Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Calling, Courtship, and Condition in the Fall Field Cricket, Gryllus pennsylvanicus

Abstract

Theoretically, sexual signals should provide honest information about mating benefits and many sexually reproducing species use honest signals when signalling to potential mates. Male crickets produce two types of acoustic mating signals: a long-distance mate attraction call and a short-range courtship call. We tested whether wild-caught fall field cricket (Gryllus pennsylvanicus) males in high condition (high residual mass or large body size) produce higher effort calls (in support of the honest signalling hypothesis). We also tested an alternative hypothesis, whether low condition males produce higher effort calls (in support of the terminal investment hypothesis). Several components of long-distance mate attraction calls honestly reflected male body size, with larger males producing louder mate attraction calls at lower carrier frequencies. Long-distance mate attraction chirp rate dishonestly signalled body size, with small males producing faster chirp rates. Short-range courtship calls dishonestly reflected male residual mass, as chirp rate and pulse rate were best explained by a curvilinear function of residual mass. By producing long-distance mate attraction calls and courtship calls with similar or higher effort compared to high condition males, low condition males (low residual mass or small body size) may increase their effort in current reproductive success at the expense of their future reproductive success, suggesting that not all sexual signals are honest.

Introduction

Honest signalling was termed “ubiquitous” in a recent game theory paper modeling signal evolution [1]. Biological signals are considered honest when they provide useful information to the receiver [2]. More specifically, sexual signals are considered honest when they indicate the potential benefits that a female could receive by mating with an advertising male [3][10]. Sexual signals may indicate mating benefits because (1) preferred signals are often costly to produce; (2) only males good at acquiring nutrients or using them efficiently may be able to support the costs of signal production; and (3) males with more nutrients may be of higher fitness or may be able to invest more in providing benefits to females. Males that are able to obtain more nutrients may have greater energy stores, and several studies have found a positive relationship between dietary nutrient availability and sexual signalling [11][16]. Given these points, females may benefit from selecting mates that exhibit condition-dependent signals. Here condition is defined as variation in resource acquisition ability [17], which may result from differences in resource availability in the environment and/or individual physiological differences in the ability to assimilate and utilize resources.

The cost of producing sexual signals is often dependent on available nutrients, which in turn is subject to life-history trade-offs. Allocating nutrients to sexual signalling must, therefore, be balanced against the nutrients required for growth and survival [8], [18], [19]. High condition males with an abundance of nutrients may be better able to afford the costs of allocating resources to sexual signalling than poor condition males (i.e. honest signalling [20]). Alternatively, poor condition males with reduced future reproductive potential may allocate more nutrients towards sexual signalling, thereby maximizing their current reproductive success at the expense of their future reproductive success (terminal investment hypothesis) [21][25]. When this occurs, one should see sexual signals being unreliable indicators of condition. There are several examples of poor condition males signalling to females with higher effort than high condition males [22], [26][29]. Game theory models reveal that dishonest sexual signals can be maintained provided the signals are honest on average, and the frequency of cheaters is low enough that receivers are more often likely to benefit from trusting that signals are honest [21], [25].

Here we investigate whether male sexual signals are honest indicators of condition, using body size and residual mass as proxies. Male field crickets (Gryllinae) rub their forewings together to produce two types of multicomponent acoustic signals (calls) to attract and court potential mates [30]. Males produce a long-distance mate attraction call to broadcast to distant females. Once they come into physical contact with a female they switch to quieter short-range courtship calls [30]. Variation in long-distance mate attraction calls influences male mating success, as females tend to phonolocate towards males that call most often (Gryllus integer: [31]; G. campestris: [32]; Teleogryllus commodus: [33], [34]), with higher chirp rates and longer chirp durations (G. lineaticeps: [35]), and longer signalling bout durations (G. integer: [36], [37]). While comparatively little is known about female preference for short-range courtship calls, female crickets appear to prefer courtship calls with higher chirp rates (G. lineaticeps: [38]), higher tick rates with longer durations of higher frequency ticks (G. bimaculatus: [39]), and higher sound rates with longer chirp, pulse, and trill durations (T. oceanicus: [40]). Assuming that females base their mating decisions on both long-distance mate attraction calls and short-range courtship calls, males may use these calls to honestly convey possible mating benefits to potential mates.

While several studies have shown long-distance mate attraction calls to be nutrition-dependent (e.g. [12][14], [27], [32], [41][43]), short-range courtship calls have generally been found to not be nutrition dependent (e.g. [23], [38]). This lack of support for courtship calling nutrition-dependence may result from (1) less rigorous examinations of the fine scale temporal aspects of courtship calls compared to long-distance mate attraction calling studies, (2) experimental diets not reflecting natural feeding regimes, (3) high breeding densities of laboratory-reared crickets altering selection pressures on male calls, or (4) males in poor condition artificially inflating their calls to maximize their current reproductive success at the expense of their future reproduction.

We use fall field crickets (Gryllus pennsylvanicus) to test two alternative hypotheses: (1) whether males in high condition (high residual mass or large body size) produce higher effort calls in support of the hypothesis that males signal honestly; and (2) whether low condition males (low residual mass or small body size) produce higher effort calls in support of the terminal investment hypothesis that low condition males maximize their current reproductive output [24]. We used wild-caught crickets in an attempt to circumvent potential downfalls associated with laboratory-reared crickets, such as artificial feeding regimes that test only the effect of resource abundance, not the ability to acquire resources in a natural environment. Wild-caught crickets that vary in body size and residual mass allow us to explore the effect of natural variation in resource abundance and resource acquisition ability experienced during development in the wild. We quantified the variance in sexual signalling within and between individuals, determined whether long-distance mate attraction and short-range courtship calls were correlated, and examined the condition-dependent nature of these signals. Mate attraction calls and courtship calls were highly repeatable but largely uncorrelated. Our findings reveal partial support for both hypotheses. In support of the honest signalling hypothesis large males produced louder long-distance mate attraction calls at lower carrier frequencies than small males. In support of the terminal investment hypothesis small males called to attract mates from a distance using faster chirp rates than large males. Additionally, lean (low residual mass) males produced courtship calls with pulse and chirp rates equivalent to plump (high residual mass) males. Our results suggest that not all sexual signals are honest; low condition males might maximize their current reproductive success with higher signalling effort, possibly at the expense of future reproduction.

Methods

Ethics Statement

Our study was conducted in accordance with the guidelines of the Canadian Council on Animal Care.

Collection and Husbandry

Adult Gryllus pennsylvanicus were collected at the Koffler Scientific Reserve (University of Toronto) at Jokers Hill in the Oak Ridges Moraine in King Township, north of Toronto, Ontario, Canada from 8 to 14 August, 2010 (no collecting permits required). Upon capture, adult crickets were individually housed in 520 mL clear plastic containers with crumpled unbleached paper towel for shelter and ad libitum water and food (powdered Harlan Teklad Inc. Rodent diet no. 8604M). Adults were transferred to Carleton University where they were housed in a temperature-controlled greenhouse at 28±2°C on a 14∶10 h light:dark cycle for a three day acclimation period.

Long-Distance Mate Attraction Call Recording

Male mate attraction calls were recorded for three days (72 h) immediately following acclimation to the Carleton University lab environment using the EARS II (Electronic Acoustic Recording System II; designed by Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK). The EARS II is a system of 96 sound-proof Styrofoam boxes, each lined with acoustic foam to avoid sound contamination by neighbouring crickets, that simultaneously records and monitors all mate attraction calling of individual crickets (for further details refer to [27]). Each box contains a microphone and an LED light set to the same 14∶10 h light:dark cycle as the acclimatization room. The EARS II CricketSong software (Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK) automatically filters out background noise and auto-adjusts its amplitude threshold for quiet or loud individuals. Male G. pennsylvanicus long-distance mate attraction calls are characterized by a series of ∼4.7 kHz pulses concatenated into chirps with ∼2–4 pulses per chirp (Figure 1 A & B; Table 1). Using the EARS II system we recorded nine fine scale temporal components of long-distance mate attraction calls [mean daily: pulse duration (ms), interpulse duration (ms), pulses per chirp, chirp duration (ms), interchirp duration (ms), call amplitude (dB), pulse carrier frequency (Hz), pulse rate (P/min), and chirp rate (Ch/min)] as well as three parameters indicative of calling effort [mean daily: number of pulses, number of chirps, and time spent calling]. Due to multicollinearity between several of these signal parameters, only six were used to characterize long-distance mate attraction calls in this study: chirp duration (ms), call amplitude (dB), pulse carrier frequency (Hz), pulse rate (P/min), chirp rate (Ch/min), time spent calling (min/day). Acoustic files were analyzed to produce a summary of mean calling parameters using Spike2 v6.12 (Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK). Of the 62 males quantified, 60 produced mate attraction calls on all three days, while the remaining 2 produced calls on 2/3 days.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Long-distance mate attraction and courtship calls.

Sonograms (top) and waveforms (bottom) of a G. pennsylvanicus long-distance mate attraction call (A & B) and a courtship call (C & D), showing the pulse (P) and tick (T) composition of each signal.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0060356.g001

thumbnail
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for long-distance mate attraction call and courtship call parameters of 44 male G. pennsylvanicus, including coefficient of variation (CV) and repeatability (r) estimates.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0060356.t001

Short-Range Courtship Call Recording

Males were placed with a random field-caught female immediately after being removed from the EARS II. Experimental adults were of unknown age and mating status, but are likely to have all been reproductively active because males were actively producing long-distance mate attraction calls just prior to collection, and females were collected in the vicinity of signalling males. Females received the same 72 h acclimation period as males, along with an additional 72 h period while male long-distance mate attraction calls were being recorded. Females had, therefore, been unmated for at least 6 days.

Courtship and mating trials were conducted between 1000 h and 2300 h over a two-day period. Each cricket pair was placed in a clear plastic 520 mL container without food or water. Courtship was observed continuously for 30 minutes. Using a handheld audio recorder (Handy Recorder H4, Zoom Corporation, Tokyo, Japan), we recorded at least 30 seconds of each male's short-range courtship calls immediately following initiation. A subset of males (N = 18) failed to produce courtship calls. This subset did not differ from other males in body size or residual mass and were excluded from all analyses.

Courtship calls were analyzed manually using Spike2 v. 6.12 (Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK). Gryllus pennsylvanicus courtship calls are characterized by a series of chirps, each with 1–3 pulses at ∼5 kHz, intermixed with high frequency ticks with bandwidths ranging from ∼10–22 kHz (Figure 1 C & D; Table 1). Because female G. pennsylvanicus auditory receptors are most sensitively tuned to male acoustic calls at ∼5 kHz [44], and the pulse carrier frequency of male long-distance mate attraction calls is ∼4.7 kHz (Table 1), we ran courtship recordings through a high pass second order 4 kHz filter to reduce background noise.

Male's courtship calls were often interspersed with long-distance mate attraction calls. Courtship calls could unambiguously be distinguished from mate attraction calls due to their smaller number of pulses per chirp, the presence of high frequency ticks, and lower amplitude pulses. Because males often intermix call types during courtship we could not quantify the first courtship calls produced. Instead, we identified the first 15-seconds following a high frequency tick where the male continuously produced courtship calls without long-distance mate attraction calls. We subdivided this interval into three 5-second intervals and used the first and last 5-second intervals for analysis. By measuring two 5-second intervals we obtained two sets of short-range courtship calls, which we used to quantify repeatability (see below). We could not lengthen the interval between recordings because some females mounted males quickly.

Each male's two 5-second samples of courtship calling were analyzed for chirp rate (Ch/min), tick rate (T/min), pulse rate (P/min), pulse/tick amplitude ratio, and pulse carrier frequency (Hz). Although the mechanism is unknown, males occasionally produced a pulse and a tick simultaneously such that the amplitude of each separate element could not be measured. When this happened both the pulse and tick were included in the tick rate and pulse rate measurements but their amplitude was not included in the pulse/tick amplitude ratio measure. Two males produced only one short bout of short-range courtship calling before mating, so only a single sample (3 to 5 seconds) could be analyzed for these two individuals. These two males were excluded from the repeatability analyses.

Condition Proxies

We define condition as variation in the ability to obtain, assimilate, and utilize nutritional resources. Therefore, we used body size and residual mass as proxies of condition. Male and female crickets were weighed to the nearest milligram following their mating trials using a Denver Instruments analytical balance (Pinnacle Series model PI-114; precision ±0.1 mg). Crickets were then photographed in a dorsal position using a Zeiss Discovery V12 stereo dissecting microscope (AxioVision v4.8, Carl Zeiss; magnification: ∼5x, resolution: ∼1.60 µm) from which pronotum area (mm2), width (mm), height (mm) and head capsule width (mm) was measured to the nearest micrometer. Male body size was quantified with a principal component analysis (PCA) to remove multicollinearity between the four size measurements. Size PC1 explained 91.8% of the variation (eigenvalue  = 3.67) and was loaded heavily by all measurements. Residual mass was calculated using a regression of body mass on body size (size PC1) [45], [46], a measure that appears to reflect energetic fat reserves to some degree in crickets [23].

Statistical Analyses

All statistical analyses were performed in JMP v8.0.2 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA). All acoustic call parameters except for mate attraction pulse rate and courtship pulse rate were Box Cox transformed to ensure they approximated normal distributions. To assess variability in acoustic calls among individual males we calculated the intraclass correlation coefficient, or repeatability (r), for long-distance mate attraction call parameters over three full days of recording. We quantified repeatability for short-range courtship call parameters to assess measurement error using our two five-second intervals. Repeatability was calculated as r  =  s2A/(s2+s2A) [47]. The among-groups (s2A) and within-group (s2) variance components were calculated from the mean squares from a one-way ANOVA as s2A =  (Group MS – Error MS)/no and s2  =  Error MS, where no, a coefficient related to the number of measurements for each male for each call component, was 2.00 for courtship call parameters and 2.94 for long-distance mate attraction call parameters [47].

We calculated the coefficient of variation (CV = 100* sd/|5|) for each mate attraction and courtship calling parameter across all males. We used pairwise correlations to quantify the relationships between long-distance mate attraction and short-range courtship traits. We used general linear models with a subset of the signalling traits to test whether signals honestly reflected condition. We used non-linear regression analyses to test whether males in poor condition (low residual mass or small size) maximized their current reproductive output. We corrected for multiple hypothesis tests using FDRB-Y method [48].

Results

Acoustic Call Variability and Correlations

On average, male G. pennsylvanicus long-distance mate attraction calls were characterized by a series of 4.7 kHz pulses concatenated into 111 ms chirps with 2–4 pulses per chirp (Figure 1 A & B; Table 1). Pulse and chirp rates were 1064 pulses per minute and 69 chirps per minute, respectively, and males called for an average of 255 minutes per day at 62 db (Table 1). While all long-distance mate attraction call parameters were highly repeatable within each male's signals (Table 1; all r >0.65), time spent calling and chirp rate were highly variable across males, as evidenced by high coefficients of variation (Table 1; CV >27).

On average, male G. pennsylvanicus short-range courtship calls were characterized by a series of chirps at approximately 112 chirps per minute, each with 1–3 pulses at 5 kHz at a rate of 207 pulses per minute (Figure 1 C & D; Table 1). Courtship call pulses were generally louder than ticks, with an average pulse to tick amplitude ratio of 1.85 (Table 1). Pulses were intermixed with high frequency ticks having bandwidths ranging from approximately 10–22 kHz at an average rate of 135 ticks per minute (Figure 1 C & D; Table 1). All courtship call parameters were highly repeatable within each male's signals (Table 1; all r >0.66), usually with coefficients of variation indicating a high degree of variability across males (Table 1; CV >28), particularly for pulse to tick amplitude ratio (CV  = 106).

Short-range courtship call parameters were generally not significantly correlated with mate attraction calling parameters, with two exceptions: courtship pulse carrier frequency was positively correlated with long-distance mate attraction pulse rate (R = 0.493, P = 0.001) and pulse carrier frequency (Table 2; R = 0.765, P<0.001).

thumbnail
Table 2. Pairwise correlations between G. pennsylvanicus courtship call parameters (rows) and long-distance mate attraction call parameters (columns) showing p values for Pearson correlation coefficients.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0060356.t002

Condition and Acoustic Calling

Our general linear models examining whether call parameters honestly reflect condition suggest that long-distance mate attraction calls convey information about body size. Large males produced louder long-distance mate attraction calls, at lower carrier frequencies than small males (Table 3). Small males, however, produced long-distance mate attraction calls with faster chirp rates than large males (Table 3). Male long-distance call parameters did not convey significant information about residual mass. Similarly, male courtship call parameters did not convey significant information about body size or residual mass (Table 3).

thumbnail
Table 3. General linear models showing relationships between condition measures (body size and residual mass) and mate signalling traits (long-distance mate attraction and short-range courtship signals).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0060356.t003

Our non-linear regression models examining whether low condition males signal dishonestly revealed that lean males (low residual mass) courted females at rates equivalent to plump males (high residual mass). Lean males produced courtship calls with pulse rates and chirp rates equivalent to plump males, with males of intermediate residual mass having courtship calls with the lowest pulse and chirp rates (Table 4; Figure 2). All other non-linear regression models were not statistically significant and so were not included in Table 4.

thumbnail
Figure 2. Non-linear relationship between residual mass and courtship call parameters.

Residual mass predicts courtship chirp rate (top) and courtship pulse rate (bottom) where lean males (low residual mass) have chirp and pulse rates that are equivalent to plump males (high residual mass). Statistics are presented in Table 4.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0060356.g002

thumbnail
Table 4. Models showing relationships between call parameters and linear/non-linear condition measures (only call parameters with significant models are shown).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0060356.t004

Discussion

Theory predicts that male sexual signals should honestly indicate female mating benefits because the cost associated with signalling dishonestly should be too high for males in poor condition to maintain (the handicap principle; [5], [20], [49], [50]). Females may, therefore, benefit from selecting mates on the basis of condition-dependent signals. Cricket studies have largely supported this honest signalling theory, revealing that long-distance mate attraction calls are usually nutrition dependent (e.g. [12][14], [27], [32], [42], [43]).

In support of the honest signalling theory, we found that some signal components of field-captured male G. pennsylvanicus' long-distance mate attraction calls were indicative of male body size (Table 3). Similar to previous findings in other gryllid species [51], [52], larger G. pennsylvanicus males produced louder mate attraction calls at lower carrier frequencies (Table 3). Because large adult body size in field crickets is beneficial in aggressive contests between rival males over mating territories [53], [54], and several studies have shown body size to be heritable in crickets [55][58], females may secure good genes for offspring body size by selecting mates on the basis of their signalling amplitude and carrier frequency.

In contrast to honest signalling theory, the terminal investment hypothesis suggests that poor condition males with reduced future reproductive potential may increase their effort in sexual signalling in an attempt to secure a successful mating while they are still able [21][25]. While this hypothesis has received less attention in the sexual selection literature compared to the honest signalling hypothesis, several recent cricket studies have findings consistent with it. Smaller Acheta domesticus males transferred greater numbers of viable sperm to females than larger males [59]. Gryllus assimilis increased their signalling effort with increasing age, with older males producing higher pulse and chirp rates, and longer and louder chirps [60]. Gryllus assimilis males that experienced decreased body condition after being fed low quality diets called for more long-distance mate attraction bouts per night compared to males fed high quality diets [27]. Furthermore, males experiencing an immunological threat increased their investment in current reproduction through increased fighting success (G. integer: [29]), and faster chirp rates in mate attraction calls (Allonemobius socius: [28]).

In support of the terminal investment hypothesis, we found that small males produced long-distance mate attraction calls with higher chirp rates than large males (Table 3). Given that faster chirp rates are more energetically expensive to produce [61], and small body size may reflect poor nutritional resources and/or nutrient assimilation and utilization ability during juvenile development, chirp rate appears to be a dishonest signal of male condition. Small males may be overcompensating for their small size by chirping at faster rates in an attempt to attract females. Females may be able to detect this dishonest signal using information from other mating cues.

The question that remains, however, is what information are females gleaning from courtship calls? Courtship calls occur after females have located males and our findings suggest they also convey dishonest information about male condition. A curvilinear relationship exists between residual mass and courtship pulse and chirp rates such that both lean and plump males (low and high residual mass, respectively) courted females with similarly high pulse and chirp rates, with males of intermediate residual mass having the lowest pulse and chirp rates (Table 4; Figure 2). Lean males may be enhancing their courtship rates in an attempt to secure a successful mating. Enhanced investment in courtship may maximize male current reproductive success at the expense of future reproductive success [21][24].

Overall, our findings that (1) small males produced long-distance calls with faster chirp rates, and (2) males with low residual mass courted females with higher pulse and chirp rates than males of intermediate residual mass suggests the possibility of an alternative reproductive strategy in G. pennsylvanicus. Poor condition males may be increasing their investment in current reproduction at the expense of having fewer resources to devote to future reproduction or survival. Future studies should address the long-term consequences of courtship calling and longevity for males that vary in residual mass.

Given the high production costs of cricket acoustic calls, the use of multiple signals to attract a mate may seem maladaptive. However, multiple sexual signals may be adaptive by reducing female mate choice errors, providing different types of information on male quality and condition, or reducing time and energy spent assessing males [62]. We found long-distance mate attraction and short-range courtship calling parameters to be repeatable over time (Table 1), suggesting both call types have the potential to provide reliable information to females. Further, the relative lack of significant correlations between mate attraction and courtship calls (Table 2) suggests these two call types may convey distinct information to females (i.e. the multiple messages hypothesis [63]). Future studies examining sexual signalling in crickets should investigate relationships between multiple sexual cues in different sensory modalities in order to gain a better understanding of information being conveyed to females in these signals.

We interpret our findings with caution for several reasons. First, we have no information about male age or mating history, and these factors may influence behavioural tradeoffs in investing in current versus future reproductive effort. Similarly, males with low residual mass might have had less (or more) mating experience, resulting in enhanced courtship rates. Second, we assumed that males' residual mass reflected individual differences in resource acquisition. However, if low residual mass males are good at acquiring resources, they may risk investing much of their energy in signalling because they can easily replace it. Even if our assumption is valid that residual mass reflects individual differences in resource acquisition, our ad libitum feeding regime may have provided low residual mass males the resources necessary to enhance their courtship displays. In retrospect, a superior protocol would have been to weigh each cricket immediately following collection, then re-weigh them following ad libitum feeding to ascertain how residual mass changed. Given we only weighed crickets following ad libitum feeding, we have to assume that our protocol did not greatly alter the variation in condition determined by physiological differences in assimilation and utilization of resources between males.

Formal tests of the hypotheses that small males produce higher effort long-distance mate attraction calls and lean males court females with higher effort than plump males requires experimental manipulation of male condition using nutritionally explicit dietary treatments (i.e. geometric framework, [64]). That said, lab-based nutrient manipulation studies only test the effect of resource abundance on male signals, not the effect of resource acquisition ability in a natural environment. The benefit of using field-captured animals is that they naturally vary in residual mass, and are therefore likely to reflect variation in both resources acquisition ability and resource abundance.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the Koffler Scientific Reserve for use of field and laboratory facilities, and Andrew Mikhail and Samantha Klaus for their assistance with field-collection. We would also like to thank Associate Editor Sean Walker and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on an earlier version of this paper.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: SJH IRT CMG SMB. Performed the experiments: SJH IRT CMG SMB. Analyzed the data: SJH IRT CMG SMB. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: SMB. Wrote the paper: SJH IRT CMG SMB.

References

  1. 1. Holman L (2012) Costs and constraints conspire to produce honest signalling: Insights from an ant queen pheromone. Evolution 66: 2094–2105. Available: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1558-5646.2012.01603.x/abstract Accessed 11 September 2012.
  2. 2. Otte D (1974) Effects and functions in the evolution of signaling systems. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 5: 385–417. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.2307/2096893 Accessed 29 November 2012.
  3. 3. Hill GE (1991) Plumage coloration is a sexually selected indicator of male quality. Nature 350: 337–339. Available: http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/handle/2027.42/62564 Accessed 17 August 2012.
  4. 4. Nicoletto PF (1993) Female sexual response to condition-dependent ornaments in the guppy, Poecilia reticulata. Animal Behaviour 46: 441–450. Available: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0003347283712133 Accessed 17 August 2012.
  5. 5. Grafen A (1990) Biological signals as handicaps. Journal of Theoretical Biology 144: 517–546. Available: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022519305800888 Accessed 11 September 2012.
  6. 6. Price T, Schluter D, Heckman NE (1993) Sexual selection when the female directly benefits. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 48: 187–211. Available: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1095-8312.1993.tb00887.x/abstract Accessed 17 September 2012.
  7. 7. Kokko H (1998) Should advertising parental care be honest? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 265: 1871–1878 Available: http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/cgi/doi/10.1098/rspb.1998.0515.
  8. 8. Kokko H (1998) Good genes, old age and life-history trade-offs. Evolutionary Ecology 12: 739–750. Available: http://www.springerlink.com/index/N08827088740150T.pdf Accessed 17 September 2012.
  9. 9. Kelly NB, Alonzo SH (2010) Does a trade-off between current reproductive success and survival affect the honesty of male signalling in species with male parental care? Journal of Evolutionary Biology 23: 2461–2473. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20860698 Accessed 30 July 2012.
  10. 10. Møller AP, Jennions MD (2001) How important are direct fitness benefits of sexual selection? Naturwissenschaften 88: 401–415. Available: http://www.springerlink.com/openurl.asp?genre=article&id=doi:10.1007/s001140100255 Accessed 14 July 2012.
  11. 11. Hill GE, Inouye CY, Montgomerie R (2002) Dietary carotenoids predict plumage coloration in wild house finches. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 269: 1119–1124. Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=1691014&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract Accessed 17 July 2012.
  12. 12. Scheuber H, Jacot A, Brinkhof MWG (2003) Condition dependence of a multicomponent sexual signal in the field cricket Gryllus campestris. Animal Behaviour 65: 721–727. Available: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0003347203920835 Accessed 6 December 2010.
  13. 13. Hunt J, Brooks R, Jennions MD, Smith MJ, Bentsen CL, et al. (2004) High-quality male field crickets invest heavily in sexual display but die young. Nature 432: 1024–1027 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15616562.
  14. 14. Maklakov AA, Simpson SJ, Zajitschek F, Hall MD, Dessmann J, et al.. (2008) Sex-specific fitness effects of nutrient intake on reproduction and lifespan. Current Biology 18: 1062–1066. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18635354 Accessed 2 December 2010.
  15. 15. David P, Hingle A, Greig D, Rutherford A, Pomiankowski A, et al.. (1998) Male sexual ornament size but not asymmetry reflects condition in stalk–eyed flies. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 265: 2211–2216. Available: http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/265/1411/2211.short Accessed 11 September 2012.
  16. 16. Kotiaho JS (2000) Testing the assumptions of conditional handicap theory: costs and condition dependence of a sexually selected trait. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 48: 188–194. Available: http://www.springerlink.com/index/UK27YFC8CG60TDKM.pdf Accessed 18 September 2012.
  17. 17. Tomkins JL, Radwan J, Kotiaho JS, Tregenza T (2004) Genic capture and resolving the lek paradox. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 19: 323–328. Available: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0169534704000849 Accessed 5 December 2010.
  18. 18. Williams GC (1966) Natural selection, the costs of reproduction, and a refinement of Lack's Principle. The American Naturalist 100: 687–690.
  19. 19. Stearns SC (1992) The evolution of life histories. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
  20. 20. Zahavi A (1975) Mate selection-a selection for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology 53: 205–214 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1195756.
  21. 21. Kokko H (1997) Evolutionarily stable strategies of age-dependent sexual advertisement. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 41: 99–107 Available: http://www.springerlink.com/openurl.asp?genre=article&id=doi:10.1007/s002650050369.
  22. 22. Candolin U (1999) The relationship between signal quality and physical condition: is sexual signalling honest in the three-spined stickleback? Animal Behaviour 58: 1261–1267. Available:http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10600148.
  23. 23. Gray DA, Eckhardt G (2001) Is cricket courtship song condition dependent? Animal Behaviour 62: 871–877. Available: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0003347201918251 Accessed 30 July 2010.
  24. 24. Clutton-Brock TH (1984) Reproductive effort and terminal investment in iteroparous animals. American Naturalist 123: 212–229. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.2307/2461034 Accessed 29 November 2012.
  25. 25. Johnstone RA, Grafen A (1993) Dishonesty and the handicap principle. Animal Behaviour 46: 759–764. Available: http://users.ox.ac.uk/~grafen/cv/dishonest.pdf Accessed 11 September 2012.
  26. 26. Svensson O, Nyman A, Kvarnemo C (2004) Costly courtship or dishonest display? Intensely displaying sand goby males have lower lipid content. Journal of Fish Biology 64: 1425–1429. Available: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.0022-1112.2004.00381.x/full Accessed 11 September 2012.
  27. 27. Whattam EM, Bertram SM (2011) Effects of juvenile and adult condition on long-distance call components in the Jamaican field cricket, Gryllus assimilis. Animal Behaviour 81: 135–144. Available:http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0003347210003933 Accessed 24 July 2012.
  28. 28. Copeland EK, Fedorka KM (2012) The influence of male age and simulated pathogenic infection on producing a dishonest sexual signal. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 279: 4740–4746 .
  29. 29. Pölkki M, Kortet R, Hedrick A, Rantala MJ (2013) Dominance is not always an honest signal of male quality, but females may be able to detect the dishonesty. Biology letters 9: 20121002. Available: http://rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/9/1/20121002.short Accessed 18 February 2013.
  30. 30. Alexander RD (1961) Aggressiveness, Territoriality, and Sexual Behavior in Field Crickets (Orthoptera: Gryllidae). Behaviour 17: 130–223.
  31. 31. Cade WH, Cade ES (1992) Male mating success, calling and searching behaviour at high and low densities in the field cricket, Gryllus integer. Animal Behaviour 43: 49–56. Available: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0003347205800703 Accessed 16 December 2010.
  32. 32. Holzer B, Jacot A, Brinkhof MWG (2003) Condition-dependent signaling affects male sexual attractiveness in field crickets, Gryllus campestris. Behavioral Ecology 14: 353–359. Available: http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/content/14/3/353.short Accessed 6 December 2010.
  33. 33. Brooks R, Hunt J, Blows MW, Smith MJ, Bussière LF, et al.. (2005) Experimental evidence for multivariate stabilizing sexual selection. Evolution 59: 871–880. Available:http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.0014-3820.2005.tb01760.x/abstract Accessed 26 February 2012.
  34. 34. Bentsen CL, Hunt J, Jennions MD, Brooks R (2006) Complex multivariate sexual selection on male acoustic signaling in a wild population of Teleogryllus commodus. The American Naturalist 167: E102–E116 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16670989.
  35. 35. Wagner WE (1996) Convergent song preferences between female field crickets and acoustically orienting parasitoid flies. Behavioral Ecology 7: 279–285 Available: http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/doi/10.1093/beheco/7.3.279.
  36. 36. Hedrick AV (1986) Female preferences for male calling bout duration in a field cricket. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 19: 73–77. Available: http://www.springerlink.com/index/U84T2G6044438757.pdf Accessed 27 January 2011.
  37. 37. Leonard A, Hedrick AV (2010) Long-distance signals influence assessment of close range mating displays in the field cricket, Gryllus integer. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 100: 856–865 Available: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/j.1095-8312.2010.01472.x.
  38. 38. Wagner WE, Reiser MG (2000) The importance of calling song and courtship song in female mate choice in the variable field cricket. Animal Behaviour 59: 1219–1226. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10877901 Accessed 30 July 2010.
  39. 39. Rantala MJ, Kortet R (2003) Courtship song and immune function in the field cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 79: 503–510 Available: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1046/j.1095-8312.2003.00202.x.
  40. 40. Rebar D, Bailey NW, Zuk M (2009) Courtship song's role during female mate choice in the field cricket Teleogryllus oceanicus. Behavioral Ecology 20: 1307–1314. Available: http://www.beheco.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/doi/10.1093/beheco/arp143 Accessed 6 May 2012.
  41. 41. Wagner WE, Hoback WW (1999) Nutritional effects on male calling behaviour in the variable field cricket. Animal Behaviour 57: 89–95 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10053075.
  42. 42. Bertram SM, Whattam EM, Visanuvimol L, Bennett R, Lauzon C (2009) Phosphorus availability influences cricket mate attraction displays. Animal Behaviour 77: 525–530. Available: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0003347208005551 Accessed 20 October 2010.
  43. 43. Bertram SM, Rook V (2012) Relationship between condition, aggression, signaling, courtship, and egg laying in the field cricket, Gryllus assimilis. Ethology 118: 1–13. Available: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.02019.x Accessed 7 March 2012.
  44. 44. Jeffery J, Navia B, Atkins G, Stout J (2005) Selective processing of calling songs by auditory interneurons in the female cricket, Gryllus pennsylvanicus: possible roles in behavior. Journal of Experimental Zoology A: Comparative Experimental Biology 303: 377–392. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15828009 Accessed 16 July 2012.
  45. 45. Jakob EM, Marshall SD, Uetz GW (1996) Estimating fitness: a comparison of body condition indices. Oikos 77: 61–67. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.2307/3545585 Accessed 19 September 2012.
  46. 46. Schulte-Hostedde AI, Zinner B, Millar JS, Hickling GJ (2005) Restitution of mass-size residuals: validating body condition indices. Ecology 86: 155–163. Available: http://www.esajournals.org/doi/abs/10.1890/04-0232 Accessed 19 September 2012.
  47. 47. Lessells CM, Boag PT (1987) Unrepeatable repeatabilities: a common mistake. The Auk 104: 116–121. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.2307/4087240 Accessed 18 September 2012.
  48. 48. Benjamini Y, Yekutieli D (2001) The control of the false discovery rate in multiple testing under dependency. The Annals of Statistics 29: 1165–1188.
  49. 49. Zahavi A (1977) The cost of honesty (further remarks on the handicap principle). Journal of Theoretical Biology 67: 603–605 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/904334.
  50. 50. Zahavi A (1997) The handicap principle: a missing piece of Darwin's puzzle. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
  51. 51. Gray D (1997) Female house crickets, Acheta domesticus, prefer the chirps of large males. Animal Behaviour 54: 1553–1562. Available: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0003347297905844 Accessed 11 December 2010.
  52. 52. Scheuber H, Jacot A, Brinkhof MWG (2003) The effect of past condition on a multicomponent sexual signal. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 270: 1779–1784. Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=1691443&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract Accessed 15 July 2010.
  53. 53. Jang Y, Gerhardt HC, Choe JC (2008) A comparative study of aggressiveness in eastern North American field cricket species (genus Gryllus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 62: 1397–1407. Available: http://www.springerlink.com/index/10.1007/s00265-008-0568-6 Accessed 8 March 2012.
  54. 54. Judge K., Bonanno VL (2008) Male weaponry in a fighting cricket. PLoS one 3: e3980. Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2601036&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract Accessed 15 September 2012.
  55. 55. Mousseau TA, Roff DA (1989) Adaptation to seasonality in a cricket: patterns of phenotypic and genotypic variation in body size and diapause expression along a cline in season length. Evolution 43: 1483–1496. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.2307/2409463 Accessed 4 December 2012.
  56. 56. Fedorka KM, Mousseau TA (2004) Female mating bias results in conflicting sex-specific offspring fitness. Nature 429: 65–67 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15129280.
  57. 57. del Castillo RC (2005) The quantitative genetic basis of female and male body size and their implications on the evolution of body size dimorphism in the house cricket Acheta domesticus (Gryllidae). Genetics and Molecular Biology 28: 843–848 Available: http://www.scielo.br/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1415-47572005000500030&lng=en&nrm=iso&tlng=en.
  58. 58. Simmons LW (1987) Heritability of a male character chosen by females of the field cricket, Gryllus bimaculatus. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 21: 129–133.
  59. 59. Klaus SP, Fitzsimmons LP, Pitcher TE, Bertram SM (2011) Song and sperm in crickets: a trade-off between pre- and post-copulatory traits or phenotype-linked fertility? Ethology 117: 154–162. Available: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01857.x Accessed 23 August 2011.
  60. 60. Bertram SM, Rook V (2011) Jamaican field cricket mate attraction signals provide age cues. Ethology 117: 1050–1055. Available: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.01958.x Accessed 24 July 2012.
  61. 61. Hoback WW, Wagner WE (1997) The energetic cost of calling in the variable field cricket, Gryllus lineaticeps. Physiological Entomology 22: 286–290. Available: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1365-3032.1997.tb01170.x/abstract Accessed 7 January 2011.
  62. 62. Candolin U (2003) The use of multiple cues in mate choice. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 78: 575–595 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14700392.
  63. 63. Møller AP, Pomiankowski A (1993) Why have birds got multiple sexual ornaments? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 32: 167–176. Available: http://www.springerlink.com/index/P7R1V27M7278G464.pdf Accessed 6 June 2012.
  64. 64. Raubenheimer D, Simpson SJ (1999) Integrating nutrition: a geometrical approach. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 91: 67–82 Available: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1046/j.1570-7458.1999.00467.x.