Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Loading metrics

Relationships between estimated autozygosity and complex traits in the UK Biobank

  • Emma C. Johnson ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    emma.c.johnson@wustl.edu

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, Missouri, United States of America

  • Luke M. Evans,

    Roles Data curation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, Colorado, United States of America, Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, Colorado, United States of America

  • Matthew C. Keller

    Roles Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, Colorado, United States of America, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, Colorado, United States of America

Abstract

Inbreeding increases the risk of certain Mendelian disorders in humans but may also reduce fitness through its effects on complex traits and diseases. Such inbreeding depression is thought to occur due to increased homozygosity at causal variants that are recessive with respect to fitness. Until recently it has been difficult to amass large enough sample sizes to investigate the effects of inbreeding depression on complex traits using genome-wide single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) data in population-based samples. Further, it is difficult to infer causation in analyses that relate degree of inbreeding to complex traits because confounding variables (e.g., education) may influence both the likelihood for parents to outbreed and offspring trait values. The present study used runs of homozygosity in genome-wide SNP data in up to 400,000 individuals in the UK Biobank to estimate the proportion of the autosome that exists in autozygous tracts—stretches of the genome which are identical due to a shared common ancestor. After multiple testing corrections and controlling for possible sociodemographic confounders, we found significant relationships in the predicted direction between estimated autozygosity and three of the 26 traits we investigated: age at first sexual intercourse, fluid intelligence, and forced expiratory volume in 1 second. Our findings corroborate those of several published studies. These results may imply that these traits have been associated with Darwinian fitness over evolutionary time. However, some of the autozygosity-trait relationships were attenuated after controlling for background sociodemographic characteristics, suggesting that alternative explanations for these associations have not been eliminated. Care needs to be taken in the design and interpretation of ROH studies in order to glean reliable information about the genetic architecture and evolutionary history of complex traits.

Author summary

Inbreeding is well known to increase the risk of rare, monogenic diseases, and there has been some evidence that it also affects complex traits, such as cognition and educational attainment. However, difficulties can arise when inferring causation in these types of analyses because of the potential for confounding variables (e.g., socioeconomic status) to bias the observed relationships between distant inbreeding and complex traits. In this investigation, we used single-nucleotide polymorphism data in a very large (N > 400,000) sample of seemingly outbred individuals to quantify the degree to which distant inbreeding is associated with 26 complex traits. We found robust evidence that distant inbreeding is inversely associated with fluid intelligence and a measure of lung function, and is positively associated with age at first sex, while other trait associations with inbreeding were attenuated after controlling for background sociodemographic characteristics. Our findings are consistent with evolutionary predictions that fluid intelligence, lung function, and age at first sex have been under selection pressures over time; however, they also suggest that confounding variables must be accounted for in order to reliably interpret results from these types of analyses.

Introduction

Inbreeding occurs when genetic relatives have offspring, and is associated with increased risk of disorders and decreased health and viability in offspring [13]. This effect, called inbreeding depression, is thought to occur because natural selection more efficiently removes additive and dominant deleterious alleles, leaving the remaining deleterious alleles segregating in the population at a given time more recessive than otherwise expected [4], a phenomenon called directional dominance. Inbreeding is thought to be associated with lower fitness because it leads to long stretches of the genome that are autozygous—homozygous because the genomic segments inherited from each parent are from the same ancestor. Autozygosity reveals the full deleterious effects of recessive or partially recessive alleles that exist in these regions, and so individuals with increased autozygosity are more likely to exhibit deficits in traits that have been associated with Darwinian fitness over evolutionary time. Thus, one major reason for the interest in studying the effects of inbreeding on complex traits has been that such studies can provide insight into which traits have been under natural selection.

Because all humans are related to one another, even if distantly, inbreeding is a matter of degree. In the last decade, the increasing availability of genome-wide single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) data has allowed scientists to infer degree of distant inbreeding, or the proportion of the genome that is autozygous, using runs of homozygosity (ROHs)—long stretches of SNPs that are homozygous [5]. The total proportion of the genome contained within these homozygous regions is called FROH and has been shown to be the best genome-wide estimate of autozygosity [5,6]. However, very large samples (e.g. n > 10,000) are required to detect likely effects of FROH in outbred human populations because of the low variance in levels of genome-wide autozygosity in such populations. Previous studies of FROH in humans have found evidence consistent with inbreeding depression for several complex traits, including height, forced expiratory volume in one second (FEV1), educational attainment, and cognitive ability (g) [710], with less conclusive evidence for an effect of inbreeding on psychiatric disorders [11,12] or risk factors for late-onset diseases like hypertension and other cardiovascular disease [13,14]. These observed associations with FROH may suggest that directional selection has acted on these traits ancestrally.

One challenge in autozygosity research in humans is in the causal interpretations of any observed FROH -trait relationships. It is likely that propensity to outbreed (choosing mates who are genetically dissimilar) is related to multiple sociodemographic variables in parents (e.g., education, religiosity, socioeconomic status), and these parental trait values may influence offspring trait values, thereby inducing an FROH -trait relationship that has nothing to do with the genetic effects of inbreeding depression. In a recent study conducted in the Netherlands, a relatively small, densely populated country with a strong history of latitudinal religious assortment, Abdellaoui et al. [15] found a significant association between decreased FROH (i.e. less inbred) and increased risk for major depressive disorder (MDD); this counter-intuitive association disappeared when the models accounted for religious assortment. This suggests that the original FROHMDD association occurred for sociological rather than genetic reasons: religious individuals had higher average levels of autozygosity than non-religious individuals, probably due to denominational restrictions on mate choice that were only recently relaxed [15], and religious individuals were less likely to experience MDD [16]. In another recent study, the largest (N > 300,000) FROH analysis to date, Joshi et al. (2016) found a significant relationship between FROH and four complex traits: height, FEV1, cognitive ability (g), and educational attainment [7]. When educational attainment was included as a covariate in the model as a proxy for socioeconomic status (SES), the effects for height, FEV1, and cognitive ability remained significant. Because of the persistence of these effects after accounting for educational attainment, the authors conclude that the relationship they observed between FROH and the complex traits is likely a due to a genetic mechanism, directional dominance, rather than to sociodemographic confounds. However, the FROH–trait effect sizes decreased by ~20–35% after controlling for SES; it is possible that inclusion of additional, or more relevant, sociodemographic covariates could have changed these conclusions.

The findings from our work and others on the relationship between FROH and psychiatric disorders in ascertained samples [11,12,1721] have been inconsistent and highlight concerns about the potential for unmeasured confounders to influence FROH results. Using the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (PGC) MDD data from 9 samples, Power et al.[12] found a significant positive relationship between FROH and MDD in three German samples but, strangely, a significant negative relationship between FROH and MDD in six samples from non-German sites. Similarly, in 2012 we found a small but highly significant association between schizophrenia and FROH across 17 case-control datasets (total N = 21,844 [20]). However, in 2016 we published an independent replication using the same procedures as our previous study that found little to no evidence of an FROH-schizophrenia association across 22 case-control datasets (total N = 39,830 [11]). We are uncertain how to explain these discrepancies, but we have hypothesized that unmeasured cofounding variables such as education, religiosity, and income can differentially bias such ROH findings across different sites, and that this problem is particularly salient in ascertained samples where cases and controls may be drawn from subpopulations that differ slightly on background sociodemographic characteristics. While such differences in ascertainment between cases and controls are unlikely to lead to significant allele frequency differences, and thus are unlikely to bias genome-wide association studies (GWAS), they could easily lead to systematic case-control differences in FROH, depending on the difference in degree of inbreeding in the subpopulations from which cases and controls were drawn.

Finally, a recent study by Yengo et al. [10] quantified a separate source of potential bias in FROH analyses. Using simulations, the authors show that FROH -based measures of inbreeding can overestimate the inbreeding effect size compared to SNP-based measures. The authors go on to show that a SNP-based measure of inbreeding calculated from the correlation between uniting gametes, FUNI, provides unbiased estimates of inbreeding effects when causal variants are well-tagged by measured SNPs. However, unlike FROH, which captures the effects of both common and rare recessive causal variants [5], SNP-based measures of inbreeding such as FUNI underestimate the effects of (typically rare) recessive causal variants that are poorly tagged by measured SNPs [22]. We chose to focus on FROH rather than SNP-based measures of inbreeding in the current study because we were interested in investigating the evidence for directional dominance on complex traits, which is theoretically manifested via a higher proportion of rare and recessive causal variants. We also chose to focus on FROH for comparability with previous findings, as FROH has been the standard metric in the field for assessing inbreeding depression to date.

Here, we describe the most powerful investigation to date of the association of FROH with several complex traits. We used whole-genome SNP and phenotypic data from the UK Biobank (total n ~ 100,000–400,000) to address two principal questions: (1) is there evidence consistent with directional dominance on traits related to fitness and health, such that increased FROH is associated with lower trait values? and (2) do FROH-trait relationships persist after controlling for multiple background sociodemographic variables? This sample is population-based, reducing concerns about ascertainment-induced confounds, and includes information on multiple relevant sociodemographic control variables and traits previously associated with FROH (e.g. waist-to-hip ratio, grip strength, diastolic and systolic blood pressure, and fluid intelligence [710,1214]), making it an ideal sample for investigating the relationship between distant inbreeding and complex traits.

Methods

Ethics statement

This study utilized de-identified data from the UK Biobank. UK Biobank received ethical approval from the NHS National Research Ethics Service North West (11/NW/0382).

UK Biobank sample

Our data came from the UK Biobank, a population-based sample from the United Kingdom. In total, 502,682 individuals were recruited from 2006–2010 from 22 centers across the UK. Participants were given a touchscreen interview that included questions about demographic characteristics, health history, and lifestyle information (e.g. diet, alcohol intake, sleep habits), and some anthropometric and physical measures were collected. DNA was extracted from whole blood and genotyped using either the Affymetrix UK Biobank Axiom array or the Affymetrix UK BiLEVE Axiom array. Detailed genotyping and sample QC procedures are described in Bycroft et al. [23] We analyzed data from the second release of up to 400,000 individuals (exact N varied by phenotype) with genotypes available.

Phenotypes

We examined 26 traits related to health, fitness, or sociodemographic characteristics (see S1 Text for full description and field ID of individual measures). These included 17 continuous traits (age at first sexual intercourse, waist to hip ratio, height, body mass index (BMI), basal metabolic rate (BMR), diastolic and systolic blood pressure (BP), hand-grip strength (taking the maximum of left and right grip strength measurements), county-wide socioeconomic status (SES) as measured by the Townsend Deprivation Index (TDI), total household income (an ordinal variable of income brackets recoded to be numeric, ranging from 0–4), years of educational attainment (coded using ISCED classifications as in Okbay et al. [24]), fluid intelligence score (FI), forced expiratory volume in 1 second (FEV1; a measure of lung function), FEV1 over forced vital capacity (FEV1/FVC), birth weight, neuroticism score, and body fat percentage) and 9 binary traits (ever smoked, ever drank alcohol, whether or not they were breastfed as a baby, whether or not they completed college, whether they specified participation in a religious group as a leisure activity, whether or not they had ever been diagnosed with diabetes, probable bipolar and/or major depression status, and whether they live in an urban or rural area). These traits were chosen because they had either been previously studied in ROH analyses (fluid intelligence, grip strength, FEV1, FEV1/FVC ratio, waist-to-hip ratio, height, BMI, diastolic and systolic blood pressure [710]), were diagnoses or binary traits of psychiatric and biomedical interest with large enough Ns to reasonably include in our study (diabetes, probable bipolar or major depressive disorder diagnoses, ever smoked, ever drank), or were our only available proxy measure of reproductive success (age at first sexual intercourse). Our “sociodemographic” traits were chosen because of their hypothesized (regional poverty via Townsend Deprivation Index [25], breastfed as an infant as a proxy for mother’s socioeconomic status [26], income, urbanicity) or previously demonstrated (education, religious assortment [15,27]) influence on ROH–trait associations. We excluded individuals who weighed less than 36.28 kg (~80 lbs), weighed more than 6.8 kg (~15 lbs) at birth, had systolic BP readings >200 mmHg or diastolic BP readings >120 mmHg, were shorter than 120 cm (~3.93 ft), had a hip circumference <50 cm or >175 cm, had a waist circumference <40 cm or >160 cm, had grip strength >70 kg, or reported having had sex before 12 years of age. These exclusion criteria were chosen based on thresholds typically defined as being boundaries of normal physiological, anthropometric, or behavioral ranges and by checking for obvious outliers that may have been incorrect data entries. More information on specific phenotype derivations and calculations are included in the supplemental material. We standardized all quantitative phenotypes (within sex) before calculating their relationship with FROH for ease of comparison with Joshi et al.’s and others’ results [7].

Quality control (QC)

Because the sample was predominately European ancestry, we restricted analyses to individuals of European ancestry (N = 436,065) as identified by visual inspection of plots of genomic principal components. We followed sample and genotypic quality control that has become typical in ROH analyses. In particular, we excluded SNPs if they a) deviated from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium at p<1×10−6, b) missing call rate > 0.02, or c) had a minor allele frequency (MAF) < 0.05. We also excluded individuals with discordant self-reported gender and genetic sex, a missing genotype call rate > 0.02, and we removed the minimum number of individuals so that all remaining subjects were unrelated at > 0.2 (using GCTA’s—grm-cutoff option [28]) (n = 31,541 removed in total). We also repeated these analyses after using a stricter relatedness cutoff, removing all individuals related at > 0.05 (n = 103,389 removed in total; see S1 Text), to ensure the robustness of our results.

ROH calling procedures

After QC, we pruned out SNPs that were in strong linkage disequilibrium with other SNPs by removing those that had a variance inflation factor > 10 (equivalent to an r2 of 0.90) between target SNPs and 50 surrounding SNPs (plink command:—indep 50 5 10). After these procedures, 263,609 SNPs and 404,524 individuals remained. For our main analysis, we called ROHs as being ≥65 homozygous SNPs in a row, set the minimum KB length very low (essentially ignoring the length requirements), with no heterozygote calls and three missing variant calls allowed (5% of the SNP threshold), per recommendations from Howrigan et al. (2011) for genotype data of similar SNP density. We required ROHs to have a density greater than at least 1 SNP per 200 kb (the average density across the genome in the SNPs used in the analysis was 1 per 10 kb) and split an ROH into two if a gap >500 kb existed between consecutive homozygous SNPs. These analyses used the following commands in Plink 1.9 [29]:—homozyg-window-snp 65—homozyg-snp 65—homozyg-kb 10—homozyg-gap 500—homozyg-window-missing 3—homozyg-window-het 0—homozyg-density 200 (see S1 Text for further discussion of parameter choice). After calling ROHs, we summed the total length of all autosomal ROHs for each individual and divided that by the total SNP-mappable distance (2.77x109 bases) to calculate FROH, the proportion of the genome likely to be autozygous.

We also tested the relative importance of recent vs. distant inbreeding by calculating FROH from longer ROHs (indicative of closer inbreeding) and comparing to the effect of FROH from shorter ROHs (a proxy for more distant inbreeding). We defined recent inbreeding as the proportion of the genome contained in autozygous regions longer than 8.5 Mb (FROH_long) and distant inbreeding as the proportion of the genome in autozygous regions shorter than 8.5 Mb (FROH_short), as FROH_long and FROH_short had approximately equal variances (4.5e-6 and 4.3e-6, respectively) in our sample. An autozygous segment spanning < 8.5 Mb should originate from a common ancestor at least 6 generations ago on average [30].

In addition to calling ROHs, we also calculated a measure of SNP-by-SNP homozygosity (FSNP) for each individual, using the—het flag in Plink 1.9 [29]:

FSNP = [observed homozygous count—expected count] / [total observations—expected count]

Because it is calculated with genotyped SNPs, FSNP is a measure of excess homozygosity at common SNPs (see S3 Table for the correlation matrix between FSNP, FROH_long and FROH_short).

ROH burden analysis

FROH was used as the primary predictor of the traits of interest in analyses described below. The distributions of ROH lengths and FROH are shown in S1 Fig. We regressed each trait (Y) on FROH using the model in the equation below, where is the intercept, C is a matrix of covariates (including e.g. the first 20 principal components) and ε represents the residual error term.

As noted above, all quantitative phenotypes were standardized to intra-sex z-scores for ease of comparison with previous findings in the literature. In addition, for ease of interpretation, we reverse-coded some of the phenotypes such that lower values represented what we thought were likely to be lower fitness and/or less desirable outcomes (e.g. disease diagnosis was coded as ‘0’ while no diagnosis was coded as ‘1’, and TDI was reverse-coded such that lower values represented greater material poverty). We were primarily interested in the estimate of , which represents the association of FROH with the trait, controlling for covariates (although in one set of models, described below, we were also interested in the effect of FSNP on the trait). For binary traits, we ran logistic regression models with the same covariates as in the linear regression models for quantitative traits.

We ran a total of three sets of models for each trait. The first set of models was designed to test for a simple relationship between FROH and the traits listed above. Because confounding factors such as population stratification, SNP missingness, call quality, and plate effects can influence FROH, we included the batch number, percentage of missing SNP calls per sample in the non-imputed genotype data, and the first 20 ancestry principal components (calculated within individuals of European ancestry), as well as age, age2, and sex, in all of the regression models unless explicitly stated.

In our second set of models, we tested whether background sociodemographic characteristics mediated FROH-trait relationships. In addition to the above covariates, in these models we also included income, years of educational attainment, Townsend Deprivation Index (a measure of the amount of material deprivation in a given region [25]), and whether subjects attended college, lived in an urban area, participated in a religious group as a leisure activity, and reported being breastfed as an infant. Although the covariates of true interest are those measured on the parents (whose sociodemographic traits may influence mate choice), parental information was unavailable (other than breastfeeding, which is associated with mother’s socioeconomic status [26]), and so we used the subjects’ own values on these traits as the best available proxies for characteristics of their parents.

In our third set of models, we tested the degree to which observed FROH-trait relationships were due to homozygosity at common versus rare alleles. To do this, we included FSNP as a covariate in addition to the covariates from the second set of models above. Because common SNPs can often predict (are in linkage disequilibrium with) other common SNPs but typically poorly predict rare SNPs, FSNP captures effects of homozygosity at common SNPs only whereas FROH captures the effects of homozygosity at both common and rare SNPs [5]. In the S1 Text (and S4 Table), we demonstrate via simulation that entering both FSNP and FROH as predictors simultaneously in the regression equation allows insight into the degree to which observed inbreeding effects are due to homozygosity at common versus rare alleles.

Results

The distribution of ROH lengths, FROH, and FSNP are shown in S1 and S2 Figs, and descriptive statistics are given in S1 Table. Using a Bonferroni correction based on testing 26 traits (= 0.002), we observed significant negative associations between FROH and income, grip strength, height, fluid intelligence score (FI), and forced expiratory volume in one second (FEV1), and observed significant positive associations between FROH and age at first sexual intercourse (AFS) and religious group participation (Table 1 and Fig 1). The associations we found between FROH and FI, FEV1, and height replicate three of Joshi et al.’s four significant findings. To our surprise, we did not replicate their significant relationship between FROH and educational attainment. When these analyses were repeated in the smaller sample of individuals unrelated at > 0.05, conclusions did not change (see S5 Table). When we tested the effects of recent vs. distant inbreeding, the results for more recent inbreeding were similar to the full FROH models: income, grip strength, height, FI, FEV1, AFS, and religious group participation were all associated with FROH_long, with the same direction of effect as the original models. Similarly, AFS, FEV1, FI, religious group attendance, and ever drink (such that being more autozygous was associated with a lower likelihood of having ever drank alcohol) were significantly associated with FROH_short, while its associations with income, grip strength, and height were not significant (S6 Table).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Beta FROH and 95% confidence intervals from main regression models controlling for minimal covariates (20 ancestry principal components, genotype batch, per-sample SNP missingness, age, age2, and sex).

Significant estimates (at p < 0.002—corrected for multiple testing) are starred (religious group attendance as a leisure activity, income, AFS, FEV1, FI, height, and grip strength). A. All quantitative traits were analyzed in intra-sex standardized phenotypic units in linear regression models. B. Binary traits and diagnoses were analyzed using logistic regression models (the log odds ratios are reported). AFS, age at first sexual intercourse; BMI, body mass index; BMR, basal metabolic rate; BP, blood pressure; BPD, bipolar disorder; CI, confidence interval; FEV1, forced expiratory volume in 1 second; FI, fluid intelligence; FVC, forced vital capacity; MDD, major depression; TDI, Townsend Deprivation Index.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1007556.g001

thumbnail
Table 1.

Association of FROH with 26 traits, in two sets of models: 1) controlling for age, age2, sex, the first 20 principal components, sample missingness, and batch number as covariates, and 2) also controlling for sociodemographic variables.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1007556.t001

When we included the seven sociodemographic variables as covariates in the regression models (other than those predicting sociodemographic variables), the betas associated with FROH decreased for AFS, grip strength, height, and FI (by 20.1%, 19.8%, 36.8%, and 1.2%, respectively) and increased for FEV1 (by 4.2%) (see Table 1 and Fig 2). AFS, FI, and FEV1 remained significantly associated with FROH whereas the associations with height and grip strength became non-significant. No significant indirect mediation effect of the sociodemographic variables in combination was found for the relationships between FROH and AFS, grip strength, height, FI, or FEV1 (see S1 Text for a description of these tests). Furthermore, the association between FROH_short and ever drink became non-significant after controlling for the sociodemographic covariates, as did the associations between FROH_long and grip strength, height, and FI (S8 Table). Finally, we tested whether the effect of FROH differed by sex by including sex*FROH interaction terms in each of the second set of models; we observed no significant sex-by-FROH interactions for any of the traits.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Comparison with estimates from Joshi et al. 2015, and some evidence that sociodemographic background variables attenuate the relationship between FROH and complex traits.

Plot shows the Beta FROH and 95% confidence interval in within-sex standardized phenotypic units for the five quantitative traits that were significantly associated with FROH in the main models (Fig 1), as well as educational attainment, which was significantly associated with autozygosity in Joshi et al.’s study7. Estimates that were statistically significant after multiple testing corrections are starred for each set of models. After controlling for background sociodemographic characteristics, AFS, FEV1, and FI were still statistically significant in our study. The effect sizes for AFS, grip strength, FI, and height all decreased after controlling for sociodemographic variables. The effect sizes from our analyses were smaller for all four of the phenotypes also measured in Joshi et al.’s study. FI, fluid intelligence; FEV1, forced expiratory volume in 1 second; CI, confidence interval.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1007556.g002

In our final set of models, where excess SNP-by-SNP homozygosity (FSNP) was included as an additional covariate, AFS and FI remained significantly associated with FROH after accounting for multiple testing and FEV1 was marginally significant (Table 2). Waist-to-hip ratio was significantly associated with FSNP but not FROH, suggesting that higher homozygosity at common but not rare variants is related to increased waist-to-hip ratio. When these analyses were repeated in the smaller set of individuals unrelated at a stricter cutoff ( < 0.05), our findings did not substantially change, though fluid intelligence no longer met the p-value cutoff for determining statistical significance, likely because of larger standard errors from the reduced sample size (S9 Table).

thumbnail
Table 2. Effects of both FROH and excess SNP-by-SNP homozygosity, measured by FSNP, controlling for the same covariates as in the previous models (age, age2, sex, batch number, per-sample SNP missingness, the first 20 principal components, and background sociodemographic variables.).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1007556.t002

Discussion

Overview of findings

We replicated several previous associations between FROH and fitness-related traits, identified a novel association between FROH and a reproductive phenotype (age at first sexual intercourse), and found weak evidence that background sociodemographic characteristics may be partially mediating a few of the observed relationships between FROH and complex traits (Fig 2). In particular, we found robust evidence that fluid intelligence (FI), forced expiratory volume in one second (FEV1), and age at first sexual intercourse (AFS) are associated with FROH (Tables 1 and 2), while grip strength and height’s relationships with FROH were attenuated enough to become non-significant after controlling for background sociodemographic variables. The associations of FROH with FI and FEV1 were especially robust, with the inclusion of sociodemographic covariates having little to no consequence on these effect sizes. The strength of FROH associations for more recent inbreeding was similar or stronger than those for more distant inbreeding, except, interestingly, for AFS, FI, and participation in a religious group. When we accounted for SNP-by-SNP homozygosity in the model, AFS and FI were still significantly associated with FROH, consistent with their relationships with FROH being more strongly driven by homozygosity at rare rather than common variants. Certain other associations were likely due to social rather than genetic causes; for example, it is much more plausible that non-religious individuals tend to outbreed at higher rates and have less religious offspring than that autozygosity causes individuals to be more religious.

Comparison with previous results

Our results largely agree with recent reports [710] on the relationships between estimated autozygosity and complex traits in population-based samples. Replicating Howrigan et al. [8], Joshi et al. [7], and Yengo et al.[10], as well as previous pedigree studies [31], we found a significant, negative relationship between FROH and fluid intelligence. In addition, we replicated Joshi et al.’s [7] finding of a significant relationship between increased FROH and decreased FEV1. We initially observed a significant association between increased FROH and decreased height, as did Joshi et al. [7] and Verweij et al. [9], but this association was attenuated in our sample after controlling for background sociodemographic variables and did not meet statistical significance after Bonferroni corrections (Table 1 and Fig 2). Our initial results (Table 1) were consistent with previous findings for an effect of inbreeding depression on grip strength [9], though this association appears to be more likely due to homozygosity at common rather than rare variants (Table 2).

Despite the general consistency across reports on FROH-complex trait associations, there were two differences between our results and those from earlier studies. First, educational attainment (in years of education) was not significantly associated with FROH in any of our models, contrary to several previous reports [7,9], but consistent with Yengo et al. [10]. We found a significant (p = 2.18e-4) relationship between FROH and income (which itself was correlated with years of education at r = 0.37), but we found no evidence for an association between FROH and either years of education or the binary variable measuring whether or not an individual attended college. The reason for the discrepancy in findings for education is unlikely to be due to sampling variability because the two confidence intervals do not overlap (Fig 2). One possibility is that educational attainment is less correlated with geographic mobility (and the tendency to outbreed) in the UK compared to other countries previously investigated, and Joshi et al. [7] did report significant heterogeneity of the FROH-education association across sites. Moreover, of the 5 cohorts from the UK investigated by Joshi et al. [7], two (GRAPHIC and LBC1936) showed associations in the opposite direction of the overall association (see their Extended Data Fig 2). Thus, it is possible that the FROH -educational attainment relationship might be different in the UK than is typical in other societies. Furthermore, the association we found between height and autozygosity was attenuated (by ~37%) when we accounted for sociodemographic covariates, and was somewhat smaller than that found by previous studies even when we did not control for sociodemographic variables (e.g. a 1% increase in FROH predicted a decrease of ~.03 s.d. of height in previous studies [7,32] versus a decrease of ~.02 s.d. in the current study). Nevertheless, the confidence intervals for Joshi et al.’s [7] and our observed association between height and FROH overlapped (Fig 2), suggesting that sampling variability could be a reason for the discrepant height findings.

In comparing results across recent publications and the current one, it is important to note the differences in populations, samples, and measurements across studies. Both Howrigan et al. [8] and Joshi et al. [7] took a meta-analytic approach, conducting FROH analyses in each contributing sample separately, and then combining across samples, controlling for relevant covariates (e.g. dataset, country of data collection). Joshi et al. in particular analyzed a much more diverse overall sample than the present study, including multiple cohorts from European, African, and Asian populations. Another difference is in the measurement of intelligence across studies: our measurement for general cognitive ability was the unweighted sum of the number of 13 fluid intelligence questions answered correctly, given as part of the UK Biobank’s cognitive function assessment, while Howrigan et al. [8] converted the scores from each contributing sample’s measure of general cognitive ability (e.g. WAIS-R, Cattell Culture Fair Test) into z-scores (to avoid bias from different measurement schemes across samples), and Joshi et al. used g as their measure of general cognitive ability, “calculated as the first unrotated principal component of test scores across diverse domains of cognition”. Furthermore, our regression models controlled for the first 20 ancestry principal components, while Howrigan et al. controlled for the first 10 and Joshi et al. the first 3.

Possible evolutionary interpretations

There are two major evolutionary theories for why inbreeding depression occurs [4]: the overdominance hypothesis posits that an overall loss of heterozygosity at loci governed by heterozygote advantage leads to inbreeding depression, while the partial dominance theory postulates that inbreeding depression occurs as selection acts most efficiently on the most additive and dominant deleterious mutations, purging those from the population while leaving behind the more rare, partially recessive deleterious alleles. This second hypothesis, partial dominance, is widely accepted as the more likely mechanism of inbreeding depression [3,33]. The robust associations we observed between FROH and AFS, FI, and FEV1, even after controlling for homozygosity at common variants with FSNP, suggest that the variants contributing to lower trait values are biased toward being rare and recessive, consistent with predictions from a partial dominance model of inbreeding depression [5] and consistent with the hypothesis that these traits, or traits genetically correlated with them, have been under directional selection over evolutionary time. Cognitive ability, including intelligence test scores, is a predictor of multiple Darwinian fitness-related outcomes, including overall health and lifespan [8,34]. FEV1 is correlated with mortality and lifespan [3538], traits that are components of fitness and thus more likely to have been under directional selection over evolutionary history [39]. Thus, our replication of the associations between autozygosity and FEV1 and FI adds to a body of evidence that these traits, or traits genetically correlated with them, have been under directional selection over evolutionary history, consistent with the expectation that variants that influence them are biased toward being rarer and more recessive than expected under a neutral drift model.

The positive relationship we observed between AFS and FROH is a novel finding, to the best of our knowledge, though associations between FROH and reproductive phenotypes have been observed previously, for different proxy measures (e.g. number of children fathered, in Yengo et al. [10]). The FROH-AFS association was attenuated but remained statistically significant after controlling for sociodemographic variables and homozygosity at common variants (FSNP). Our finding is consistent with a body of research suggesting that reproductive traits, like AFS, in non-human populations are under more intense selection pressures than non-fitness traits [5,40]. If autozygosity causally influences AFS (see “Limitations” below), there are two possible evolutionary interpretations. First, it is possible that early sex itself was advantageous in ancient human history due to a prolonged reproductive period. A second possibility is that the observed association between autozygosity and AFS is due to selection on a genetically correlated trait, such as sexual attractiveness [41,42]. However, it is important to note that the original effect size for the association between FROH and AFS decreased by ~20% after accounting for sociodemographic variables. Furthermore, sociodemographic variables were not measured on parents (the more relevant control; see below), and the single dichotomous variable for religious group participation that we used as a proxy for religiosity is unlikely to capture the full confounding effects of religious observance on sexual behaviors. Thus, the association between inbreeding and age at first sexual intercourse that we observed in this study should be interpreted with caution.

Limitations

There were three central limitations in the current study. The most important one, which applies equally to all other FROH studies that we are aware of, is that ROH associations might be due to third-variable explanations. Unlike GWAS analyses, where parental or offspring sociodemographic traits are unlikely to be associated with allele frequencies and therefore are unlikely to bias GWAS results, it takes only a single generation of parental inbreeding to strongly influence FROH levels in offspring. For example, higher income might be associated with greater opportunities to meet mates of diverse origins and to higher outbreeding; offspring of higher income parents might thereby have not only lower levels of autozygosity, on average, but might also differ on any traits influenced genetically or environmentally by parental income. While sociodemographic confounding is particularly problematic in ascertained samples where cases and controls are drawn from different populations (e.g. cases drawn from a psychiatric hospital, controls from a nearby university), the possibility of confounding cannot be eliminated, even in population-based samples, unless relevant sociodemographic variables among parents are measured and controlled for or other (e.g., within-family) designs are used. For example, in a study of approximately 2,000 individuals of Dutch ancestry, Abdellaoui et al. [27] found only a weak association between FROH and the subjects’ own educational attainment (p = 0.045), but found highly significant negative associations between the subject’s FROH and their parents’ educational attainment (pfather < 10−5, pmother = 9e-5). These relationships were entirely mediated by the geographic distance between parents’ birthplaces, such that parents with higher educational attainment tended to be more geographically mobile, increasing their chances of mating with someone genetically dissimilar from themselves and thus creating systematic differences in levels of inbreeding across levels of educational attainment in their offspring.

Having information on parents’ birth location, education, income, mobility, level of religious involvement, and so forth is important in order to control for the possibility that these sociodemographic variables are associated with both higher levels of (distant) inbreeding and lower offspring trait values. Unfortunately, the UK Biobank has limited parental information other than indirect measures such as whether one was breastfed. In the current study, we used sociodemographic responses of the offspring as imperfect proxies for parental responses, which is effective only to the degree that offspring values on these sociodemographic variables are positively correlated with their parents’ values. For example, parental educational (r = 0.25–0.40; [43,44]), income (r = .60; [44]), and religiosity [45] are imperfectly correlated between parents and offspring in Great Britain. These imperfect correlations imply that the true mediating influences of the sociodemographic variables on observed FROH -trait relationships were likely to be underestimated in the present report, and thus causal interpretation of our results may not be warranted.

Still, while FROH_short is an imperfect measure of truly distant inbreeding (as recent inbreeding can also produce short ROHs), it may be that FROH_short is less susceptible to confounding from recent assortative mating than FROH_long (a measure of more recent inbreeding). The three traits (AFS, FI, and FEV1) that were significantly associated with FROH even after the inclusion of sociodemographic covariates were also significantly associated with FROH_short (S6 and S8 Tables). This bolsters our hypothesis that autozygosity is causally influencing these three traits through inbreeding depression, while the evidence for height and grip strength (which were associated with recent but not distant inbreeding, S6 Table) is weaker.

A second limitation is the potential for a collider bias in the models in which we controlled for sociodemographic variables. Collider bias occurs when a covariate in a linear model is causally influenced by both the outcome and the predictor of interest, which creates a bias in the estimated association between the predictor and the outcome [46,47]. In the context of the current study, it is possible that income is negatively influenced by autozygosity and positively influenced by intelligence. If so, then the regression coefficient for FROH predicting fluid intelligence would be upwardly biased when income is included as a covariate. Unfortunately, it was not possible to know the degree to which our results were influenced by a collider bias given that the directions of causation between variables are unknown. Nevertheless, it has been argued that the potential bias from failing to adjust for a covariate is likely to be greater than the bias introduced when controlling for a collider [48,49]. Thus, our solution was to present results from models that both controlled and did not control for the sociodemographic covariates, and to highlight the potential for biases in models that controlled for sociodemographic variables.

The third limitation to the current study is that we did not have access to all of the phenotypes studied in recent articles such as Yengo et al. [10], Verweij et al. [9], Joshi et al. [7] (e.g. the cholesterol measures in Joshi et al.), so we could not attempt to fully replicate these previous investigations. In addition, as mentioned earlier, the covariate that we had available as a proxy for religiosity was unlikely to account for all possible confounding of religiosity on FROH-trait associations.

Summary

We found several significant associations between estimated autozygosity and several sociodemographic, anthropometric, health, and otherwise fitness-related traits. All effects were in the direction that would be predicted by evolutionary hypotheses (i.e. higher inbreeding associated with lower fitness). When controlling for measures of background sociodemographic characteristics (educational attainment, college education, income, urbanicity, TDI, religious participation, and whether an individual was breastfed)–which should at least partially reflect parental characteristics–we found that two (height and grip strength) of the five significant FROH-trait associations were attenuated and became non-significant, while AFS, FI, and FEV1 remained significantly associated with FROH. The fact that the associations between estimated autozygosity and both grip strength and height were reduced after controlling for the additional covariates suggests that these relationships might not hold up if relevant confounder variables in parents had been controlled for, and we cannot eliminate the possibility that the other FROH-trait associations we report here would not also be attenuated or eliminated in this situation.

Nevertheless, our results generally replicate previous findings in humans [79] and are consistent with similar ones from non-human populations [40,50,51]. This cumulative evidence may well reflect the detrimental effects of autozygosity on complex traits, revealing ancient selection pressures on these or correlated traits. However, the fact remains that even in very large, well-powered, unascertained samples such as this one, it is exceedingly difficult to make definitive statements about the underlying causal mechanism of observed relationships between FROH and complex traits.

Supporting information

S1 Text. Additional information on phenotype derivation, choice of autozygosity detection method and parameters, FROH_long vs. FROH_short, mediation analysis and testing for indirect effect, FSNP simulations.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1007556.s001

(DOCX)

S1 Table. Descriptive statistics of the UK Biobank sample–continuous variables.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1007556.s002

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Descriptive statistics of the UK Biobank sample–binary variables.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1007556.s003

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Correlations between FROH from longer ROHs (FROH_long, indicative of more recent inbreeding), FROH from shorter ROHs (FROH_short, a proxy for more distant inbreeding), and FSNP, a measure of excess homozygosity at common SNPs.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1007556.s004

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Results from simulations of fully recessive quantitative phenotypes caused exclusively by homozygosity at either common (minor allele frequency (MAF) > 0.05) or rare (MAF < 0.05) variants.

Linear regression models included both FROH and FSNP, as well as age, age2, sex, batch number, sample missingness, and the first 20 principle components. We report the average Beta and p-value across the 100 simulations for each MAF class of causal variants (CVs).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1007556.s005

(DOCX)

S5 Table.

Association of FROH with 26 traits in smaller sample of individuals unrelated at pihat > 0.05, for two sets of models: 1) controlling for age, age2, sex, the first 20 principal components, sample missingness, and batch number as covariates, and 2) also controlling for sociodemographic variables. The quantitative traits (analyzed via linear regression) are listed first in the table, followed by diagnoses and binary traits (analyzed via logistic regression models). Phenotypes with a significant relationship with FROH (p < 0.002 after multiple testing correction) are bolded; those with an asterisk are also significantly associated with FROH after controlling for sociodemographic covariates (income, educational attainment, college degree, urbanicity, TDI, religious group participation, and whether or not they were breastfed as an infant). BP, blood pressure; FEV1, forced expiratory volume in 1 second; FVC, forced vital capacity; BPD, bipolar disorder; MDD, major depressive disorder; df, degrees of freedom; SE, standard error.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1007556.s006

(DOCX)

S6 Table. Effects of FROH from recent and distant inbreeding (analyzed in two separate models), defined as the proportion of the genome contained in autozygous segments longer than or shorter than 8.5 Mb, respectively.

All models controlled for age, age2, sex, batch number, per-sample SNP missingness, and the first 20 principal components. Phenotypes with a significant relationship (p < 0.002 after multiple testing correction) with FROH from recent inbreeding are bolded, while those with a significant relationship with FROH from distant inbreeding are starred. The quantitative traits (analyzed via linear regression) are listed first in the table, followed by diagnoses and binary traits (analyzed via logistic regression models). BP, blood pressure; FEV1, forced expiratory volume in 1 second; FVC, forced vital capacity; BPD, bipolar disorder; MDD, major depressive disorder; df, degrees of freedom; SE, standard error.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1007556.s007

(DOCX)

S7 Table. Effects of FROH from recent and distant inbreeding, when both FROH_long and FROH_short are simultaneously entered into the regression model.

All models controlled for age, age2, sex, batch number, per-sample SNP missingness, and the first 20 principal components. Phenotypes with a significant association (p < 0.002 after multiple testing correction) with FROH_long are bolded, while those with a significant relationship with FROH_short are starred. The quantitative traits (analyzed via linear regression) are listed first in the table, followed by diagnoses and binary traits (analyzed via logistic regression models). BP, blood pressure; FEV1, forced expiratory volume in 1 second; FVC, forced vital capacity; BPD, bipolar disorder; MDD, major depressive disorder; df, degrees of freedom; SE, standard error.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1007556.s008

(DOCX)

S8 Table. Effects of FROH from recent and distant inbreeding (analyzed in two separate models), controlling for background sociodemographic characteristics.

All models controlled for age, age2, sex, batch number, per-sample SNP missingness, the first 20 principal components, and seven sociodemographic variables. Phenotypes with a significant relationship (p < 0.003 after multiple testing correction) with FROH from recent inbreeding are bolded, while those with a significant relationship with FROH from distant inbreeding are starred. The quantitative traits (analyzed via linear regression) are listed first in the table, followed by diagnoses and binary traits (analyzed via logistic regression models). BP, blood pressure; FEV1, forced expiratory volume in 1 second; FVC, forced vital capacity; BPD, bipolar disorder; MDD, major depressive disorder; df, degrees of freedom; SE, standard error.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1007556.s009

(DOCX)

S9 Table. Effects of both FROH and excess SNP-by-SNP homozygosity, measured by FSNP, in smaller sample of individuals unrelated at pihat > 0.05, controlling for the covariates in the previous models (age, age2, sex, batch number, per-sample SNP missingness, the first 20 principal components, and background sociodemographic variables.).

Phenotypes with a significant relationship with FROH (p < 0.003 after multiple testing correction) are bolded, while those with a significant relationship with FSNP are italicized. The quantitative traits (analyzed via linear regression) are listed first in the table, followed by diagnoses and binary traits (analyzed via logistic regression models). BP, blood pressure; FEV1, forced expiratory volume in 1 second; FVC, forced vital capacity; BPD, bipolar disorder; MDD, major depressive disorder; df, degrees of freedom; SE, standard error.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1007556.s010

(DOCX)

S1 Fig. Distribution of ROH lengths in Kb (left) and FROH (right) in the UK Biobank sample.

The histogram of ROH lengths is cut off at 6500 Kb for clarity; similarly, the histogram of FROH is cut off at 0.05 for clarity. There were 583 individuals who had FROH > 0.05; these individuals were not excluded from analyses.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1007556.s011

(DOCX)

S2 Fig. Distribution of FSNP in the UK Biobank sample.

The histogram of FSNP is cut off at |0.01| for clarity; 77 individuals had levels of SNP-by-SNP homozygosity greater than 0.1 or less than -0.1.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1007556.s012

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We thank Chick Judd for his statistical guidance on testing mediation models, Loic Yengo for helpful discussions regarding different methods of quantifying genome-wide autozygosity, as well as Brooke Huibregtse for her valuable input regarding some of the UK Biobank phenotypes. This research has been conducted using the UK Biobank Resource under Application Numbers ‘1665’, ‘16651’, and ‘24795’. This work utilized the RMACC Summit supercomputer, which is supported by the National Science Foundation (awards ACI-1532235 and ACI-1532236), the University of Colorado Boulder, and Colorado State University. The Summit supercomputer is a joint effort of the University of Colorado Boulder and Colorado State University.

References

  1. 1. Darwin, C. The effects of cross and self fertilisation in the vegetable kingdom. (J. Murray, 1876).
  2. 2. Walsh B. Evolutionary Quantitative Genetics. in Handbook of Statistical Genetics: Third Edition 1, 533–586 (2008).
  3. 3. Charlesworth D. & Willis J. H. The genetics of inbreeding depression. Nat. Rev. Genet. 10, 783–96 (2009). pmid:19834483
  4. 4. Sewall-Wright, S. Evolution and the genetics of populations. Vol. 3: experimental results and evolutionary deductions. (1977).
  5. 5. Keller M. C., Visscher P. M. & Goddard M. E. Quantification of inbreeding due to distant ancestors and its detection using dense single nucleotide polymorphism data. Genetics 189, 237–249 (2011). pmid:21705750
  6. 6. Kirin M. et al. Genomic runs of homozygosity record population history and consanguinity. PLoS One 5, (2010).
  7. 7. Joshi P. K. et al. Directional dominance on stature and cognition in diverse human populations. Nature 523, 459–462 (2015). pmid:26131930
  8. 8. Howrigan D. P. et al. Genome-wide autozygosity is associated with lower general cognitive ability. Mol Psychiatry (2015).
  9. 9. Verweij K. J. H. et al. The Association of Genotype-Based Inbreeding Coefficient with a Range of Physical and Psychological Human Traits. PLoS One 9, e103102 (2014). pmid:25062308
  10. 10. Yengo L. et al. Detection and quantification of inbreeding depression for complex traits from SNP data. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 114, 8602 LP-8607 (2017).
  11. 11. Johnson E. C. et al. No Reliable Association between Runs of Homozygosity and Schizophrenia in a Well-Powered Replication Study. PLoS Genet. 12, (2016).
  12. 12. Power R. A. et al. A recessive genetic model and runs of homozygosity in major depressive disorder. Am. J. Med. Genet. Part B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 165, 157–166 (2014).
  13. 13. Rudan I. et al. Inbreeding and the genetic complexity of human hypertension. Genetics 163, 1011–1021 (2003). pmid:12663539
  14. 14. Campbell H. et al. Effects of genome-wide heterozygosity on a range of biomedically relevant human quantitative traits. Hum. Mol. Genet. 16, 233–241 (2007). pmid:17220173
  15. 15. Abdellaoui A. et al. Association between autozygosity and major depression: Stratification due to religious assortment. Behav. Genet. 43, 455–467 (2013). pmid:23978897
  16. 16. Koenig H. G. Research on religion, spirituality, and mental health: A review. Can. J. Psychiatry 54, 283–291 (2009). pmid:19497160
  17. 17. Vine A. E. et al. No evidence for excess runs of homozygosity in bipolar disorder. Psychiatr. Genet. 19, 165–170 (2009). pmid:19451863
  18. 18. Heron E. A. et al. No evidence that runs of homozygosity are associated with schizophrenia in an Irish genome-wide association dataset. Schizophr. Res. 154, 79–82 (2014). pmid:24560374
  19. 19. McQuillan R. et al. Runs of Homozygosity in European Populations. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 83, 359–372 (2008). pmid:18760389
  20. 20. Keller M. C. et al. Runs of homozygosity implicate autozygosity as a schizophrenia risk factor. PLoS Genet. 8, (2012).
  21. 21. Ruderfer D. M. et al. No evidence for rare recessive and compound heterozygous disruptive variants in schizophrenia. Eur. J. Hum. Genet. 23, 1–3 (2014).
  22. 22. Yengo L. et al. Reply to Kardos et al.: Estimation of inbreeding depression from SNP data. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. (2018).
  23. 23. Bycroft C. et al. Genome-wide genetic data on ~500,000 UK Biobank participants. bioRxiv (2017).
  24. 24. Okbay A. et al. Genome-wide association study identifies 74 loci associated with educational attainment. Nature 533, 539–542 (2016). pmid:27225129
  25. 25. Phillimore P., Beattie A. & Townsend P. Widening inequality of health in northern England, 1981–91. BMJ 308, 1125 (1994). pmid:8173452
  26. 26. Gudnadottir M., Sigurdur Gunnarsson B. & Thorsdottir I. Effects of sociodemographic factors on adherence to breastfeeding and other important infant dietary recommendations. Acta Pædiatrica 95, 419–424 (2006). pmid:16720488
  27. 27. Abdellaoui A. et al. Educational attainment influences levels of homozygosity through migration and assortative mating. PLoS One 10, (2015).
  28. 28. Yang J., Lee S. H., Goddard M. E. & Visscher P. M. GCTA: A tool for genome-wide complex trait analysis. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 88, 76–82 (2011). pmid:21167468
  29. 29. Chang C. C. et al. Second-generation PLINK: rising to the challenge of larger and richer datasets. Gigascience 4, 7 (2015). pmid:25722852
  30. 30. Fisher R. A. A fuller theory of ‘Junctions’ in inbreeding. Heredity (Edinb). 8, 187–197 (1954).
  31. 31. Afzal M. Consequences of consanguinity on cognitive behavior. Behav. Genet. 18, 583–594 (1988). pmid:3190642
  32. 32. McQuillan R. et al. Evidence of inbreeding depression on human height. PLoS Genet. 8, (2012).
  33. 33. Charlesworth B. & Charlesworth D. The genetic basis of inbreeding depression. Genet. Res. 74, 329–340 (1999). pmid:10689809
  34. 34. Gottfredson L. S. & Deary I. J. Intelligence Predicts Health and Longevity, but Why? Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 13, 1–4 (2004).
  35. 35. Stavem K. et al. Lung function, smoking and mortality in a 26-year follow-up of healthy middle-aged males. Eur. Respir. J. 25, 618–625 (2005). pmid:15802334
  36. 36. Beaty T. H. et al. Impaired pulmonary function as a risk factor for mortality. Am. J. Epidemiol. 116, 102–113 (1982). pmid:7102646
  37. 37. Hole D. J. et al. Impaired lung function and mortality risk in men and women: findings from the Renfrew and Paisley prospective population study. BMJ 313, 711–715 (1996). pmid:8819439
  38. 38. Higgins M. W. & Keller J. B. Predictors of mortality in the adult population of Tecumseh: respiratory symptoms, chronic respiratory disease, and ventilatory lung function. Arch. Environ. Heal. An Int. J. 21, 418–424 (1970).
  39. 39. Keller M. C. Evolutionary Perspectives on Genetic and Environmental Risk Factors for Psychiatric Disorders. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. (2018).
  40. 40. DeRose M. A. & Roff D. A. A Comparison of Inbreeding Depression in Life-History and Morphological Traits in Animals. Evolution (N. Y). 53, 1288 (1999).
  41. 41. Rhodes G., Simmons L. W. & Peters M. Attractiveness and sexual behavior: Does attractiveness enhance mating success? Evol. Hum. Behav. 26, 186–201 (2018).
  42. 42. Weeden J. & Sabini J. Subjective and objective measures of attractiveness and their relation to sexual behavior and sexual attitudes in university students. Arch. Sex. Behav. 36, 79–88 (2007). pmid:17136591
  43. 43. Dubow E. F., Boxer P. & Huesmann L. R. Long-term Effects of Parents’ Education on Children’s Educational and Occupational Success: Mediation by Family Interactions, Child Aggression, and Teenage Aspirations. Merrill. Palmer. Q. (Wayne State Univ. Press) 55, 224–249 (2009).
  44. 44. Dearden L., Machin S. & Reed H. Intergenerational mobility in Britain. Econ. J. 107, 47–66 (1997).
  45. 45. Voas D. & Crockett A. Religion in Britain: Neither Believing nor Belonging. Sociology 39, 11–28 (2005).
  46. 46. Aschard H., Vilhjálmsson B. J., Joshi A. D., Price A. L. & Kraft P. Adjusting for heritable covariates can bias effect estimates in genome-wide association studies. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 96, 329–339 (2015). pmid:25640676
  47. 47. Munafò M. R., Tilling K., Taylor A. E., Evans D. M. & Davey Smith G. Collider scope: when selection bias can substantially influence observed associations. Int. J. Epidemiol. (2017). pmid:29040562
  48. 48. Glymour M. M. Using causal diagrams to understand common problems in social epidemiology. Methods Soc. Epidemiol. 387–422 (2006).
  49. 49. Greenland S. Quantifying biases in causal models: Classical confounding vs collider-stratification bias. Epidemiology 14, 300–306 (2003). pmid:12859030
  50. 50. Bjelland D. W., Weigel K. A., Vukasinovic N. & Nkrumah J. D. Evaluation of inbreeding depression in Holstein cattle using whole-genome SNP markers and alternative measures of genomic inbreeding. J. Dairy Sci. 96, 4697–4706 (2013). pmid:23684028
  51. 51. Howard J. T., Pryce J. E., Baes C. & Maltecca C. Invited review: Inbreeding in the genomics era: Inbreeding, inbreeding depression, and management of genomic variability. J. Dairy Sci. (2017).