Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Regional White Matter Volumes Correlate with Delay Discounting

  • Rongjun Yu

    rongjun.yu@gmail.com

    Affiliations Department of Psychology and Center for Studies of Psychological Application, South China Normal University, GuangZhou, GuangDong, China, MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom

Abstract

A preference for immediate gratification is a central feature in addictive processes. However, the neural structures underlying reward delay tolerance are still unclear. Healthy participants (n = 121) completed a delay discounting questionnaire assessing the extent to which they prefer smaller immediate rewards to larger delayed reward after undergoing magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scanning. Whole brain voxel-based morphometric analysis shows that delay discounting severity was negatively correlated with right prefrontal subgyral white matter volume and positively correlated with white matter volume in parahippocampus/hippocampus, after whole brain correction. This study might better our understanding of the neural basis of impulsivity and addiction.

Introduction

People prefer to obtain reward immediately rather than in future. Such preference has been vividly demonstrated in the delay of gratification paradigm commonly used in developmental and personality psychology. In the famous ‘marshmallow test’, which measures how long a child can resist eating a small immediate reward in order to get a larger reward some time later, it has been shown that 4-year-old children who delayed gratification longer developed into more cognitively and socially competent adolescents, achieving higher scholastic performance and coping better with frustration and stress [1]. The interpretation of delaying gratification focuses on individuals' willpower to resist the temptations of the immediate reward. Although not the equivalent measures [2], delay discounting procedures are used in behavioural economics and nonhuman animal behavioural studies to precisely assess delay-related impulsive behaviour. Delay discounting refers to that phenomenon that the present value of a future reward reduces as the delay to that reward increases. Delay discount rates are relatively stable over time periods of more than 1 year [3], [4]. They correlate negatively with college grade point average [5], as well as adolescent academic performance [6]. A number of studies have found steeper discounting functions among various substance abusers, such as alcoholics, drug addicts, or heavy smokers, than among those of matched control groups [7], [8], [9], [10], [11], [12], [13], [14], [15]. Although making choices in favor of long-term goals is important for both individuals and our society, the underlying neural correlates of delay related impulsive behaviours in humans are still not clear.

Since the delay of gratification paradigm (e.g. Stanford marshmallow experiment) only provides qualitative measure of impulsivity, few neuroimaging studies have been conducted using this paradigm. In a recent longitudinal study using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), low delayers, those who have low resistance to temptation, as measured originally by the delay of gratification task 40 years ago, showed lesser recruitment of inferior frontal gyrus and greater recruitment of the ventral striatum when resisting alluring cues [16], suggesting that resisting temptation is supported by frontostriatal circuitries.

A large number of human neuroimaging studies have used the delay discounting paradigm to investigate the neural associates of delay discounting choices but the findings are inconsistent. Some studies found specific brain regions, such as ventral striatum, medial orbitofrontal cortex, medial prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, and left posterior hippocampus, are preferentially activated by decisions involving immediately available rewards [17], [18], [19]. However, another study found that brain regions, including the ventral striatum, medial prefrontal cortex and posterior cingulate cortex are engaged in evaluating both delay reward and immediate reward [20]. Lesion studies in humans also found mixed results. One study reported that delay discounting is not affected by lesion to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex [21], whereas another study found that damage to medial orbitofrontal cortex increased significantly the preference for small-immediate over larger-delayed rewards [22]. These studies raise questions on whether there are some specific brain regions dedicated to delay discounting processing or delay discounting is supported by a general brain network, e.g. brain regions that support cognitive calculation and general decision making. One approach to answer this question is to correlate individual differences in delay discounting with brain structure characteristics.

To date, only one preliminary study has investigated the correlation between delay discounting rates and prefrontal grey matter volume in a small group (n = 29) using a region of interest (ROI) approach [23]. Here, voxel-based morphometry (VBM) is used to examine the associations between regional white matter (WM) and gray matter (GM) volumes and individual differences in delay discounting. Compared to the ROI approach, which manually delineate GM/WM volumes in pre-specified regions only, VBM allows for examining the entire brain on a voxel-by-voxel basis in a fully automated manner, without having to specify in advance regions of interest. Delay discounting was chosen because it provides a precise quantitative measurement of delay-based impulsive behaviours. Previous neuroimaging studies in humans and animal studies have identified candidate brain regions in which activity was associated with delay discounting, especially the prefrontal and temporal regions [24], [25]. I hypothesized that the local structure of these regions might reflect an individual's ability to tolerate reward delay.

Methods

Participants

One hundred and twenty one healthy right-handed volunteers (58 male, mean age and SD 25.27±5.16, ranging from 18 to 49) participated in the experiment. Participants were recruited from the community through advertisements. All subjects were screened for psychiatric and nonpsychiatric medical disorders using the Structured Clinical Interview for the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (DSM-IV; SCID). All subjects were right-handed, had no history of any neurological or psychiatric disorder, had no drug dependence, and were not currently taking any medications. Participants were compensated for their participation with 20 pounds. All recruited participants were informed about all procedures of the experiment and written informed consents were obtained. This study was approved by the Cambridge Research Ethics Committee review board.

Questionnaire

Outside scanner, participants were asked to fill in the delay discounting questionnaire, which has good to excellent internal consistency [6]. There are 27 choice trials in the questionnaire. Each trial consists of one smaller, immediate reward and one larger, delayed reward (i.e., “Would you prefer £54 today, or £55 in 117 days?”). Participants indicate which alternative they would prefer to receive by circling it. The behavioural choices were fitted to the hyperbolic function: V = A/(1+kD), where V is the value of the delayed outcome (i.e., the indifference value), A is the fixed delayed reward, D is the length of the delay, and k expresses the steepness of the discount function. For example, one offered participants a choice between ‘£33 today’ and ‘£80 in 14 days’. A participant with a discount rate of 0.10 would be indifferent between these two rewards. Therefore, if a participant chose the immediate reward on this trial, then one could infer that this person has a discount rate greater than 0.10. Another question offered participants a choice between ‘£31 today’ and ‘£85 in 7 days’. A participant with a discount rate of 0.25 would be indifferent between these two rewards. Therefore, if the same participant chose the delayed reward on this trial, then one could infer that this person has a discount rate less than 0.25. The two trials together imply that this person has a discount rate between 0.10 and 0.25. We used the geometric mean of this interval as our estimate of the person's k value. In the example, this yields k = 0.16. The participant was assigned to the value that yielded the highest consistency among his or her choices. Finally, to obtain a single rate estimate for each participant, we computed the geometric mean of the three rates for the small, medium and large reward magnitudes [15]. Higher values of k indicate that the delayed rewards are being discounted more steeply, meaning that the subject is more impulsive. Because raw k values were skewed toward low values and were not normally distributed, the discounting parameter k was normalized by logarithmic transformation (hereafter lnK) for subsequent analysis. Individuals with consistency lower than 75% were excluded in the following analysis.

Scanning Procedure

A high-resolution structural magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo scan (voxel size = 1.3×1.3×1 mm, repetition time = 2250 ms, echo time = 2.99 ms, inversion time = 900 ms, flip angle = 9°, total scan time = 4 min 16 s) was acquired on a Siemens 3-T MRI scanner for all participants.

Data Analysis

VBM analysis was performed in SPM5 (Welcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London, UK), which enables automated spatial normalization, tissue classification, and radiofrequency bias correction to be combined within the segmentation step. The segmented images were smoothed using a Gaussian kernel of 8-mm full-width at half-maximum (FWHM). An absolute threshold mask was set at 0.1 to ensure that the voxels included in the analysis had a higher probability of being WM or GM, respectively. Global WM/GM volume was included as a covariate of no interest in the analysis of WM/GM volume differences, to account for any gross differences in total WM/GM volume across participants [26]. For example, the correlation between WM and Ink was controlled for global WM (and not GM) and the converse was done for the correlation between GM and Ink. Age and gender were also included as covariates of no interest in all models [27]. A false discovery rate (FDR) corrected threshold of P<0.05 at cluster level was set, corresponding to voxel-wise P<0.001, cluster size = 162, shared edge.

Results

The lnK was −4.80±SD 1.40, ranging from −8.29 to −1.84. There was no gender difference, p>0.3. The lnK was not associated with age, global WM volume, or global GM volume (p values >0.05). Similar to previous studies, the consistency of choices are very high [15], [28]. Mean consistencies were 98%±4%, 98%±4%, and 97%±5%, for the small, medium, and large delayed reward conditions, respectively. The lowest consistency was 78%. The high consistency in participants' choices suggests that they were generally very careful in making their choices, even though rewards were hypothetical.

The whole brain analysis revealed that WM in the right parahippocampus extending to the right hippocampus [40,−22,−22, peak z = 4.57, cluster size = 365 voxels, cluster level corrected PFDR<0.005] showed a positively correlation with the lnK, whereas WM volumes of the right prefrontal subgyral area [20,−6,38, peak z = 3.46, cluster size = 162 voxels, cluster level corrected PFDR<0.05] showed a negative correlation with the lnK (see Figure 1). Prefrontal cluster still significantly correlated with discounting rates even after the data point that may be an influential outlier was removed from the analysis. These results are whole brain corrected and controlled for age, gender and absolute whole brain WM volumes. No significant correlation between regional GM volume and the InK was found after whole brain correction. For the completeness' sake, results were also shown at P<0.001 with a 10 voxel extent threshold, uncorrected (see Table 1).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Regional white matter volume correlated with discounting severity.

White matter volume in the parahippocampus/hippocampus (A) and right prefrontal subgyral area (B) correlated with delay discounting rates after whole brain correction. Scatterplot shows the size of white matter volume changes in the parahippocampus/hippocampus (C) and the right prefrontal subgyral area (D) at the peak voxel (a voxel with local maxima) as a function of delay discounting rates (i.e. the Ink). For display purposes, maps are thresholded at p<0.001, uncorrected.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0032595.g001

thumbnail
Table 1. Regional white matter (WM) and grey matter (GM) volume showing significant correlations with discounting severity (i.e. the Ink) at P<0.001 with a 10 voxel extent threshold, uncorrected.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0032595.t001

Discussion

In this VBM study, I found that individuals who have steeper delay discounting rates (more impulsive) have smaller WM volumes in the prefrontal cortex. They also have relatively larger WM volumes in the right parahippocampus and hippocampus.

The prefrontal cortex has been implicated in regulating cognitive and emotional processes [29]. A recent diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) study showed that WM integrity in the right frontal region cluster and in the left temporal lobe cluster was negatively correlated with discounting rates [30]. Another morphometric study (n = 29) using region of interest (ROI) analysis has revealed a negative partial correlation between lnK and the GM volume in the lateral frontal cortex [23]. Our finding of reduced WM volume in the right prefrontal subgyral area converges with these results and suggests that the cognitive control dysfunction may lead to the inability to resist the temptation of immediate reward [29].

It is interesting to note that WM volumes in the parahippocampus/hippocampus were positively correlated with discounting rates. Lesion of hippocampus in rats leads to high impulsive behaviours, such as more likely to choose small but immediate reward [24]. Neuroimaging studies in humans have shown that the parahippocampus/hippocampus plays an important role in self-projection into the future [31]. Increased functional coupling between ACC and hippocampus was associated with increasing shifts toward more future-minded choice behaviour [32]. The parahippocampal gyri exhibited enhanced activity for long versus short delay, suggesting that during intertemporal choice, decision makers simulate the impending delay via a process of prospection [33]. Patients with hippocampal amnesia also cannot imagine new experiences [34]. It is possible that an individual's discounting of future events depends on his or her ability to remember past events. Consideration and valuation of the future events has been shown to overlap with processes associated with memory or valuation of the past [35]. Discounting of future rewards are correlated with discounting of past rewards [36]. A recent study demonstrated that rates of discounting of delayed rewards were significantly reduced among those who received memory training but were unchanged among those who received control training [37]. An interesting hypothesis for future studies to test is that hippocampal human patients would show abnormal temporal discounting. Together with the present work, these findings emphasize an important role for parahippocampus/hippocampus in delay discounting. Because the functional significance of larger white matter volumes is still not clear, the directionality of the correlation should be interpreted with caution. It is worth mentioning that size does not always correlate positively with performance. Larger regional white matter volumes are not always beneficial and they do not necessarily suggest better functions associated with those regions. For example, previous studies found that larger white matter volume was positively associated with number of intrusions [38]. Further research is needed to systematically relate white matter volume to memory ability before any firm conclusions can be made.

The basal ganglia also play a crucial in reward processing. Specially, ventral striatum has been found to mediate the delay discounting in several fMRI studies [39], [40]. Individuals who discounted future reward more steeply, as measured outside the scanner, exhibited more recruitment in the ventral striatum in response to both gains and losses [39]. Caudate lesion rats showed abnormal preference to immediate reward [41]. It is surprising that no significant difference in WM or GM among these regions was found in the present study. One possibility is that dynamic BOLD responses to immediate rewards at the individual level are highly variable and may highly depend on the experimental contexts [17], [20], [42], [43]. Whether there is a structural correlation in this region with delay discounting or not waits for future investigation.

There are some limitations of this study which should be considered in interpreting our results. First, I used hypothetical rather than real rewards. Previous studies suggest that hypothetical and real rewards are discounted similarly [9], [44], [45], [46]. Nevertheless, future studies are needed to replicate the current investigation using real rewards. Second, one may argue that the monetary delay discounting task used was quite short and may not provide the most precise estimate of discount rates. Although the present version of task has been used extensively in previous studies [15], [47], [48], [49], future studies may use a longer task to replicate the current findings. Third, the IQ scores were not measured in this study. IQ is associated with delay discounting [50], [51], as well as with brain structural features including grey matter density and thickness [52], [53]. Other information regarding demographic characteristics, such as years of education [54], ethnic background [55], was not collected. These variables may contribute to the effects observed in the current study. Future studies should carefully control for these variables and examine their influence on the brain structures. Finally, our sample may not be representative of the general population, since individuals with a history of any psychiatric disorder and individuals taking any medications were excluded. However, a large number of studies have demonstrated that psychiatric disorders and medications modulate brain structures [56]. Using a healthy sample allows controlling for confounding factors such as psychiatric disorders and medications.

In conclusion, this study provides further evidence that individual differences in delay discounting are associated with the volume of specific brain structures. It highlights the possibility that our ability to tolerant reward delay may be associated with brain regions implicated in executive control and mental simulation.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: RY. Performed the experiments: RY. Analyzed the data: RY. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: RY. Wrote the paper: RY.

References

  1. 1. Mischel W, Shoda Y, Rodriguez MI (1989) Delay of gratification in children. Science 244: 933–938.
  2. 2. Reynolds B, Schiffbauer R (2005) Delay of Gratification and Delay Discounting: A unifying feedback model of delay-related impulsive behavior. Psychological Record 55: 439–460.
  3. 3. Kirby KN (2009) One-year temporal stability of delay-discount rates. Psychon Bull Rev 16: 457–462.
  4. 4. Ohmura Y, Takahashi T, Kitamura N, Wehr P (2006) Three-month stability of delay and probability discounting measures. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 14: 318–328.
  5. 5. Kirby K, Winston G, Santiesteban M (2005) Impatience and grades: Delay-discount rates correlate negatively with college GPA. Learning and Individual Differences 213–222.
  6. 6. Duckworth AL, Seligman ME (2005) Self-discipline outdoes IQ in predicting academic performance of adolescents. Psychological Science 16: 939–944.
  7. 7. Odum AL, Madden GJ, Bickel WK (2002) Discounting of delayed health gains and losses by current, never- and ex-smokers of cigarettes. Nicotine Tob Res 4: 295–303.
  8. 8. Bickel WK, Odum AL, Madden GJ (1999) Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 146: 447–454.
  9. 9. Madden GJ, Begotka AM, Raiff BR, Kastern LL (2003) Delay discounting of real and hypothetical rewards. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 11: 139–145.
  10. 10. Petry NM (2002) Discounting of delayed rewards in substance abusers: relationship to antisocial personality disorder. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 162: 425–432.
  11. 11. Petry NM (2001) Delay discounting of money and alcohol in actively using alcoholics, currently abstinent alcoholics, and controls. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 154: 243–250.
  12. 12. Petry NM, Casarella T (1999) Excessive discounting of delayed rewards in substance abusers with gambling problems. Drug Alcohol Depend 56: 25–32.
  13. 13. Ohmura Y, Takahashi T, Kitamura N (2005) Discounting delayed and probabilistic monetary gains and losses by smokers of cigarettes. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 182: 508–515.
  14. 14. Kirby KN, Petry NM (2004) Heroin and cocaine abusers have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than alcoholics or non-drug-using controls. Addiction 99: 461–471.
  15. 15. Kirby KN, Petry NM, Bickel WK (1999) Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General 128: 78–87.
  16. 16. Casey BJ, Somerville LH, Gotlib IH, Ayduk O, Franklin NT, et al. (2011) Behavioral and neural correlates of delay of gratification 40 years later. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108: 14998–15003.
  17. 17. McClure SM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD (2004) Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science 306: 503–507.
  18. 18. Ballard K, Knutson B (2009) Dissociable neural representations of future reward magnitude and delay during temporal discounting. Neuroimage 45: 143–150.
  19. 19. McClure SM, Ericson KM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD (2007) Time discounting for primary rewards. Journal of Neuroscience 27: 5796–5804.
  20. 20. Kable JW, Glimcher PW (2007) The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice. Nature Neuroscience 10: 1625–1633.
  21. 21. Fellows LK, Farah MJ (2005) Dissociable elements of human foresight: a role for the ventromedial frontal lobes in framing the future, but not in discounting future rewards. Neuropsychologia 43: 1214–1221.
  22. 22. Sellitto M, Ciaramelli E, di Pellegrino G (2010) Myopic Discounting of Future Rewards after Medial Orbitofrontal Damage in Humans. Journal of Neuroscience 30: 16429–16436.
  23. 23. Bjork JM, Momenan R, Hommer DW (2009) Delay discounting correlates with proportional lateral frontal cortex volumes. Biological Psychiatry 65: 710–713.
  24. 24. Mariano TY, Bannerman DM, McHugh SB, Preston TJ, Rudebeck PH, et al. (2009) Impulsive choice in hippocampal but not orbitofrontal cortex-lesioned rats on a nonspatial decision-making maze task. European Journal of Neuroscience 30: 472–484.
  25. 25. Peters J, Buchel C (2009) Overlapping and distinct neural systems code for subjective value during intertemporal and risky decision making. Journal of Neuroscience 29: 15727–15734.
  26. 26. von dem Hagen EA, Nummenmaa L, Yu R, Engell AD, Ewbank MP, et al. (2011) Autism spectrum traits in the typical population predict structure and function in the posterior superior temporal sulcus. Cerebral Cortex 21: 493–500.
  27. 27. Steinberg L, Graham S, O'Brien L, Woolard J, Cauffman E, et al. (2009) Age Differences in Future Orientation and Delay Discounting. Child Development 80: 28–44.
  28. 28. Kirby KN, Marakovic NN (1996) Delay-discounting probabilistic rewards: Rates decrease as amounts increase. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 3: 100–104.
  29. 29. Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND (2002) Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry 159: 1642–1652.
  30. 30. Olson EA, Collins PF, Hooper CJ, Muetzel R, Lim KO, et al. (2009) White matter integrity predicts delay discounting behavior in 9- to 23-year-olds: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21: 1406–1421.
  31. 31. Schacter DL, Addis DR, Buckner RL (2007) Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8: 657–661.
  32. 32. Peters J, Buchel C (2010) Episodic future thinking reduces reward delay discounting through an enhancement of prefrontal-mediotemporal interactions. Neuron 66: 138–148.
  33. 33. Luhmann CC, Chun MM, Yi DJ, Lee D, Wang XJ (2008) Neural dissociation of delay and uncertainty in intertemporal choice. J Neurosci 28: 14459–14466.
  34. 34. Hassabis D, Kumaran D, Vann SD, Maguire EA (2007) Patients with hippocampal amnesia cannot imagine new experiences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104: 1726–1731.
  35. 35. Buckner RL, Carroll DC (2007) Self-projection and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11: 49–57.
  36. 36. Bickel WK, Yi R, Kowal BR, Gatchalian KM (2008) Cigarette smokers discount past and future rewards symmetrically and more than controls: Is discounting a measure of impulsivity? Drug and Alcohol Dependence 96: 256–262.
  37. 37. Bickel WK, Yi R, Landes RD, Hill PF, Baxter C (2011) Remember the Future: Working Memory Training Decreases Delay Discounting Among Stimulant Addicts. Biological Psychiatry 69: 260–265.
  38. 38. Walhovd KB, Fjell AM, Reinvang I, Lundervold A, Fischl B, et al. (2004) Size does matter in the long run - Hippocampal and cortical volume predict recall across weeks. Neurology 63: 1193–1197.
  39. 39. Hariri AR, Brown SM, Williamson DE, Flory JD, de Wit H, et al. (2006) Preference for immediate over delayed rewards is associated with magnitude of ventral striatal activity. J Neurosci 26: 13213–13217.
  40. 40. Wittmann M, Lovero KL, Lane SD, Paulus MP (2010) Now or later? Striatum and insula activation to immediate versus delayed rewards. J Neurosci Psychol Econ 3: 15–26.
  41. 41. Cardinal RN, Pennicott DR, Sugathapala CL, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ (2001) Impulsive choice induced in rats by lesions of the nucleus accumbens core. Science 292: 2499–2501.
  42. 42. Knutson B, Adams CM, Fong GW, Hommer D (2001) Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. Journal of Neuroscience 21: art. no.-RC159.
  43. 43. Bjork JM, Smith AR, Chen G, Hommer DW (2010) Adolescents, Adults and Rewards: Comparing Motivational Neurocircuitry Recruitment Using fMRI. Plos One 5:
  44. 44. Lagorio CH, Madden GJ (2005) Delay discounting of real and hypothetical rewards III: steady-state assessments, forced-choice trials, and all real rewards. Behav Processes 69: 173–187.
  45. 45. Madden GJ, Raiff BR, Lagorio CH, Begotka AM, Mueller AM, et al. (2004) Delay discounting of potentially real and hypothetical rewards: II. Between- and within-subject comparisons. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 12: 251–261.
  46. 46. Johnson MW, Bickel WK (2002) Within-subject comparison of real and hypothetical money rewards in delay discounting. J Exp Anal Behav 77: 129–146.
  47. 47. Reynolds B, Leraas K, Collins C, Melanko S (2009) Delay discounting by the children of smokers and nonsmokers. Drug Alcohol Depend 99: 350–353.
  48. 48. Housden CR, O'Sullivan SS, Joyce EM, Lees AJ, Roiser JP (2010) Intact reward learning but elevated delay discounting in Parkinson's disease patients with impulsive-compulsive spectrum behaviors. Neuropsychopharmacology 35: 2155–2164.
  49. 49. Epstein LH, Richards JB, Saad FG, Paluch RA, Roemmich JN, et al. (2003) Comparison between two measures of delay discounting in smokers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 11: 131–138.
  50. 50. Olson EA, Hooper CJ, Collins P, Luciana M (2007) Adolescents' performance on delay and probability discounting tasks: Contributions of age, intelligence, executive functioning, and self-reported externalizing behavior. Personality and Individual Differences 43: 1886–1897.
  51. 51. de Wit H, Flory JD, Acheson A, McCloskey M, Manuck SB (2007) IQ and nonplanning impulsivity are independently associated with delay discounting in middle-aged adults. Personality and Individual Differences 42: 111–121.
  52. 52. Narr KL, Woods RP, Thompson PM, Szeszko P, Robinson D, et al. (2007) Relationships between IQ and regional cortical gray matter thickness in healthy adults. Cerebral Cortex 17: 2163–2171.
  53. 53. Shaw P, Greenstein D, Lerch J, Clasen L, Lenroot R, et al. (2006) Intellectual ability and cortical development in children and adolescents. Nature 440: 676–679.
  54. 54. Jaroni JL, Wright SM, Lerman C, Epstein LH (2004) Relationship between education and delay discounting in smokers. Addictive Behaviors 29: 1171–1175.
  55. 55. Weatherly JN, McDonald JD (2011) Delay Discounting of Different Outcomes in a Sample of American Indian and Non-Indian College Students. American Indian and Alaska Native Mental Health Research 17: 43–55.
  56. 56. Bodnar M, Harvey PO, Malla AK, Joober R, Lepage M (2011) The parahippocampal gyrus as a neural marker of early remission in first-episode psychosis: a voxel-based morphometry study. Clin Schizophr Relat Psychoses 4: 217–228.