Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Eye-Blink Behaviors in 71 Species of Primates

Abstract

The present study was performed to investigate the associations between eye-blink behaviors and various other factors in primates. We video-recorded 141 individuals across 71 primate species and analyzed the blink rate, blink duration, and “isolated” blink ratio (i.e., blinks without eye or head movement) in relation to activity rhythms, habitat types, group size, and body size factors. The results showed close relationships between three types of eye-blink measures and body size factors. All of these measures increased as a function of body weight. In addition, diurnal primates showed more blinks than nocturnal species even after controlling for body size factors. The most important findings were the relationships between eye-blink behaviors and social factors, e.g., group size. Among diurnal primates, only the blink rate was significantly correlated even after controlling for body size factors. The blink rate increased as the group size increased. Enlargement of the neocortex is strongly correlated with group size in primate species and considered strong evidence for the social brain hypothesis. Our results suggest that spontaneous eye-blinks have acquired a role in social communication, similar to grooming, to adapt to complex social living during primate evolution.

Introduction

Support for the social brain hypothesis has come from the fact that the neocortex is strongly correlated with group size in primate species [1][3]. Eye-blinks in humans are influenced by a variety of factors, including peripheral, central, and environmental factors [4][7]. In addition, many studies have identified marked interindividual differences in eye-blink behavior [8][10], although few have directly examined the origin of these differences. Most of these studies compared a pathological group with a healthy control group and showed that compared with the control groups, the blink rate is higher in individuals with schizophrenia or Huntington's chorea [11][15] and lower in individuals with parkinsonism or progressive supranuclear palsy [4], [7], [16][20].

Numerous developmental studies on human blinking have been performed [4], [16], [21][25]. Many of these studies confirmed the conclusions of Ponder and Kennedy [4] that blinking is virtually absent at birth. However, no consistent findings have been reported regarding the subsequent development of blinks. One study indicated that the blinking rate increased during development until adulthood [21], whereas another study showed that the peak rate of blinking occurred in preadolescence [24]. Nevertheless, both studies showed that after the peak, no significant differences in blink rates occur at older ages. However, another study in subjects from 5 to 87 years old indicated no differences in the blink rate with age [22].

The most systematic study of eye-blinking in nonhuman animals is that performed in 1927 by Blount [26], who observed blinking in 39 species, including amphibians and primates, and described their blinking behaviors in detail. His observations revealed marked changes in blinking behaviors. For example, the interblink interval was prolonged in dairy cows during feeding and neck licking, in pre-ruminating bison, and in wolves watching birds. Based on these results, Blount concluded that these blinks have a central rather than peripheral origin [26]. These results were consistent with those reported by Ponder and Kennedy [4], who focused on human blinking and suggested the importance of central processes. Blount also suggested that blink rates show an evolutionary history, albeit with some exceptions [26]. Using these results, we calculated the mean blink rate and standard deviation (SD) of 39 species [approximately 6.8 ± 9.1 blinks per minute (bpm)]. The coefficient of variation (CV  =  SD/mean × 100) was 134%, which suggested marked interspecies variations in blink rates.

Surprisingly, very few studies have determined the blink rates in animals. One series of studies investigated the dopamine hypothesis of blinking in animals [27][33]. Another series directly measured the blink rates in animals, including cows [34], birds [35], dogs [36], guinea pigs [37], rabbits [37], [38], and 31 mammalian species [13]. These studies confirmed that nocturnal animals blink much less frequently than diurnally active animals, blinking frequency being one-tenth lower in nocturnal versus diurnal mammals [13] and birds [35]. Carrington et al. [36] reported that the mean ± SD blink rate in dogs is 14.5 ± 5.63 bpm, and approximately 66% of all blinks were incomplete. Note that interindividual differences are quite large in dogs [36], but much smaller in birds [35]. Mowrer reported a detailed description of the nictitating membrane (i.e., the third eyelid) of birds [39].

Studies on blinking in nonhuman primates will help us to understand human eye behaviors. Several studies have focused on head-cocking behaviors in nonhuman primates [40] and the properties of the sclera in primates [41]. Several studies on animal blinking support this phylogenetic view [26], [34], [35], but no systematic investigations have examined nonhuman primates, the group of species most closely related to humans. Therefore, studies on nonhuman primates may be particularly important because previously reported studies did not distinguish among various types of blinks, including pseudo-blinks (nictitating membrane activities), half blinks, winks (one eye only, involuntarily blinks), and flickers [29], [39] and did not examine the relationship between eye-blink behaviors and other factors. Therefore, the present study was performed to examine the factors affecting blinking behaviors in other primates, the group most closely related to humans, with the most similar eye-blink behaviors.

Methods

Subjects

We video-recorded 141 individuals across 71 species of primates housed at the Japan Monkey Centre (JMC), Inuyama, Aichi, Japan, in addition to two gorillas (one male and one female) and three chimpanzees (one male and two females) housed at Yagiyama Zoological Park, Sendai, and two orangutans (one male and one female) housed at Chiba Zoological Park, Chiba, Japan. All of the species are listed in Table 1. Phylogenetic classification was determined on the basis of Rowe [42] and Groves [43].

thumbnail
Table 1. Data on the three measures of eye-blinking behavior, two body-size measures, and group size for each primate species.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0066018.t001

Ethical Statements

The present study adhered to the 2002 Version of the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Primates by the Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, and was approved by the Animal Welfare and Animal Care Committee of the Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, and by the Animal Research Committee of Kyoto University. All the zoos studied are members of the Japanese Association of Zoos and Aquariums (JAZA), and daily care also adhered to the Ethical Guidelines of JAZA. Our study was conducted according to these guidelines and the Ethical Guidelines for the Conduct of Research on Animals by Zoos and Aquariums of the World Association of Zoos and Aquariums [44].

Blink Recording

Video recorders (e.g., Handycam DCR-VX2000 and TRV-10; Sony, Tokyo, Japan) were used to record eye-blinks. Video recording was done between 09:30 and 17:00 in winter or until 18:00 in summer. Video recording was mainly done in daylight, although infrared lamps were used under dim light conditions when the individuals had returned to their sleeping quarters after dinner or when we recorded nocturnal primates. The accumulated recording time for each individual was at least 5 min because some studies have recommended that the minimum sampling time should be 3 min with the ideal time being 5 min for humans [39], [45]. Indeed, a previous study investigating the correlation between the blink rate and interblink interval indicated that a 5-min recording time is essential for humans [45]. Therefore, although the blink rate of other primates is generally lower than that in humans, we decided that 5 min of recording was appropriate.

Data Reduction and Analysis

The videotaped eye-blink data were transferred to a PC using Adobe Premiere (Adobe Systems Co., Tokyo, Japan) and DVgate Plus (Sony) software. Data were reduced to obtain blink wave attributes, including amplitude, closing duration, reopening duration, and blink rate using a Blink Detection Program (Mizuno Measurement Co., Sendai, Japan), which was developed specifically for the present study. Using this program, we could precisely determine the necessary blink measures through playback of the video recording. Videos were viewed at slow speed (½ or ¼ of the original speed) because blink velocities of primates are generally high and it was possible to miss blinks at the normal playback speed and the same blink events may have been counted several times. Using the video recordings, we determined the following blink parameters: the number of blinks, the number of frames from the start to completion of each blink, and whether the blink was associated with specific body movements, including head and eye movements. Therefore, we obtained three different measures of blinks: the blink rate (bpm), blink duration (ms), and “isolated” blink ratio (%; proportion of blinks not associated with any type of body movement).

Factors Contributing to Eye-Blinks

Three factors potentially linked to eye-blink behaviors [i.e., body size (body weight), activity rhythm, and habitat type] were selected based on previous reports [42], [43], [46][50]. Mean body weight values were determined for each species and averaged across males and females. Activity rhythm was classified as nocturnal (n  =  5) or diurnal (n  =  66). Habitat type was classified as arboreal (n  =  45), semiarboreal (n  =  19), or terrestrial (n  =  7). We also used the group size as a social factor. The mean group size for each species was calculated on the basis of Rowe [39], Smuts et al. [49], Campbell et al. [50], and Rowe and Myers [51].

Results

Overall Characteristics of Eye-Blink Behaviors in Primates

Figure 1 shows two examples of eye-blinks by the primate species the black-and-white ruffed lemur (Varecia variegata) and drill (Mandrillus leucophaeus). Data for each species are shown in Table 1. The mean values for eye-blink measures and body size and group size measures for 141 individuals of 71 species are given at the bottom of Table 1. The mean ± SD blink rate was 10.9 ± 6.8 bpm and was highest in white-fronted capuchin (Cebus albifrons; 29.8 bpm) and western gorilla (Gorilla gorilla; 29.4 bpm), and lowest in potto (Perodicticus potto; 0 bpm), lesser slow loris (Nycticebus pygmaeus; 0.2 bpm), and brown greater galago (Otolemur crassicaudatus; 0.3 bpm). The potto did not blink during the 5-min recordings. The mean blink rate was approximately half of that in humans, which was 20.8 bpm in our own earlier study performed in approximately 1400 adults [24] and as reported elsewhere [6], [7], [52]. However, the variation in blink rate was fairly large, with SD and coefficient of variation (CV) values of 6.8 bpm and 62.3%, respectively. In our earlier study, the respective SD and CV values in humans were 14.7 bpm and 70.7% [24].

thumbnail
Figure 1. Two examples of eye-blinking behaviors in nonhuman primates.

Left: Black-and-white ruffed lemur (Varecia variegata); right: the drill (Mandrillus leucophaeus).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0066018.g001

The mean blink duration of the 71 species of primates was 214.0 ± 52.8 ms (CV  =  24.7%). Blink duration was longest in the brown greater galago (341.0 ms), western gorilla (335.0 ms), and black-and-white ruffed lemur (323.4 ms), and shortest in the Guinea baboon (Papio papio; 138.6 ms), Goeldi's marmoset (Callimico goeldii; 141.9 ms), and Bolivian squirrel monkey (Saimiri boliviensis boliviensis; 148.5 ms). In comparison, the blink duration and CV in humans were 403.6 ± 52.6 ms and 13.0% [21], respectively. Thus, the blink duration and CV in primates were approximately half and about double those in humans, respectively.

The mean isolated blink ratio in primates was 28.3% ± 19.3%, and was highest in the lesser slow loris (100%), Sumatran orangutan (Pongo abelii; 70.6%), and Tibetan macaque (Macaca thibetana; 68.9%), and lowest in the grand galago (Galago crassicaudatus; 0%), black-capped squirrel monkey (Saimiri boliviensis; 0%), and ring-tailed lemur (Lemur catta; 0%). We do not yet have sufficient data to determine the isolated blink ratio in humans, but from our experience, the isolated blink ratio in humans, at least under experimental conditions, is high in adults. Based on limited data obtained from 408 subjects, including very young babies [24], the isolated blink ratio seems to increase with age, particularly in young children.

Effect of Activity Rhythms

For subsequent analyses, we transformed all of the data into common logarithmic values for data analyses, although retransformed values are presented in the text.

First, we examined the effects of activity rhythms on eye-blink behaviors, although analyses of blink duration and isolated blink ratio were not conducted because of the small numbers of species. Figure 2 shows the mean blink rate as a function of activity rhythms. Diurnal primates exhibited more eye-blinks than nocturnal primates [9.62 vs. 0.47; t(68)  =  7.74, P < 0.001]. The results of multiple regression analysis using activity rhythms as dummy variables to control for body size factors indicated that this difference was still significant [R2  =  0.515, F(2,67)  =  35.59, P < 0.001; regression coefficient for body weights  =  0.169, P  =  0.013; regression coefficient for activity rhythm  =  1.167, P < 0.001]. Based on these results, subsequent analyses focused only on the data from diurnal primates.

thumbnail
Figure 2. Mean blink rate as a function of activity rhythms.

Error bars show standard deviations.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0066018.g002

Effects of Habitat Type

Figure 3 shows the mean blink rate, blink duration, isolated blink ratio, and body weight as a function of habitat type. Some correlations were observed between these three measures and habitat types. However, body size factors were also correlated with habitat type. We also conducted multiple regression analyses for each measure using body size and habitat type as independent variables. Three habitat types were used as dummy variables. Table 2 shows the results of multiple regression analyses. For the blink rate, no significant multiple regression were noted, whereas regressions were significant for the other two measures. In these two measures, however, only the regression coefficients for body weight were significant. These results clearly indicated that habitat types do not affect eye-blink behaviors.

thumbnail
Figure 3. Three eye-blink measures and body weight as functions of habitat type.

SD: standard deviation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0066018.g003

thumbnail
Table 2. Results of multiple regression analyses on body size and habitat type factors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0066018.t002

Effects of Group Size

Figure 4 shows scatterplots of three measures against body weight and group size. Simple correlation coefficient and simple regression line are shown in each panel. These three eye-blink measures were significantly correlated with body weight. Group size, however, was only significantly correlated with blink rate, as shown in Figure 4. For further investigation, multiple regression analyses with body weight and group size as independent variables were conducted for these data. The results indicated significant multiple regressions for blink rate [R2  =  0.154, F(2,63)  =  5.73, P  =  0.005], blink duration [R2  =  0.099, F(2,61)  =  3.34, P  =  0.042], and isolated blink ratio [R2  =  0.181, F(2,62)  =  6.83, P  =  0.002]. Note that the contributions of each independent variable varied across eye-blink measures. Figure 5 shows the unstandardized regression coefficients for each variable. As indicated in this figure, the effect of body weight was significant for blink duration and isolated blink ratio, while the group size significantly affected the blink rate only. Figure 4 also shows partial correlation coefficients in parentheses after partialling out the other variable.

thumbnail
Figure 4. Scatterplots of the three measures for eye-blinking behaviors in primates as a function of body size and group size factors.

Red lines indicate the simple regression lines. ***: P < 0.001, **: P < 0.01, *: P < 0.05. A: arboreal; sA: semiarboreal; T: terrestrial.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0066018.g004

thumbnail
Figure 5. Regression coefficients for body weight and group size factors for each eye-blink measure.

Error bars show 95% confidence intervals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0066018.g005

Discussion

The results of this study can be summarized as follows. First, eye-blink behaviors in primates are affected by the activity rhythms (especially with regard to blink rate), but not habitat type. The effects of activity rhythms on spontaneous blinks were reported previously in nonhuman animals, including other primates [13]. Our results clearly replicated the previous results. Furthermore, these tendencies are not “virtual,” as the effect remained after partialling out body size factors.

Second, group size factors also affected eye-blink behaviors, but the effects varied across behavioral measures. Blink duration and isolated blink ratio are correlated with body weight but not with group size, an important social factor [1]. As in the other allometric parameters, such as locomotor speed and life span [53], [54], variations in these two measures were simply explained by the body size factors. In contrast, however, the blink rate was not correlated with body weight but with the group size. This result is quite suggestive. From a physiological perspective, a rate of 2 bpm has been reported to provide sufficient corneal wetting for human adults [55], [56]. Regardless of whether this basic rate is constant against body size factors, the observed blink rate was clearly higher than this rate in 65 of 66 diurnal primates. Our data suggest that these “additional” blinks may be related to social factors. Dunbar [1] reported that group size is significantly correlated with the neocortex ratio (i.e., the ratio between the volume of the neocortex and the rest of the brain) after partialling out body weight factors. Their results are considered as evidence for the “social brain hypothesis” or “social (Machiavellian) intelligence hypothesis” [1][3]. The observation that neocortex size, a simple indicator of the cognitive capabilities, was correlated with group size, a simple indicator of complexity of social lives, suggests that a large brain (neocortex) evolved as a result of social complexity.

Many reports have been published regarding the relationship between the effect of group size and related factors, and social and visual behaviors in primates. For example, Dunbar reported that the group size was significantly positively correlated with time spent in social grooming in nonhuman primates [57]. With regard to vigilance or visual monitoring behavior, Kutsukake reported that vigilance duration in chimpanzees increased significantly as a function of the number of individuals [58]. Vigilance is not simply governed by external threats, such as predators, but by conspecific factors in chimpanzees. A similar tendency was observed in capuchins [59], but the reversed tendency was also reported in nonprimate species, such as ungulates [60]. Furthermore, Kobayashi and Hashiya [61] reported that wide eye-shape in primates, including humans, was also correlated with group size. Humans have the widest eyes among primates and are the only species with white sclera [38]. Kobayashi and colleagues also found that the rate of gaze behavior without head or body motion was correlated with the width of the eyes [38] and group size [61], and suggested that gaze may play the role in “remote” grooming.

Taken together, these previous findings suggest that eye-blinks, a looking behavior along with vigilance and gazing, may be governed by social factors. For monitoring to detect inter- or intraspecific risks, vigilance behavior should increase with increases in potential threats, as shown previously [58][60]. Blinking, however, shuts down external information even though it is for a very short duration. Humans are known to show greater difficulty in detecting changes in scenes during blinking (i.e., change blindness [62], [63]), and spontaneous eye-blinks are highly synchronized with scene breaks when watching videos to minimize the chance of losing critical information [64]. If primates increased monitoring activity, the blink rate would have decreased to maintain a higher vigilance level as the group size increased. Human pilots in flight simulators were shown to blink more frequently and with longer durations when flying over “friendly” (less vigilant) skies than when flying over “enemy” territory [65]. Furthermore, when they were targeted by enemy radar, the pilots blinked very infrequently and the blinks were of very short duration [65]. Under some specific vigilant contexts, eye-blink rates actually decreased in humans. Our results, however, showed the opposite tendency, i.e., the blink rate increased as a function of group size. Thus, an increased blink rate may have a different or additional social role to visual monitoring. One candidate role may be for social communication, similar to grooming or gazing [57], [61]. Eye-blink behavior may have developed as a means of remote visual communication. The observation that the blink rate affects the formation of a person's impression in humans is a suggestive example of eye-blinking as a social communication tool [66]. This hypothesis may also be indirectly supported by the “colorful” primates [67]. Some species of primates (e.g., mangabeys, baboons, and some species of macaques) have very colorful faces and especially bright white eyelids [67]. Some studies have suggested that this coloration may have evolved to adapt to visually “noisy” environments [68]. However, bright color around the eyes may enhance attention to the eye region, as in the human sclera. Furthermore, this eyelid color is most enhanced when the monkeys are blinking, and this coloration may be indicative of the use of eye-blinks for social communication. Although whether species with such highlighted eyelids show more eye-blinks than other species is unclear, our preliminary data suggest that species with contrasting bright eyelids (mangabeys, gelada baboons) show lower blink rates (9.9 bpm) than other species (genus Macaca, family Cercopithecidae, 14.5 bpm, n  =  11); this difference, however, was not significant [t(12)  =  1.44, P  =  0.174].

In summary, we found significant relationships between some aspects of eye-blink behaviors (blink rate) and group size, as well as body size and activity rhythm factors. In addition, we propose that spontaneous eye-blinks in primates have some role in social communication. Further studies are required to test this possibility under more controlled settings. For example, our data on blink rate were correlated with the “average” group size. As in the case for vigilance behavior in the study by Kutsukake [58], one will have to examine how eye-blink behaviors in primates are affected by the “actual” social contexts of each individual, such as the number of spatially proximate conspecifics. Also, investigating the developmental origin of eye-blink behaviors from the standpoint of comparative cognitive science will be necessary [69]. Humans show increased blink rate from the minimum level to around 20 bpm during the course of development [6], [7], [24], [52]. Unfortunately, no data concerning the development of eye-blink behaviors in nonhuman primates are available. Spontaneous eye-blinking is affected by cognitive demands [64], [70], [71]. One will need to examine how the varied cognitive demands affect the eye-blink behaviors in various primates. We also emphasized the relationship between facial (especially eyelid) coloration of some primate species and eye-blink behavior, although definitive conclusions cannot yet be reached based on our results. Further detailed observations will provide more insight into the social role of eye-blinks in primates, including humans.

Acknowledgments

We are very grateful to Mr. Akira Kato, Director, and the staff at the Japan Monkey Centre, Gen'ichiro Endo, the Director of Yagiyama Zoological Park, and Chiaki Aso, the Director of Chiba Zoological Park, for their permission and research support, and to Dr. Tetsuro Matsuzawa for his invaluable comments. We also thank the anonymous reviewers for their critical comments regarding earlier versions of this manuscript.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: HT YO KH MT. Performed the experiments: HT YO KH HO. Analyzed the data: HT YO KH MT. Wrote the paper: HT KH HO MT.

References

  1. 1. Dunbar RIM (1992) Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in primates. J Hum Evol 22: 469–493.
  2. 2. Byrne RW, Whiten A (editors) (1988) Machiavellian Intelligence: Social Expertise and the Evolution of Intellect in Monkeys, Apes, and Humans. Oxford, UK:Oxford University Press.
  3. 3. WhitenA, ByrneRW(editors) (1997) Machiavellian Intelligence II: Extensions and Evaluations.Cambridge, UK:Cambridge University Press.
  4. 4. Ponder E, Kennedy WP (1927) On the act of blinking. Q J Exp Physiol 18: 99–110.
  5. 5. Hall RJ, Cusack BL (1972) The measurement of eye behavior: Critical and selected reviews of voluntary eye movement and blinking. .U. S. Army Technical Memorandum No. 18–72.Aberdeen, MD:Human Engineering Laboratory, Aberdeen Research and Development Center.
  6. 6. Stern JA, Walrath LC, Goldstein R (1984) The endogenous eyeblink. Psychophysiology 21: 22–33.
  7. 7. Karson CN (1988) Physiology of normal and abnormal blinking. Adv Neurol 49: 25–37.
  8. 8. Drew C (1951) Variations in reflex blink-rate during visual-motor tasks. Q J Exp Psychol 3: 73–88.
  9. 9. Appel H, McCarron LT, Manning BA (1968) Eyeblink rate: Behavioral index of threat? J Couns Psychol 15: 153–157.
  10. 10. Brandt R, Fenz WD (1969) Specificity in verbal and physiological indicants of anxiety. Percept Mot Skills 29: 663–675.
  11. 11. Ostow M (1945) The frequency of blinking in mental illness: A measurable somatic aspect of attitude. J Nerv Ment Dis 102: 294–301.
  12. 12. Deiker T, Bruno RD (1976) Sensory reinforcement of eyeblink rate in a decorticate human. Am J Ment Defic 80: 665–667.
  13. 13. Stevens R, Livermore A (1978) Eyeblinking and rapid eye movement: Pulsed photic stimulation of the brain. Exp Neurol 60: 541–556.
  14. 14. Karson CN (1979) Oculomotor signs in a psychiatric population: A preliminary report. Am J Psychiatry 136: 1057–1060.
  15. 15. Karson CN, Dykman RA, Paige S (1990) Blink rates in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 16: 345–354.
  16. 16. Knorr A (1929) Beitrage zur kenntnis des lidschlages unter normalen und pathologisehcn verhaltnissen. Wurzuburger Abhandlungen aus dem Gesmatgebiet der Medizine 25: 197–223.
  17. 17. Karson CN, LeWitt PA, Calne DB, Wyatt RJ (1982) Blink rates in parkinsonism. Ann Neurol 12: 645–653.
  18. 18. Karson CN (1983) Spontaneous eye-blink rates and dopaminergic systems. Brain 106: 643–653.
  19. 19. Segraves T, Abdel-Dayem M (1996) Critique of DSM-IV: Associated features of neuroleptic-induced parkinsonism. Am J Psychiatry 153: 844.
  20. 20. Lasncman ME, Cristiano E, Golimostok A, Granillo RJ (1993) Effects of monotherapy and polytherapy on the blink reflex in epileptic patients. Epilepsia 34: 1085–1088.
  21. 21. Zametkin AJ, Stevens JR, Pittman R (1979) Ontogeny of spontaneous blinking and of habituation of the blink reflex. Ann Neurol 5: 453–457.
  22. 22. Bentivoglio AR, Bressman SB, Cassetta E, Cerretta D, Tonali P, et al. (1997) Analysis of blink rate pattern in normal subjects. Mov Disord 12: 1028–1034.
  23. 23. Bacher LF, Smotherman WP (2004) Systematic temporal variation in the rate of spontaneous eye blinking in human infants. Dev Psychobiol 44: 140–145.
  24. 24. Sugiyama T, Kashiwagura E, Ohsaga A, Tada H (2008) Life-long development and gender difference in endogenous eyeblinks from three month infants to 93 year-old aged. Int J Psychophysiol 69: P232.
  25. 25. Bacher LF (2010) Factors regulating eye blink rate in young infants. Optom Vis Sci 87: 337–343.
  26. 26. Blount P (1927) Studies of the movements of the eyelids of animals: Blinking. Q J Exp Physiol 18: 111–125.
  27. 27. Karson CN, Staub RA, Kleinman JE, Wyatt RJ (1981) Drug effect on blink rates in rhesus monkeys: Preliminary studies. Biol Psychiatry 16: 249–254.
  28. 28. Karson CN, Staub RA, Kleinman JE, Wyatt RJ (1981) Blink rates and receptor supersensitivity. Neuropharmacology 20: 91–93.
  29. 29. Freed WJ, Karson CN, Kleinman JE, Wyatt R (1981) Increased spontaneous eye-blinks in cerebellectomized cats. Biol Psychiatry 16: 789–792.
  30. 30. Karson CN, Berman KF, LeWitt PA (1983) Blink rate: Neuropharmacological and clinical evidence of dopaminergic control. Pharmacol Bull 19: 620–622.
  31. 31. Elsworth JD, Lawrence MS, Roth RH, Taylor JR (1991) D1 and D2 dopamine receptors independently regulate spontaneous blink rate in the vervet monkey. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 259: 595–600.
  32. 32. Lawrence MS, Redmond DE (1991) The D1 receptor antagonist, SCH 23390, induces signs of parkinsonism in African green monkeys. Life Sci 49: PL229–PL234.
  33. 33. Taylor JR, Elsworth JD, Lawrence MS, Sladek JR, Roth RH, et al. (1999) Spontaneous blink rates correlate with dopamine levels in the caudate nucleus of MPTP-treated monkeys. Exp Neurol 158: 214–220.
  34. 34. Haberich J, Fischer MH (1956) Beobachtung uber den Lidschlag des Rindes (Observation of the eyeblink of cows). Zeitschrift fur vergleichende Physiologie 39: 209–225.
  35. 35. Kirsten J, Kirsten EB (1983) Spontaneous blink rates of birds. Condor 85: 92–93.
  36. 36. Carrington D, Bedford PGC, Guillon JPW (1987) Polarized light biomicroscopic observations on the pre-corneal tear film I. The normal tear film of the dog. J Small Anim Pract 28: 605–622.
  37. 37. Evinger C (1984) Blinking and associated eye movements in humans, guinea pigs, and rabbits. J Neurophysiol 52: 323–339.
  38. 38. Ison R, Lonard DW (1971) Effect of auditory stimuli on the amplitude of the nictitating membrane reflex of the rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus). J Comp Physiol Psychol 75: 157–164.
  39. 39. Mowrer OH (1933) A comparison of the visual function of nictitating and blinking. J Comp Psychol 15: 75–93.
  40. 40. Menzel C (1980) Head-cocking and visual perception in primates. Anim Behav 28: 151–159.
  41. 41. Kobayashi H, Koshima S (1997) Unique morphology of the human eye. Nature 387: 767–768.
  42. 42. Rowe N (1996) The Pictorial Guide to the Living Primates. Charlestown, NH:Pergamon Press.
  43. 43. Groves CP (2001) Primate Taxonomy. Washington, DC:Smithsonian Institute Press.
  44. 44. Sato Y, Tomonaga M (2010) WAZA (World Association of Zoos and Aquariums) Ethical guidelines for the conduct of research on animals by zoos and aquariums (Translation in Japanese). Jpn J Anim Psychol 60: 139–146.
  45. 45. Zaman ML, Doughty MJ (1997) Some methodological issues in the assessment of spontaneous eyeblink frequency in man. Ophthal Physiol Opt 17: 421–432.
  46. 46. Napier JR, Napier PH (1985) The Natural History of the Primates. London, UK:British Museum.
  47. 47. Macdonald DW (1984) The Encyclopedia of Animals.London, UK:Andromeda Oxford .
  48. 48. Fleagle JG (1999) Primate Adaptation and Evolution (2nd edition). London, UK:Academic Press.
  49. 49. Smuts BB, Wrangham RW, Cheney DL, Struhsaker T, Seyfarth RM (1987) Primate Societies.Chicago, IL:University of Chicago Press .
  50. 50. Campbell CJ, Fuentes A, Mackinnon KC, Panger M, Bearder SK (2007) Primates in Perspective. New York, NY:Oxford University Press.
  51. 51. Rowe N, Myers M (2011) All the World's Primates (Web site). Available: http://www.alltheworldsprimates.org.Accessed 2013 May 8.
  52. 52. Hart WM Jr (1992) The eyelids. In: Hart WM Jr, editor. Adler's Physiology of the Eye (9th edition) St. Louis, MO:C.V. Mosby Company, pp. 1–17.
  53. 53. Heglund NC, Taylor CR (1988) Speed, stride frequency and energy cost per stride: How do they change with body size and gait? J Exp Biol 138: 301–318.
  54. 54. West GB, Brown JH (2005) The origin of allometric scaling laws in biology from genomes to ecosystems: Towards a quantitative unifying theory of biological structure and organization. J Exp Biol 208: 1575–1592.
  55. 55. Norn MS (1969) Desiccation of the precorneal tear film. Acta Ophthalmol 47: 865–880.
  56. 56. Doane G (1980) Interaction of eyelids and tears in corneal wetting and the dynamics of the normal eyeblink. Am J Ophthalmol 89: 507–516.
  57. 57. Dunbar RIM (1991) Functional significance of social grooming in primates. Folia Primatol 57: 121–131.
  58. 58. Kutsukake N (2007) Conspecific influences on vigilance behavior in wild chimpanzees. Int J Primatol 28: 907–918.
  59. 59. Hirsch BT (2002) Social monitoring and vigilance behavior in brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 52: 458–464.
  60. 60. Hunter LTB, Skinner JD (1998) Vigilance behaviour in African ungulates: The role of predation pressure. Behaviour 135: 195–211.
  61. 61. Kobayashi H, Hashiya K (2011) The gaze that grooms: Contribution of social factors to the evolution of primate eye morphology. Evol Hum Behav 32: 157–165.
  62. 62. O′Regan JK, Deubel H, Clark JJ, Rensink RA (2000) Picture changes during blinks: Looking without seeing and seeing without looking. Vis Cogn 7: 191–211.
  63. 63. Simons DJ, Levin DT (1997) Change blindness. Trends Cogn Sci 1: 261–267.
  64. 64. Nakano T, Yamamoto Y, Kitajo K, Takahashi T, Kitazawa S (2009) Synchronization of spontaneous eyeblinks while viewing video stories. Proc R Soc Lond B 276: 3635–3644.
  65. 65. Stern JA, Brown TB, Wang L, Russo MB (2005) Eye and head movements in the acquisition of visual information. Psychologia 48: 133–145.
  66. 66. Omori Y, Miyata Y (2001) Estimates of impressions based on frequency of blinking. Soc Behav Pers 29: 159–167.
  67. 67. Ankel-Simons F (2007) Primate Anatomy, 3rd edition. Burlington, MA:Academic Press.
  68. 68. Kingdon J (2007) Primate visual signals in noisy environments. Folia Primatol 78: 389–404.
  69. 69. MatsuzawaT, TomonagaM, TanakaM(editors) (2006) Cognitive development in chimpanzees. Tokyo, Japan:Springer.
  70. 70. Ohira H (1996) Eyeblink activity in a word-naming tasks as a function of semantic priming and cognitive load. Percept Mot Skills 82: 835–842.
  71. 71. Paas F, Tuovinen JE, Tabbers H, Van Gerven PWM (2003) Cognitive load measurement as a means to advance cognitive load theory. Educ Psychol 38: 63–71.