objects in the ASD population are valuable from a diagnosis point of view, but also the emotional sensitivity found in these children is significant from an intervention perspective, as it may help practitioners to guide their interventions emphasizing the role of emotions, taking advantage of those more engaging for children with ASD, and avoiding the most repelling ones. The worthy applicability of this kind of studies alongside the widely reported advantages of early intervention for children future outcomes and development substantiate the need of continuing this line of research enriching it with larger samples, deeper cognitive assessments, and more comparison groups.

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge to all families who agreed that their children took part in this study, as well as the Early Intervention Centres and the school which also accepted our request of participation very kindly.

References

  1. 1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013.
  2. 2. Autism Europe. Prevalence rate of Autism. Belgium: Autism-Europe a.i.s.b.l.; 2015.
  3. 3. Charman T, Baird G. Practitioner Review: Diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder in 2- and 3-year-old children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2002 Mar; 43(3):289–305. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00022 pmid:11944873
  4. 4. World Health Organization. International Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders (ICD-10). Geneva: World Health Organization; 1992.
  5. 5. Bennett TA, Szatmari P, Georgiades K, Hanna S, Janus M, Georgiades S, et al. Language Impairment and Early Social Competence in Preschoolers with Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Comparison of DSM-5 Profile. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014 May; 44:2797–2808. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2138-2 pmid:24865586
  6. 6. Corsello CM. Early Intervention in Autism. Infants Young Child. 2005 Apr-Jun; 18(2):74–85. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001163-200504000-00002
  7. 7. Mandell DS, Novak MM, Zubritsky CD. Factors Associated with Age of Diagnosis Among Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Pediatrics. 2005 Dec; 116(6):1480–6. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2005-0185 pmid:16322174
  8. 8. Nicholas JS, Carpenter LA, King LB, Jenner W, Charles JM. Autism Spectrum Disorders in Preschool-Aged Children: Prevalence and Comparison to a School-Aged Population. Ann Epidemiol. 2009 Nov; 19(11):808–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.annepidem.2009.04.005 pmid:19541501
  9. 9. Özerk K. The Issue of Prevalence of Autism/ASD. International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education. 2016 Dec; 9(2):263–306. Retrieved from: https://www.iejee.com/index.php/IEJEE/article/view/158
  10. 10. Zablotsky B, Colpe LJ, Pringle BA, Kogan MD, Rice C, Blumberg SJ. Age of parental concern, diagnosis, and service initiation among children with autism spectrum disorder. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2017 Jan; 122(1):49–61. https://doi.org/10.1352/1944-7558-122.1.49 pmid:28095057
  11. 11. Aslin RN, McMurray B. Automated Corneal-Reflection Eye Tracking in Infancy: Methodological Developments and Applications to Cognition. Infancy. 2004 Sep; 6(2):155–163. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in0602_1 pmid:33430532
  12. 12. Gredebäck G, Johnson S, von Hofsten C. Eye Tracking in Infancy Research. Dev Neuropsychol. 2009 Dec; 35(1):1–19. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565640903325758
  13. 13. Hepach R, Vaish A, Tomasello M. Novel paradigms to measure variability of behavior in early childhood: posture, gaze, and pupil dilation. Front Psychol. 2015 Jul; 6:12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00858 pmid:25657635
  14. 14. Bolte S, Bartl-Pokorny KD, Jonsson U, Berggren S, Zhang D, Kostrzewa E, et al. How can clinicians detect and treat autism early? Methodological trends of technology use in research. Acta Paediatr. 2015 Oct; 105(2):137–144. https://doi.org/10.1111/apa.13243 pmid:26479859
  15. 15. Falck-Ytter T, Bolte S, Gredeback G. Eye tracking in early autism research. J Neurodev Disord. 2013 Sep; 5:28. https://doi.org/10.1186/1866-1955-5-28 pmid:24069955
  16. 16. Harms MB, Martin A, Wallace GL. Facial EmotionRecognition in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Review of Behavioral and Neuroimaging Studies. Neuropsychol Rev. 2015 Sep; 20(3):290–322. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-010-9138-6
  17. 17. Klin A, Jones W, Schultz R, Volkmar F, Cohen D. Defining and Quantifying the Social Phenotype in Autism. Am J Psychiatry. 2002 Jun; 159:895–908. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.159.6.895 pmid:12042174
  18. 18. Chevallier C, Parish-Morris J, McVey A, Rump KM, Sasson NJ, Herrington JD, et al. Measuring social attention and motivation in autism spectrum disorder using eye-tracking: Stimulus type matters. Autism Res. 2015 Jun; 8(5):620–8. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1479 pmid:26069030
  19. 19. Chita-Tegmark M. Attention Allocation in ASD: a Review and Meta-analysis of Eye-Tracking Studies. Rev J Autism Dev Disord. 2016 Apr; 3:209–223. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40489-016-0077-x
  20. 20. Chita-Tegmark M. Social attention in ASD: A review and meta-analysis of eye-tracking studies. Res Dev Disabil. 2016 Jan; 48:79–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2015.10.011 pmid:26547134
  21. 21. Guillon Q, Hadjikhani N, Baduel S, Roge B. Visual social attention in autism spectrum disorder: Insights from eye tracking studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014 May; 42:279–297. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.013 pmid:24694721
  22. 22. Guillon Q, Roge B, Afzali MH, Baduel S, Kruck J, Hadjikhani N. Intact perception but abnormal orientation towards face-like objects in young children with ASD. Sci Rep. 2016 Feb; 6:22119. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep22119 pmid:26912096
  23. 23. Nakano T, Tanaka K, Endo Y, Yamane Y, Yamamoto T, Nakano Y, et al. Atypical gaze patterns in children and adults with autism spectrum disorders dissociated from developmental changes in gaze behaviour. Proc Biol Sci. 2010 May; 277(1696):2935–2943. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.0587 pmid:20484237
  24. 24. Zantinge G, van Rijn S, Stockmann L, Swaab H. Psychophysiological responses to emotions of others in young children with autism spectrum disorders: Correlates of social functioning. Autism Res. 2017 Apr; 10(9):1499–1509. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1794 pmid:28383171
  25. 25. Sasson NJ, Turner-Brown LM, Holtzclaw TN, Lam KS, Bodfish JW. Children With Autism Demonstrate Circumscribed Attention During Passive Viewing of Complex Social and Nonsocial Picture Arrays. Autism Res. 2008 Feb; 1(1):31–42. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.4 pmid:19360648
  26. 26. Franchini M, Glaser B, Wood de Wilde H, Gentaz E, Eliez S, Schaer M. Social orienting and joint attention in preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders. PLoS ONE. 2017 Jun; 12(6):e0178859. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0178859 pmid:28599002
  27. 27. Moore A, Wozniak M, Yousef A, Barnes CC, Cha D, Courchesne E, et al. The geometric preference subtype in ASD: identifying a consistent, early-emerging phenomenon through eye tracking. Mol Autism. 2018 Mar; 9:19. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-018-0202-z pmid:29581878
  28. 28. Pierce K, Conant D, Hazin R, Stoner R, Desmond J. Preference for Geometric Patterns Early in Life as a Risk Factor for Autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011 Jan; 68(1):101–9. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.113 pmid:20819977
  29. 29. Pierce K, Marinero S, Hazin R, McKenna B, Barnes CC, Malige A. Eye Tracking Reveals Abnormal Visual Preference for Geometric Images as an Early Biomarker of an Autism Spectrum Disorder Sub-type Associated With Increased Symptom Severity. Biol Psychiatry. 2016 Apr; 79(8):657–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.03.032 pmid:25981170
  30. 30. Liberati A, Fadda R, Doneddu G, Congiu S, Javarone MA, Striano T, et al. A Statistical Physics Perspective to Understand Social Visual Attention in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Perception. 2017 Jan; 46(8):889–913. https://doi.org/10.1177/0301006616685976 pmid:28056653
  31. 31. Frith CD, Frith U. The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron. 2006 May; 50(4):531–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.05.001 pmid:16701204
  32. 32. Klin A. In the Eye of the Beholden: Tracking Developmental Psychopathology. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008 Apr; 47(4):362–3. https://doi.org/10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181648dd1 pmid:18356702
  33. 33. Johnson MH. Cortical specialization for higher cognitive functions: Beyond the maturational model. Brain Cogn. 2000 Feb; 42(1):124–7. https://doi.org/10.1006/brcg.1999.1180 pmid:10739617
  34. 34. Nelson CA. The Development and Neural Bases of Face Recognition. Infant Child Dev. 2001 Mar-Jun; 10(1–2):3–18. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.239
  35. 35. Schultz RT. Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: the role of the amygdala and fusiform face area. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2005 Feb; 23(2–3):125–141. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.12.012 pmid:15749240
  36. 36. Franchini M, Glaser B, Gentaz E, Wood H, Eliez S, Schaer M. The effect of emotional intensity on responses to joint attention in preschoolers with an autism spectrum disorder. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2017 Mar; 35:13–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2016.11.010
  37. 37. Vivanti G, McCormick C, Young GS, Abucayan F, Hatt N, Nadig A, et al. Intact and Impaired Mechanisms of Action Understanding in Autism. Dev Psychol. 2011 May; 47(3):841–856. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023105 pmid:21401220
  38. 38. von Hofsten C, Uhlig H, Adell M, Kochukhova O. (2009). How children with autism look at events. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2009 Apr-Jun; 3(2):556–569. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2008.12.003
  39. 39. Nuske HJ, Vivanti G, Dissanayake C. Reactivity to fearful expressions of familiar and unfamiliar people in children with autism: an eye-tracking pupillometry study. J Neurodev Disord. 2014 May; 6:14. https://doi.org/10.1186/1866-1955-6-14 pmid:24982695
  40. 40. Farran EK, Branson A, King BJ. Visual search for basic emotional expressions in autism; impaired processing of anger, fear and sadness, but a typical happy face advantage. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2011 Jan-Mar; 5(1):455–462. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2010.06.009 pmid:19036491
  41. 41. Li YM, Jing J, Jin Y, Zou XB, Igarashi K, Chan RCK. Visual Attention, Emotional and Behavioral Responses to Facial Expression in Young Children with Autism. Psychologia. 2011 Dec;54(3):156–165. https://doi.org/10.2117/psysoc.2011.156
  42. 42. Uljarevic M, Hamilton A. Recognition of Emotions in Autism: A Formal Meta-Analysis. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;43:1517–1526. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1695-5 pmid:23114566
  43. 43. Black MH, Chen NTM, Iyer KK, Lipp OV, Bolte S, Falkmer M, et al. Mechanisms of facial emotion recognition in autism spectrum disorders: Insights from eye tracking and electroencephalography. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017 Sep; 80:488–515. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.06.016 pmid:28698082
  44. 44. Sasson NJ. The Development of Face Processing in Autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2006 Mar; 36(3):381–394. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0076-3 pmid:16572261
  45. 45. Jones W, Carr K, Klin A. Absence of Preferential Looking to the Eyes of Approaching Adults Predicts Level of Social Disability in 2-Year-Old Toddlers With Autism Spectrum Disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008 Aug; 65(8):946–954. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.65.8.946 pmid:18678799
  46. 46. Sasson NJ, Elison JT, Turner-Brown LM, Ditcher GS, Bodfish JW. Brief Report: Circumscribed Attention in Young Children with Autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2011 Feb; 41(2):242–7. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1038-3 pmid:20499147
  47. 47. Sasson NJ, Touchstone EW. Visual Attention to Competing Social and Object Images by Preschool Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014 Mar; 44(3):584–592. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1910-z pmid:23918441
  48. 48. Pierce K, Courchesne E. Evidence for a Cerebellar Role in Reduced Exploration and Stereotyped Behavior in Autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2001 Apr; 49(8):655–664. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(00)01008-8 pmid:11313033
  49. 49. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007 May; 39(2):175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146 pmid:17695343
  50. 50. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang AG. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods. 2009 Nov; 41(4), 1149–1160. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149 pmid:19897823
  51. 51. Cho IYK, Jelinkova K, Schuetze M, Vinette SA, Rahman S, McCrimmon A, et al. Circumscribed interests in adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A look beyond trains, planes, and clocks. PLoS ONE. 2017 Nov; 12(11):e0187414. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187414 pmid:29095880
  52. 52. DeLoache JS, Simcock G, Macari S. Planes, trains, automobiles—and tea sets: Extremely intense interests in very young children. Dev Psychol. 2007 Nov; 43(6):1579–1586. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1579 pmid:18020834
  53. 53. Sasson NJ, Dichter GS, Bodfish JW. Affective responses by adults with autism are reduced to social images but elevated to images related to circumscribed interests. PLoS ONE. 2012 Aug; 7(8):e42457. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042457 pmid:22870328
  54. 54. Van der Schalk J, Hawk ST, Fischer AH, Doosje BJ. Moving faces, looking places: The Amsterdam Dynamic Facial Expressions Set (ADFES). Emotion. 2011 Aug; 11(4):907–920. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023853 pmid:21859206
  55. 55. South M, Ozonoff S, McMahon WM. Repetitive behavior profiles in Asperger syndrome and high-functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2005 Apr; 35(2):145–158. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-004-1992-8 pmid:15909401
  56. 56. Dunn LM, Dunn LK. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Third Edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1997.
  57. 57. Korkman M, Kirk U, Kemp S. NEPSY-II: A developmental neuropsychological assessment. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 2007.
  58. 58. Shi L, Zhou Y, Ou J, Gong J, Wang S, Cui X, et al. Different Visual Preference Patterns in Response to Simple and Complex Dynamic Social Stimuli in Preschool-Aged Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. PLoS One. 2015 Mar; 10(3): e0122280. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0122280 pmid:25781170
  59. 59. Evers K, Steyaert J, Noens I, Wagemans J. Reduced Recognition of Dynamic Facial Emotional Expressions and Emotion-Specific Response Bias in Children with an Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015 Jan; 45(6):1774–1784. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2337-x pmid:25563453
  60. 60. Lozier LM, Vanmeter JW, Marsh AA. Impairments in facial affect recognition associated with autism spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis. Dev Psychopathol. 2014 Nov; 26(4):933–945. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579414000479 pmid:24915526
  61. 61. Parish-Morris J, Chevallier C, Tonge N, Letzen J, Pandey J, Schultz RT. Visual attention to dynamic faces and objects is linked to face processing skills: a combined study of children with autism and controls. Front. Psychol. 2013 Apr; 4:185. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00185 pmid:23596436
  62. 62. Stagg SD, Linnell KJ, Heaton P. Investigating eye movement patterns, language, and social ability in children with autism spectrum disorder. Dev Psychopathol. 2014 May; 26(2):529–537. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579414000108 pmid:24622054
  63. 63. Davidson D, Hilvert E, Misiunaite I, Kerby K, Giordano M. Recognition of facial emotions on human and canine faces in children with and without autism spectrum disorders. Motiv Emot. 2019 Feb; 43:19–202. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-018-9736-9
  64. 64. Trevisan DA, Birmingham E. Are emotion recognition abilities related to everyday social functioning in ASD? A meta-analysis. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2016 Dec, 32:24–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2016.08.004
  65. 65. Klin A, Jones W, Schultz R, Volkmar F, Cohen D. Visual Fixation Patterns During Viewing of Naturalistic Social Situations as Predictors of Social Competence in Individuals With Autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002 Sep; 59(9):809–16. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.59.9.809 pmid:12215080
  66. 66. Magrelli S, Jermann P, Noris B, Ansermet F, Hentsch F, Nadel J, et al. Social orienting of children with autism to facial expressions and speech: a study with a wearable eye-tracker in naturalistic settings. Front Psychol. 2013 Nov; 4:840. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00840 pmid:24312064