Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Association between Parkinson’s Disease and Cigarette Smoking, Rural Living, Well-Water Consumption, Farming and Pesticide Use: Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis

Abstract

Objective

Bradford Hill’s viewpoints were used to conduct a weight-of-the-evidence assessment of the association between Parkinson’s disease (PD) and rural living, farming and pesticide use. The results were compared with an assessment based upon meta-analysis. For comparison, we also evaluated the association between PD and cigarette smoking as a “positive control” because a strong inverse association has been described consistently in the literature.

Methods

PubMed was searched systematically to identify all published epidemiological studies that evaluated associations between Parkinson’s disease (PD) and cigarette smoking, rural living, well-water consumption, farming and the use of pesticides, herbicides, insecticides, fungicides or paraquat. Studies were categorized into two study quality groups (Tier 1 or Tier 2); data were abstracted and a forest plot of relative risks (RRs) was developed for each risk factor. In addition, when available, RRs were tabulated for more highly exposed individuals compared with the unexposed. Summary RRs for each risk factor were calculated by meta-analysis of Tier 1, Tier 2 and all studies combined, with sensitivity analyses stratified by other study characteristics. Indices of between-study heterogeneity and evidence of reporting bias were assessed. Bradford Hill’s viewpoints were used to determine if a causal relationship between PD and each risk factor was supported by the weight of the evidence.

Findings

There was a consistent inverse (negative) association between current cigarette smoking and PD risk. In contrast, associations between PD and rural living, well-water consumption, farming and the use of pesticides, herbicides, insecticides, fungicides or paraquat were less consistent when assessed quantitatively or qualitatively.

Conclusion

The weight of the evidence and meta-analysis support the conclusion that there is a causal relationship between PD risk and cigarette smoking, or some unknown factor correlated with cigarette smoking. There may be risk factors associated with rural living, farming, pesticide use or well-water consumption that are causally related to PD, but the studies to date have not identified such factors. To overcome the limitations of research in this area, future studies will have to better characterize the onset of PD and its relationship to rural living, farming and exposure to pesticides.

Introduction

In 1817, James Parkinson identified “the shaking palsy,” later named Parkinson’s disease (PD), as a unique clinical entity in patients who presented four cardinal symptoms: tremor, bradykinesia, rigor and postural instability [1]. The disease is a major cause of morbidity in the elderly and its progression can impose a distressing burden of ill health. Death of dopaminergic (DA) neurons in the substantia nigra pars compacta (SNpc) and degeneration of its projections to the basal ganglia are the major, but not exclusive, findings in PD. Non-motor symptoms have been attributed to early changes in the olfactory tract and hindbrain, and late-stage changes have been described in subcortical and cortical structures [2]. Both the incidence and prevalence of PD increase with age, more so in men than in women [3].

Substantial advances have been made in understanding the role of gene mutations in familial PD [4, 5]. Studies on kindred subgroups have established that selected mutations of a number of parkin genes are linked to early- [6, 7] and late-onset [8] PD. Mutation-based impairment of chaperone proteins [9], as well as proteins involved with the ubiquitin proteasomal [10, 11] and the autophagy lysosomal [12, 13] systems, has been described. Mutation of α-synuclein [14], the protein aggregate present in Lewy bodies [15], has been characterized. In addition, proteins that play a role in mitochondrial function [16] and in the cellular response to oxidative stress [17] have been implicated in PD.

For idiopathic PD, where none of the established causal factors of parkinsonism have been identified, it is assumed that the combination of individual genetic susceptibility factors and environmental exposure to chemicals, pathogens or other factors may trigger or accelerate the onset of the disease. A major difficulty in investigating risk factors for PD is that some studies rely on self-reported PD [1820], rather than including only patients whose PD diagnosis has been clinically confirmed. Litvan et al. emphasized that “this limitation strongly affects epidemiologic studies” [21]; also see Burn et al. [22]. Patients with multiple system atrophy (MSA), progressive supranuclear palsy, cortico-basal ganglionic degeneration, Lewy body disease or vascular disease may display parkinsonism. Even where diagnosis of PD is certain, there appear to be several PD subtypes that may have different etiologies [23].

Barbeau et al. [24, 25] were among the first researchers to report a positive correlation between PD prevalence in nine hydrographic basins in Quebec and the amount of pesticide sold within each basin. They discussed several limitations or “sources of error” in their study, including diagnostic accuracy, coding accuracy, accessibility to medical or specialist care, reporting completeness, migration of the patients, variation in prescription medicine doses, mean age of the populations and incomplete reporting of PD on death certificates. Many or all of these limitations also exist in epidemiologic studies of PD conducted by other researchers in subsequent years. Barbeau et al. [25] reported an approximate 20 percent difference in the number of prevalent PD cases ascertained between indirect methods (medical records of the province of Quebec or records of L-dopa sales) and direct diagnosis based on neurological examination. PD prevalence was greater in urban communities (cities with a population >50,000) than in the nine hydrographic basins, based on the state reporting system and L-dopa sales, but not when the cases were diagnosed by neurological examination.

Since Barbeau’s seminal work, numerous studies have evaluated the association between PD and rural living, well-water consumption, farming and pesticide use [2630]. In the current study, we conducted a comprehensive assessment of the association between agriculture-related risk factors and PD because there is evidence in the literature to suggest that some component of the agricultural lifestyle contributes to the occurrence of PD. In order to contextualize the results from the analysis of agriculture-related factors, we also evaluated the association between PD and cigarette smoking because a consistent, strong inverse association has been reported in the published literature.

A systematic review of the published English epidemiological literature was done using standard methods for study identification, data retrieval and selection [31, 32]. In contrast to recent studies [2830], this study used three complementary approaches to assess the potential causal relationship between PD, rural living and agricultural practices in any population. The narrative assessment approach utilized by Li et al. [26], Brown et al. [27], Wirdefeldt et al. [33], Friere and Koifman [34], and Moretto and Colosio [35] was augmented by a qualitative assessment of the weight of the evidence using the Bradford Hill viewpoints. The results from qualitative assessments were compared with those based on published meta-analyses [2830, 3638]. To judge the relative strength of associations with PD for a number of factors related to rural living and agricultural practices, the results were compared with those from studies assessing the relationship between PD and cigarette smoking, which consistently has shown an inverse association with PD [33, 37, 39, 40]. By using this analogy, we hoped to provide the reader with a reference point for judging whether the strength and consistency of the findings described in the present study are sufficient to conclude that causal relationships exist.

Materials and Methods

A comprehensive search of the English-language literature was conducted to identify studies that evaluated associations between PD and a number of potential risk factors: cigarette smoking, rural living, well-water consumption, farming, any pesticide use and use of herbicides, fungicides, insecticides or paraquat (see S1 Appendix for search terms). In addition, bibliographies in recently published reviews and meta-analyses of pesticide use [2629, 33] were evaluated for papers that fit the categories of interest.

Three searches were conducted in PubMed to identify articles describing smoking, rural (or associated agricultural) factors or paraquat in relation to PD; these searches identified a combined total of 1,145 potentially relevant articles, including 252 duplicates, leaving 893 unique articles. For each eligible study included in the analysis, the numbers of PD cases and comparison subjects with the potential risk factor were determined within the total number of subjects evaluated. All data were extracted and entered on a standardized form by one investigator, and independently confirmed by a second investigator. The estimated relative risk (RR), or odds ratio (OR) for case-control studies (hereafter both are referred to as RR), and the 95 percent CI were recorded, and forest plots were constructed for each risk factor. The herbicide paraquat was included in this analysis because it has been identified in some epidemiological studies as a potential risk factor for PD [41]. The organochlorine insecticide DDT and its metabolite dichlorodiphenyldichloroethylene (DDE) [33]; the dithiocarbamate fungicides maneb, ziram and mancozeb [42, 43]; and the natural insecticide rotenone [44], which have been reported as potential risk factors for PD, were not included because there were limited epidemiological data. To evaluate whether results were consistent with an exposure-response trend, RRs for heavier or longer-term cigarette smoking (e.g., more packs, years or pack-years smoked) and greater exposure to herbicides, fungicides, insecticides and paraquat (e.g., higher frequency or longer duration of use) were recorded separately.

When both unadjusted and adjusted RRs were reported in a study, the adjusted RR was selected. If adjusted RRs were not reported, crude RRs were included as reported or calculated from available raw data. If data for males and females were combined, this RR was selected. In some cases where stratified RRs were reported, weighted average RRs were calculated to combine stratum-specific RRs. When multiple RRs for related but distinct exposures were reported in a study, the RR judged to be closest to the exposure category of interest or to entail the highest level of exposure was selected. In some cases, more than one informative RR was selected from a given study (or from overlapping study populations described in multiple publications), but only non-overlapping RRs were included in the meta-analysis.

Evidence of bias in the reporting of associations with PD was evaluated by fitting a regression to the selected RRs using the unweighted least squares method described by Egger et al. [45]. We conducted a Monte Carlo simulation that showed that the unweighted least squares method was superior to weighted least squares analyses described by Egger et al. [45]. Funnel plots were used to appraise the asymmetry in the distribution of RRs. For each risk factor, an overall RR and 95% CI were calculated using the fixed effects meta-analysis procedure described by Egger et al. [31].

Publication bias was evaluated by testing the null hypothesis that publication bias does not exist, using the Egger et al. [45] regression test of unweighted ordinary least squares. According to Egger et al. [45], the null hypothesis is rejected if the p-value is less than 0.10. However, as a sensitivity analysis, we reported meta-analysis RRs and 95% CIs for Tier 1 studies (explained below), both with and without correction for publication bias for the fixed and random effects models, irrespective of whether Egger’s publication bias test statistic had a p-value of less than 0.10.

Publication bias was estimated and corrected for each Tier 1 (explained below) study dataset by using the trim-and-fill procedure employed by Pezzoli and Cereda [30], and described by Duval and Tweedie [46]. Accordingly, an iterative mathematical procedure was used, based upon a rank-based data augmentation technique, to estimate the number of missing studies and to fill in the missing RRs. These filled-in RRs might be either above or below the mean RR calculated from the original published studies. For example, for heavy/long-term smoking (p = 0.01), there were ten Tier 1 studies (fixed effects RR = 0.64; 95% CI = 0.58–0.71). After three iterations, using the L0+ estimator described by Duval and Tweedie [46], the procedure estimated that six studies were missing. The estimated RRs for these “missing studies” were all assigned values by the procedure that were greater that the RRs reported for the ten Tier 1 studies. In particular, the six filled-in RR values were as far above the mean as the six lowest published RRs were below the mean. In this example, the corrected meta-analysis RR and 95% CI were 0.70 and 0.64–0.76, respectively.

The percentage of the between-study variance in RRs attributable to study heterogeneity (I2) was calculated by replacing the Mantel-Haenszel measure of central tendency with the measure of central tendency based on inverse-variance, as described by Higgins et al. [47]. This method was used because it required only knowledge of the RRs and standard errors (SEs). The natural logarithm of the RR estimate [ln(RR)] was weighted proportionally to the reciprocal of its estimated variance, which was calculated as the square of the estimated SE. Statistically significant heterogeneity was indicated when the probability of obtaining the observed I2 was less than 0.05 (two-sided test).

To aid in the evaluation of heterogeneity in the observed associations, we classified all studies for each risk factor into two tiers according to their methodological rigor. Tier 1, indicating higher study quality, included studies with incident PD cases classified according to clinical data (e.g., medical records, physician confirmation and/or neurological examination) for diagnostic confirmation, as well as individual-level exposure assessment. Tier 2 included all other studies, i.e., those with prevalent PD cases, PD classified based on self-reporting or death certificates only, and/or ecologic (group-level) exposure assessment. We adopted this methodology because it was successfully used in a previous systematic review [48] based on the premise that causality cannot be inferred unless there is a clear determination that 1) individuals within studies were actually exposed to the risk factor; 2) diagnosis of PD in cases was confirmed by a PD specialist; 3) a suitable latency period existed between initial exposure to the risk factor and the occurrence of PD; and 4) the non-cases were confirmed to be disease-free. Furthermore, to reduce the potential for selection bias, we put more weight on studies that reported incident cases discovered during the study, rather than relying on cases that existed (prevalent cases) at the time of study commencement.

We conducted a sensitivity analysis to determine whether study characteristics, other than those used for the Tier 1/Tier 2 classification described above, affected meta-analysis results. We reviewed study attributes described in the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale [49] and the Research Triangle Institute Item Data Bank [50], and selected the following five additional attributes to categorize studies: 1) whether information on exposure was collected by personal interview or by some other means, such as a self-administered questionnaire; 2) for studies that assessed pesticide exposure, whether it was based on self-reported recall or was assigned by the investigator using a job-exposure matrix, a geographical model or some other method; 3) whether the study source population was based in a general population (i.e., population-based) or another setting; 4) for case-control studies, whether controls were population- or cohort-based (i.e., selected from an existing study cohort) or selected from another source (e.g., hospital-based or friend-based); and 5) whether the investigators adjusted for confounding by at least age, gender and cigarette smoking, or adjusted for fewer factors. These study characteristics were extracted from each publication and entered on a standardized form by one investigator, and were independently confirmed by a second investigator. We did not combine these attributes into a single scale, as others have done, because we could not be certain that any specific study feature would necessarily result in a “better-quality study.” Furthermore, we found that increasing the number of criteria used to categorize the studies resulted in few studies that fulfilled all the criteria and hence the subsequent meta-analysis was of limited utility.

Heterogeneity among studies was also judged by comparing the overall RR and 95% CI calculated using the fixed effects model to the RR and 95% CI calculated using the random effects model described by DerSimonian and Laird [51]. In a fixed effects model, all of the studies are assumed to be identical (i.e., with no heterogeneity) in the sense that they have an identical design aimed at estimating the magnitude of a fixed effect of treatment [52, 53]. In this situation, the weight assigned to an individual study for calculating the weighted average meta-analysis RR is proportional to the study’s precision (i.e., inversely proportional to its variance). The assumption that observational studies have identical design is rarely true, so as an alternative, a random effects model was also used. In the random effects model, because the individual studies may vary in design and conduct, it is assumed that each study represents a random sample from a distribution of all potential studies. In meta-analysis calculations based on the random effects model, the variance of ln(RR) is the sum of the variance (σ2) of the study ln(RR) around the study’s underlying mean ln(RR) (i.e., within-study variability) plus the variance (τ2) of that study mean around the underlying mean of the population of study means from which the particular study mean is randomly selected (i.e., between-study variability). Thus the weight assigned to a study in the random effects model is dependent on its precision and the between-study variance. The results of the fixed effects model were compared with those from the random effects model overall and within each tier of study quality or stratum of study characteristics in order to estimate the contribution of between-study heterogeneity to the calculated meta-analysis RR. The measure of between-study variability based on the random effects model (τ2) was also considered as an indicator of study heterogeneity. Overall, we adapted the guidance developed by Higgins and Green [53] on how to conduct meta-analysis for intervention studies to the present set of observational epidemiological studies.

In addition to these statistical analyses, a qualitative assessment of the weight of evidence for a causal relationship between each risk factor and PD was conducted. Bradford Hill’s “viewpoints”, as summarized in Table 1, were used to assess whether the studies supported a causal exposure-disease association. These viewpoints include the strength of the association, the consistency of results among studies, evidence of a biological gradient, evidence that “exposure” preceded the occurrence of disease with an appropriate latency period, the specificity of the risk factor being evaluated, the coherence of the relationship, the biological plausibility that the risk factor could cause PD and whether analogies exist to other exposure-disease associations that are interpreted to be causal. There were no experimental studies among those identified (Table 1). In this assessment, higher-quality studies, as defined previously, were given more weight than were studies of lower quality.

Results

Study Identification and Eligibility

Three searches were conducted in PubMed to identify articles on smoking, rural living (or associated agricultural factors) or paraquat in relation to PD; these searches identified a combined total of 1,145 potentially relevant articles, including 252 duplicates, leaving 893 unique articles (S2 Appendix). An additional 30 potentially relevant articles were identified from published bibliographies. Based on a review of the titles and abstracts of all 923 articles, we excluded 677 articles without pertinent information, leaving 246 articles for full-text review. After reviewing these articles, we further excluded 133 that were not relevant or did not contain sufficient data for meta-analysis (i.e., RRs and 95% CIs, or raw frequencies), leaving 113 articles eligible for meta-analysis. Eight of these reported sets of results completely overlapped with results from another eligible article and were ultimately excluded. Overall, 105 articles were included in meta-analyses of any of the 14 exposures considered. Many of these articles contained data relating to multiple risk factors.

Study Classification

Key information used to categorize each study as either a Tier 1 or Tier 2 study is summarized in Tables A-I in S3 File. Overall, approximately 20 percent of the studies were in Tier 1 (Table 2). Thirty-three percent of studies on current cigarette smoking (11 studies) were Tier 1 studies, whereas rural living, well-water consumption and farming had 14 percent (four studies), 14 percent (five studies) and 17 percent (eight studies) Tier 1 studies, respectively. The percentage of studies on pesticides that were Tier 1 ranged from eight percent for studies on paraquat (one study) to 30 percent for studies on fungicides (three studies). There were 11 Tier 1 studies (22%) that reported any pesticide use. Among studies that evaluated high pesticide use, there were either no Tier 1 studies (paraquat) or only one Tier 1 study (herbicide, fungicide or insecticide use).

thumbnail
Table 2. Number of studies assigned to each category (Tier 1, Tier 2, exposure interview technique, pesticide exposure assessment, source population, type of controls and adjustment for confounders) for each environmental or lifestyle risk factor.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151841.t002

Most of the studies (88%) of pesticide exposure relied on self-reported pesticide use obtained either through personal interviews (49%) or by some other method. Twenty-two percent of all included studies were population-based and 29 percent of case-control studies used population- or cohort-based controls. Fifty-two percent of all studies adjusted the RRs for age, gender and cigarette smoking, with fewer adjustments being made in the other 48 percent of the studies.

Heterogeneity of Study Results

The heterogeneity of results for Tier 1 and Tier 2 studies combined, as measured by I2, was statistically significant for all risk factors examined except fungicides (Table 3). Using the random effects model, the between-study variances (τ2) of the individual study RRs for Tier 1, Tier 2 and Tier 1 & Tier 2 studies combined were calculated (Table 4, Figs A-J in S1 File). The results indicate that τ2 tended to be larger (i.e., the study RRs were more heterogeneous) when all studies were combined than when τ2 was calculated for Tier 1 studies. However, τ2 was greater in Tier 1 studies on current cigarette smoking and rural living than it was in either Tier 2 studies or across all studies combined.

thumbnail
Table 3. Fixed and random effects meta-analysis relative risk (RR) estimates and their 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for the association between Parkinson’s disease and each risk factor based on Tier 1 studies and all studies combined.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151841.t003

thumbnail
Table 4. Heterogeneity estimates for each risk factor based on between-study variance (τ2) among Tier 1, Tier 2 or Tiers 1&2 studies combined.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151841.t004

Between-study variance of RRs was lowest for fungicide use, independent of study classification, and greatest for insecticide use, rural living and paraquat use (Table 4). For insecticide use, the Tier 2 study by Das et al. [58] was an “outlier” study that contributed the most to the Tier 2 between-study heterogeneity; removal of this study resulted in a 49 percent reduction in τ2 from 0.57 to 0.29. As another example, the Tier 2 study by Liou et al. [59] contributed the most to the Tier 2 between-study variability for paraquat; when this study was removed, τ2 was reduced almost 10-fold, from 0.24 to 0.03.

Publication Bias

A statistically significant regression test statistic for publication bias in Tier 1 studies (p = 0.01) was observed for heavy/long-term smoking, but not for any other potential risk factor (Table 3). Correction for publication bias for heavy/long-term smoking did not substantially change the magnitude of the RR or the range of the 95% CI (Table 3, columns 4 and 5 vs. columns 9 and 10). For other possible risk factors, the meta-analysis RR based on Tier 1 studies was no longer statistically significant after publication bias correction for pesticide use (random effects model) and for herbicide use (random and fixed effects models); however, all changes were small in magnitude and largely inconsequential.

Cigarette Smoking

In the majority of studies, the risk of PD was statistically significantly reduced in current cigarette smokers compared with non-smokers (Fig 1). Sixty-seven percent of the 33 epidemiological studies evaluated and 10 out of 11 (the exception is Benedetti et al. [60]) Tier 1 studies reported statistically significantly decreased PD risk in cigarette smokers (Table A in S4 File and Table A in S5 File). In studies where the cigarette smoking “dose” was estimated based on packs, years and/or pack-years of smoking, PD risk was statistically significantly reduced in heavy or long-term smokers compared with non-smokers in 73 percent of the studies (90% of Tier 1 studies) (Fig 2, Table B in S4 File and Table B in S5 File). Overall, current cigarette smokers had a statistically significantly reduced risk of PD compared with non-smokers, irrespective of whether risk was assessed using a fixed effects (RR = 0.46; 95% CI = 0.42–0.51) or a random effects model (RR = 0.41; 95% CI = 0.34–0.48) (Table 3). Among Tier 1 studies, the fixed effects RR was 0.41 (95% CI = 0.35–0.48) and the random effects RR was 0.35 (95% CI = 0.25–0.49). Results were similar in studies that separately classified heavy or long-term smokers. The overall (Tiers 1 and 2 combined) estimate of the association between heavy/long-term cigarette smoking and PD was robust and insensitive to stratification by other study characteristics. However, the overall RR estimates tended to be slightly lower when calculated using the random effects model than when using the fixed effects model (Fig A in S2 File).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Association between current cigarette smoking and Parkinson’s disease.

The natural logarithm of the estimated relative risk [ln(RR)] and the 95% confidence interval for each study are displayed. Current smokers have a significantly greater or lower risk of PD than non-smokers if the horizontal line for the study is to the right or to the left, respectively, of the bold vertical line [ln(RR) = 0] and does not cross it. PD risk for current smokers is similar to that in non-smokers if the horizontal line for the study crosses the bold vertical axis. An asterisk (*) denotes RR estimates that are not included in the meta-analysis due to study overlap with another RR estimate shown in the figure. RR = relative risk, LCL = lower limit of the 95% confidence interval, UCL = upper limit of the 95% confidence interval, HPFS = Health Professionals Follow-Up Study, M & F = males and females, NHS = Nurses’ Health Study, PD = Parkinson’s disease. Citations for studies appearing in this figure can be found here: [3, 18, 20, 6095].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151841.g001

thumbnail
Fig 2. Association between the heavy or long-term cigarette smoking and Parkinson’s disease.

The natural logarithm of the estimated relative risk [ln(RR)] and the 95% confidence interval for each study are displayed (see the legend for Fig 1 for instructions on how to interpret forest plots). An asterisk (*) denotes RR estimates that are not included in the meta-analysis due to study overlap with another RR estimate shown in the figure. RR = relative risk, LCL = lower limit of the 95% confidence interval, UCL = upper limit of the 95% confidence interval, M & F = males and females, PD = Parkinson’s disease. Citations for studies appearing in this figure can be found here: [18, 55, 5962, 6466, 68, 70, 71, 74, 75, 77, 80, 82, 8488, 90, 91, 93, 95111].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151841.g002

The regression test statistic for publication bias was not statistically significant in Tier 1 studies of current smokers vs. never smokers (Table 3). However, there was statistically significant publication bias in the assessment of the association between heavy/long-term smoking and PD. The RR adjusted for publication bias using the trim-and-fill procedure was still statistically significantly negative, irrespective of whether a fixed (RR = 0.70; 95% CI = 0.64–0.76) or random effects model (RR = 0.69; 95% CI = 0.57–0.83) was used (Table 3).

An assessment of PD risk in cigarette smokers based on Bradford Hill’s viewpoints (Table 5) indicates a consistent, approximately two-fold reduction in PD risk. We did not formally assess biological gradient (dose-response) in these studies, although when PD risk was evaluated in heavy or long-term smokers (Fig 2), it was reduced, but not to a greater extent than was observed in current smokers (Table 3). Despite compelling statistical evidence of an inverse relationship between the risk of developing PD and cigarette smoking, the underlying biological basis for this relationship is unknown.

thumbnail
Table 5. Weight-of-the-evidence assessment of causality based upon Bradford Hill viewpoints.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151841.t005

Rural Living, Farming and Well-Water Consumption

Rural living, farming and well-water consumption were assessed together because of potential inter-dependence of findings related to these three aspects of a rural or agricultural lifestyle. Within studies that evaluated at least two of the three exposures, there was a positive, but not statistically significant, correlation between RRs for rural living and well-water consumption (r = 0.27; p = 0.24; 21 studies). There was no correlation between RRs for rural living and farming (r = - 0.02; p = 0.94; 14 studies) or between RRs for well-water consumption and farming (r = - 0.02; p = 0.91; 23 studies).

Forest plots of the RRs from studies that evaluated the association between PD risk and rural living, well-water consumption or farming are provided in Figs 3, 4 and 5, respectively, and the meta-analysis results are summarized in Table 3. Statistically significant associations were observed between PD risk and rural living (RR = 1.17; 95% CI = 1.10–1.24) and farming (RR = 1.08; 95% CI = 1.04–1.11), but not well-water consumption (RR = 1.02; 95% CI = 0.98–1.07), based on the fixed effects model. The overall meta-analysis RR, based on the random effects model, was statistically significant for all three variables: rural living (RR = 1.43; 95% CI = 1.22–1.69), farming (RR = 1.24; 95% CI = 1.12–1.37) and well-water consumption (RR = 1.30; 95% CI = 1.12–1.51). Similar results were obtained for Tier 1 studies, both before and after correction for publication bias.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Association between rural living and Parkinson’s disease.

The natural logarithm of the estimated relative risk [ln(RR)] and the 95% confidence interval for each study are displayed (see the legend for Fig 1 for instructions on how to interpret forest plots). RR = relative risk, LCL = lower limit of the 95% confidence interval, UCL = upper limit of the 95% confidence interval. Citations for studies appearing in this figure can be found here: [54, 56, 58, 59, 64, 76, 98100, 102, 104, 114130].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151841.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. Association between well-water consumption and Parkinson’s disease.

The natural logarithm of the estimated relative risk [ln(RR)] and the 95% confidence interval for each study are displayed (see the legend for Fig 1 for instructions on how to interpret forest plots). An asterisk (*) denotes RR estimates that are not included in the meta-analysis due to study overlap with another RR estimate shown in the figure. RR = relative risk, LCL = lower limit of the 95% confidence interval, UCL = upper limit of the 95% confidence interval, PD = Parkinson’s disease. Citations for studies appearing in this figure can be found here: [55, 58, 59, 63, 69, 79, 81, 83, 84, 99, 100, 102, 104, 114, 115, 117137].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151841.g004

thumbnail
Fig 5. Association between farming and Parkinson’s disease.

The natural logarithm of the estimated relative risk [ln(RR)] and the 95% confidence interval for each study are displayed (see the legend for Fig 1 for instructions on how to interpret forest plots). An asterisk (*) denotes RR estimates that are not included in the meta-analysis due to study overlap with another RR estimate shown in the figure. RR = relative risk, LCL = lower limit of the 95% confidence interval, UCL = upper limit of the 95% confidence interval, M & F = males and females. Citations for studies appearing in this figure can be found here: [19, 20, 55, 5759, 63, 73, 7981, 84, 87, 92, 104, 107, 114116, 118, 121, 122, 124, 127132, 134, 135, 137156].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151841.g005

Meta-analysis RRs calculated based on Tier 2 studies (Tables C-E in S4 File) were lower than those based on Tier 1 studies (Table 3), largely due to the fact that among the Tier 2 studies for each type of exposure, there was at least one study with a near null result that was weighted heavily, based on a narrow 95% CI. Thus, for rural living, the RR reported in a Tier 2 study by Taylor et al. [102] (RR = 1.07; 95% CI = 0.99–1.15) received a weight of 63.1 percent in the fixed effects model versus 6.7 percent in the random effects model (Table C in S4 File). Similarly, for well-water consumption, the RR reported by Taylor et al. [102] (RR = 0.93; 95% CI = 0.88–0.98) drove the fixed effects meta-analysis value for Tier 1 and Tier 2 studies combined (RR = 1.02; 95% CI = 0.98–1.07), because this RR was assigned a weight of 68.8 percent (Table D in S5 File). In contrast, a 4.7 percent weight was given to this RR in the random effects model. Likewise, the RR for farming from the Tier 2 study by Lee et al. [148] (RR = 0.86; 95% CI = 0.81–0.92) had a large impact (27.9%) on the fixed effects RR and less of an effect (4.8%) on the random effects RR for Tier 1 and Tier 2 studies combined (Table E in S5 File).

The overall estimate of the association between rural living and PD (Fig B in S2 File) was insensitive to stratification by study characteristics, with fixed effects model estimates having generally lower RRs and narrower 95% CIs. In the case of well-water consumption (Fig C in S2 File), fixed effects estimates of the RR tended to be centered around the null, whereas random effects estimates were generally greater than 1.0, but 95% CIs were wide. RR estimates of the association between farming and PD were in general statistically significantly greater than 1.0, independent of the statistical model or after stratification by study characteristics (Fig D in S2 File).

The weight-of-the-evidence assessment of Tier 1 studies on the association between PD and rural living, farming or well-water consumption, according to Bradford Hill’s viewpoints, indicates that there are inadequate data to reach a conclusion of causality (Table 5). The associations reported were small and the biological gradient and the temporality of disease onset have not been adequately investigated. The results are inconsistent for rural living (Fig 3). Rural living, well-water consumption and farming lack specificity.

Pesticide Use

The RRs for the association between use of any pesticide and PD were not statistically significantly correlated with RRs for farming (r = 0.07; p = 0.72; 33 studies) or herbicide use (r = 0.11; p = 0.74; 12 studies). Positive, but not statistically significant, correlations with pesticide use were observed for rural living (r = 0.39; p = 0.12; 17 studies), well-water use (r = 0.34; p = 0.12; 22 studies) and fungicide use (r = 0.66; p = 0.10; 7 studies). There were statistically significant positive correlations between the RRs for use of any pesticide and use of insecticides (r = 0.82; p = 0.001; 12 studies) or paraquat (r = 0.84; p = 0.005; 9 studies).

Forty-nine of the 56 RRs of the association between the use of any pesticide and PD were greater than 1.0, with 24 RRs being statistically significant. Only seven studies had RRs less than 1.0 and none was statistically significant (Fig 6). There were 11 Tier 1 studies and 38 Tier 2 studies among the 49 studies that reported independent estimates of the RR for pesticide use. The association between pesticide use and PD was statistically significant using the fixed effects model for Tier 1 studies (RR = 1.32; 95% CI = 1.16–1.52) and for all studies combined (RR = 1.22; 95% Cl = 1.18–1.27) (Table 3). Using the random effects model, the RRs were also statistically significant, and slightly greater than the fixed effects RRs for Tier 1 studies (RR = 1.40; 95% Cl = 1.06–1.85) and all studies (RR = 1.56; 95% Cl = 1.37–1.77). When corrected for publication bias, the association between pesticide use and PD was statistically significant for Tier 1 studies using the fixed effects model (RR = 1.14; 95% Cl = 1.01–1.29), but it was not statistically significant when the random effects model was used (RR = 1.11; 95% Cl = 0.82–1.50). The meta-analysis estimates were insensitive to stratification by study characteristics, but the random effects model tended to produce larger RRs with wider 95% CIs than the fixed effects model (Fig E in S2 File).

thumbnail
Fig 6. Association between pesticide use and Parkinson’s disease.

The natural logarithm of the estimated relative risk [ln(RR)] and the 95% confidence interval for each study are displayed (see the legend for Fig 1 for instructions on how to interpret forest plots). An asterisk (*) denotes RR estimates that are not included in the meta-analysis due to study overlap with another RR estimate shown in the figure. RR = relative risk, LCL = lower limit of the 95% confidence interval, UCL = upper limit of the 95% confidence interval. Citations for studies appearing in this figure can be found here: [18, 19, 5459, 6870, 73, 7981, 84, 87, 89, 91, 92, 94, 100, 102, 107, 114116, 118, 120, 123, 125130, 132, 134137, 139141, 143, 149151, 154158].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151841.g006

The weight-of-the-evidence assessment of Tier 1 studies indicated there was a consistent positive association between pesticide use and PD (Table 5). The results lacked specificity or biological plausibility. Neither the biological gradient nor the latency period until PD diagnosis following pesticide use was adequately assessed.

Herbicide, Fungicide and Insecticide Use

Herbicide, fungicide and insecticide data are presented together in this section, because for most crops, it is likely that a grower would have applied more than one of these classes of pesticide sometime during the growing season, thereby leading to a correlation between exposures and, potentially, their corresponding RRs. Statistically significant positive correlations were observed between RRs for herbicide and insecticide use (r = 0.66; p = 0.008; 15 studies) and those for fungicide and insecticide use (r = 0.90; p = 0.001; 9 studies). A positive correlation between the RRs for herbicide and fungicide use (r = 0.58; p = 0.08; 10 studies) and a negative correlation between the RRs for paraquat and herbicide use (r = - 0.61; p = 0.14; 7 studies) were observed, but these correlations were not statistically significant.

Use of herbicides (RR = 1.20; 95% CI = 1.06–1.36) or insecticides (RR = 1.32; 95% CI = 1.14–1.52) was associated with statistically significantly increased PD risk, based on all Tier 1 and Tier 2 studies using the fixed effects model (Table 3, Fig 7). Similar results were obtained using the random effects model. An evaluation of Tier 1 studies using the fixed effects model also yielded a statistically significant positive association for herbicides (RR = 1.30; 95% CI = 1.01–1.68), but not for insecticides (RR = 1.04; 95% CI = 0.83–1.31). After correction for publication bias, the RRs between use of herbicides, fungicides or insecticides and PD in Tier 1 studies did not change appreciably, but none of them were statistically significant, based on either random or fixed effects models (Table 3).

thumbnail
Fig 7. Association between herbicide (Panel a), fungicide (Panel b) or insecticide use (Panel c) and Parkinson’s disease.

The natural logarithm of the estimated relative risk [ln(RR)] and the 95% confidence interval for each study are displayed (see the legend for Fig 1 for instructions on how to interpret forest plots). RR = relative risk, LCL = lower limit of the 95% confidence interval, UCL = upper limit of the 95% confidence interval. Citations for studies appearing in this figure can be found here: [55, 58, 80, 81, 92, 102, 104, 114, 115, 119, 121, 122, 132, 135, 137, 143, 155158].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151841.g007

The results for both herbicides and insecticides were insensitive to stratification by study characteristics. The stratum-specific meta-analysis RRs based on the fixed effects model were similar to those based on the random effects model for herbicides (Fig F in S2 File) and insecticides (Fig H in S2 File), although 95% CIs tended to be wider for insecticides when calculated using the random effects model.

High herbicide use was statistically significantly positively associated with PD risk among Tier 1 and Tier 2 studies combined (Table 3, Fig 8 Panel a), but not in the single Tier 1 study (RR = 2.8; 95% CI = 0.6–12.8) conducted by Vlajinac et al. [115], which lacked precision. In contrast, Vlajinac et al. [115] reported a statistically significant positive association between high use of insecticides and PD (RR = 4.5; 95% CI = 1.5–13.9) in the only Tier 1 study of this association, but again, precision in this study was low. High use of insecticides, based upon Tier 1 and Tier 2 studies combined, was statistically significantly associated with PD in both fixed effects and random effects models (Table 3, Fig 8 Panel c).

thumbnail
Fig 8. Association between high herbicide (Panel a), high fungicide (Panel b) or high insecticide use (Panel c) and Parkinson’s disease.

The natural logarithm of the estimated relative risk [ln(RR)] and the 95% confidence interval for each study are displayed (see the legend for Fig 1 for instructions on how to interpret forest plots). RR = relative risk, LCL = lower limit of the 95% confidence interval, UCL = upper limit of the 95% confidence interval. Citations for studies appearing in this figure can be found here: [55, 81, 98, 115, 121, 130, 135, 143, 150].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151841.g008

Use of fungicides (based on Tiers 1 and 2 studies combined or Tier 1 studies alone) and high use of fungicides (based on one Tier 1 study) were not statistically significantly associated with PD risk in any statistical analysis (Table 3, Fig 7 and Fig 8 Panel b). The results were insensitive to use of the fixed effects or random effects model and to stratification by study characteristics (Fig G in S2 File).

The weight-of-the-evidence assessment of Tier 1 studies on the association between herbicide, fungicide or insecticide use and PD, using Bradford Hill’s viewpoints, indicated that there were insufficient high-quality studies to warrant the determination of causation (Table 5). Among the four Tier 1 studies on herbicide use, the overall RRs were comparable in magnitude before and after correction for reporting bias, although the RR was statistically non-significant after correction (uncorrected RR = 1.30; 95% CI = 1.01–1.68; corrected RR = 1.26; 95% CI = 0.99–1.60) (Table 3). Based on the three Tier 1 studies on fungicide use, the RRs were not statistically significant. For insecticides, only one of the four Tier 1 studies [115] had a statistically significantly elevated risk (RR = 3.2; 95% CI = 1.3–7.9) (Fig 7 Panel c).

For all three exposures, the meta-analysis RRs for Tier 1 studies were heavily influenced by the study by Brighina et al. [158], which was assigned a study weight of greater than 75 percent in five of the six fixed and random effects models (Tables G-I in S4 File), and 39 percent in the random effects model for insecticides (Table I in S4 File). Biological gradients and temporality were inadequately assessed for these exposures. The results lack specificity and plausibility [159].

Paraquat Use

The one independent Tier 1 study by Firestone et al. [57] found no association between paraquat use and PD (RR = 0.9; 95% CI = 0.14–5.43) (Fig 9 Panel a). Three of the 12 (25%) independent Tier 2 studies [59, 160, 161] reported a statistically significantly elevated risk of PD in individuals who used paraquat. There was a statistically significantly positive meta-analysis RR between PD and paraquat use based on the fixed effects model for all studies (RR = 1.69; 95% CI = 1.44–1.98) (Table 3). Similar results were obtained using the random effects model (RR = 1.47; 95% CI = 1.01–2.13). Results were comparable when overlapping estimates were included from four publications from a case-control study based in California [68, 69, 161, 162], two publications from a nested case-control study in the Agricultural Health Study [160, 163], two publications from a case-control study in Washington [57, 114], and two publications from a case-control study of agriculture workers in France [55, 164], as well as estimates for incident and prevalent PD from the Agricultural Health Study [18] (20 estimates; RRs shown in Fig 9 Panel a).

thumbnail
Fig 9. Association between ever use (Panel a) or high use (Panel b) of paraquat and Parkinson’s disease.

The natural logarithm of the estimated relative risk [ln(RR)] and the 95% confidence interval for each study are displayed (see the legend for Fig 1 for instructions on how to interpret forest plots). An asterisk (*) denotes RR estimates that are not included in the meta-analysis due to study overlap with another RR estimate shown in the figure. RR = relative risk, LCL = lower limit of the 95% confidence interval, UCL = upper limit of the 95% confidence interval. Citations for studies appearing in this figure can be found here: [18, 55, 57, 59, 68, 69, 81, 92, 114, 132, 135, 154, 156, 160165].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151841.g009

Among the four studies (all Tier 2) that evaluated the association between high use of paraquat and PD risk (Fig 9 Panel b), two of the studies [59, 163] reported statistically significant positive RRs. Overall, the fixed effects meta-analysis RR was statistically significant (RR = 1.75; 95% CI = 1.19–2.57) (Table 3); the random effects RR was not statistically significant (RR = 1.99; 95% CI = 0.84–4.71), although it was comparable in magnitude to the fixed effects RR.

Sensitivity analyses indicated that the meta-analysis RRs stratified by study characteristics were similar to RRs calculated for all Tier 2 studies (Fig I in S2 File). However, the meta-analysis RRs were not statistically significant after stratification by source population (population-based) or control type (cohort), whereas RRs calculated using the fixed effects model were statistically significant regardless of interview type (in-person or other), method of paraquat use ascertainment (self-reported or other) and confounder adjustment (age, gender and cigarette smoking or fewer covariates).

An assessment of the weight of the evidence according to Bradford Hill’s viewpoints (Table 5) indicates there is an inadequate basis to draw an inference of causality between PD and paraquat use based upon one Tier 1 study [57], which was null but imprecise. Biological gradients and temporality were not assessed. Although the hypothesis that paraquat might cause PD is highly specific, there is disagreement concerning biological plausibility [166].

Discussion

In this study we found statistically significant inverse associations between cigarette smoking and PD. Meta-analysis RRs, after adjustment for publication bias, ranged from 0.54 to 0.55 for current vs. never smokers. The consistency of the association between smoking cigarettes and PD was maintained when the studies were stratified by different study characteristics, including the Tier 1/Tier 2 categories that we defined and factors used by others to assess study quality (i.e., exposure assessment methods, source population, type of controls and degree of confounder adjustment [49, 50]). Our meta-analysis results for cigarette smoking are comparable to those reported by others (Table 6; [37, 39, 40]).

thumbnail
Table 6. Comparison of meta-analysis results from this study to results in the published literature1: meta-relative risk (RR; random effects model), 95 percent confidence interval (CI) and number of studies (N).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151841.t006

The strength and consistency of the weight of evidence suggest that there is an inverse causal relationship between cigarette smoking and PD. Hernán et al. [61] and Liu et al. [74] each evaluated the combined population in two large prospective cohorts, and found that among current cigarette smokers, the strength of the inverse association between cigarette smoking and PD significantly increased with the number of cigarettes smoked per day. Among past smokers, Hernán et al. [61] reported that there was a significant decrease in the strength of the association with a greater amount of time since having quit smoking. In spite of this strong and consistent evidence, the association between cigarette smoking and PD risk lacks specificity given that cigarette smoke comprises many structurally diverse chemicals. Mechanisms underlying the potential neuroprotective effect of cigarette smoke have been postulated, including the activation of nicotinic receptors [169] or cigarette smoke-induced decreased rate of formation of potentially neurotoxic metabolites of endogenous agents [170]. To date, no constituent of cigarette smoke or any other agent has been identified as being neuroprotective against PD [112].

Recently, it has been postulated that individuals who will develop PD find it easier to quit cigarette smoking than individuals who will not develop PD [171]. According to this hypothesis, the consistent inverse association between cigarette smoking and PD is due to reverse causation, whereby prodromal disease leads to an effect on smoking behavior. Under this hypothesis, cigarette smoking per se is not neuroprotective, but rather cigarette smoking occurs less frequently in PD patients, perhaps through early impairment in the quality of olfaction [2] or dysfunction of dopaminergic reward circuitry [172, 173]. To date, no specific genetic or psychological factor has been identified that might prevent individuals who are prone to develop PD from engaging in cigarette smoking and no factor has been identified that might make it easier for undiagnosed PD patients to quit cigarette smoking.

In our study, random effects meta-analysis results for Tier 1 and Tier 2 studies combined were statistically significant for all agricultural lifestyle factors and types of pesticide used (Table 6). Excluding fungicides, based on Tier 1 studies alone, RRs were all greater than unity but were not statistically significant, except for rural living. RRs from individual studies were considerably less consistent than observed in studies on cigarette smoking. Correction for publication bias and stratification by study characteristics had little impact, although RRs were attenuated in Tier 2 studies compared with Tier 1 RRs. Our combined-studies results were comparable to those reported by other investigators (Table 6). The exception was the study by Van Maele-Fabry et al. [29], who evaluated only cohort studies (N = 2 to 8). In their study, RRs for farming and the use of pesticides, herbicides, fungicides or paraquat were not statistically significant.

Studies of farming occupation as a risk factor rarely distinguish among the diverse tasks and the variety of chemical and pathogen exposures that may occur in different types of farming activities. For example, farming may result in many other exposures, such as an increased likelihood of head injury [174], vibrational stress [175], infection [176] and soil-borne pathogens [54, 177179], that could be causally related to PD. Individuals engaged in these occupations may also be exposed to a large number of chemicals including herbicides, fungicides, insecticides, rodenticides, fumigants, fertilizers and fuels. Participants in such occupations may also exhibit unique lifestyle factors such as dietary preference, tobacco use or sunlight exposure that could alter their susceptibility to PD [33]. Similarly, rural living and well-water consumption lack specificity and correspond to a wide range of exposures, some of which may plausibly be causally linked to PD.

Positive, statistically significant associations (uncorrected RRs = 1.2 to 1.6) were observed between pesticide use and PD. Correction for publication bias reduced the strength of the association (corrected RRs = 1.1) and rendered the result non-significant based upon the random effects model but not the fixed effects model. Similar results were obtained for use of herbicides and insecticides, whereas use of fungicides was not statistically significantly associated with PD in any model. Stratification by study quality or other characteristics had little impact on the magnitude of the RRs, although the stratified RRs were often based on few studies and, therefore, the RRs were often statistically non-significant. The analysis of biological gradient (high use of herbicides, fungicides or insecticides vs. none) was limited by the number of available studies.

Pesticides are a broad group of chemicals that are structurally and functionally diverse and may not share a common mechanism of action, toxicity [159] or common use. The idea that “pesticides,” or even all herbicides, fungicides or insecticides as a class, could cause PD is inconsistent with the understanding that the mechanism of toxicity is highly specific to the chemical structure and the biochemical pathway(s) perturbed by a causative agent [180]. Although a specific pesticide or its metabolites within any of these categories could be causally related to PD, no such compound has yet been identified. Statistically significant correlations observed between RRs derived from the same studies (e.g., pesticide use and insecticide use [r = 0.82]; pesticide use and paraquat use [r = 0.84]; herbicide use and insecticide use [r = 0.66]; and insecticide use and fungicide use [r = 0.90]) illustrate the lack of specificity among these factors.

It is also plausible that a factor correlated with pesticide use, such as exposure to a highly active specific pesticide or some other factor associated with farming, could be causally related to PD. However, the available data do not permit the identification of such a factor. Biologically plausible mechanisms for PD causation have been postulated for specific pesticides, such as the inhibition of mitochondrial complex I by rotenone [44], the inhibition of aldehyde dehydrogenase by the dithiocarbamate fungicides ferbam, macozeb and maneb [42, 43] and the induction of oxidative stress by paraquat [181, 182]. There is disagreement on the utility of animal models for predicting risk in humans [183185], who may have had limited or intermittent exposure to these pesticides. Furthermore, results obtained using some models appear to be less reliable [186, 187] than originally reported [181, 182], and there is a paucity of epidemiological data for most specific pesticides.

When we focused on the 20 published RR estimates (from 13 independent study populations) on paraquat and PD, we found statistically significant positive associations overall, but not in some strata of study characteristics. For example, the one Tier 1 study by Firestone et al. [57] did not find an association between paraquat and PD. Studies that assessed the association between paraquat exposure and a second potential risk factor for PD, such as co-exposure to the fungicide maneb [68], the occurrence of head injury [161] or the presence of genetic susceptibility factors [70, 163], were not included because of limited data. Overall, the epidemiological data are inconsistent across studies, and collectively, they do not support a conclusion that a causal relationship exists between exposure to paraquat and PD.

Statistical associations between PD and exposure to pesticides or factors related to rural or agricultural living do not necessarily indicate cause-and-effect relationships. Consideration must be given to alternative explanations such as random error (chance), systematic error (bias) and confounding, as well as to the issues of validity, precision and reliability. Sir Austin Bradford Hill developed “viewpoints” to evaluate whether an association between an exposure and an outcome was likely to be causal [188]. He asked, “What aspects of that association should we especially consider before deciding that the most likely interpretation of its causation?” ([188], p.295). Other approaches for evaluating causality have been proposed [189192], but the Bradford Hill viewpoints are often used. Based upon our weight-of-the-evidence assessment using Bradford Hill’s viewpoints, we conclude that none of the risk factors that we assessed provide sufficient evidence of a causal relationship with PD. This conclusion is consistent with those of other investigators who conducted systematic reviews and/or meta-analyses (Table 7).

thumbnail
Table 7. Conclusions from published systematic reviews and meta-analyses of epidemiology studies on Parkinson’s disease.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151841.t007

A comparison of the results from our meta-analysis with those in the published literature (Table 6) indicates that there is general agreement among studies. Overall, cigarette smoking is consistently inversely associated with PD [37, 39, 40], whereas positive associations have been reported PD between and rural living, well-water consumption, farming or pesticide use [2830, 36, 37]. As expected, the RRs were more likely to be statistically significant when the meta-analysis included more studies (e.g., Noyce et al. [37], Pezzoli and Cereda [30], and Breckenridge et al., 2016 [all studies]) than when fewer studies were included (e.g., Van Maele-Fabry et al. [29] [cohort studies] and Breckenridge et al., 2016 [Tier 1 studies]).

There are limitations to meta-analysis, especially as applied to observational epidemiology [193]. Such limitations are exemplified by the fact that the heterogeneity of results in Tier 1 and Tier 2 studies, as measured by I2 in our analyses, was statistically significant for all risk factors except for fungicides. Between-study variances (τ2) calculated based on the random effects model and a review of the distribution of RRs permitted the identification of individual studies that contributed the most to such heterogeneity. However, detailed evaluation of the study protocols and individual study records, which usually are not available in publications or directly from investigators, would be necessary before one could decide if there was a scientific basis for either excluding or assigning extraordinary weight to a study that deviates widely from the majority of studies. This unresolvable problem raises questions about the scientific merit of calculating a single summary RR for a set of observational epidemiologic studies.

Statistically significant heterogeneity was observed in Tier 1 studies alone and in Tiers 1 and 2 studies combined for several risk factors (i.e., current smoking, heavy or long-term smoking, rural living, well-water consumption, any pesticide use and insecticide use). Thus, for these risk factors, study heterogeneity did not depend on study categorization. In contrast, we found that while heterogeneity was statistically significant for combined Tiers 1 and 2 studies of farming and herbicide use, I2 was not statistically significant for Tier 1 studies alone, suggesting that differences in heterogeneity of results between studies was dependent on the study category. The observation that τ2 was usually smaller for Tier 1 studies than for Tier 1 and Tier 2 studies combined suggests, from a statistical perspective, that Tier 1 studies should not be combined with Tier 2 studies for these risk factors.

In our sensitivity analysis we compared the effect on the meta-analysis RRs of using criteria that other investigators have used to classify studies (i.e., exposure assessment method, source population, control type and confounder adjustment) with the criteria that we selected to define study tiers (i.e., enrollment of incident vs. prevalent PD cases, individual vs. ecological exposure assessment, and the basis of PD diagnosis). We found that these different study characteristics did not substantially alter the results of the overall meta-analysis.

It is known that the selection of studies and results for publication, and hence their use in meta-analysis, can be biased [31]. In this evaluation, there was evidence of reporting bias for heavy/long-term cigarette smoking. Furthermore, the practice of combining results from several studies into one estimate of risk may be inappropriate under the component cause model, where there are potentially multiple causal factors [194]. Using meta-analysis for causal inference based up observational studies generally is not warranted because it tends to give a false impression of the consistency of studies [195]. As Greenland pointed out, “no statistical technique can compensate for fundamental limitations of the input data” [196]. Greenland suggested that meta-analysis can be used appropriately as a basis for comparing results from a variety of studies, and not as a method to produce a single RR estimate with narrower CIs. There may be a greater benefit from examining sources of heterogeneity among study results than by merely computing a summary estimate [31]. In addition, it is important to examine studies to ensure that within-study selection bias is minimized, particularly when the results from a post-hoc subgroup analysis is the main focus of the study [197, 198].

Meta-analysis artificially increases precision by combining RR estimates from epidemiological studies [2730, 36, 37, 39, 40, 111, 199]. In spite of the increased statistical power arising from the combination of studies in meta-analysis, the results reported in this paper and by other researchers are consistent with each other only with respect to cigarette smoking, rural living, well-water consumption and pesticide use when we analyzed Tier 1 studies. Contradictory evidence for rural living has been reported in a large study of US Medicare beneficiaries aged 65 years and older. In this study, the incidence and prevalence of PD were statistically significantly greater in urban (population greater than 1 million) than in rural (population less than 2500) communities [200]. However, in general, our meta-analysis results were comparable to those published by other investigators, with mostly minor differences in meta-analysis RRs that were probably due to slight differences in the selection of studies and RR estimates.

Increasing statistical power by combining studies cannot overcome problems in the design and conduct of the individual studies. Failure to adjust for study deficiencies may be responsible, at least in part, for many false positive findings in the published epidemiological literature [201205]. The need for higher standards and a more critical appraisal of individual studies was recognized in a series of recommendations for Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE), which were developed to address transparency in reporting the results of epidemiological studies [206].

To establish a causal role of any factor in the etiology of PD, particularly those related to rural living and farming, substantial improvements are needed in the design and conduct of observational epidemiologic studies. Future research must 1) better characterize specific past exposure to suspected agents to permit a more accurate assessment of the dose-response relationship; 2) utilize neurologists or movement disorder specialists to diagnose PD and to confirm the absence of diseases in controls in order to minimize disease misclassification; 3) determine with greater accuracy the date of onset of PD so that the latency between exposure and PD onset and progression can be assessed; and 4) enroll incident PD cases close to the time of diagnosis (if not onset) to increase the likelihood that reported exposures preceded disease development. Without improvements in methodology, further research will encounter similar shortcomings, and the identification of causal environmental risk factors for idiopathic PD will not be achieved.

Supporting Information

S2 Appendix. PRISMA Flow Chart: Study Identification, Screening, Eligibility, Inclusion and Exclusion.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151841.s002

(PDF)

S1 File.

Figs A to J: Frequency distribution of the relative risk estimates (RRs) from individual epidemiological studies assessing the association between risk factors and Parkinson’s disease. Fig A: Frequency distribution of individual study relative risk estimates: Current cigarette smoking. Fig B: Frequency distribution of individual study relative risk estimates: Heavy or long-term smoking. Fig C: Frequency distribution of individual study relative risk estimates: Rural living. Fig D: Frequency distribution of individual study relative risk estimates: Well-water consumption. Fig E: Frequency distribution of individual study relative risk estimates: Farming. Fig F: Frequency distribution of individual study relative risk estimates: Pesticide use. Fig G: Frequency distribution of individual study relative risk estimates: Herbicide use. Fig H: Frequency distribution of individual study relative risk estimates: Fungicide use. Fig I: Frequency distribution of individual study relative risk estimates: Insecticide use. Fig J: Frequency distribution of individual study relative risk estimates: Paraquat use.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151841.s003

(PDF)

S2 File.

Figs A to I: Sensitivity analyses of estimated relative risks (RRs) stratified by study characteristics (study quality tier, exposure interview technique, source population, type of controls and extent of confounder adjustment) in fixed and random effects models. Fig A: Sensitivity Analyses–Heavy Smoking. Fig B: Sensitivity Analyses–Rural Living. Fig C: Sensitivity Analyses–Well-Water Consumption. Fig D: Sensitivity Analyses–Farming. Fig E: Sensitivity Analyses–Pesticide Use. Fig F: Sensitivity Analyses–Herbicide Use. Fig G: Sensitivity Analyses–Fungicide Use. Fig H: Sensitivity Analyses–Insecticide Use. Fig I: Sensitivity Analyses–Paraquat Use.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151841.s004

(PDF)

S3 File.

Tables A to I: Metadata characteristics of Tier 1 and Tier 2 studies on the association between risk factors and Parkinson’s disease. Table A: Metadata for Tier 1 and Tier 2 studies: Current cigarette smoking. Table B: Metadata for Tier 1 and Tier 2 studies: Heavy or long-term cigarette smoking. Table C: Metadata for Tier 1 and Tier 2 studies: Rural living. Table D: Metadata for Tier 1 and Tier 2 studies: Well-water consumption. Table E: Metadata for Tier 1 and Tier 2 studies: Farming. Table F: Metadata for Tier 1 and Tier 2 studies: Pesticide use. Table G: Metadata for Tier 1 and Tier 2 studies: Herbicide, fungicide or insecticide use. Table H: Metadata for Tier 1 and Tier 2 studies: High herbicide, fungicide or insecticide use. Table I: Metadata for Tier 1 and Tier 2 studies: Paraquat ever use or high use.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151841.s005

(PDF)

S4 File.

Tables A to N: Normalized study weights based upon fixed and random effects models and RRs and 95% CIs for individual Tier 1 or Tier 2 studies. Table A: RRs, 95% CIs and fixed or random effects study weights for Tier 1 or Tier 2 studies: Current cigarette smoking. Table B: RRs, 95% CIs and fixed or random effects study weights for Tier 1 or Tier 2 studies: Heavy or long-term cigarette smoking. Table C: RRs, 95% CIs and fixed or random effects study weights for Tier 1 or Tier 2 studies: Rural living. Table D: RRs, 95% CIs and fixed or random effects study weights for Tier 1 or Tier 2 studies: Well-water consumption. Table E: RRs, 95% CIs and fixed or random effects study weights for Tier 1 or Tier 2 studies: Farming. Table F: RRs, 95% CIs and fixed or random effects study weights for Tier 1 or Tier 2 studies: Pesticide use. Table G: RRs, 95% CIs and fixed or random effects study weights for Tier 1 or Tier 2 studies: Herbicide use. Table H: RRs, 95% CIs and fixed or random effects study weights for Tier 1 or Tier 2 studies: Fungicide use. Table I: RRs, 95% CIs and fixed or random effects study weights for Tier 1 or Tier 2 studies: Insecticide use. Table J: RRs, 95% CIs and fixed or random effects study weights for Tier 1 or Tier 2 studies: High herbicide use. Table K: RRs, 95% Cls and fixed or random effects study weights for Tier 1 or Tier 2 studies: High fungicide use. Table L: RRs, 95% CIs and fixed or random effects study weights for Tier 1 or Tier 2 studies: High insecticide use. Table M: RRs, 95% CIs and fixed or random effects study weights for Tier 1 or Tier 2 studies: Paraquat use. Table N: RRs, 95% CIs and fixed or random effects study weights for Tier 1 or Tier 2 studies: High paraquat use.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151841.s006

(PDF)

S5 File.

Tables A to J: Normalized study weights for Tier 1 and Tier 2 studies combined (i.e., all studies) based upon fixed and random effects models and RRs and 95% CLs for all individual studies. Table A: RRs, 95% CIs and fixed or random effects study weights for all studies: Current cigarette smoking. Table B: RRs, 95% CIs and fixed or random effects study weights for all studies: Heavy or long-term cigarette smoking. Table C: RRs, 95% CIs and fixed or random effects study weights for all studies: Rural living. Table D: RRs, 95% CIs and fixed or random effects study weights for all studies: Well-water consumption. Table E: RRs, 95% CIs and fixed or random effects study weights for all studies: Farming. Table F: RRs, 95% CIs and fixed or random effects study weights for all studies: Pesticide use. Table G: RRs, 95% CIs and fixed or random effects study weights for all studies: Herbicide use. Table H: RRs, 95% CIs and fixed or random effects study weights for all studies: Fungicide use. Table I: RRs, 95% CIs and fixed or random effects study weights for all studies: Insecticide use. Table J: RRs, 95% CIs and fixed or random effects study weights for all studies: Paraquat use.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151841.s007

(PDF)

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: CBB CB RLS JSM. Performed the experiments: CBB CB ETC RLS JSM. Analyzed the data: CBB CB ETC RLS JSM. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: CBB CB ETC RLS JSM. Wrote the paper: CBB CB ETC RLS JSM. Literature searches: CBB ETC JSM. Selection of data for study inclusion: ETC JSM. Independent study quality categorization: ETC JSM. Data tabulation: CBB ETC. Data and manuscript quality control: CBB ETC RLS. Agriculture, farming, pesticide use: CBB CB. Parkinson's disease, neuropathology: CBB CB. Interpretation of results: CBB CB ETC RLS JSM. Bradford Hill assessment: CBB CB ETC JSM.

References

  1. 1. Parkinson J. An essay on the shaking palsy. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2002;14(2):223–36. Epub 2002/05/02. pmid:11983801.
  2. 2. Braak H, Ghebremedhin E, Rub U, Bratzke H, Del Tredici K. Stages in the development of Parkinson's disease-related pathology. Cell Tissue Res. 2004;318(1):121–34. Epub 2004/09/01. pmid:15338272.
  3. 3. Ascherio A, Weisskopf MG, O'Reilly EJ, McCullough ML, Calle EE, Rodriguez C, et al. Coffee consumption, gender, and Parkinson's disease mortality in the cancer prevention study II cohort: the modifying effects of estrogen. Am J Epidemiol. 2004;160(10):977–84. Epub 2004/11/04. pmid:15522854.
  4. 4. Klein C. Genetics of Parkinson's disease—An overview. In: Fahn S, Lang AE, Schapira AHV, editors. Movement Disorders 4. Philadelphia: Saunders/Elsevier; 2010. p. 15–39.
  5. 5. Martin I, Dawson VL, Dawson TM. The impact of genetic research on our understanding of Parkinson's disease. Prog Brain Res. 2010;183:21–41. Epub 2010/08/11. pmid:20696313.
  6. 6. Farrer M, Chan P, Chen R, Tan L, Lincoln S, Hernandez D, et al. Lewy bodies and parkinsonism in families with parkin mutations. Ann Neurol. 2001;50(3):293–300. Epub 2001/09/18. pmid:11558785.
  7. 7. Gasser T. Identifying PD-causing genes and genetic susceptibility factors: current approaches and future prospects. Prog Brain Res. 2010;183:3–20. Epub 2010/08/11. pmid:20696312.
  8. 8. Farrer MJ. LRRK2 and Parkinson's disease. In: Fahn S, Lang AE, Schapira AHV, editors. Movement Disorders 4. 4. Philadelphia: Saunders/Elsevier; 2010. p. 102–12.
  9. 9. Uversky V. Intrinsically Disordered Chaperones and Neurodegeneration In: Witt S, editor. Protein Chaperones and Protection from Neurodegenerative Diseases. Wiley series on protein and peptide science. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2011. p. 1–63.
  10. 10. Cookson MR. The biochemistry of Parkinson's disease. Annu Rev Biochem. 2005;74:29–52. Epub 2005/06/15. pmid:15952880.
  11. 11. Hattori N, Hatano T, Machida Y, Sato S, Kubo S- I. PARK 2: Parkin mutations responsible for familial Parkinson's disease. In: Fahn S, Lang AE, Schapira AHV, editors. Movement Disorders 4. Philadelphia: Saunders/Elsevier; 2010. p. 54–65.
  12. 12. Martinez-Vicente M, Wong E. Chaperone-Mediated Autophagy and Parkinson’s Disease. In: Witt S, editor. Protein Chaperones and Protection from Neurodegenerative Diseases. Wiley series on protein and peptide science. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2011. p. 101–38.
  13. 13. Cuervo AM. Chaperone-mediated autophagy: selectivity pays off. Trends Endocrinol Metab. 2010;21(3):142–50. Epub 2009/10/28. pmid:19857975; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2831144.
  14. 14. Polymeropoulos MH, Lavedan C, Leroy E, Ide SE, Dehejia A, Dutra A, et al. Mutation in the α-Synuclein Gene Identified in Families with Parkinson's Disease. Science. 1997;276(5321):2045–7. pmid:9197268
  15. 15. Spillantini MG, Crowther RA, Jakes R, Hasegawa M, Goedert M. α-Synuclein in filamentous inclusions of Lewy bodies from Parkinson’s disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998;95(11):6469–73. pmid:9600990.
  16. 16. Valente EM, Ferraris A. PINK1 (PARK6) and Parkinson's Disease. In: Fahn S, Lang AE, Schapira AHV, editors. Movement Disorders 4. Philadelphia: Saunders/Elsevier; 2010. p. 66–82.
  17. 17. Bonifati V. DJ-1 (PARK7) and Parkinson’s Disease. In: Fahn S, Lang AE, Schapira AHV, editors. Movement Disorders 4. Philadelphia: Saunders/Elsevier; 2010. p. 83–101.
  18. 18. Kamel F, Tanner C, Umbach D, Hoppin J, Alavanja M, Blair A, et al. Pesticide exposure and self-reported Parkinson's disease in the agricultural health study. Am J Epidemiol. 2007;165(4):364–74. Epub 2006/11/23. pmid:17116648.
  19. 19. Chaturvedi S, Ostbye T, Stoessl AJ, Merskey H, Hachinski V. Environmental exposures in elderly Canadians with Parkinson's disease. Can J Neurol Sci. 1995;22(3):232–4. Epub 1995/08/01. pmid:8529177.
  20. 20. Kyrozis A, Ghika A, Stathopoulos P, Vassilopoulos D, Trichopoulos D, Trichopoulou A. Dietary and lifestyle variables in relation to incidence of Parkinson's disease in Greece. Eur J Epidemiol. 2013;28(1):67–77. Epub 2013/02/05. pmid:23377703.
  21. 21. Litvan I, Bhatia KP, Burn DJ, Goetz CG, Lang AE, McKeith I, et al. Movement Disorders Society Scientific Issues Committee report: SIC Task Force appraisal of clinical diagnostic criteria for Parkinsonian disorders. Mov Disord. 2003;18(5):467–86. Epub 2003/05/02. pmid:12722160.
  22. 22. Burn DJ, Sawle GV, Brooks DJ. Differential diagnosis of Parkinson's disease, multiple system atrophy, and Steele-Richardson-Olszewski syndrome: discriminant analysis of striatal 18F-dopa PET data. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1994;57(3):278–84. Epub 1994/03/01. pmid:8158173; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1072814.
  23. 23. Thenganatt MA, Jankovic J. Parkinson disease subtypes. JAMA Neurol. 2014;71(4):499–504. Epub 2014/02/12. pmid:24514863.
  24. 24. Barbeau A, Roy M, Cloutier T, Plasse L, Paris S. Enviromental and Genetic Factors in the Etiology of Parkinson's Disease. Advances in Neurology. 45. New York: Raven Press; 1986. p. 299–306.
  25. 25. Barbeau A, Roy M, Cloutier T, Plasse L, Paris S. Enviromental and Genetic Factors in the Etiology of Parkinson's Disease. Adv Neurol. 1987;45:299–306. pmid:3493629
  26. 26. Li AA, Mink PJ, McIntosh LJ, Teta MJ, Finley B. Evaluation of epidemiologic and animal data associating pesticides with Parkinson's disease. J Occup Environ Med. 2005;47(10):1059–87. Epub 2005/10/12. doi: 00043764-200510000-00013 [pii]. pmid:16217247.
  27. 27. Brown TP, Rumsby PC, Capleton AC, Rushton L, Levy LS. Pesticides and Parkinson's disease—is there a link? Environ Health Perspect. 2006;114(2):156–64. Epub 2006/02/03. pmid:16451848; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC1367825.
  28. 28. van der Mark M, Brouwer M, Kromhout H, Nijssen P, Huss A, Vermeulen R. Is pesticide use related to Parkinson disease? Some clues to heterogeneity in study results. Environ Health Perspect. 2012;120(3):340–7. Epub 2012/03/06. pmid:22389202; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3295350.
  29. 29. Van Maele-Fabry G, Hoet P, Vilain F, Lison D. Occupational exposure to pesticides and Parkinson's disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis of cohort studies. Environ Int. 2012;46:30–43. Epub 2012/06/16. pmid:22698719.
  30. 30. Pezzoli G, Cereda E. Exposure to pesticides or solvents and risk of Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2013;80(22):2035–41. Epub 2013/05/29. pmid:23713084.
  31. 31. Egger M, Smith GD, Phillips AN. Meta-analysis: principles and procedures. BMJ. 1997;315(7121):1533–7. Epub 1998/02/12. pmid:9432252; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2127925.
  32. 32. Rothman KJ, Greenland S, Lash TL. Modern epidemiology. 3rd ed., thoroughly rev. and updated ed. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2008.
  33. 33. Wirdefeldt K, Adami HO, Cole P, Trichopoulos D, Mandel J. Epidemiology and etiology of Parkinson's disease: a review of the evidence. Eur J Epidemiol. 2011;26 Suppl 1:S1–58. Epub 2011/06/03. pmid:21626386.
  34. 34. Freire C, Koifman S. Pesticide exposure and Parkinson's disease: epidemiological evidence of association. Neurotoxicology. 2012;33(5):947–71. Epub 2012/05/26. pmid:22627180.
  35. 35. Moretto A, Colosio C. The role of pesticide exposure in the genesis of Parkinson's disease: epidemiological studies and experimental data. Toxicology. 2013;307:24–34. Epub 2012/12/19. pmid:23246862.
  36. 36. Priyadarshi A, Khuder SA, Schaub EA, Priyadarshi SS. Environmental risk factors and Parkinson's disease: a metaanalysis. Environ Res. 2001;86(2):122–7. Epub 2001/07/05. pmid:11437458.
  37. 37. Noyce AJ, Bestwick JP, Silveira-Moriyama L, Hawkes CH, Giovannoni G, Lees AJ, et al. Meta-analysis of early nonmotor features and risk factors for Parkinson disease. Ann Neurol. 2012;72(6):893–901. Epub 2012/10/17. pmid:23071076; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3556649.
  38. 38. Allen MT, Levy LS. Parkinson's disease and pesticide exposure—a new assessment. Crit Rev Toxicol. 2013;43(6):515–34. Epub 2013/07/13. pmid:23844699.
  39. 39. Hernán MA, Takkouche B, Caamano-Isorna F, Gestal-Otero JJ. A meta-analysis of coffee drinking, cigarette smoking, and the risk of Parkinson's disease. Ann Neurol. 2002;52(3):276–84. Epub 2002/09/03. pmid:12205639.
  40. 40. Allam MF, Campbell MJ, Hofman A, Del Castillo AS, Fernandez-Crehuet Navajas R. Smoking and Parkinson's disease: systematic review of prospective studies. Mov Disord. 2004;19(6):614–21. Epub 2004/06/16. pmid:15197698.
  41. 41. Mandel JS, Adami HO, Cole P. Paraquat and Parkinson's disease: an overview of the epidemiology and a review of two recent studies. Regul Toxicol Pharmacol. 2012;62(2):385–92. Epub 2011/10/26. pmid:22024235.
  42. 42. Fitzmaurice AG, Rhodes SL, Lulla A, Murphy NP, Lam HA, O'Donnell KC, et al. Aldehyde dehydrogenase inhibition as a pathogenic mechanism in Parkinson disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110(2):636–41. Epub 2012/12/26. pmid:23267077; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3545765.
  43. 43. Fitzmaurice AG, Rhodes SL, Cockburn M, Ritz B, Bronstein JM. Aldehyde dehydrogenase variation enhances effect of pesticides associated with Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2014;82(5):419–26. Epub 2014/02/05. pmid:24491970; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3917685.
  44. 44. Betarbet R, Sherer TB, MacKenzie G, Garcia-Osuna M, Panov AV, Greenamyre JT. Chronic systemic pesticide exposure reproduces features of Parkinson's disease. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3(12):1301–6. Epub 2000/12/02. pmid:11100151.
  45. 45. Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. 1997;315(7109):629–34. Epub 1997/10/06. pmid:9310563; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2127453.
  46. 46. Duval S, Tweedie R. Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics. 2000;56(2):455–63. Epub 2000/07/06. pmid:10877304.
  47. 47. Higgins JP, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ. 2003;327(7414):557–60. Epub 2003/09/06. pmid:12958120; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC192859.
  48. 48. Goodman M, Mandel JS, DeSesso JM, Scialli AR. Atrazine and pregnancy outcomes: a systematic review of epidemiologic evidence. Birth Defects Res B Dev Reprod Toxicol. 2014;101(3):215–36. Epub 2014/05/07. pmid:24797711; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4265844.
  49. 49. Wells GA, Shea B, O'Connell D, Peterson J, Welch V, Losos M, et al. The Newcastle-Ottawa scale (NOS) for assessing the quality of nonrandomized studies in meta-analysis [April 13, 2013]. Available from: http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_epidemiology/oxford.asp.
  50. 50. Viswanathan M, Berkman ND. Development of the RTI Item Bank on Risk of Bias and Precision of Observational Studies. Research Triangle Park, NC: RTI International–University of North Carolina Evidence-based Practice Center, 2011 Sep Studies. Report No.
  51. 51. DerSimonian R, Laird N. Meta-analysis in clinical trials. Control Clin Trials. 1986;7(3):177–88. Epub 1986/09/01. pmid:3802833.
  52. 52. Borenstein M, Hedges L, Rothstein H. Meta-Analysis: Fixed effect vs. random effects www.Meta-Analysis.com2007 [cited 2015 May 14,]. Available from: http://www.meta-analysis.com/downloads/Meta-analysis%20fixed%20effect%20vs%20random%20effects.pdf.
  53. 53. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. Higgins PT, Green S, editors. West Sussex, England: Wiley-Blackwell; 2008.
  54. 54. Hubble JP, Cao T, Hassanein RE, Neuberger JS, Koller WC. Risk factors for Parkinson's disease. Neurology. 1993;43(9):1693–7. Epub 1993/09/01. pmid:8414014.
  55. 55. Elbaz A, Clavel J, Rathouz PJ, Moisan F, Galanaud JP, Delemotte B, et al. Professional exposure to pesticides and Parkinson disease. Ann Neurol. 2009;66(4):494–504. Epub 2009/10/23. pmid:19847896.
  56. 56. Baldi I, Lebailly P, Mohammed-Brahim B, Letenneur L, Dartigues JF, Brochard P. Neurodegenerative diseases and exposure to pesticides in the elderly. Am J Epidemiol. 2003;157(5):409–14. Epub 2003/03/05. pmid:12615605.
  57. 57. Firestone JA, Lundin JI, Powers KM, Smith-Weller T, Franklin GM, Swanson PD, et al. Occupational factors and risk of Parkinson's disease: A population-based case-control study. Am J Ind Med. 2010;53(3):217–23. Epub 2009/12/22. pmid:20025075; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3299410.
  58. 58. Das K, Ghosh M, Nag C, Nandy SP, Banerjee M, Datta M, et al. Role of familial, environmental and occupational factors in the development of Parkinson's disease. Neurodegener Dis. 2011;8(5):345–51. Epub 2011/02/25. pmid:21346317.
  59. 59. Liou HH, Tsai MC, Chen CJ, Jeng JS, Chang YC, Chen SY, et al. Environmental risk factors and Parkinson's disease: a case-control study in Taiwan. Neurology. 1997;48(6):1583–8. Epub 1997/06/01. pmid:9191770.
  60. 60. Benedetti MD, Bower JH, Maraganore DM, McDonnell SK, Peterson BJ, Ahlskog JE, et al. Smoking, alcohol, and coffee consumption preceding Parkinson's disease: a case-control study. Neurology. 2000;55(9):1350–8. Epub 2000/11/23. pmid:11087780.
  61. 61. Hernán MA, Zhang SM, Rueda-deCastro AM, Colditz GA, Speizer FE, Ascherio A. Cigarette smoking and the incidence of Parkinson's disease in two prospective studies. Ann Neurol. 2001;50(6):780–6. Epub 2002/01/05. pmid:11761476.
  62. 62. Checkoway H, Powers K, Smith-Weller T, Franklin GM, Longstreth WT Jr., Swanson PD. Parkinson's disease risks associated with cigarette smoking, alcohol consumption, and caffeine intake. Am J Epidemiol. 2002;155(8):732–8. Epub 2002/04/12. pmid:11943691.
  63. 63. Park J, Yoo CI, Sim CS, Kim HK, Kim JW, Jeon BS, et al. Occupations and Parkinson's disease: a multi-center case-control study in South Korea. Neurotoxicology. 2005;26(1):99–105. Epub 2004/11/06. pmid:15527877.
  64. 64. Wirdefeldt K, Gatz M, Pawitan Y, Pedersen NL. Risk and protective factors for Parkinson's disease: a study in Swedish twins. Ann Neurol. 2005;57(1):27–33. Epub 2004/11/03. pmid:15521056.
  65. 65. Thacker EL, O'Reilly EJ, Weisskopf MG, Chen H, Schwarzschild MA, McCullough ML, et al. Temporal relationship between cigarette smoking and risk of Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2007;68(10):764–8. Epub 2007/03/07. pmid:17339584; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2225169.
  66. 66. Tan LC, Koh WP, Yuan JM, Wang R, Au WL, Tan JH, et al. Differential effects of black versus green tea on risk of Parkinson's disease in the Singapore Chinese Health Study. Am J Epidemiol. 2008;167(5):553–60. Epub 2007/12/25. pmid:18156141; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2737529.
  67. 67. Sääksjärvi K, Knekt P, Rissanen H, Laaksonen MA, Reunanen A, Männistö S. Prospective study of coffee consumption and risk of Parkinson's disease. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2008;62(7):908–15. Epub 2007/05/25. pmid:17522612.
  68. 68. Costello S, Cockburn M, Bronstein J, Zhang X, Ritz B. Parkinson's Disease and Residential Exposure to Maneb and Paraquat From Agricultural Applications in the Central Valley of California. Am J Epidemiol. 2009;169:919–26. Epub 2009/03/10. pmid:19270050.
  69. 69. Gatto NM, Cockburn M, Bronstein J, Manthripragada AD, Ritz B. Well-Water Consumption and Parkinson's Disease in Rural California. Environ Health Perspect. 2009;117(12):1912–8. Epub 2010/01/06. pmid:20049211.
  70. 70. Ritz BR, Manthripragada AD, Costello S, Lincoln SJ, Farrer MJ, Cockburn M, et al. Dopamine transporter genetic variants and pesticides in Parkinson's disease. Environ Health Perspect. 2009;117(6):964–9. Epub 2009/07/11. pmid:19590691; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2702414.
  71. 71. Chen H, Huang X, Guo X, Mailman RB, Park Y, Kamel F, et al. Smoking duration, intensity, and risk of Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2010;74(11):878–84. Epub 2010/03/12. pmid:20220126; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2836869.
  72. 72. Shino MY, McGuire V, Van Den Eeden SK, Tanner CM, Popat R, Leimpeter A, et al. Familial aggregation of Parkinson's disease in a multiethnic community-based case-control study. Mov Disord. 2010;25(15):2587–94. Epub 2010/09/16. pmid:20842689; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2978761.
  73. 73. Feldman AL, Johansson AL, Nise G, Gatz M, Pedersen NL, Wirdefeldt K. Occupational exposure in parkinsonian disorders: a 43-year prospective cohort study in men. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2011;17(9):677–82. Epub 2011/07/08. pmid:21733735; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3200471.
  74. 74. Liu R, Guo X, Park Y, Huang X, Sinha R, Freedman ND, et al. Caffeine intake, smoking, and risk of Parkinson disease in men and women. Am J Epidemiol. 2012;175(11):1200–7. Epub 2012/04/17. pmid:22505763; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3370885.
  75. 75. Mayeux R, Tang MX, Marder K, Cote LJ, Stern Y. Smoking and Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 1994;9(2):207–12. Epub 1994/03/01. pmid:8196685.
  76. 76. Martyn CN, Osmond C. Parkinson's disease and the environment in early life. J Neurol Sci. 1995;132(2):201–6. Epub 1995/10/01. pmid:8543949.
  77. 77. Hellenbrand W, Seidler A, Robra BP, Vieregge P, Oertel WH, Joerg J, et al. Smoking and Parkinson's disease: a case-control study in Germany. Int J Epidemiol. 1997;26(2):328–39. Epub 1997/04/01. pmid:9169168.
  78. 78. Tzourio C, Rocca WA, Breteler MM, Baldereschi M, Dartigues JF, Lopez-Pousa S, et al. Smoking and Parkinson's disease. An age-dependent risk effect? The EUROPARKINSON Study Group. Neurology. 1997;49(5):1267–72. Epub 1997/12/31. pmid:9371906.
  79. 79. Chan DK, Woo J, Ho SC, Pang CP, Law LK, Ng PW, et al. Genetic and environmental risk factors for Parkinson's disease in a Chinese population. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1998;65(5):781–4. Epub 1998/11/12. pmid:9810958; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2170330.
  80. 80. Fall PA, Fredrikson M, Axelson O, Granerus AK. Nutritional and occupational factors influencing the risk of Parkinson's disease: a case-control study in southeastern Sweden. Mov Disord. 1999;14(1):28–37. Epub 1999/01/26. pmid:9918341.
  81. 81. Kuopio AM, Marttila RJ, Helenius H, Rinne UK. Environmental risk factors in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 1999;14(6):928–39. Epub 1999/12/10. pmid:10584666.
  82. 82. Paganini-Hill A. Risk factors for parkinson's disease: the leisure world cohort study. Neuroepidemiology. 2001;20(2):118–24. Epub 2001/05/19. doi: 54770. pmid:11359079.
  83. 83. Tsai CH, Lo SK, See LC, Chen HZ, Chen RS, Weng YH, et al. Environmental risk factors of young onset Parkinson's disease: a case-control study. Clin Neurol Neurosurg. 2002;104(4):328–33. Epub 2002/07/26. pmid:12140099.
  84. 84. Dong JQ, Zhang ZX, Zhang KL. Parkinson's disease and smoking: an integral part of PD's etiological study. Biomed Environ Sci. 2003;16(2):173–9. Epub 2003/09/11. pmid:12964791.
  85. 85. Ragonese P, Salemi G, Morgante L, Aridon P, Epifanio A, Buffa D, et al. A case-control study on cigarette, alcohol, and coffee consumption preceding Parkinson's disease. Neuroepidemiology. 2003;22(5):297–304. Epub 2003/08/07. doi: 71193. pmid:12902625.
  86. 86. Scott WK, Zhang F, Stajich JM, Scott BL, Stacy MA, Vance JM. Family-based case-control study of cigarette smoking and Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2005;64(3):442–7. Epub 2005/02/09. pmid:15699372.
  87. 87. Galanaud JP, Elbaz A, Clavel J, Vidal JS, Correze JR, Alperovitch A, et al. Cigarette smoking and Parkinson's disease: a case-control study in a population characterized by a high prevalence of pesticide exposure. Mov Disord. 2005;20(2):181–9. Epub 2004/10/07. pmid:15468111.
  88. 88. Hancock DB, Martin ER, Stajich JM, Jewett R, Stacy MA, Scott BL, et al. Smoking, caffeine, and nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs in families with Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol. 2007;64(4):576–80. Epub 2007/04/11. pmid:17420321.
  89. 89. Fong CS, Wu RM, Shieh JC, Chao YT, Fu YP, Kuao CL, et al. Pesticide exposure on southwestern Taiwanese with MnSOD and NQO1 polymorphisms is associated with increased risk of Parkinson's disease. Clin Chim Acta. 2007;378(1–2):136–41. Epub 2006/12/26. pmid:17188257.
  90. 90. Powers KM, Kay DM, Factor SA, Zabetian CP, Higgins DS, Samii A, et al. Combined effects of smoking, coffee, and NSAIDs on Parkinson's disease risk. Mov Disord. 2008;23(1):88–95. Epub 2007/11/08. pmid:17987647.
  91. 91. Petersen MS, Halling J, Bech S, Wermuth L, Weihe P, Nielsen F, et al. Impact of dietary exposure to food contaminants on the risk of Parkinson's disease. Neurotoxicology. 2008;29(4):584–90. Epub 2008/05/06. pmid:18455239.
  92. 92. Dhillon AS, Tarbutton GL, Levin JL, Plotkin GM, Lowry LK, Nalbone JT, et al. Pesticide/environmental exposures and Parkinson's disease in East Texas. J Agromedicine. 2008;13(1):37–48. Epub 2008/12/02. pmid:19042691.
  93. 93. Tanaka K, Miyake Y, Fukushima W, Sasaki S, Kiyohara C, Tsuboi Y, et al. Active and passive smoking and risk of Parkinson's disease. Acta Neurol Scand. 2010;122(6):377–82. Epub 2010/02/24. pmid:20175761.
  94. 94. Kiyohara C, Miyake Y, Koyanagi M, Fujimoto T, Shirasawa S, Tanaka K, et al. GST polymorphisms, interaction with smoking and pesticide use, and risk for Parkinson's disease in a Japanese population. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2010;16(7):447–52. Epub 2010/05/18. pmid:20472488.
  95. 95. Nicoletti A, Pugliese P, Nicoletti G, Arabia G, Annesi G, De Mari M, et al. The FRAGAMP study: environmental and genetic factors in Parkinson's disease, methods and clinical features. Neurol Sci. 2010;31(1):47–52. Epub 2009/11/20. pmid:19924504.
  96. 96. Grandinetti A, Morens DM, Reed D, MacEachern D. Prospective study of cigarette smoking and the risk of developing idiopathic Parkinson's disease. Am J Epidemiol. 1994;139(12):1129–38. Epub 1994/06/15. pmid:8209872.
  97. 97. Sasco AJ, Paffenbarger RS Jr. Smoking and Parkinson's disease. Epidemiology. 1990;1(6):460–5. Epub 1990/11/01. pmid:2090284.
  98. 98. Butterfield PG, Valanis BG, Spencer PS, Lindeman CA, Nutt JG. Environmental antecedents of young-onset Parkinson's disease. Neurology. 1993;43(6):1150–8. Epub 1993/06/01. pmid:8170560.
  99. 99. Wang WZ, Fang XH, Cheng XM, Jiang DH, Lin ZJ. A case-control study on the environmental risk factors of Parkinson's disease in Tianjin, China. Neuroepidemiology. 1993;12(4):209–18. Epub 1993/01/01. pmid:8272180.
  100. 100. Morano A, Jiménez-Jiménez FJ, Molina JA, Antolin MA. Risk-factors for Parkinson's disease: case-control study in the province of Caceres, Spain. Acta Neurol Scand. 1994;89(3):164–70. Epub 1994/03/01. pmid:8030397.
  101. 101. Gorell JM, Rybicki BA, Johnson CC, Peterson EL. Smoking and Parkinson's disease: a dose-response relationship. Neurology. 1999;52(1):115–9. Epub 1999/01/28. pmid:9921857.
  102. 102. Taylor CA, Saint-Hilaire MH, Cupples LA, Thomas CA, Burchard AE, Feldman RG, et al. Environmental, medical, and family history risk factors for Parkinson's disease: a New England-based case control study. Am J Med Genet. 1999;88(6):742–9. Epub 1999/12/03. pmid:10581500.
  103. 103. Vanacore N, Bonifati V, Fabbrini G, Colosimo C, Marconi R, Nicholl D, et al. Smoking habits in multiple system atrophy and progressive supranuclear palsy. European Study Group on Atypical Parkinsonisms. Neurology. 2000;54(1):114–9. Epub 2000/01/15. pmid:10636135.
  104. 104. Behari M, Srivastava AK, Das RR, Pandey RM. Risk factors of Parkinson's disease in Indian patients. J Neurol Sci. 2001;190(1–2):49–55. Epub 2001/09/28. pmid:11574106.
  105. 105. Tan EK, Tan C, Fook-Chong SM, Lum SY, Chai A, Chung H, et al. Dose-dependent protective effect of coffee, tea, and smoking in Parkinson's disease: a study in ethnic Chinese. J Neurol Sci. 2003;216(1):163–7. Epub 2003/11/11. pmid:14607318.
  106. 106. Pals P, Van Everbroeck B, Grubben B, Viaene MK, Dom R, van der Linden C, et al. Case-control study of environmental risk factors for Parkinson's disease in Belgium. Eur J Epidemiol. 2003;18(12):1133–42. Epub 2004/02/05. pmid:14758870.
  107. 107. Baldereschi M, Di Carlo A, Vanni P, Ghetti A, Carbonin P, Amaducci L, et al. Lifestyle-related risk factors for Parkinson's disease: a population-based study. Acta Neurol Scand. 2003;108(4):239–44. Epub 2003/09/06. pmid:12956856.
  108. 108. Gorell JM, Peterson EL, Rybicki BA, Johnson CC. Multiple risk factors for Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Sci. 2004;217(2):169–74. Epub 2004/01/07. pmid:14706220.
  109. 109. Evans AH, Lawrence AD, Potts J, MacGregor L, Katzenschlager R, Shaw K, et al. Relationship between impulsive sensation seeking traits, smoking, alcohol and caffeine intake, and Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2006;77(3):317–21. Epub 2006/02/18. pmid:16484638; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2077692.
  110. 110. Ma L, Zhang L, Gao XH, Chen W, Wu YP, Wang Y, et al. Dietary factors and smoking as risk factors for PD in a rural population in China: a nested case-control study. Acta Neurol Scand. 2006;113(4):278–81. Epub 2006/03/18. pmid:16542169.
  111. 111. Ritz B, Ascherio A, Checkoway H, Marder KS, Nelson LM, Rocca WA, et al. Pooled analysis of tobacco use and risk of Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol. 2007;64(7):990–7. Epub 2007/07/11. pmid:17620489.
  112. 112. Ravina BM, Fagan SC, Hart RG, Hovinga CA, Murphy DD, Dawson TM, et al. Neuroprotective agents for clinical trials in Parkinson's disease: a systematic assessment. Neurology. 2003;60(8):1234–40. Epub 2003/04/23. pmid:12707423.
  113. 113. Berry C, Botham PA, Breckenridge CB, Smith LL. Re: "Paths from Pesticides to Parkinson's" [online comment] 2013 [updated November 4, 2013; cited 2015 May 14,]. Available from: http://comments.sciencemag.org/content/10.1126/science.1243619.
  114. 114. Firestone JA, Smith-Weller T, Franklin G, Swanson P, Longstreth WT Jr., Checkoway H. Pesticides and risk of Parkinson disease: a population-based case-control study. Arch Neurol. 2005;62(1):91–5. Epub 2005/01/12. pmid:15642854.
  115. 115. Vlajinac HD, Sipetic SB, Maksimovic JM, Marinkovic JM, Dzoljic ED, Ratkov IS, et al. Environmental factors and Parkinson's disease: a case-control study in Belgrade, Serbia. Int J Neurosci. 2010;120(5):361–7. Epub 2010/04/21. pmid:20402575.
  116. 116. Ho SC, Woo J, Lee CM. Epidemiologic study of Parkinson's disease in Hong Kong. Neurology. 1989;39(10):1314–8. Epub 1989/10/01. pmid:2797455.
  117. 117. Tanner CM, Chen B, Wang W, Peng M, Liu Z, Liang X, et al. Environmental factors and Parkinson's disease: a case-control study in China. Neurology. 1989;39(5):660–4. Epub 1989/05/01. pmid:2710356.
  118. 118. Koller W, Vetere-Overfield B, Gray C, Alexander C, Chin T, Dolezal J, et al. Environmental risk factors in Parkinson's disease. Neurology. 1990;40(8):1218–21. Epub 1990/08/01. pmid:2381528.
  119. 119. Stern M, Dulaney E, Gruber SB, Golbe L, Bergen M, Hurtig H, et al. The epidemiology of Parkinson's disease. A case-control study of young-onset and old-onset patients. Arch Neurol. 1991;48(9):903–7. Epub 1991/09/01. pmid:1953412.
  120. 120. Jiménez-Jiménez FJ, Mateo D, Giménez-Roldán S. Exposure to well water and pesticides in Parkinson's disease: a case-control study in the Madrid area. Mov Disord. 1992;7(2):149–52. Epub 1992/01/01. pmid:1584237.
  121. 121. Seidler A, Hellenbrand W, Robra BP, Vieregge P, Nischan P, Joerg J, et al. Possible environmental, occupational, and other etiologic factors for Parkinson's disease: a case-control study in Germany. Neurology. 1996;46(5):1275–84. Epub 1996/05/01. pmid:8628466.
  122. 122. Gorell JM, Johnson CC, Rybicki BA, Peterson EL, Richardson RJ. The risk of Parkinson's disease with exposure to pesticides, farming, well water, and rural living. Neurology. 1998;50(5):1346–50. Epub 1998/05/22. pmid:9595985.
  123. 123. De Palma G, Mozzoni P, Mutti A, Calzetti S, Negrotti A. Case-control study of interactions between genetic and environmental factors in Parkinson's disease. Lancet. 1998;352(9145):1986–7. Epub 1999/01/01. pmid:9872254.
  124. 124. Marder K, Logroscino G, Alfaro B, Mejia H, Halim A, Louis E, et al. Environmental risk factors for Parkinson's disease in an urban multiethnic community. Neurology. 1998;50(1):279–81. Epub 1998/01/27. pmid:9443493.
  125. 125. McCann SJ, LeCouteur DG, Green AC, Brayne C, Johnson AG, Chan D, et al. The epidemiology of Parkinson's disease in an Australian population. Neuroepidemiology. 1998;17(6):310–7. Epub 1998/10/21. pmid:9778597.
  126. 126. Werneck AL, Alvarenga H. Genetics, drugs and environmental factors in Parkinson's disease. A case-control study. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 1999;57(2B):347–55. Epub 1999/08/18. pmid:10450337.
  127. 127. Preux PM, Condet A, Anglade C, Druet-Cabanac M, Debrock C, Macharia W, et al. Parkinson's disease and environmental factors. Matched case-control study in the Limousin region, France. Neuroepidemiology. 2000;19(6):333–7. Epub 2000/11/04. pmid:11060508.
  128. 128. Zorzon M, Capus L, Pellegrino A, Cazzato G, Zivadinov R. Familial and environmental risk factors in Parkinson's disease: a case-control study in north-east Italy. Acta Neurol Scand. 2002;105(2):77–82. Epub 2002/03/21. pmid:11903115.
  129. 129. Wright JM, Keller-Byrne J. Environmental determinants of Parkinson's disease. Arch Environ Occup Health. 2005;60(1):32–8. Epub 2006/09/12. pmid:16961006.
  130. 130. Sanyal J, Chakraborty DP, Sarkar B, Banerjee TK, Mukherjee SC, Ray BC, et al. Environmental and familial risk factors of Parkinsons disease: case-control study. Can J Neurol Sci. 2010;37(5):637–42. Epub 2010/11/10. pmid:21059511.
  131. 131. Park J, Yoo CI, Sim CS, Kim JW, Yi Y, Jung KY, et al. Occupations and Parkinson's disease: a case-control study in South Korea. Ind Health. 2004;42(3):352–8. Epub 2004/08/07. pmid:15295907.
  132. 132. Hertzman C, Wiens M, Snow B, Kelly S, Calne D. A case-control study of Parkinson's disease in a horticultural region of British Columbia. Mov Disord. 1994;9(1):69–75. Epub 1994/01/01. pmid:8139607.
  133. 133. De Michele G, Filla A, Volpe G, De Marco V, Gogliettino A, Ambrosio G, et al. Environmental and genetic risk factors in Parkinson's disease: a case-control study in southern Italy. Mov Disord. 1996;11(1):17–23. Epub 1996/01/01. pmid:8771062.
  134. 134. Smargiassi A, Mutti A, De Rosa A, De Palma G, Negrotti A, Calzetti S. A case-control study of occupational and environmental risk factors for Parkinson's disease in the Emilia-Romagna region of Italy. Neurotoxicology. 1998;19(4–5):709–12. Epub 1998/09/24. pmid:9745932.
  135. 135. Engel LS, Checkoway H, Keifer MC, Seixas NS, Longstreth WT Jr., Scott KC, et al. Parkinsonism and occupational exposure to pesticides. Occup Environ Med. 2001;58(9):582–9. Epub 2001/08/21. pmid:11511745; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1740189.
  136. 136. Dick FD, De Palma G, Ahmadi A, Scott NW, Prescott GJ, Bennett J, et al. Environmental risk factors for Parkinson's disease and parkinsonism: the Geoparkinson study. Occup Environ Med. 2007;64(10):666–72. Epub 2007/03/03. pmid:17332139; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2078401.
  137. 137. Hancock DB, Martin ER, Mayhew GM, Stajich JM, Jewett R, Stacy MA, et al. Pesticide exposure and risk of Parkinson's disease: a family-based case-control study. BMC Neurol. 2008;8:6. Epub 2008/04/01. pmid:18373838; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC2323015.
  138. 138. Frigerio R, Elbaz A, Sanft KR, Peterson BJ, Bower JH, Ahlskog JE, et al. Education and occupations preceding Parkinson disease: a population-based case-control study. Neurology. 2005;65(10):1575–83. Epub 2005/11/23. pmid:16301484.
  139. 139. Ascherio A, Chen H, Weisskopf MG, O'Reilly E, McCullough ML, Calle EE, et al. Pesticide exposure and risk for Parkinson's disease. Ann Neurol. 2006;60(2):197–203. Epub 2006/06/28. pmid:16802290.
  140. 140. Skeie GO, Muller B, Haugarvoll K, Larsen JP, Tysnes OB. Differential effect of environmental risk factors on postural instability gait difficulties and tremor dominant Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2010;25(12):1847–52. Epub 2010/07/30. pmid:20669310.
  141. 141. Hertzman C, Wiens M, Bowering D, Snow B, Calne D. Parkinson's disease: A case-control study of occupational and environmental risk factors. Am J Ind Med. 1990;17(3):349–56. pmid:BIOSIS/90/11444 DOCNO- BIOSIS/90/11444.
  142. 142. Tanner CM, Langston JW. Do environmental toxins cause Parkinson's disease? A critical review. Neurology. 1990;40(10 Suppl 3):suppl 17–30; discussion -1. Epub 1990/10/01. pmid:2215971.
  143. 143. Semchuk KM, Love EJ, Lee RG. Parkinson's disease and exposure to agricultural work and pesticide chemicals. Neurology. 1992;42(7):1328–35. Epub 1992/07/01. pmid:1620342.
  144. 144. Rocca WA, Anderson DW, Meneghini F, Grigoletto F, Morgante L, Reggio A, et al. Occupation, education, and Parkinson's disease: a case-control study in an Italian population. Mov Disord. 1996;11(2):201–6. Epub 1996/03/01. pmid:8684392.
  145. 145. Tsui JK, Calne DB, Wang Y, Schulzer M, Marion SA. Occupational risk factors in Parkinson's disease. Can J Public Health. 1999;90(5):334–7. Epub 1999/11/26. pmid:10570579.
  146. 146. Tuchsen F, Jensen AA. Agricultural work and the risk of Parkinson's disease in Denmark, 1981–1993. Scand J Work Environ Health. 2000;26(4):359–62. Epub 2000/09/20. pmid:10994803.
  147. 147. Kirkey KL, Johnson CC, Rybicki BA, Peterson EL, Kortsha GX, Gorell JM. Occupational categories at risk for Parkinson's disease. Am J Ind Med. 2001;39(6):564–71. Epub 2001/06/01. pmid:11385640.
  148. 148. Lee E, Burnett CA, Lalich N, Cameron LL, Sestito JP. Proportionate mortality of crop and livestock farmers in the United States, 1984–1993. Am J Ind Med. 2002;42(5):410–20. Epub 2002/10/17. pmid:12382254.
  149. 149. Baldi I, Cantagrel A, Lebailly P, Tison F, Dubroca B, Chrysostome V, et al. Association between Parkinson's disease and exposure to pesticides in southwestern France. Neuroepidemiology. 2003;22(5):305–10. Epub 2003/08/07. pmid:12902626.
  150. 150. Duzcan F, Zencir M, Ozdemir F, Cetin GO, Bagci H, Heutink P, et al. Familial influence on parkinsonism in a rural area of Turkey (Kizilcaboluk-Denizli): a community-based case-control study. Mov Disord. 2003;18(7):799–804. Epub 2003/06/20. pmid:12815659.
  151. 151. Park RM, Schulte PA, Bowman JD, Walker JT, Bondy SC, Yost MG, et al. Potential occupational risks for neurodegenerative diseases. Am J Ind Med. 2005;48(1):63–77. Epub 2005/06/09. pmid:15940722.
  152. 152. Goldman SM, Tanner CM, Olanow CW, Watts RL, Field RD, Langston JW. Occupation and parkinsonism in three movement disorders clinics. Neurology. 2005;65(9):1430–5. Epub 2005/09/16. pmid:16162857.
  153. 153. Dick S, Semple S, Dick F, Seaton A. Occupational titles as risk factors for Parkinson's disease. Occup Med (Lond). 2007;57(1):50–6. Epub 2006/10/19. pmid:17046990.
  154. 154. Tanner CM, Ross GW, Jewell SA, Hauser RA, Jankovic J, Factor SA, et al. Occupation and Risk of Parkinsonism: A Multicenter Case-Control Study Arch Neurol. 2009;66(9):1106–13. pmid:19752299
  155. 155. Tanaka K, Miyake Y, Fukushima W, Sasaki S, Kiyohara C, Tsuboi Y, et al. Occupational risk factors for Parkinson's disease: a case-control study in Japan. BMC Neurol. 2011;11:83. Epub 2011/07/08. pmid:21733194; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3171313.
  156. 156. Rugbjerg K, Harris MA, Shen H, Marion SA, Tsui JK, Teschke K. Pesticide exposure and risk of Parkinson's disease—a population-based case-control study evaluating the potential for recall bias. Scand J Work Environ Health. 2011;37(5):427–36. Epub 2011/01/18. pmid:21240453.
  157. 157. Frigerio R, Sanft KR, Grossardt BR, Peterson BJ, Elbaz A, Bower JH, et al. Chemical exposures and Parkinson's disease: a population-based case-control study. Mov Disord. 2006;21(10):1688–92. Epub 2006/06/15. pmid:16773614.
  158. 158. Brighina L, Frigerio R, Schneider NK, Lesnick TG, de Andrade M, Cunningham JM, et al. Alpha-synuclein, pesticides, and Parkinson disease: a case-control study. Neurology. 2008;70(16 Pt 2):1461–9. Epub 2008/03/07. pmid:18322262.
  159. 159. Stevens JT, Stevens TD, Breckenridge CB. Crop Protection Chemicals: Mechanism of Action and Hazard Profiles. Hayes' Principles and Methods of Toxicology, Sixth Edition: CRC Press; 2014. p. 711–824.
  160. 160. Tanner CM, Kamel F, Ross GW, Hoppin JA, Goldman SM, Korell M, et al. Rotenone, paraquat, and Parkinson's disease. Environ Health Perspect. 2011;119(6):866–72. Epub 2011/01/29. pmid:21269927; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3114824.
  161. 161. Lee PC, Bordelon Y, Bronstein J, Ritz B. Traumatic brain injury, paraquat exposure, and their relationship to Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2012;79(20):2061–6. Epub 2012/11/15. pmid:23150532; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3511918.
  162. 162. Wang A, Costello S, Cockburn M, Zhang X, Bronstein J, Ritz B. Parkinson's disease risk from ambient exposure to pesticides. Eur J Epidemiol. 2011;26(7):547–55. Epub 2011/04/21. pmid:21505849; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3643971.
  163. 163. Goldman SM, Kamel F, Ross GW, Bhudhikanok GS, Hoppin JA, Korell M, et al. Genetic modification of the association of paraquat and Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2012;27(13):1652–8. Epub 2012/10/10. pmid:23045187; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3572192.
  164. 164. Elbaz A, Levecque C, Clavel J, Vidal JS, Richard F, Amouyel P, et al. CYP2D6 polymorphism, pesticide exposure, and Parkinson's disease. Ann Neurol. 2004;55(3):430–4. Epub 2004/03/03. pmid:14991823.
  165. 165. Tomenson JA, Campbell C. Mortality from Parkinson's disease and other causes among a workforce manufacturing paraquat: a retrospective cohort study. BMJ Open. 2011;1(2):e000283. Epub 2011/11/15. pmid:22080539; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3211049.
  166. 166. Kamel F. Epidemiology. Paths from pesticides to Parkinson's. Science. 2013;341(6147):722–3. Epub 2013/08/21. pmid:23950519.
  167. 167. Ntzani EE, Chondrogiorgi M, Ntritsos G, Evangelou E, Tzoulaki I. Literature review on epidemiological studies linking exposure to pesticides and health effects. EFSA supporting publication. 2013;(EN-497):159.
  168. 168. Kiyohara C, Kusuhara S. Cigarette smoking and Parkinson's disease: a meta-analysis. Fukuoka Igaku Zasshi. 2011;102(8):254–65. Epub 2011/10/05. pmid:21966751.
  169. 169. Quik M. Smoking, nicotine and Parkinson's disease. Trends Neurosci. 2004;27(9):561–8. Epub 2004/08/28. pmid:15331239.
  170. 170. Fowler JS, Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Pappas N, Logan J, MacGregor R, et al. Inhibition of monoamine oxidase B in the brains of smokers. Nature. 1996;379(6567):733–6. Epub 1996/02/22. pmid:8602220.
  171. 171. Ritz B, Lee PC, Lassen CF, Arah OA. Parkinson disease and smoking revisited: ease of quitting is an early sign of the disease. Neurology. 2014;83(16):1396–402. Epub 2014/09/14. pmid:25217056; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4206154.
  172. 172. Torta DM, Castelli L. Reward pathways in Parkinson's disease: clinical and theoretical implications. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2008;62(2):203–13. Epub 2008/04/17. pmid:18412844.
  173. 173. Ray NJ, Strafella AP. Imaging impulse control disorders in Parkinson's disease and their relationship to addiction. J Neural Transm (Vienna). 2013;120(4):659–64. Epub 2012/12/13. pmid:23232664.
  174. 174. Stern RA, Riley DO, Daneshvar DH, Nowinski CJ, Cantu RC, McKee AC. Long-term consequences of repetitive brain trauma: chronic traumatic encephalopathy. PM R. 2011;3(10 Suppl 2):S460–7. Epub 2011/11/09. pmid:22035690.
  175. 175. Harris MA, Cripton PA, Teschke K. Retrospective assessment of occupational exposure to whole-body vibration for a case-control study. J Occup Environ Hyg. 2012;9(6):371–80. Epub 2012/05/11. pmid:22571854.
  176. 176. Whitton PS. Inflammation as a causative factor in the aetiology of Parkinson's disease. Br J Pharmacol. 2007;150(8):963–76. Epub 2007/03/07. pmid:17339843; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2013918.
  177. 177. Hubble JP, Cao T, Kjelstrom JA, Koller WC, Beaman BL. Nocardia species as an etiologic agent in Parkinson's disease: serological testing in a case-control study. J Clin Microbiol. 1995;33(10):2768–9. Epub 1995/10/01. pmid:8567923; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC228573.
  178. 178. Beaman BL. Nocardia: An environmental bacterium possibly associated with neurodegenerative diseases in humans. In: Isaacson RL, Jensen KF, editors. The vulnerable brain and environmental risks. 2: Springer; 1992. p. 147–66.
  179. 179. Beaman BL, Canfield D, Anderson J, Pate B, Calne D. Site-specific invasion of the basal ganglia by Nocardia asteroides GUH-2. Med Microbiol Immunol. 2000;188(4):161–8. Epub 2000/08/05. pmid:10917152.
  180. 180. Hatcher JM, Pennell KD, Miller GW. Parkinson's disease and pesticides: a toxicological perspective. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences. 2008;29(6):322–9. pmid:18453001
  181. 181. McCormack AL, Thiruchelvam M, Manning-Bog AB, Thiffault C, Langston JW, Cory-Slechta DA, et al. Environmental risk factors and Parkinson's disease: selective degeneration of nigral dopaminergic neurons caused by the herbicide paraquat. Neurobiol Dis. 2002;10(2):119–27. Epub 2002/07/20. pmid:12127150.
  182. 182. McCormack AL, Atienza JG, Johnston LC, Andersen JK, Vu S, Di Monte DA. Role of oxidative stress in paraquat-induced dopaminergic cell degeneration. J Neurochem. 2005;93(4):1030–7. Epub 2005/04/29. pmid:15857406.
  183. 183. Jackson-Lewis V, Blesa J, Przedborski S. Animal models of Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2012;18 Suppl 1:S183–5. Epub 2011/12/23. pmid:22166429.
  184. 184. Sharma V. Parkinson's disease and neurotoxic animal models: A mechanistic view. Int J Pharm Pharm Sci. 2012;4(Suppl. 1):55–60. pmid:2012178693.
  185. 185. Le W, Sayana P, Jankovic J. Animal models of Parkinson's disease: a gateway to therapeutics? Neurotherapeutics. 2014;11(1):92–110. Epub 2013/10/26. pmid:24158912; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3899493.
  186. 186. Breckenridge CB, Sturgess NC, Butt M, Wolf JC, Zadory D, Beck M, et al. Pharmacokinetic, neurochemical, stereological and neuropathological studies on the potential effects of paraquat in the substantia nigra pars compacta and striatum of male C57BL/6J mice. Neurotoxicology. 2013;37:1–14. Epub 2013/03/26. pmid:23523781.
  187. 187. Minnema DJ, Travis KZ, Breckenridge CB, Sturgess NC, Butt M, Wolf JC, et al. Dietary administration of paraquat for 13 weeks does not result in a loss of dopaminergic neurons in the substantia nigra of C57BL/6J mice. Regul Toxicol Pharmacol. 2014;68(2):250–8. Epub 2014/01/07. pmid:24389362.
  188. 188. Hill AB. The Environment and Disease: Association or Causation? Proc R Soc Med. 1965;58:295–300. Epub 1965/05/01. pmid:14283879; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1898525.
  189. 189. Cole P. Causality in epidemiology, health policy and law. Environ Law Report. 1997;27(6):10279–85.
  190. 190. Rothman KJ, Greenland S. Causation and Causal Inference in Epidemiology. Am J Public Health. 2005;95(S1):S144–S50. PubMed Central PMCID: PMC16030331.
  191. 191. Weed DL. Weight of Evidence: A Review of Concept and Methods. Risk Anal. 2005;25(6):1545–57. pmid:16506981.
  192. 192. Adami HO, Berry SC, Breckenridge CB, Smith LL, Swenberg JA, Trichopoulos D, et al. Toxicology and epidemiology: improving the science with a framework for combining toxicological and epidemiological evidence to establish causal inference. Toxicol Sci. 2011;122(2):223–34. Epub 2011/05/13. pmid:21561883; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3155086.
  193. 193. Shapiro S. Meta-analysis/Shmeta-analysis. Am J Epidemiol. 1994;140(9):771–8. Epub 1994/11/01. pmid:7977286.
  194. 194. Olsen J. What characterises a useful concept of causation in epidemiology? J Epidemiol Community Health. 2003;57(2):86–8. pmid:12540681.
  195. 195. Weed DL. Meta-analysis and causal inference: a case study of benzene and non-Hodgkin lymphoma. Ann Epidemiol. 2010;20(5):347–55. Epub 2010/04/13. pmid:20382335.
  196. 196. Greenland S. Can meta-analysis be salvaged? Am J Epidemiol. 1994;140(9):783–7. Epub 1994/11/01. pmid:7977288.
  197. 197. Williamson PR, Gamble C, Altman DG, Hutton JL. Outcome selection bias in meta-analysis. Stat Methods Med Res. 2005;14(5):515–24. Epub 2005/10/27. pmid:16248351.
  198. 198. Kivimaki M, Singh-Manoux A, Ferrie JE, Batty GD. Post hoc decision-making in observational epidemiology—is there need for better research standards? Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42(2):367–70. Epub 2013/04/10. pmid:23569177; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3619956.
  199. 199. Priyadarshi A, Khuder SA, Schaub EA, Shrivastava S. A meta-analysis of Parkinson's disease and exposure to pesticides. Neurotoxicology. 2000;21(4):435–40. Epub 2000/10/07. pmid:11022853.
  200. 200. Wright Willis A, Evanoff BA, Lian M, Criswell SR, Racette BA. Geographic and ethnic variation in Parkinson disease: a population-based study of US Medicare beneficiaries. Neuroepidemiology. 2010;34(3):143–51. Epub 2010/01/22. pmid:20090375; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2865395.
  201. 201. Boffetta P, McLaughlin JK, La Vecchia C, Tarone RE, Lipworth L, Blot WJ. False-positive results in cancer epidemiology: a plea for epistemological modesty. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2008;100(14):988–95. Epub 2008/07/10. pmid:18612135; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2467434.
  202. 202. Boffetta P, McLaughlin JK, La Vecchia C, Tarone RE, Lipworth L, Blot WJ. A further plea for adherence to the principles underlying science in general and the epidemiologic enterprise in particular. Int J Epidemiol. 2009;38(3):678–9. Epub 2009/01/17. pmid:19147704.
  203. 203. Ioannidis JP. Why most published research findings are false. PLoS Med. 2005;2(8):e124. Epub 2005/08/03. pmid:16060722; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1182327.
  204. 204. Ioannidis JPA. Why Most Discovered True Associations Are Inflated. Epidemiology. 2008;19(5):640–8. pmid:18633328
  205. 205. Ioannidis JPA, Tarone R, McLaughlin JK. The False-positive to False-negative Ratio in Epidemiologic Studies. Epidemiology. 2011;22(4):450–6. pmid:21490505
  206. 206. von Elm E, Altman DG, Egger M, Pocock SJ, Gøtzsche PC, Vandenbroucke JP, et al. The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) Statement: Guidelines for Reporting Observational Studies. PLoS Med. 2007;4(10):1623–7.