Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Tail Biting in Pigs: Blood Serotonin and Fearfulness as Pieces of the Puzzle?

  • Winanda W. Ursinus ,

    nanda.ursinus@wur.nl

    Affiliations Adaptation Physiology Group, Department of Animal Sciences, Wageningen University, Wageningen, The Netherlands, Animal behaviour & Welfare, Wageningen UR Livestock Research, Wageningen, The Netherlands

  • Cornelis G. Van Reenen,

    Affiliation Animal behaviour & Welfare, Wageningen UR Livestock Research, Wageningen, The Netherlands

  • Inonge Reimert,

    Affiliation Adaptation Physiology Group, Department of Animal Sciences, Wageningen University, Wageningen, The Netherlands

  • J. Elizabeth Bolhuis

    Affiliation Adaptation Physiology Group, Department of Animal Sciences, Wageningen University, Wageningen, The Netherlands

Abstract

Tail biting in pigs is a widespread problem in intensive pig farming. The tendency to develop this damaging behaviour has been suggested to relate to serotonergic functioning and personality characteristics of pigs. We investigated whether tail biting in pigs can be associated with blood serotonin and with their behavioural and physiological responses to novelty. Pigs (n = 480) were born in conventional farrowing pens and after weaning at four weeks of age they were either housed barren (B) or in straw-enriched (E) pens. Individual pigs were exposed to a back test and novel environment test before weaning, and after weaning to a novel object (i.e. bucket) test in an unfamiliar arena. A Principal Component Analysis on behaviours during the tests and salivary cortisol (novel object test only) revealed five factors for both housing systems, labeled ‘Early life exploration’, ‘Near bucket’, ‘Cortisol’, ‘Vocalizations & standing alert’, and ‘Back test activity’. Blood samples were taken at 8, 9 and 22 weeks of age to determine blood platelet serotonin. In different phases of life, pigs were classified as tail biter/non-tail biter based on tail biting behaviour, and as victim/non-victim based on tail wounds. A combination of both classifications resulted in four pig types: biters, victims, biter/victims, and neutrals. Generally, only in phases of life during which pigs were classified as tail biters, they seemed to have lower blood platelet serotonin storage and higher blood platelet uptake velocities. Victims also seemed to have lower blood serotonin storage. Additionally, in B housing, tail biters seemed to consistently have lower scores of the factor ‘Near bucket’, possibly indicating a higher fearfulness in tail biters. Further research is needed to elucidate the nature of the relationship between peripheral 5-HT, fearfulness and tail biting, and to develop successful strategies and interventions to prevent and reduce tail biting.

Introduction

Aberrant behaviours such as tail biting, i.e. the harmful oral manipulations of the group mates’ tails, can both reflect and contribute to health and welfare problems in pigs [1]. Tail biting is caused by many factors [2], but the lack of exploration possibilities in the home pen is likely the main risk factor for its development [3]. Accordingly, tail biting is generally seen as redirected explorative behaviour [4]. Indeed, pigs kept in pens enriched with materials suitable for chewing and rooting, perform much less tail biting compared to pigs kept in rather barren pens [3], [5]. However, in barren housing systems not all pigs develop tail biting behaviour, and, conversely, in enriched housing systems still some pigs perform the behaviour [6]. Previously, the existence of different types of tail biters was suggested [2] and there may be different underlying motivations to display the damaging behaviour by pigs kept in diverging housing systems. Individual pigs may therefore be predisposed to develop tail biting behaviour. Tail biting behaviour has comparable characteristics with for instance feather pecking in laying hens [7] and feather picking in parrots [8]. The behaviours involved may be perceived as maladaptive as they seem an inadequate response given the living circumstances [9], [10]. However, an important commonality of the problematic behaviours may be a malfunctional neurotransmitter system [11], [12] which would imply that in some individuals the behaviours may be malfunctional rather than maladaptive only [10]. Furthermore, it has been suggested that the propensity to develop aberrant behaviour is related to personality traits, such as the strategy (or ‘coping style’ [13]) an animal adopts in challenging situations [11], [14]. Animals may be classified in either proactive or reactive individuals [13]. Briefly, proactive animals cope more actively with acute mild stress, develop more easily routines and seem more rigid in their responses to changes in their environment, whereas reactive animals respond more passively to acute stressors and seem more flexible in their behavioural responses [15], [16]. These coping styles may also relate to the functioning of the brain neurotransmitter systems [17], [18] and it was previously suggested that these proactive animals may be more vulnerable to develop compulsive disorders, which show similarities with feather pecking in laying hens and tail biting in pigs [11]. Another personality trait that may contribute to the tendency of animals to develop aberrant behaviours is fearfulness or anxiety [19], [20] which may be reflected in the response to novel situations [21], [22]. Additionally, in pigs the behaviour in novelty tests has been associated with coping styles [23], tail biting [20], and serotonergic blood and brain parameters [22]. Therefore, the main aim of our study was to explore whether tail biting in pigs, in a longitudinal study, is associated with behavioural and physiological responses to challenges, and also with blood serotonin. Blood serotonin is relatively easy to measure compared to brain serotonin and behaviours and may, thus, be valuable in understanding the mechanisms of tail biting. Pigs were subjected to a back test as the behaviour performed during this test may reflect a pig’s coping style [16], [24], and they were exposed to two novelty tests, one before and one after weaning. After weaning, salivary cortisol measured around the novelty test was used to assess the pig’s adrenal response to a challenging situation [25]. Tail biting behaviour, tail damage, and blood serotonin were measured at different time points in life as we recently found that tail biting in pigs is not always consistent over different life stages [6]. Previously, in laying hens [26] and pigs [27] differences were found in gene expression profiles of the animals that performed damaging behaviours (feather peckers/tail biters), animals that received the damaging behaviours (victims of feather pecking/tail biting), and animals that were not involved in performing or receiving the damaging behaviours (neutral animals). Apart from focussing on pigs displaying tail biting behaviour, it seems, therefore, highly relevant to explore other types of pigs as well. Accordingly, our pigs were classified in tail biters/non tail biters and victims/non-victims of tail biting. Combining both classifications resulted in biters, victims, a combination of both (biter/victims), and pigs that never engaged in tail biting or receiving the behaviour (neutrals). We chose to use both barren and enriched pens as environmental enrichment strongly affects the prevalence of tail biting behaviour (e.g. [6], [28]), and may reveal different types of tail biters [2].

Materials and Methods

Ethics Statement

The experimental protocol followed during this study was approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of Wageningen University (no. 2010055f) and then also adopted by the Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Groningen, the Netherlands. Blood samples were taken near the home pens of the pigs and as quickly as possible to minimize stress. Pigs with severe tail wounds (i.e. tip of tail missing) were removed from the experiment and all pens (barren and enriched) received a jute sack from 8 weeks of age onward to keep tail biting in barren housing at an acceptable level.

Animals and housing

Pigs (n = 480) were born in 5 rounds at the experimental farm of TOPIGS Research Center IPG in Beilen (the Netherlands). Briefly, piglets were housed in a conventional (barren) farrowing pen with a sow crate. Tails and teeth were kept intact, but males were castrated. At four weeks of age piglets were transported to the experimental farm “De Haar” in Wageningen (the Netherlands). After weaning pigs were kept either barren (B) or enriched (E). The difference between E and B housing was the provision of wood shavings (12 kg at start, 3 kg added daily) and straw (1.5 kg daily) in E housing. B housed pigs received two handfuls of wood shavings daily, from six weeks of age onward. Additionally, from week 8 onward both B and E pens received a jute sack, to keep tail biting at an acceptable level in B pens. More details on housing of the pigs both pre- and post-weaning have previously been described (see [6], [24], [29]). Groups of pigs in a pen also differed in Indirect Genetic Effects (IGE) for growth; IGE results are presented elsewhere (see [30], [31]). Each pen consisted of six unrelated pigs with a 1∶1 sex ratio, and at least two pigs of each back test (see below) classification (LR:HR ratio [24]).

Tail biting behaviour and tail damage

Four life phases were distinguished: one pre-weaning, and three post-weaning (1–3). These phases were, roughly, according to general production stages: piglet (0–4 weeks), weaner (4–8 weeks), grower (8–16 weeks), finisher (16–23 weeks). Pigs were, per phase, identified as tail biters and victims of being bitten based on home pen observations and tail damage scores, respectively [6]. Tail biters were pigs involved in more than one tail biting incidence (i.e. nibbling, sucking or chewing at the tail of a pen mate) during a phase post-weaning (>1 out of 360 samples). Pre-weaning, tail biters could not be identified properly and were, therefore, not considered in this study. Tail biting behaviour was observed during instantaneous scan samples with an interval of two min (30 samples/h) while using a Psion Workabout with Observer software (Noldus Information Technology, Wageningen, The Netherlands). In total six observation days (at 4, 5, 8, 11, 16, and 21 weeks of age) of six hours each (in total 1080 samples per pig) were considered and per phase two observation days (i.e. 360 samples per pig) were used. Victims of tail biting were pigs with a tail wound (i.e. any skin damage involving (clotted) blood, which refers to the severest tail damage score recorded, see [6]) at time of weaning, or at least one time during a phase post-weaning (weeks 5–7, 8–15, and 16–23 for phase 1, 2 and 3, respectively) [6].

Behavioural tests

Pre-weaning back test.

Piglets (n = 480) were subjected to a back test at approximately 14 days of age (Figure 1A) (see [24]). The test was carried out on two consecutive days, except for the final round where all 96 piglets were tested in one day. Preliminary analyses showed that time of day did not significantly effect the results. From each litter, individual piglets were placed in supine position for one min to observe their behavioural response which may range from vigorous struggling and screaming to immobility (see for more details [32], [33]). Two observers conducted the test, one observer held the piglet and counted the number of struggles, and the other observer counted the number of vocalizations and registered latency times to first struggle and vocalization. Latency to first struggle was strongly correlated with number of struggles (r = −0.84, P<0.001) and latency to first vocalization was strongly correlated with number of vocalizations (r = −0.76, P<0.001) (analysed with Spearman’s rank correlation on residuals from a GLM with round as fixed effect, n = 480). Therefore, further analysis was restricted to numbers of struggles and vocalizations.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Pigs subjected to three behavioural tests.

A: Back test. B: Pre-weaning novel environment exposure (pNEe). C: Novel object exposure (NOe).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0107040.g001

Pre-weaning novel environment exposure.

Pre-weaning, individual piglets (n = 470) were, by litter, exposed to a novel environment (pNEe) at the age of 3.5 weeks (see also [24]). Testing was, per round, carried out on two consecutive days. Preliminary analyses showed that time of day did not significantly effect the results. The novel environment consisted of a 1.25 m×1.25 m arena with dark wooden walls of 62.5 cm height, and concrete flooring (Figure 1B). A heating lamp with yellow lighting was placed above the arena. The test lasted for 2.5 min. Behaviours included in our study were percentage of time spent on walking (all forms of moving), standing alert (standing with head upward and ears pricked), exploring (walking or standing while nosing, licking or rooting floor or walls), and total number of vocalizations (grunts, grunt-squeals, squeals, and screams). Behaviours were recorded by two observers (one for continuous behaviours and one for behavioural events) who had each a Psion Workabout with Observer software (Noldus Information Technology, Wageningen, The Netherlands).

Post-weaning novel object exposure.

Post-weaning, individual pigs (n = B: 224, E: 227) were subjected to a novelty test at 13 weeks of age (see also [30]). The order of testing was balanced for housing condition and sex. Testing was, per round, carried out on five consecutive days. The size of the test arena was 5 m×5 m, made of wooden walls (approximately 1.5 m high) and concrete flooring (Figure 1C). The arena (walls + floor) was darkened with grey painting. The first 5 min pigs were exposed to the test arena (i.e. novel environment exposure or NEe), then a metal bucket was dropped from the ceiling (i.e. novel object exposure or NOe) and pigs were left in the arena for another 5 min. Total duration of the test was 10 min. In a previous study large behavioural differences were found between NEe and NOe when pigs were 11 weeks of age, pointing in the direction of NOe being more fearful for pigs post-weaning compared to NEe [22]. Since we were especially interested in behavioural responses to potentially stressful situations, only the behaviours recorded during the NOe were used for further analysis. Behaviours considered were percentage of time spent on standing alert (standing with head upward and ears pricked), exploring the arena (nosing and rooting floor, nosing and rooting walls, and chewing), exploring the novel object (nosing, sniffing, rooting, licking or chewing the metal bucket), and total number of vocalizations (barks, grunts, grunt-squeals, squeals, and screams). Two observers with a Psion hand-held computer recorded continuous behaviours or behavioural events. Additionally, total distance covered (i.e. locomotion), and percentage of time spent near the door (∼1.25×2.25 m in front of the door, with the door centred in the ∼2.25, see also [30]) and directly adjacent to the walls (excluding the door zone) were recorded by video tracking using EthoVision XT 8.5 (Noldus Information Technology, Wageningen, The Netherlands). Housing (B/E) effects on behavioural parameters of the NOe were not present, which was previously described elsewhere [30], and are therefore not further considered in our study.

Physiological measures

Serotonin measures.

Blood samples were taken when the pigs were approximately 8 (T1), 9 (T2), and 22 (T3) weeks of age. The first (n = B: 237, E: 235 pigs) and last blood samples (n = B: 213, E: 221 pigs) were considered as basal samples. The second sample (n = B: 233, E: 230 pigs) was taken three days after a regrouping test, where pairs of pigs were, for another study, mixed with unfamiliar pigs during 24 h and then returned to their original home pen (see for more details [29]), but no short-term effects on blood serotonergic measures were expected as they are thought to be rather stable over time [34], [35]. Samples were taken by venipuncture while the pigs were either placed on their backs (T1 and T2) or when held in a nose sling (T3). Order of blood collection was always balanced for housing. Blood was collected in EDTA containing tubes (9 ml) and stored on ice until transfer to the lab for further processing. Platelets (expressed in 109 cells/l) were counted using a Sysmex F-820 Counter (Sysmex Corporation, Kobe, Japan).

Blood platelet serotonin level. Platelet serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine or 5-HT) level was determined using a fluorimetric assay based on [36], [37] as previously described [22]. Platelet 5-HT level was expressed in nmol/109 platelets.

Whole blood serotonin level. Serotonin in platelets represents >95% of 5-HT found in blood [38], [39]. Therefore, we multiplied platelet 5-HT level by the number of platelets counted in whole blood (109 cells/l) to obtain whole blood 5-HT. Whole blood 5-HT was expressed in nmol/ml blood.

Blood platelet serotonin uptake velocity. Serotonin uptake velocity in blood platelets was measured by a radioactive uptake assay based on [40], [41] as previously described [22]. Platelet 5-HT uptake velocity was expressed as pmol/109 platelets/min.

Salivary cortisol measures.

Before (t = 0 min) and after (t = 15, 30, and 60 min) the start of the novelty test at 13 weeks of age, pigs (n = B: 218, E: 218) were allowed to chew on two cotton buds to obtain saliva samples to measure cortisol concentrations by a radioimmunoassay kit [30], [42]. Pigs were previously habituated to chewing on the cotton buds. For each pig the deltas between the first and second sample (Δ t = 15–0) (i.e. cortisol rise from the first sample to the highest observed peak), and between the second and final sample (Δ t = 15–60) (i.e. cortisol recovery from the highest observed peak to the final sample) were calculated. The area under the curve (AUC) from time 0 to 60 was determined per pig using the linear trapezoidal rule. Housing (B/E) effects on salivary cortisol parameters were previously described elsewhere [30] and therefore not further considered in our study. Shortly, B housed pigs had consistently higher values compared to E housed pigs, but cortisol responses to novelty did not differ between the pigs from the two different housing types.

Statistical Analysis

SAS version 9.2 (Statistical Analysis System Institute and Inc., 2002–2008) was used for all statistical analyses. The effect of type of pig (see below) was considered per phase as tail biting behaviour observed in individual pigs was inconsistent throughout life resulting in different classifications per pig per phase [6]. Additionally, an ‘overall’ score per pig (ever (i.e. in one of the three phases) a tail biter, and ever a victim or not) was used to assess the overall effect of type of pig on variables considered per phase. Number of pigs varied among analyses due to several reasons: some pigs did not participate in the novel environment test pre-weaning (i.e. they were initially not selected for follow up), pigs were removed from the experiment due to health reasons and tail wounds, blood samples were not suitable for analysis, some blood and saliva samples were lost or could not be analysed due to technical problems.

The effect of housing and of type of pig on serotonin measures.

The effect of housing (B/E) on serotonin measures was tested in a mixed model with a fixed effect of round (1–5) and random effect of pen (nested within round). Subsequently, the effect of type of pig with respect to tail biting (per phase and over all phases) on serotonin measures was tested separately for both types of housing (B/E). At time of weaning piglets were classified as either a victim (i.e. piglet had a tail wound; n = B: 14, E: 30) or non-victim of tail biting (i.e. piglet had no tail wound; n = B: 226, E: 210). This classification in non-victim/victim (0/1) was tested as fixed effect in a mixed model, together with round (1–5) and the random effect of pen (nested within round). In each phase (1–3) post-weaning, pigs were classified as either a tail biter (at least twice involved in biting incident) (n = B: 29, 40, and 38, E: 0, 6, and 8 in phase 1–3, respectively) or a non-tail biter (n = B: 211, 199, and 183, E: 240, 232, and 223 in phase 1–3, respectively). In addition, all pigs were classified in each phase as either a victim (at least once a tail wound) (n = B: 76, 132, and 154, E: 26, 29, and 58 in phase 1–3, respectively) or a non-victim (no tail wounds) (n = B: 164, 107 and 68, E: 210, 214, 209 and 173 in phase 1–3, respectively). Both 0/1 classifications were treated as fixed effects with two levels, i.e. the effects of ‘biter’ (non-tail biter = 0 versus tail biter = 1) and of ‘victim’ (non-victim = 0 versus victim = 1), respectively. These two fixed effects and their interaction were included in a mixed model, together with the fixed effect of round (1–5) and the random effect of pen (nested within round). Including the interaction between the effects of ‘biter’ and of ‘victim’ in the model made it possible to distinguish between the following four types of pigs: biter (‘biter’ = 1, ‘victim’ = 0) (n = B: 23, 22, and 11, E: 0, 5, and 2 for phase 1–3, respectively), victim (‘biter’ = 0 and ‘victim’ = 1) (n = B: 70, 114, and 126, E: 26, 28, and 52 for phase 1–3, respectively), a combination of both, here referred to as biter/victim (‘biter’ = 1 and ‘victim’ = 1) (n = B: 6, 18, and 27, E: 0, 1, and 6 for phase 1–3, respectively) and pigs not involved in tail biting or being tail bitten, here referred to as neutral pigs (‘biter’ = 0 and ‘victim’ = 0) (n = B: 141, 85, and 57, E: 214, 204, and 171 for phase 1–3, respectively). A similar approach was used to test the effect of an overall classification of each pig. Pigs were classified as tail biter if they were at least once a tail biter in any of the phases (n = B: 88, E: 14). Similarly, pigs were classified in victims if they were at least once a victim in any of the phase (n = B: 200, E: 109). Including the interaction of the effects of ‘biter’ and ‘victim’ resulted in biters (n = B: 15, E: 4), victims (n = B: 127, E: 99), biter/victims (n = B: 73, E: 10) and neutrals (n = B: 25, E: 127). Serotonin measures were logarithmically transformed if necessary to approach normal distribution of residuals.

Principal Component Analysis.

A Principal Component Analysis (PCA) [43] was conducted by type of housing (B/E) on variables from the back test, pre-weaning novel environment test (pNEe), and novel object test (NOe) including salivary cortisol measures, to examine whether variation in behavioural and physiological responses of the pigs (17 in total) could be summarized in a limited number of different factors [44]. Prior to PCA, variables were, if necessary, square root (number of vocalizations), arcsine square root (proportion of time spent exploring the bucket, and proportion of time spent near the door zone during NOe) or logarithmically (cortisol measures) transformed, and all variables were subjected to a general linear model with round (1–5) as fixed effect to obtain residuals used for the PCA. After extraction, principal components were scaled by their standard deviations (square roots of associated Eigenvalues) and subjected to orthogonal rotation (varimax) to obtain independent factors.

The effect of type of pig on principal component factors.

A mixed model was performed to test whether type of pig (over all phases and per phase) with respect to tail biting, had an effect on the factors that were retained from the PCA. Similar to the analysis of serotonin measures, 0/1 classifications of pigs as ‘biter’ (non-tail biter = 0 versus tail biter = 1) and ‘victim’ (non-victim = 0 versus victim = 1) were included in the model as fixed effects, and their interaction was considered as well (resulting in the four pig types: biters, victims, biter/victims, neutrals). Furthermore, the random effect of pen (nested within round) was included in the model. Round (1–5) was not included in the model, as variables were already corrected for the effect of round prior to PCA.

Correlations between serotonin measures and principal component factors.

To assess consistency over time (T1, T2, and T3) with respect to blood serotonin storage (both expressed in whole blood and in blood platelets), a general linear model per type of housing (B/E) with round (1–5) as fixed effect was run on raw data of serotonin variables to obtain residuals. Thereafter, Spearman’s rank correlation coefficients (as not all variables approached normality) were calculated between residuals of serotonin measures. A similar procedure was used to determine possible relationships between serotonin measures and factors obtained by the PCA. Spearman’s rank correlation coefficients were calculated between residuals of serotonin measures and factors.

Results

The effect of housing and of type of pig on serotonin measures

Housing significantly affected platelet 5-HT uptake velocity (determined at T3 only), where B housed pigs had higher velocities compared to E housed pigs (P<0.05) (Table 1). No other significant effects of housing were found with respect to 5-HT measures, except that B housed pigs tended to have higher platelet 5-HT levels at T1 (P<0.10).

thumbnail
Table 1. Blood 5-HT measures at 8 (T1), 9 (T2), and 22 (T3) weeks of age in barren or enriched housed pigsa.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0107040.t001

Whole blood 5-HT measured at T1 was significantly (P<0.001) positively correlated with whole blood 5-HT measured at T2 (B: r = 0.42, E: r = 0.44) and T3 (B: r = 0.40, E: r = 0.36), and T2 and T3 were also significantly (P<0.001) correlated (B: r = 0.43, E: r = 0.38). Corresponding results were found for blood platelet 5-HT measures (all P<0.001), where T1 was correlated with T2 (B: r = 0.43, E: r = 0.41) and T3 (B: r = 0.37, E: r = 0.38), and T2 was also correlated with T3 (B: r = 0.37, E: r = 0.29).

Barren housing.

Considering a pig’s life as a whole, no significant main effects of ‘biter’ (i.e. non-tail biters versus tail biters) or ‘victim’ (i.e. non-victims versus victims), and no significant interaction between these effects (i.e. allowing for the distinction between tail biters, victims, biter/victims, and neutral pigs) were found with respect to the blood 5-HT measures (data not shown). However, considering each phase of life separately did reveal relationships between type of pigs and serotonin measures. Victims at time of weaning had, when B housed post-weaning, lower whole blood and platelet 5-HT levels at T1 and T3 compared to non-victims (Table 2). Tail biters classified during phase 1 post-weaning tended to have a lower platelet 5-HT level at T1 compared to non-tail biters (Table 3). Tail biters of phase 2 had a significantly lower platelet 5-HT level at T2 and they tended to have a lower whole blood 5-HT level at T2 compared to non-tail biters. Furthermore, phase 3 tail biters had significantly lower platelet 5-HT levels at T2 compared to non-tail biters. Victims of phase 3 had significantly lower platelet 5-HT uptake velocities, and tended to have higher platelet 5-HT levels at T2 compared to non-victims. Finally, a significant interaction between ‘biter’ and ‘victim’ revealed that tail biters of phase 3 had lower whole blood 5-HT at T2, and tended to have higher platelet 5-HT uptake velocities at T3 compared to victims, biter/victims, and neutral pigs.

thumbnail
Table 2. Blood 5-HT measures at 8 (T1), 9 (T2), and 22 (T3) weeks of age in barren or enriched housed victims and non-victims of tail biting at time of weaninga.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0107040.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Blood 5-HT measures at 8 (T1), 9 (T2) and 22 (T3) weeks of life and given per type of pig (with respect to tail biting) in barren housing over three phases of life (weaner, grower, finisher) post-weaninga.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0107040.t003

Enriched housing.

Also when considering E housed pigs throughout life, no relationships were found between type of pigs (in terms of either significant fixed effects of ‘biter’ and ‘victim’ or their interaction) and 5-HT measures, although a tendency for an interaction between tail biters and victims was found with respect to platelet 5-HT measured at T3 (P<0.10, but post-hoc analysis revealed no pairwise differences; untransformed LSmeans ± SEM: biters: 14.4±6.62, victims: 19.3±1.55, biter/victims: 26.7±4.85, neutrals: 19.3±1.38) (non-significant findings not shown). Victims at time of weaning tended to have a higher platelet 5-HT uptake velocity at T3 compared to non-victims when E housed post-weaning (Table 2). Pigs identified as victim of tail biting during phase 2 post-weaning had lower whole blood 5-HT levels and platelet 5-HT levels at T3 compared to non-victims (Table 4). Tail biters identified during phase 2 had higher platelet 5-HT uptake velocities at T3 compared to non-tail biters. In phase 3, a significant interaction was observed between ‘biter’ and ‘victim’, where tail biters tended to have lower whole blood 5-HT levels at T3 compared to biter/victims and neutral pigs. A significant interaction between ‘biter’ and ‘victim’ was also found for platelet 5-HT levels at T3 (P<0.01), where tail biters had significantly lower levels compared to biter/victims and neutral pigs, and tended to have lower levels compared to victims of tail biting.

thumbnail
Table 4. Blood 5-HT measures at 8 (T1), 9 (T2) and 22 (T3) weeks of life and given per type of pig (with respect to tail biting) in enriched housing over three phases of life (weaner, grower, finisher) post-weaninga.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0107040.t004

Performance in behavioural tests by different types of pigs

Principal component analysis.

In B housing, six factors were retained from the PCA (Eigenvalue or EV>1). However, only five factors were retained from the PCA to maintain an equal number of factors for both B and E housing as in E housing five factors had an EV>1 (Table 5 and 6). The factors of B housing together explained 87% of the total variance and of E housing this was 93%. Factors retained in B and E housing were largely similar. Each factor was labelled according to the importance of measures defined by loadings. The factor ‘Early life exploration’ had high positive loadings for the proportion of time exploring the novel arena and walking, and a high negative loading for the proportion of time spent standing alert during the pNEe. The proportion of time spent exploring the bucket presented during NOe loaded positively, and proportions of time spent near the wall zone and standing alert loaded negatively (although this was for the latter variable less prominent in E housing), on the factor ‘Near bucket. The factor ‘Cortisol’ summarized mainly basal cortisol and the area under the curve, and to a lesser extent also changes (i.e. recovery) in cortisol determined around the novelty test post-weaning. In B housed pigs, high scores on the factor ‘Vocalizations & standing alert’ were associated with high frequencies of vocalizing during pNEe and NOe and with a low proportion of time spent exploring the arena during NOe. The loading pattern of this factor was slightly different in E housed pigs: here the factor ‘Vocalizations and standing alert’ was less clearly determined by frequencies of vocalizations during pNEe and NOe (with moderate loadings of 0.20 and 0.41, respectively), but had a high positive loading of the proportion of time spent standing alert, and a high negative loading of the proportion of time spent exploring the arena during NOe. Variables recorded during the back test, i.e. numbers of struggles and vocalizations, exclusively loaded on the factor labelled ‘Back test activity’.

thumbnail
Table 5. Loadingsa on the first five factors extracted by principal component analysis, after orthogonal rotation, of variables recorded in barren housed pigs (n = 212) during a back test at 2 weeks of age, a novel environment test at 3 weeks of age and a novel object test at 13 weeks of age, including saliva cortisol variables.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0107040.t005

thumbnail
Table 6. Loadingsa on the first five factors extracted by principal component analysis, after orthogonal rotation, of variables recorded in enriched housed pigs (n = 212) during a back test at 2 weeks of age, a novel environment test at 3 weeks of age and a novel object test at 13 weeks of age, including saliva cortisol variables.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0107040.t006

Barren housing.

Overall, B housed pigs classified as tail biters had significantly lower ‘Near bucket’ scores compared to non-tail biters (tail biters: −0.24±0.14, non-tail biters: 0.12±0.11, P<0.05). Furthermore, tail biters tended to have a higher ‘Back test Activity’ compared to non-tail biters (tail biters: 0.21±0.13, non-tail biters: −0.09±0.11, P<0.10) and more specifically it tended to be so compared to victims and neutral pigs (biter: 0.42±0.24, victim: −0.02±0.09, neutrals: −0.17±0.19, P<0.10). No other significant (or tendencies to) relationships between the overall classification of pigs and PCA factors were found (data not shown). When considering phases of life separately, piglets with a tail wound at weaning had lower ‘Vocalizations & standing alert’ scores (P<0.05) and tended to have lower ‘Cortisol’ scores (P<0.10) (Figure 2A). Tail biters identified during phase 1 tended to have lower ‘Near bucket’ scores (P<0.10), and tail biters of phase 3 had significantly (P<0.05) lower ‘Near bucket’ scores compared to non-tail biters (Figure 3A and 3C). During phase 2 post-weaning, an interaction between ‘biter’ and ‘victim’ was found with respect to ‘Early life exploration’, where victims had significantly lower scores of this factor compared to neutral pigs (P<0.05), but not compared to tail biters and biter/victims (Figure 3B). Furthermore, victims identified during phase 2 tended to have lower ‘Vocalizations & standing alert’ scores compared to non-victims (P<0.10), but an interaction between ‘biter’ and ‘victim’ tended to be present as well and revealed that tail biters seemed to have higher scores compared to victims, biter/victims and neutrals (all P<0.10) (Figure 3B).

thumbnail
Figure 2. Factor scores of victims and non-victims of tail biting at time of weaning.

Behavioural and physiological responses of pigs to novelty (pre- and post-weaning) were summarized in five factors using a PCA. Factor scores are presented per type of pig, i.e. victim (with tail wound) or non-victim (without tail wound) at time of weaning. A: Barren housing. B: Enriched housing. +P<0.10, *P<0.05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0107040.g002

thumbnail
Figure 3. Factor scores of barren housed pigs classified as biters, victims, biter/victims or neutrals with respect to tail biting post-weaning.

Behavioural and physiological responses of pigs to novelty (pre- and post-weaning) were summarized in five factors using a PCA. Factor scores are presented per type of pig, i.e. tail biter, victim (with tail wound), both tail biter and victim (biter/victim), or pigs that were neither tail biter or victim (neutral). A: Phase 1 post-weaning. B: Phase 2 post-weaning. C: Phase 3 post-weaning. +P<0.10, *P<0.05. Pig types lacking a common letter differ significantly (a/b) or tend to do so (x/y/z).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0107040.g003

Enriched housing.

Classifying the E housed pigs once (‘overall’) with respect to tail biting and thereby combining all phases of life, did not reveal any relationships with the factors obtained from the PCA (‘Early life exploration’, ‘Cortisol’, ‘Near bucket’, ‘Vocalizations & standing alert’, and Back test activity’) (data not shown). Neither having a tail wound at the time of weaning (Figure 2B) nor type of pig post-weaning had an effect on the five factors (Figure 4).

thumbnail
Figure 4. Factor scores of enriched housed pigs classified as biters, victims, biter/victims or neutrals with respect to tail biting post-weaning.

Behavioural and physiological responses of pigs to novelty (pre- and post-weaning) were summarized in five factors using a PCA. Factor scores are presented per type of pig, i.e. tail biter, victim (with tail wound), both tail biter and victim (biter/victim), or pigs that were neither tail biter or victim (neutral). A: Phase 1 post-weaning. B: Phase 2 post-weaning. C: Phase 3 post-weaning.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0107040.g004

Correlations between blood 5-HT and performance in behavioural tests

Barren housing.

In B housed pigs, ‘Vocalizations & standing alert’ was significantly positively correlated with platelet 5-HT levels determined at T1 (r = 0.15, P<0.05) and it tended to be so for T2 (r = 0.13, P<0.10) and T3 (r = 0.12, P<0.10). Furthermore, also ‘Early life exploration’ tended to be positively correlated with platelet 5-HT levels determined at T1 (r = 0.12, P<0.10) and T2 (r = 0.12, P<0.10). No other correlations were found between the factors retained from the PCA and 5-HT measures.

Enriched housing.

In E housed pigs, ‘Back test activity’ was significantly positively correlated with whole blood 5-HT measured at T1 (r = 0.23, P<0.001) and T2 (r = 0.20, P<0.01), and platelet 5-HT levels of T1 (r = 0.20, P<0.01) and T2 (r = 0.23, P<0.001). Furthermore, ‘Cortisol’ tended to be negatively correlated with whole blood 5-HT levels at T2 (r = −0.13, P<0.10) and with platelet 5-HT levels at T2 (r = −0.14, P<0.10).

Discussion

The main aim of our study was to explore whether tail biting in pigs is associated with behavioural and physiological responses to challenges, and also with blood serotonin. To our knowledge, this experiment is the first to provide evidence suggesting that tail biting in pigs is possibly linked to both fearfulness and the blood 5-HT system.

The effect of housing

Platelet serotonin (5-HT) uptake velocities were significantly higher in barren (B) compared to enriched (E) housed pigs. Except for one tendency for higher 5-HT platelet storage in B housed pigs than in E housed pigs, no housing effects on blood 5-HT levels were found. In a previous study in pigs [22], housing affected blood platelet 5-HT measures also only marginally and not significantly. Blood 5-HT has been associated with, amongst others stress physiology [45], [46], gut motility [47], and immune function [48] which may be affected by type of housing. B housed pigs are likely to suffer from stress due to the inability to perform species specific behaviours [49]. In humans, stress may reduce platelet 5-HT uptake sites (patients with PTSD [45]) and velocity (after surgery [46]). This altered platelet 5-HT uptake may be related to changes in HPA-axis responses, although cause and effect remain undecided [46]. Environmental enrichment in pigs, as compared with barren housing, has been shown to affect HPA-axis (re)activity at different levels indeed [50], [51] and also in our E and B pigs differences in cortisol levels (but not in response to novelty) were found (see [30]). Furthermore, in E housed pigs a tendency for a negative correlation between salivary cortisol and 5-HT storage was observed, suggesting a link between HPA-axis functioning and platelet 5-HT. In addition, E housed pigs likely have an increased gut motility [52] due to the availability of highly fibrous straw. In human babies, increased gut motility was related to increased platelet 5-HT levels [47], and in chickens the availability of fibrous litter for foraging, which likely affects gut motility, also affected levels of blood 5-HT [53]. Also in pigs, provision of dietary fibres has been shown to affect whole blood 5-HT [54]. Hence, also gut motility may relate to blood 5-HT (uptake) in different ways. Collectively, these findings suggest that the effect of housing on platelet 5-HT uptake velocity in our pigs may be related to both stress (B pigs) and the presence of fibrous foraging material, i.e. straw (E pigs). However, the exact underlying mechanisms remain unclear and the existence of other aspects involved cannot be excluded, as peripheral 5-HT also serves other functions and plays, for instance, a role in immune responses [48].

Relationships between behaviours may be affected by the environment (e.g. [55], [56]), which emphasizes for our study the importance of studying relationships between tail biting and other pig characteristics in different housing systems. Nevertheless, in E housing far less tail biting and tail damage was observed and only few E housed tail biters were identified [6]. Consequently, in E housed pigs, relations between tail biting and serotonergic, behavioural and cortisol measures were explored, but results should, given these low numbers of tail biters, be considered with caution.

Tail biting and its relation with blood serotonin

Classifications of pigs according to performing and/or being the victim of tail biting at any phase of life, i.e. over the whole observation period, did not relate to peripheral serotonergic measures. Tail biting behaviour observed in individual pigs was, however, inconsistent throughout life [6], which may explain the lack of relationships between this ‘overall’ characterization with respect to being the actor and recipient of tail biting (combination of all phases in life) and 5-HT measures. However, our results also show that relationships between tail biting and 5-HT seemed to be present in pigs of both housing systems when specific phases in a pig’s life were considered. Generally, in several phases of life (although not all), tail biters had lower whole blood and platelet 5-HT levels and higher platelet 5-HT uptake velocities compared to either non-tail biters or other types of pigs (victims, biter/victims, neutrals). This seems in line with lower 5-HT values in whole blood or platelets found in humans with mental disorders such as obsessive compulsive disorders (OCD) [57] and depression [58], [59], and in laying hens that perform feather pecking behaviour [60], [61]. Additionally, in laying hens selected for a low mortality due to severe feather pecking and other injurious behaviours, lower platelet 5-HT uptake velocities were found [60], [62]. Therefore, tail biters may suffer from a (temporary) change in the blood 5-HT system. Remarkably though, victims of tail biting (i.e. pigs with a tail wound) also seemed to have lower whole blood and platelet 5-HT levels, but had a significantly lower or tendency for a higher platelet 5-HT uptake velocity, compared to non-victims. It must be noted, though, that non-victims were possibly also tail bitten and could have bite marks, but they were not as severely bitten as the pigs we identified as victims. It is possible, that victims with tail wounds may suffer from depressive-like symptoms (e.g. due to B housing, to being tail bitten, or a combination of both) and also develop a malfunctional blood 5-HT system, although not entirely identical to that observed in tail biters. In pigs, especially the neutral animals not involved in the damaging behaviour at all, were found to differ in gene expression profiles (some of the genes were associated with production, sociality, and novelty seeking) compared to animals that either displayed or received the damaging behaviour [27]. Although in our study both tail biters and victims were found to differ from non-tail biters or non-victims, or other pig types, a clear and consistent difference between neutral pigs and both tail biters and victims was not found. This difference in findings may be explained by the type of pigs used and the relatively low tail biting threshold (>1 tail biting incidence per phase of life) that was used. In the current study, we used all available animals including a group of pigs that were both tail biter and tail bite victim, instead of a selected set of (possibly the most extreme) animals (biter, victim, neutral) as was done by others [27].

In B housed pigs, whole blood and platelet 5-HT measured at T2 (i.e. week 9) were related to tail biting in phases 2 and 3 post-weaning. In E housing, associations between type of pig, with respect to tail biting and 5-HT measures were found at T3 (i.e. week 22) only. Additionally, 5-HT measures determined at T1 (i.e. week 8) were not significantly related to any type of pig in any phase, except for pigs that were victims of tail biting at time of weaning and subsequently B housed. It seems, therefore, plausible that, rather than suffering from a malfunctional blood 5-HT system from early life onward, pigs of both housing systems develop changes in their blood 5-HT system during life. Average blood 5-HT values determined at T1 and T2 were similar, most likely because the time span between the two samples was very short and decreases in 5-HT levels due to aging, as found in humans [63], were not yet present. However, significant associations between tail biting and blood 5-HT were present in B housed pigs at T2. This suggests that temporarily mixing unfamiliar pigs can trigger relationships between tail biting and blood 5-HT when also unfavourably housed. As our blood samples were taken 3 days after mixing the pigs and the half life time of 5-HT in blood is approximately 3–4 days [47], [64], it is not impossible that some changes in the blood 5-HT system of individual pigs developed after mixing. Especially when mixing resulted in a lower feed intake [65] and consequently limited dietary tryptophan intake that is needed to synthesize 5-HT [66]. Dietary tryptophan levels [67] and blood tryptophan measures have been associated with tail biting behaviour in pigs [68]. Additionally, correlations between blood 5-HT values determined at different time points were not extremely high (although clearly and significantly present), suggesting fluctuations in blood 5-HT levels of individual pigs. Remarkably, in E housing also some relations were found between type of pig and 5-HT measures in later life. Here, changes in the 5-HT system may have developed due to a change in tryptophan availability or demand caused by a depressed feed intake, stress [69] (e.g. due to restricted space [70] caused by a pig’s growth) or gut (mal)functioning [47], [71], although this may also be true in B housed pigs. According to our results, pigs involved in tail biting do not consistently exhibit changes in the blood 5-HT system that may have led to tail biting behaviour or becoming a victim of tail biting. However, in phases of life during which individual pigs display tail biting, a relationship with the 5-HT system seems to exist, suggesting that tail biting with its accompanying blood 5-HT levels (lower 5-HT storage and higher platelet uptake velocity in tail biters) fluctuate throughout time. The present findings seem to support the existence of a relationship between the blood 5-HT system and tail biting [68], similar to the relationship between 5-HT and feather pecking in laying hens [60], [61], but in our pigs this relationship seems more state-like than trait-like. This does, however, not exclude the possible existence of trait-like differences in 5-HT system functioning in other types of tail biters (such as obsessive tail biters, see also [2]) than observed in our study (likely two-stage tail biters, see [6]), which may predispose these animals to perform tail biting behaviour.

Tail biting and its relation with responsiveness in pigs and blood serotonin

A fairly consistent relationship was observed in B housed tail biters with the factor ‘Near bucket’ (retained from a PCA) during a novel object test post-weaning, i.e. tail biting pigs spent or tended to spend less time near a metal bucket introduced during a novel environment test, and spent more time standing alert (a vigilance behaviour [72]), and in the wall zones compared to non-tail biters. In E housing no such relation was found, probably due to the low level of tail biting observed. Avoiding a novel object [73], standing alert [74], [75], and seeking the walls of an arena (also called ‘Thigmotaxis’) [76], [77] altogether suggests the presence of a higher fearfulness in B housed tail biters at times of challenge in an unfamiliar environment and without other pigs present. Also in humans with behavioural disorders [78], [79] or other animals displaying maladaptive behaviour (e.g. poultry: [19], [80], dogs: [81], and cats: [82]) higher levels of fear or anxiety were suggested. Furthermore, the lower blood 5-HT levels in our tail biters in combination with a higher fearfulness seem to support previous findings in pigs where lower levels of exploring an unfamiliar arena (indicative of higher fearfulness [83]) was associated with lower whole blood and platelet 5-HT levels [22]. However, in a different study, it was suggested that tail biting pigs were less fearful compared to victim pigs, which was based on the shorter latency time to touch a novel object in the home pen, the longer contact duration with the novel object and lower levels of locomotion [20]. Notably, however, in this particular study tail biters responded to exposure to the novel object test with a decrease of heart rate variability in comparison with victims; this indicates suppression of the parasympathetic nervous system which is also found in human patients with panic disorders [84]. Moreover, different measures of heart rate variability were found to be significantly intercorrelated in tail biters but not in victims or control pigs [20]; similar intercorrelations were demonstrated in human panickers [84]. The present finding in tail biting pigs, therefore, seem to agree with the notion that altered 5-HT system functioning, high fearfulness and high levels of impulsive behaviours are related (see [85] for a review).

Over all phases (but not in separate phases), being a tail biter in our study tended to be associated with a higher ‘Back test activity’ (indicative of a proactive coping strategy [16], [24]), compared to a non-tail biter. This is in line with previous findings in parrots [86], where feather pickers likely had a proactive coping strategy. In laying hens it was suggested that the initial (first order) feather peckers may be proactive copers [87], whereas the hens attracted to already damaged or ruffled feathers (second order peckers) may be reactive copers [12]. Interestingly, in one life phase, tail biters also tended to have higher ‘Vocalizations & standing alert’ scores compared to victims, biter/victims and neutrals, whereas victims had lower scores of this factor compared to non-victims. Moreover, B housed pigs with higher ‘Vocalizations & standing alert’ scores had higher platelet 5-HT levels, and E housed pigs with a higher ‘Back test activity’ also had higher whole blood and platelet 5-HT levels. Altogether, this suggests that blood 5-HT measures in pigs may be related to more than one personality dimension, which was also proposed by others [85]. Although the relationship between tail biting and coping strategy was not significantly nor consistently present, considering interactions between 5-HT, fearfulness and other personality dimensions may be relevant in understanding problematic behaviours such as tail biting in pigs.

Conclusions

Generally, within specific phases of life, tail biters and to a lesser extent also victims seemed to have lower levels of blood serotonin compared to non-performers/receivers. Tail biters also seemed to have higher blood platelet uptake velocities. Furthermore, our results show the importance of considering different phases in a pig’s life with respect to relationships between (problematic) behaviours and serotonergic measures as both may fluctuate in time. Additionally, barren housed tail biters seemed more fearful after a challenging event. Taken together, considering both behavioural responses to challenging events and blood serotonergic measures in pigs may help in characterizing and identifying individuals at risk for developing damaging behaviours such as tail biting. Further research is needed to elucidate the nature of the relationship between peripheral 5-HT and tail biting, and to develop successful preventive strategies and interventions.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Fleur Bartels, Irene Camerlink, Monique Ooms, Merel Verhoeven, Rudie Koopmanschap, and all students involved, especially Marjoke Scherpenzeel, for their assistance during this experiment. We also gratefully acknowledge the personnel of the experimental farms in Beilen (TOPIGS Research Center IPG) and Wageningen (De Haar).

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: WWU CGR IR JEB. Performed the experiments: WWU IR JEB. Analyzed the data: WWU CGR JEB. Wrote the paper: WWU CGR IR JEB.

References

  1. 1. EFSA (2007) Scientific report on the risks associated with tail biting in pigs and possible means to reduce the need for tail docking considering the different housing and husbandry systems. Annex to the EFSA Journal 611, 1–13 Question No EFSA-Q-2006-013: 1–98.
  2. 2. Taylor NR, Main DCJ, Mendl M, Edwards SA (2010) Tail-biting: A new perspective. Vet J 186: 137–147.
  3. 3. Zonderland JJ, Wolthuis-Fillerup M, Van Reenen CG, Bracke MBM, Kemp B, et al. (2008) Prevention and treatment of tail biting in weaned piglets. Appl Anim Behav Sci 110: 269–281.
  4. 4. Studnitz M, Jensen MB, Pedersen LJ (2007) Why do pigs root and in what will they root? A review on the exploratory behaviour of pigs in relation to environmental enrichment. Appl Anim Behav Sci 107: 183–197.
  5. 5. Van De Weerd HA, Docking CM, Day JEL, Edwards SA (2005) The development of harmful social behaviour in pigs with intact tails and different enrichment backgrounds in two housing systems. Anim Sci 80: 289–298.
  6. 6. Ursinus WW, Van Reenen CG, Kemp B, Bolhuis JE (2014) Tail biting behaviour and tail damage in pigs and the relationship with general behaviour: Predicting the inevitable? Appl Anim Behav Sci 156: 22–36.
  7. 7. Brunberg E, Jensen P, Isaksson A, Keeling LJ (2013) Brain gene expression differences are associated with abnormal tail biting behavior in pigs. Genes Brain Behav 12: 275–281.
  8. 8. Van Zeeland YRA, Spruit BM, Rodenburg TB, Riedstra B, Van Hierden YM, et al. (2009) Feather damaging behaviour in parrots: A review with consideration of comparative aspects. Appl Anim Behav Sci 121: 75–95.
  9. 9. Crespi BJ (2000) The evolution of maladaptation. Heredity 84: 623–629.
  10. 10. Mills DS (2003) Medical paradigms for the study of problem behaviour: A critical review. Appl Anim Behav Sci 81: 265–277.
  11. 11. Korte SM, Prins J, Vinkers CH, Olivier B (2009) On the origin of allostasis and stress-induced pathology in farm animals: Celebrating Darwin’s legacy. Vet J 182: 378–383.
  12. 12. Kops MS, De Haas EN, Rodenburg TB, Ellen ED, Korte-Bouws GAH, et al. (2013) Effects of feather pecking phenotype (severe feather peckers, victims and non-peckers) on serotonergic and dopaminergic activity in four brain areas of laying hens (Gallus gallus domesticus). Physiol Behav 120: 77–82.
  13. 13. Koolhaas JM, Korte SM, De Boer SF, Van Der Vegt BJ, Van Reenen CG, et al. (1999) Coping styles in animals: Current status in behavior and stress-physiology. Neurosci Biobehav R 23: 925–935.
  14. 14. Korte SM, Ruesink W, Blokhuis HJ (1998) Heart rate variability during manual restraint in chicks from high- and low-feather pecking lines of laying hens. Physiol Behav 65: 649–652.
  15. 15. Koolhaas JM, Meerlo P, De Boer SF, Strubbe JH, Bohus B (1997) The temporal dynamics of the stress response. Neurosci Biobehav R 21: 775–782.
  16. 16. Bolhuis JE, Schouten WGP, Leeuw JAD, Schrama JW, Wiegant VM (2004) Individual coping characteristics, rearing conditions and behavioural flexibility in pigs. Behav Brain Res 152: 351–360.
  17. 17. Bolhuis JE, Schouten WGP, De Jong IC, Schrama JW, Cools AR, et al. (2000) Responses to apomorphine of pigs with different coping characteristics. Psychopharmacology 152: 24–30.
  18. 18. Koolhaas JM, De Boer SF, Coppens CM, Buwalda B (2010) Neuroendocrinology of coping styles: Towards understanding the biology of individual variation. Front Neuroendocrin 31: 307–321.
  19. 19. Rodenburg TB, De Haas EN, Nielsen BL, Buitenhuis AJ (2010) Fearfulness and feather damage in laying hens divergently selected for high and low feather pecking. Appl Anim Behav Sci 128: 91–96.
  20. 20. Zupan M, Janczak AM, Framstad T, Zanella AJ (2012) The effect of biting tails and having tails bitten in pigs. Physiol Behav 106: 638–644.
  21. 21. Forkman B, Boissy A, Meunier-Salaün MC, Canali E, Jones RB (2007) A critical review of fear tests used on cattle, pigs, sheep, poultry and horses. Physiol Behav 92: 340–374.
  22. 22. Ursinus WW, Bolhuis JE, Zonderland JJ, Rodenburg TB, de Souza AS, et al. (2013) Relations between peripheral and brain serotonin measures and behavioural responses in a novelty test in pigs. Physiol Behav 118: 88–96.
  23. 23. Jansen J, Bolhuis JE, Schouten WGP, Spruijt BM, Wiegant VM (2009) Spatial learning in pigs: Effects of environmental enrichment and individual characteristics on behaviour and performance. Anim Cogn 12: 303–315.
  24. 24. Reimert I, Rodenburg TB, Ursinus WW, Duijvesteijn N, Camerlink I, et al. (2013) Backtest and novelty behavior of female and castrated male piglets with diverging social breeding values for growth. J Anim Sci. 91: 4589–4597.
  25. 25. Dickerson SS, Kemeny ME (2004) Acute stressors and cortisol responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychol Bull 130: 355–391.
  26. 26. Brunberg E, Jensen P, Isaksson A, Keeling L (2011) Feather pecking behavior in laying hens: Hypothalamic gene expression in birds performing and receiving pecks. Poultry Sci 90: 1145–1152.
  27. 27. Brunberg E, Jensen P, Isaksson A, Keeling LJ (2013) Behavioural and Brain Gene Expression Profiling in Pigs during Tail Biting Outbreaks - Evidence of a Tail Biting Resistant Phenotype. PLoS ONE 8: e66513.
  28. 28. Beattie VE, Walker N, Sneddon IA (1996) An investigation of the effect of environmental enrichment and space allowance on the behaviour and production of growing pigs. Appl Anim Behav Sci 48: 151–158.
  29. 29. Camerlink I, Turner SP, Bijma P, Bolhuis JE (2013) Indirect Genetic Effects and Housing Conditions in Relation to Aggressive Behaviour in Pigs. PLoS ONE 8: e65136.
  30. 30. Reimert I, Rodenburg TB, Ursinus WW, Kemp B, Bolhuis JE (2013) Responses to novel situations of female and castrated male pigs with divergent social breeding values and different backtest classifications in barren and straw-enriched housing. Appl Anim Behav Sci 151: 24–35.
  31. 31. Camerlink I, Ursinus WW, Bijma P, Kemp B, Bolhuis JE (2014) Indirect genetic effects for growth rate in domestic pigs alter aggressive and manipulative biting behaviour. Accepted for publication in Behav Genet.
  32. 32. Bolhuis JE, Parmentier HK, Schouten WGP, Schrama JW, Wiegant VM (2003) Effects of housing and individual coping characteristics on immune responses of pigs. Physiol Behav 79: 289–296.
  33. 33. Melotti L, Oostindjer M, Bolhuis JE, Held S, Mendl M (2011) Coping personality type and environmental enrichment affect aggression at weaning in pigs. Appl Anim Behav Sci 133: 144–153.
  34. 34. Yuwiler A, Brammer GL, Morley JE, Raleigh MJ, Flannery JW, et al. (1981) Short-term and repetitive administration of oral tryptophan in normal men. Effects on blood tryptophan, serotonin, and kynurenine concentrations. Arch Gen Psychiat 38: 619–626.
  35. 35. Jernej B, Cicin-Sain L (1990) Platelet serotonin level in rats is under genetic control. Psychiat Res 32: 167–174.
  36. 36. Curzon G, Green AR (1970) Rapid method for the determination of 5-hydroxytryptamine and 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid in small regions of rat brain. Brit J Pharmacol 39: 653–655.
  37. 37. Kluge H, Bolle M, Reuter R, Werner S, Zahlten W, et al. (1999) Serotonin in platelets: Comparative analyses using new enzyme immunoassay and HPLC test kits and the traditional fluorimetric procedure. LaboratoriumsMedizin 23: 360–364.
  38. 38. Da Prada M, Picotti GB (1979) Content and subcellular localization of catecholamines and 5-hydroxytryptamine in human and animal blood platelets: monoamine distribution between platelets and plasma. Brit J Pharmacol 65: 653–662.
  39. 39. Celada P, Martín F, Artigas F (1994) Effects of chronic treatment with dexfenfluramine on serotonin in rat blood, brain and lung tissue. Life Sci 55: 1237–1243.
  40. 40. Butler J, O’Halloran A, Leonard BE (1992) The Galway Study of Panic Disorder II: Changes in some peripheral markers of noradrenergic and serotonergic function in DSM III-R Panic Disorder. J Affect Disorders 26: 89–99.
  41. 41. Thies-Flechtner K, Weigel I, Muller-Oerlinghausen B (1994) 5-HT uptake in platelets of lithium-treated patients with affective disorders and of healthy controls. Pharmacopsychiatry, Supplement 27: 4–6.
  42. 42. Ruis MAW, Te Brake JHA, Engel B, Ekkel ED, Buist WG, et al. (1997) The circadian rhythm of salivary cortisol in growing pigs: Effects of age, gender, and stress. Physiol Behav 62: 623–630.
  43. 43. Jolliffe I (1986) Principal component analyses. New York, Springer-Verlag.
  44. 44. Van Reenen CG, Engel B, Ruis-Heutinck LFM, Van Der Werf JTN, Buist WG, et al. (2004) Behavioural reactivity of heifer calves in potentially alarming test situations: A multivariate and correlational analysis. Appl Anim Behav Sci 85: 11–30.
  45. 45. Fichtner CG, O’Connor FL, Yeoh HC, Arora RC, Crayton JW (1995) Hypodensity of platelet serotonin uptake sites in posttraumatic stress disorder: Associated clinical features. Life Sci 57: PL–37–PL-44.
  46. 46. Naesh O, Hindberg I, Bruun AB (2001) Decreased reuptake of serotonin in human platelets after surgery. Clin Physiol 21: 39–43.
  47. 47. Kemperman RFJ, Bruins S, Te Lintelo JTV, Van der Dijs FPL, Erwich JJHM, et al. (2007) Relation between platelet serotonin and feeding mode in newborns suggests that gut motor activity is a determinant of platelet serotonin content. Biogenic Amines 21: 260–272.
  48. 48. León-Ponte M, Ahern GP, O’Connell PJ (2007) Serotonin provides an accessory signal to enhance T-cell activation by signaling through the 5-HT7 receptor. Blood 109: 3139–3146.
  49. 49. EFSA (2007) Scientific report on animal health and welfare aspects of different housing and husbandry systems for adult breeding boars, pregnant, farrowing sows and unweaned piglets. The EFSA Journal 572, 1–107 Question No EFSA-Q-2006-28.
  50. 50. De Jong IC, Prelle IT, Van De Burgwal JA, Lambooij E, Korte SM, et al. (2000) Effects of environmental enrichment on behavioral responses to novelty, learning, and memory, and the circadian rhythm in cortisol in growing pigs. Physiol Behav 68: 571–578.
  51. 51. Morrison RS, Johnston LJ, Hilbrands AM (2007) The behaviour, welfare, growth performance and meat quality of pigs housed in a deep-litter, large group housing system compared to a conventional confinement system. Appl Anim Behav Sci 103: 12–24.
  52. 52. Pond WG (1987) Thoughts on fiber utilization in swine. J Anim Sci 65: 497–499.
  53. 53. De Haas EN, Bolhuis JE, Kemp B, Groothuis TGG, Rodenburg TB (2014) Parents and early life environment affect behavioral development of laying hen chickens. PLoS ONE 9: e90577.
  54. 54. Souza da Silva C, Haenen D, Koopmans SJ, Hooiveld GJEJ, Bosch G, et al. (2014) Effects of resistant starch on behaviour, satiety-related hormones and metabolites in growing pigs. Animal. doi:10.1017/S1751731114001116.
  55. 55. Bell AM, Sih A (2007) Exposure to predation generates personality in threespined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Ecol Lett 10: 828–834.
  56. 56. Pearish S, Hostert L, Bell AM (2013) Behavioral type-environment correlations in the field: A study of three-spined stickleback. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 67: 765–774.
  57. 57. Delorme R, Betancur C, Callebert J, Chabane N, Laplanche JL, et al. (2005) Platelet serotonergic markers as endophenotypes for obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacol 30: 1539–1547.
  58. 58. Coppen A, Turner P, Rowsell AR, Padgham C (1976) 5 Hydroxytryptamine (5 HT) in the whole blood of patients with depressive illness. Postgrad Med J 52: 156–158.
  59. 59. Cleare AJ (1997) Reduced whole blood serotonin in major depression. Depress and Anxiety 5: 108–111.
  60. 60. Bolhuis JE, Ellen ED, Van Reenen CG, De Groot J, Napel JT, et al. (2009) Effects of genetic group selection against mortality on behavior and peripheral serotonin in domestic laying hens with trimmed and intact beaks. Physiol Behav 97: 470–475.
  61. 61. Rodenburg TB, Bolhuis JE, Koopmanschap RE, Ellen ED, Decuypere E (2009) Maternal care and selection for low mortality affect post-stress corticosterone and peripheral serotonin in laying hens. Physiol Behav 98: 519–523.
  62. 62. Uitdehaag KA, Rodenburg TB, Van Reenen CG, Koopmanschap RE, De Vries GR, et al. (2011) Effects of genetic origin and social environment on behavioral response to manual restraint and monoamine functioning in laying hens. Poultry Sci 90: 1629–1636.
  63. 63. Jernej B, Banović M, Cicin-Šain L, Hranilović D, Balija M, et al. (2000) Physiological characteristics of platelet/circulatory serotonin: Study on a large human population. Psychiat Res 94: 153–162.
  64. 64. Koe BK, Weissman A (1966) p-Chlorophenylalanine: a specific depletor of brain serotonin. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 154: 499–516.
  65. 65. Hyun Y, Ellis M, Riskowski G, Johnson RW (1998) Growth Performance of Pigs Subjected to Multiple Concurrent Environmental Stressors. J Anim Sci 76: 721–727.
  66. 66. Azmitia EC (2010) Evolution of Serotonin: Sunlight to Suicide. In Müller C and Jacobs B (Eds) Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology of Serotonin: 3–22.
  67. 67. Martínez-Trejo G, Ortega-Cerrilla ME, Rodarte-Covarrubias LF, Herrera-Haro JG, Figueroa-Velasco JL, et al. (2009) Aggressiveness and productive performance of piglets supplemented with tryptophan. J Anim Vet Adv 8: 608–611.
  68. 68. Valros A, Palander PA, Heinonen M, Brunberg E, Keeling L, et al. (2013) Preliminary evidence of an altered serotonin metabolism in the prefrontal cortex of tail biting pigs. In: Hötzel MJ and Pinheiro Machado Filho LC (Eds) Proceedings of the 47th Congress of the International Society for Applied Ethology (ISAE), Florianopolis, Brazil:105.
  69. 69. Keszthelyi D, Troost FJ, Jonkers DM, Van Donkelaar EL, Dekker J, et al. (2012) Does acute tryptophan depletion affect peripheral serotonin metabolism in the intestine? Am J Clin Nutr 95: 603–608.
  70. 70. Morgan KN, Tromborg CT (2007) Sources of stress in captivity. Appl Anim Behav Sci 102: 262–302.
  71. 71. Spiller R (2008) Serotonin and GI clinical disorders. Neuropharmacology 55: 1072–1080.
  72. 72. Reimert I, Bolhuis JE, Kemp B, Rodenburg TB (2013) Indicators of positive and negative emotions and emotional contagion in pigs. Physiol Behav 109: 42–50.
  73. 73. Van Reenen CG, Hopster H, Van der Werf JTN, Engel B, Buist WG, et al. (2009) The benzodiazepine brotizolam reduces fear in calves exposed to a novel object test. Physiol Behav 96: 307–314.
  74. 74. Hashimoto S, Inoue T, Koyama T (1999) Effects of conditioned fear stress on serotonin neurotransmission and freezing behavior in rats. Eur J Pharmacol 378: 23–30.
  75. 75. Vicens-Costa E, Martínez-Membrives E, López-Aumatell R, Guitart-Masip M, Cañete T, et al. (2011) Two-way avoidance acquisition is negatively related to conditioned freezing and positively associated with startle reactions: A dissection of anxiety and fear in genetically heterogeneous rats. Physiol Behav 103: 148–156.
  76. 76. Gershenfeld HK, Paul SM (1997) Mapping quantitative trait loci for fear-like behaviors in mice. Genomics 46: 1–8.
  77. 77. Kallai J, Makany T, Csatho A, Karadi K, Horvath D, et al. (2007) Cognitive and affective aspects of thigmotaxis strategy in humans. Behav Neurosci 121: 21–30.
  78. 78. Torres AR, Prince MJ, Bebbington PE, Bhugra D, Brugha TS, et al. (2006) Obsessive-compulsive disorder: Prevalence, Comorbidity, impact, and help-seeking in the British National Psychiatric Morbidity Survey of 2000. Am J Psychiat 163: 1978–1985.
  79. 79. Ashwani A, Tarun K, Ajay M, Anil H (2011) Anxiety Disorders: A Review. IRJP 2: 18–23.
  80. 80. De Haas EN, Kemp B, Bolhuis JE, Groothuis T, Rodenburg TB (2013) Fear, stress, and feather pecking in commercial white and brown laying hen parent-stock flocks and their relationships with production parameters. Poultry Sci 92: 2259–2269.
  81. 81. Seksel K, Lindeman MJ (2001) Use of clomipramine in treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder, separation anxiety and noise phobia in dogs: A preliminary, clinical study. Aust Vet J 79: 252–256.
  82. 82. Ghaffari MS, Sabzevari A (2010) Successful management of psychogenic alopecia with buspirone in a crossbreed cat. Comp Clin Path 19: 317–319.
  83. 83. Korte SM, De Boer SF (2003) A robust animal model of state anxiety: Fear-potentiated behaviour in the elevated plus-maze. Eur J Pharmacol 463: 163–175.
  84. 84. Friedman BH, Thayer JF (1998) Anxiety and autonomic flexibility: A cardiovascular approach. Biol Psychol 47: 243–263.
  85. 85. Carver CS, Miller CJ (2006) Relations of serotonin function to personality: Current views and a key methodological issue. Psychiat Res 144: 1–15.
  86. 86. Van Zeeland YRA, Van der Aa MMJA, Vinke CM, Lumeij JT, Schoemaker NJ (2013) Behavioural testing to determine differences between coping styles in Grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus erithacus) with and without feather damaging behaviour. Appl Anim Behav Sci 148: 218–231.
  87. 87. Kops MS (2014) Feather pecking and monoamines: A behavioral and neurobiological approach. Doctoral Thesis, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands.