Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Problematic Social Media Use: Results from a Large-Scale Nationally Representative Adolescent Sample

Abstract

Despite social media use being one of the most popular activities among adolescents, prevalence estimates among teenage samples of social media (problematic) use are lacking in the field. The present study surveyed a nationally representative Hungarian sample comprising 5,961 adolescents as part of the European School Survey Project on Alcohol and Other Drugs (ESPAD). Using the Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale (BSMAS) and based on latent profile analysis, 4.5% of the adolescents belonged to the at-risk group, and reported low self-esteem, high level of depression symptoms, and elevated social media use. Results also demonstrated that BSMAS has appropriate psychometric properties. It is concluded that adolescents at-risk of problematic social media use should be targeted by school-based prevention and intervention programs.

Introduction

Social media use

Social media use is currently one of the most popular leisure activities among adolescents (e.g., [13]). Social media (e.g., Facebook, Instagram, Snapchat, etc.) host virtual communities where users can create individual public and/or private profiles [46]. Users can access social media on different platforms (mobile or computer devices), for different activities (e.g., interacting with real-life friends, meeting others based on shared interest, chatting, mailing, sharing or creating pictures / videos, blogging, dating, playing games, gambling; [79]).

Facebook is one of the most popular social media among 13–17 years old adolescents in the USA [2]. According to a recent report, 71% of teenage social media users access more than one social media and 24% of adolescents are “almost constantly” online due to the widespread use and popularity of smartphones [2]. Furthermore, there is an increasing interest to explore and assess the characteristics and prevalence of problematic/excessive use of social media (e.g., [4, 1014]).

Problematic social media use

To date, there is no consensus among researchers regarding the definition of problematic social media use due to the conceptual confusion surrounding the classification of problematic internet use [15, 16]. Negative outcomes triggered by the excessive use of social media may have a detrimental effect on the personal, social, and/or professional lives of the users [8, 13, 1720]. Lee, Cheung, and Thadani [21] argued that obsessive Facebook users had troubles in work, academic performance. and interpersonal relationships. For instance, Pantic and Damjanovic [22], Wegmann and Stodt [16], and Andreassen and Billieux [23] reported a significant positive correlation between depression symptoms and social media use, while Malik and Khan [24] found negative relationship between self-esteem and high levels of social media use.

Due to the lack of consistency in empirical studies, diagnosis of internet-related disorders has yet to be established based on the aforementioned theoretical constructs. Internet Use Disorder was suggested for consideration in the latest (fifth) edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; [25]). However, only one internet-related disorder—Internet Gaming Disorder—was included in Section 3 of the DSM-5. Another problem is that various synonyms of problematic social media use exist in the literature with different diagnostic suggestions including (among others) Facebook dependence [26], Facebook addiction [10], social networking addiction [27], Twitter addiction [28], social media addiction [11], and Social Media Disorder [29].

Different theoretical models provide explanations for the development of problematic social media use (e.g., cognitive-behavioral, social skill, or socio-cognitive models; [30]. These theoretical models have been developed from a clinical perspective, while the biopsychosocial model concerns behavioral addictions in general [14]. According to the biopsychosocial model [31], problematic social media use can be determined by a range of addiction symptoms including: mood modification (i.e., excessive social media use leading to specific changes in mood states), salience (i.e., total preoccupation with social media use), tolerance (i.e., increasing amounts of time using social media), withdrawal symptoms (i.e., negative feelings and psychological symptoms such as irritability, anxiety when social media use is restricted), conflict (i.e., interpersonal problems as a direct result of social media usage), and relapse (i.e., returning to excessive social media use after a period of abstinence).

Assessing problematic social media use

To obtain a reliable prevalence rate of problematic social media usage, it is important to use psychometrically valid measurement tools. Due to the problem of inconsistencies regarding the definition of problematic, excessive, or addictive social media use, there is also a lack of reliable and valid psychometric scales to assess the phenomenon of problematic social media use. More specifically, the existing assessment tools are based on different diagnostic suggestions such as problematic internet use (e.g., Internet Addiction Test; [3234], Internet Gaming Disorder [29]), or other aspects of addictive tendencies (e.g., withdrawal, loss of control, salience; [35, 36]. In addition, some of the measurement tools focus only on specific social media (e.g., Facebook; [14] such as the Facebook Addiction Symptoms Scale [37], the Facebook Addiction Scale [38], the Bergen Facebook Addiction Scale [10], and the Facebook Intrusion Questionnaire [39]).

Although the most recent data show that Facebook is the most popular and frequently used social media among adolescents [2], empirical research has shown that adolescents use more than one social media frequently (e.g., [2]). Therefore, the assessment tools are unable to follow the ever-changing trends in the area of social media use. Considering the increased usage of various social media among adolescents [13, 5] the questionnaires should assess all available social media and the total range of activities on these social media instead of one specific social media such as Facebook [14].

Prevalence of problematic social media use

It is difficult to estimate the prevalence of problematic social media use due to the use of various assessment tools and the lack of a consensual definition of problematic social media use. Furthermore, recent research has demonstrated that problematic social media use has a higher prevalence among female users than males [11, 13, 40, 41]. Unfortunately, in studies that have assessed different aspects of problematic social media use, the gender distribution was usually frequently imbalanced in that women were typically over-represented [11, 12, 15, 26, 29, 37, 4244], and may be explained by the higher willingness of females to participate in such studies.

Due to different theoretical frameworks and psychometric assessments, the prevalence of problematic social media use might be underestimated or overestimated. Previous studies have reported different prevalence rates relating to problematic social media use. For instance, Olowu and Seri [44] reported a prevalence rate of 2.8% of addicted social media use among college students, while Jafarkarimi and Sim [43] reported a prevalence rate of 47% being addicted to Facebook among a sample of college students. Explanations for the large difference in problematic social media use prevalence rates might be the non-representative (self-selected and typically small participant) samples and different cultural groups examined (e.g., Chinese, Australian, Nigerian college students, Dutch adolescents [12, 15, 26, 29, 37, 4244]. Moreover, to date, there has only been one nationwide survey assessing problematic (ie., addictive) social media use [11] that examined the associations between problematic social media use, narcissism, and self-esteem, and between problematic use of social media, attention-deficit/hyperactivity, obsessive-compulsiveness, anxiety, and depression on cross-sectional convenience sample of 23,532 Norwegians (although the sample was not nationally representative).

Furthermore, no studies have examined the prevalence of problematic social media use utilizing a representative adolescent sample. Furthermore, only a few studies exist concerning problematic social media use among adolescents (e.g., [29]). Previous studies have reported an increased popularity of social media use among adolescents [14, 6] and the increased number of adolescent social media users could explain the higher prevalence of problematic usage in this group [4, 6]. Consequently, the aim of the present study was twofold:

  1. To test the psychometric properties of the Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale (BSMAS) using a nationally representative (Hungarian) adolescent sample.
  2. To assess the prevalence of problematic social media use in a nationally representative adolescent sample.

Methods

Participants and procedure

The data were collected in March 2015 as part of the European School Survey Project on Alcohol and Other Drugs (ESPAD; [45]) that included a nationally representative adolescent sample. The target population was adolescents aged 16 years. In 2015, Hungary included a short section to assess internet and social media use in addition to the original questionnaire developed by the ESPAD Committee.

To obtain a representative sample, two different grades (9th-10th) were included in the Hungarian data collection, each containing a proportion of the target population. To reduce sampling error, the grades were divided into non-overlapping, homogeneous subgroups. The variables to ensure the representativeness of the adolescent sample were as follows: region (central/western/eastern Hungary), grade (9th, 10th), and type of class (secondary general, secondary vocational, vocational classes). The data were collected anonymously from the students in the classrooms of the schools by research assistants.

The refusal rate was 7% on the level of the primary sampling unit (i.e., classes) that led to skewed nonresponse. To match the composition of the respondents with the sampling frame, data were weighted by strata with the matrix weighting method recommended by the Education Information System 2014/2015 (KIR-STAT; Elekes, 2015). The total sample consisted of 6,664 participants (50.94% male). The youngest participants were 15 years old, while the oldest were 22 years (mean age 16.62 years; SD = 0.96). The wide age range was due to a very small number of older students still attending the 9th or 10th grades at the age of 19 years or older at the time of data collection. The questions concerning internet use and social media use were included for this nationally representative sample of 9th-10th graders in secondary general and secondary vocational schools. Participant data with severe incompleteness or inconsistencies were excluded (3.72% of the sample), in addition to those participants who did not use the internet and/or any social media (an additional 6.83% of the original sample). After removing these participants, the final sample size was 5,961 (89.45% of the total sample).

This study was approved by the Scientific Ethical Committee of Corvinus University of Budapest. The study design was based on an international protocol approved by the European School Survey Project on Alcohol and Other Drugs (ESPAD) Assembly, which was conducted in full compliance with the principles expressed in the Declaration of Helsinki. Written informed consent was requested from both the students and their parents (passive on behalf of their children).

Measures

Socio-demographics questions.

Information regarding gender, age, grade, and residence were collected.

Weekly social media use.

To assess the adolescents’ weekly time spent on social media on computer or other devices (e.g., handled devices) two variables were combined: (i) ‘The last 7 days how many days did you use the internet for social networking?’ (Categories were ‘never’, ‘1 day’, ‘2 days’, …‘7 days’); (ii) ‘In the last 30 days on an average day how many hours did you use the internet for social networking?’ (Categories were ‘I don’t use’, ‘less than half an hour’, ‘1 hour’, ‘2–3 hours’, ‘4–5 hours’ and ‘more than 6 hours’).

Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale (BSMAS).

To assess problematic social media use, the Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale (BSMAS; [11]) was used. The 6-item scale was adapted from the previously validated Bergen Facebook Addiction Scale (BFAS; [10]). The original scale specifically assessed problematic Facebook use during the last year. The scale incorporated the theoretical framework of the addiction components of the biopsychosocial model [31]. The BFAS was developed by selecting the items with the highest possible factor loadings for each component (i.e., salience, mood modification, tolerance, withdrawal symptoms, conflict, and relapse) from an item-pool of 18 initial items. In the present study, the Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale (BSMAS) which is based on rephrasing of the BFAS, was to assess social media use in general over the past 12 months. The scale was translated to Hungarian and then back-translated by independent translators. The back-translation was then compared with the original scale and adjustments were made as necessary. The items are answered on a 5-point scale (“never” to “always”). The Cronbach’s alpha of the translated BSMAS was .85 in the present sample.

Rosenberg’s Self-Esteem Scale.

Self-worth was assessed by the Hungarian version of the Rosenberg’s Self-Esteem Scale (RSES-HU; [46], Hungarian version, [47]). The RSES assessed global self-esteem (i.e., feeling of self-worth and self-acceptance) with 10 items on a 4-point scale (“strongly agree” to “strongly disagree”). The score range is between 10–40 and the higher the score, the higher the self-esteem. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.87 in the present sample.

Center of Epidemiological Studies Depression-Scale.

Depressive mood was assessed with the 6-item short-form of the Center of Epidemiological Studies Depression-Scale (CES-D; [48]). The scale assesses the level of depressive symptoms but it was not designed to diagnose clinical depression. The instrument was translated and then back-translated by Hungarian experts in the addiction field. The back-translation was then compared to the original instrument and adjustments were made where necessary. The items of CES-D were answered on a 4-point scale (“rarely or never” to “most of the time”). The score range is 4–24, and a higher score indicates higher level of depressive symptoms. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.84 in the present sample.

Statistical analysis

To test the one-factor model of the BSMAS, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was performed with maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard error (MLR) in Mplus 7.3 [49]. To evaluate the model fit, a p-value of Chi-square (χ2) higher than .05 was used for the test of close fit [50]. Additional fit indices were also included: the comparative fit index (CFI), the Tukey-Lewis Fit Index (TLI), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) and its 95% confidence interval (90% CI), and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). To indicate a good fit of the model, both CFI and TLI values have to be over than .90 or over .95 [51], while the values of RMSEA and SRMR should be less than .05 and .10 respectively [5153].

In order to identify the groups of adolescents with high risk of problematic social media use, a mixture modeling technique called latent profile analysis (performed in Mplus 7.3) was used. Latent profile analysis is a mixture modeling technique to identify groups of people (categorical output variable of the analysis) according to their responses to certain continuous variables (in the present study’s case, the scores given on the six items of the BSMAS). Individuals with similar responses are classified in the same group [54]. Latent profile analysis was performed with 2 to 4 classes in the full sample (n = 5,961). To determine the number of latent classes, several indices were used, such as the measures of parsimony of each model (i.e., Akaike Information Criteria—AIC, Bayesian Information Criteria—BIC, and the Sample Size Adjusted Bayesian Information Criteria—SSABIC). The lower values on these indicators, the more parsimonious the model. The entropy criterion and the interpretability of clusters were also examined. In the final determination of the number of classes, the likelihood-ratio difference test (Lo-Mendell-Rubin Adjusted LRT Test) was used that statistically compares the fit of the estimated model with a model having one less class than the estimated one. The p-value of less than .05 suggests the tested model fits better than the model with one less class [49].

To test the construct validity of the BSMAS, the LPA classes were compared along a number of variables relevant to the phenomenon of SOCIAL MEDIA use (i.e., gender, SOCIAL MEDIA use hours/ week, level of self-esteem, level of depression). For these comparisons, Wald’s Chi-square test of mean equality for latent class predictors in mixture modeling was used because it takes into consideration the probabilistic nature of the LPA classes (for description of the analysis, see www.statmodel.com/download/meantest2.pdf).

To determine the optimal cut-off point for the BSMAS, a sensitivity analysis was performed and the group with the highest risk of problematic social media use (based on the results of the LPA analysis) was considered as the “gold standard”. The present authors are aware that this method does not replace the clinical validation process, however, the authors believe that this is better than using completely ad-hoc cut-off points as several other studies do. The sensitivity, specificity, positive and negative predictive value (PPV and NPV, respectively), and the accuracy of each cut-off threshold were calculated and compared to identify the cut-off value with the best indicators. Sensitivity is the proportion of true positive cases belonging to the at-risk group based on the LPA (the “gold standard” group in this case). Specificity is the proportion of the true negatives among those who do not belong to the at-risk group based on the LPA [55, 56]. PPV is the proportion of true positives among all participants who scored positive on the test. NPV was defined as the proportion of true negatives among all participants with negative test results [56, 57]. Finally, accuracy measures the proportions of true negatives and true positives among all participants [56, 57].

All analyses were conducted on the weighted sample. Missing data were treated with Full-information maximum likelihood (FIML) method [49]. Statistical analyses were carried out with Mplus 7.3 [49] and IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 22.0 [58].

Results

Descriptive statistics

The final sample only comprised those participants who reported using the internet and social media (n = 5,961, 89.45% of the total sample). Approximately half (49.17%) of the sample was male (n = 2931). Age ranged between 15 and 22 years (mean age 16.60; SD = 0.94). The mean number of hours using social media was 23.16 hours per week (SD = 15.57). There was a significant difference in weekly social media use between male and female adolescents (mean timemale = 20.53 hours, SDmale = 15.71; mean timefemale = 25.71 hours, SDfemale = 15.00; U = 3672101, p< 0.001; r = -0.17).

Confirmatory factor analysis

A one-factor model with the six components (salience, tolerance, mood modification, relapse, withdrawal, conflict) as indicator variables was tested with confirmatory factor analysis. The analysis provided an acceptable fit to the data (χ2 = 5836.190 df = 15 p< 0.001; CFI = 0.950; TLI = 0.917; RMSEA = 0.073 (0.066–0.080) Cfit>0.90; SRMR = 0.034). All factor loadings were above the recommended threshold (>.50) and ranged from .598 to .814.

Latent profile analysis

The latent profile analysis was performed on the six items of the BSMAS, and according to the criteria, the three-class solution was selected as the best-fitting model (see Table 1). The AIC, BIC, and SSABIC values decreased continuously as more classes were added to the analysis. However, the scale of decrease somewhat diminished after the third latent class was added. Based on the L-M-R test, the three-class solution was accepted. The entropy of the two-class solution was the highest, but the entropy of the three-class solution was also adequate.

The features of the three classes are presented in Fig 1 and Table 2. The first class named ‘no-risk’ class represents the majority of social media users (78.3% of social media users; 70.7% of the total sample) who had the lowest scores on the BSMAS. The second class of social media users represents ‘low risk’ of problematic use (17.2% and 15.5% respectively), while the third class represents the population of ‘at-risk’ problematic social media users (4.5% and 4.1%, respectively). In the ‘at-risk’ group, ‘withdrawal’ and ‘tolerance’ criteria showed elevated levels compared to the other dimensions. Members of this class (i.e., those at-risk of problematic use) were likely to (a) be female, (b) use the internet and social media for more than 30 hours per week, (c) have lower self-esteem and higher level of depressive symptoms than social media users of the other two classes (Table 2).

thumbnail
Fig 1. The Three Classes Obtained from the Latent Profile Analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169839.g001

thumbnail
Table 2. Comparison of the Three Latent Classes: Testing Equality for Latent Class Predictors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169839.t002

Suggesting a cutoff score for classification: Sensitivity and specificity analysis

Because of the lack of a clinically diagnosed group of problematic social media users, the third LPA class (i.e., those at-risk of problematic social media use) was used as the ‘gold standard’ to determine the optimal cut-off threshold to classify those at-risk of problematic use. Sensitivity, specificity, positive predictive value (PPV), negative predictive value (NPV), and accuracy of the BSMAS at all possible cut-off points were calculated (Table 3). Based on this analysis, a cut-off score of 19 points was suggested as the ideal threshold at and above which individuals are classified as at-risk of problematic social media use.

thumbnail
Table 3. Cut-off points based on the third class (i.e., those at-risk of problematic social media use) derived from the Latent Profile Analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169839.t003

In this case, the specificity is 99% and the sensitivity is 83% (i.e., only 1% of the non-problematic social media users are identified incorrectly as being at-risk of problematic use by the scale, while 17% of true cases of problematic social media users are missed). At this value, PPV is 73% and the NPV is 99%. In other words, 27% of the individuals with a positive test result are identified incorrectly, while only 1% of individuals with negative test result are identified incorrectly. This yields an accuracy of 98%. Increasing the cut-off point would result in more false negative cases, while decreasing it would increase the number of social media users labeled incorrectly (as being at-risk) by the screening instrument.

Discussion

To assess the prevalence of problematic social media use in a reliable and a valid way, the psychometric properties of the BSMAS were tested. According to the results, the BSMAS demonstrated adequate psychometric properties regarding its factor structure, reliability, and validity.

Using a latent profile analysis on the six items of the Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale (BSMAS), the adolescent social media users were divided into three different classes, and the analysis demonstrated that 4.5% of participants could be classified as being at-risk. Previous studies have shown a wide range of prevalence rates due to various methodological issues such as convenience sampling, targeting mainly college students, and/ or having small sample sizes [15, 12, 26, 42, 43]. For instance, the prevalence of problematic social media users among Nigerian University undergraduates was 1.6% [37], whereas among Malaysian college students the reported prevalence was 47% [43]. The results of the present study belong to the more conservative prevalence estimations. Moreover, they correspond to the prevalence rates of the general problematic and/or addictive Internet use, that range between 1% [59] and 18.7% [60] according to the recent review [61].

Regarding validity of the BSMAS, the at-risk group showed the lowest self-esteem and the highest level of depressive symptoms and the most time spent on internet and social media use, and was therefore in line with previous research findings [22, 24].

In addition, adolescents that were at-risk of social media use were mainly female, and reported the greatest amount of internet and social media usage. Previous studies have found similar gender differences in problematic social media use [4, 6] and problematic internet use [61]. For instance, Rehbein and Mößle [62] found that among adolescents with problematic internet use, girls indicated that social media contributed most to their addiction, while boys also cited online pornography as a primary source of their problems.

Furthermore, the results of the present study showed that within the at-risk group the withdrawal component had the highest score. Therefore, withdrawal symptoms should be highlighted when developing prevention and treatment programs in school environments for adolescents being at-risk of problematic social media use. A cut-off point was calculated using the third LPA class as the “gold standard” to categorize the risky or problematic social media users among adolescents in the sample. The suggested cut-off value with the most adequate sensitivity and specificity values was 19 points. Although, the calculated score cannot replace a clinically validated cut-off point, it may be more beneficial than using completely ad-hoc thresholds [63].

Despite the study’s strengths (most notably the large nationally representative sample using psychometrically validated instruments), the study is not without its limitations. The study only included Hungarian adolescents as participants. Therefore, to further test the psychometric properties of the BSMAS, cross-cultural studies should also be conducted in the future using different adolescent groups in different countries and cultures. Moreover, the data were all self-report and self-report measures may lead to different response biases [64], such as social desirability bias (e.g., reporting more favorable behavior than the truth), memory recall bias (difficulty in remembering past events), and response style bias (e.g., scores may show central tendency or extreme response style).

It is also important to highlight that psychometric screening tools tend to overestimate the prevalence rates of disorders when the true prevalence rate of the disorder is low. For instance, an instrument with moderate sensitivity and specificity (i.e., 80.5% and 82.4%, respectively) at a 2.1% prevalence level of the problematic behavior has a positive predictive value of 8.9%. This means that out of 100 respondents who score positive on the test only about 9 are true clinical cases [65]. Consequently, survey-based prevalence rates should be interpreted cautiously to avoid overpathologizing every day behaviors [66]. More generally, the issue of addiction to social networking and social media is a controversial issue and many papers have questioned whether the activity can be considered an addiction at all [4, 27, 6668].

The study had a cross-sectional design, therefore causality cannot be established regarding the risk factors. Future research should apply longitudinal designs to identify the contributing factors of the problematic behavior among social media users. It should also be noted that when completing the BSMAS, the participants may have had a different conception of social media use than intended by the developers of the BSMAS. For instance, on sites such as Facebook, many different activities can be carried out such as social networking, gaming and gambling. Although the BSMAS is only concerned with social networking, there is always the possibility that participants’ conception of social media use included some or all of these other activities and therefore problematic use might be including non-social networking activities.

Conclusion

In conclusion, the results of the present study suggest that the Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale [11] is a psychometrically valid scale that is an appropriate tool to identify the signs of risky social media use among adolescents. This instrument may be especially useful in school environments to identify those adolescents who are at-risk of problematic social media use and therefore could be utilized in prevention and intervention programs (i.e., content-control software, counseling, cognitive-behavioral therapy; [69]).

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the Hungarian National Research, Development and Innovation Office (Grant numbers: K111938, K111740). Ágnes Zsila was supported by the New National Excellence Program awarded by the Ministry of Human Resources.

The funding institutions had no role in the study design or the collection, analysis and interpretation of the data, writing the manuscript, or the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: ZD MDG OK.
  2. Data curation: ZE FB ÁZ AM OK.
  3. Formal analysis: FB ÁZ OK AM.
  4. Funding acquisition: ZE ZD.
  5. Investigation: ZE ZD AM.
  6. Methodology: ZE ZD AM CSA OK.
  7. Project administration: ZE ZD.
  8. Resources: ZD ZE.
  9. Software: ZE ZD.
  10. Supervision: ZE ZD.
  11. Validation: ZE ZD.
  12. Visualization: ZD.
  13. Writing – original draft: FB ÁZ OK AM.
  14. Writing – review & editing: MDG CSA ZD ZE.

References

  1. 1. Lenhart A, Purcell K, Smith A, Zickuhr K. Social Media & Mobile Internet Use among Teens and Young Adults. Washington, DC: Pew Internet & American Life Project; 2010.
  2. 2. Lenhart A, Duggan M, Perrin A, Stepler R, Rainie H, Parker K. Teens, social media and technology overview 2015. Smartphones facilitate shifts in communication landscape for teens. Washington, DC: Pew Internet & American Life Project; 2015.
  3. 3. Ahn J. The effect of social network sites on adolescents' social and academic development: Current theories and controversies. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology. 2011;62(8):1435–45.
  4. 4. Kuss DJ, Griffiths MD. Online social networking and addiction—a review of the psychological literature. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2011;8(9):3528–52. pmid:22016701
  5. 5. Boyd DM, Ellison NB. Social network sites: Definition, history, and scholarship. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication. 2007;13(1):210–30.
  6. 6. Kuss DJ, Griffiths MD. Excessive online social networking: Can adolescents become addicted to Facebook. Education and Health. 2011;29(4):68–71.
  7. 7. Allen KA, Ryan T, Gray DL, McInerney DM, Waters L. Social media use and social connectedness in adolescents: The positives and the potential pitfalls. The Australian Educational and Developmental Psychologist. 2014;31(01):18–31.
  8. 8. Ryan T, Chester A, Reece J, Xenos S. The uses and abuses of Facebook: A review of Facebook addiction. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2014;3(3):133–48. pmid:25317337
  9. 9. Griffiths MD. Adolescent gambling and gambling-type games on social networking sites: Issues, concerns, and recommendations. Aloma: Revista de Psicologia, Ciències de l'Educació i de l'Esport. 2015;33(2):31–7.
  10. 10. Andreassen CS, Torsheim T, Brunborg GS, Pallesen S. Development of a Facebook addiction scale 1, 2. Psychological Reports. 2012;110(2):501–17. pmid:22662404
  11. 11. Andreassen CS, Pallesen S, Griffiths MD. The relationship between addictive use of social media, narcissism, and self-esteem: Findings from a large national survey. Addictive Behaviors. in press.
  12. 12. Wilson K, Fornasier S, White KM. Psychological predictors of young adults' use of social networking sites. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2010;13(2):173–7.
  13. 13. Griffiths MD, Kuss DJ, Demetrovics Z. Social networking addiction: An overview of preliminary findings. Behavioral Addictions Criteria, Evidence, and Treatment. New York: Elsevier; 2014. p. 119–41.
  14. 14. Griffiths MD. Social networking addiction: Emerging themes and issues. Journal of Addiction Research and Therapy. 2013;4(5):1–2.
  15. 15. Wan C. Gratifications & loneliness as predictors of campus-SNS websites addiction & usage pattern among Chinese college students. Hong Kong: Chinese University of Hong Kong, China. 2009.
  16. 16. Wegmann E, Stodt B, Brand M. Addictive use of social networking sites can be explained by the interaction of Internet use expectancies, Internet literacy, and psychopathological symptoms. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2015;4(3):155–62. pmid:26551905
  17. 17. Hormes JM. Under the influence of Facebook? Excess use of social networking sites and drinking motives, consequences, and attitudes in college students. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2016;5(1):122–9.
  18. 18. Steers M-LN, Wickham RE, Acitelli LK. Seeing everyone else's highlight reels: How Facebook usage is linked to depressive symptoms. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 2014;33(8):701.
  19. 19. Muench F, Hayes M, Kuerbis A, Shao S. The independent relationship between trouble controlling Facebook use, time spent on the site and distress. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2015;4(3):163–9. pmid:26551906
  20. 20. Wu AM, Cheung VI, Ku L, Hung EP. Psychological risk factors of addiction to social networking sites among Chinese smartphone users. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2013;2(3):160–6. pmid:25215198
  21. 21. Lee ZW, Cheung CM, Thadani DR, editors. An Investigation into the Problematic Use of Facebook. System Science (HICSS), 2012 45th Hawaii International Conference; 2012 4–7 January; Maui, HI: IEEE.
  22. 22. Pantic I, Damjanovic A, Todorovic J, Topalovic D, Bojovic-Jovic D, Ristic S, et al. Association between online social networking and depression in high school students: behavioral physiology viewpoint. Psychiatria Danubina. 2012;24(1):90–3.
  23. 23. Andreassen CS, Billieux J, Griffiths MD, Kuss DJ, Demetrovics Z, Mazzoni E, et al. The relationship between addictive use of social media and video games and symptoms of psychiatric disorders: A large-scale cross-sectional study. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2016;30(2):252–62. pmid:26999354
  24. 24. Malik S, Khan M. Impact of facebook addiction on narcissistic behavior and self-esteem among students. Journal of Pakistan Medical Association. 2015;65(3):260–3.
  25. 25. Association AP. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5®). Fifth ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.
  26. 26. Wolniczak I, Caceres-DelAguila JA, Palma-Ardiles G, Arroyo KJ, Solís-Visscher R, Paredes-Yauri S, et al. Association between Facebook dependence and poor sleep quality: a study in a sample of undergraduate students in Peru. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(3):e59087. pmid:23554978
  27. 27. Griffiths MD. Facebook addiction: concerns, criticism, and recommendations—a response to Andreassen and colleagues. Psychological Reports. 2012;110(2):518–20. pmid:22662405
  28. 28. Saaid SA, Al-Rashid NAA, Abdullah Z. The impact of addiction to Twitter among university students. Future Information Technology. New York: Springer; 2014. p. 231–6.
  29. 29. van den Eijnden RJ, Lemmens JS, Valkenburg PM. The Social Media Disorder Scale: Validity and psychometric properties. Computers in Human Behavior. 2016;61:478–87.
  30. 30. Turel O, Serenko A. The benefits and dangers of enjoyment with social networking websites. European Journal of Information Systems. 2012;21(5):512–28.
  31. 31. Griffiths MD. A ‘components’ model of addiction within a biopsychosocial framework. Journal of Substance Use. 2005;10(4):191–7.
  32. 32. Young KS. Internet addiction: The emergence of a new clinical disorder. CyberPsychology & Behavior. 1998;1(3):237–44.
  33. 33. Armstrong L, Phillips JG, Saling LL. Potential determinants of heavier Internet usage. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies. 2000;53(4):537–50.
  34. 34. Morahan-Martin J, Schumacher P. Incidence and correlates of pathological Internet use among college students. Computers in human behavior. 2000;16(1):13–29.
  35. 35. Ehrenberg A, Juckes S, White KM, Walsh SP. Personality and self-esteem as predictors of young people's technology use. CyberPsychology & Behavior. 2008;11(6):739–41.
  36. 36. Walsh SP, White KM, Young RM. Young and connected: Psychological influences of mobile phone use amongst Australian youth. In: Goggin G, Hjorth L, editors. Proceedings Mobile Media 2007: University of Sydney; 2007. p. 125–34.
  37. 37. Alabi OF. A survey of Facebook addiction level among selected Nigerian University undergraduates. New Media and Mass Communication. 2013;10:70–80.
  38. 38. Çam E, Isbulan O. A New Addiction for Teacher Candidates: Social Networks. Turkish Online Journal of Educational Technology. 2012;11(3):14–9.
  39. 39. Elphinston RA, Noller P. Time to face it! Facebook intrusion and the implications for romantic jealousy and relationship satisfaction. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2011;14(11):631–5.
  40. 40. Andreassen CS. Online social network site addiction: A comprehensive review. Current Addiction Reports. 2015;2(2):175–84.
  41. 41. McAndrew FT, Jeong HS. Who does what on Facebook? Age, sex, and relationship status as predictors of Facebook use. Computers in Human Behavior. 2012;28(6):2359–65.
  42. 42. Zhou SX. Gratifications, loneliness, leisure boredom and self-esteem as predictors of SNS-game addiction and usage pattern among Chinese college students: Hong Kong: The Chinese University of Hong Kong; 2010.
  43. 43. Jafarkarimi H, Sim ATH, Saadatdoost R, Hee JM. Facebook Addiction among Malaysian Students. International Journal of Information and Education Technology. 2016;6(6):465–9.
  44. 44. Olowu AO, Seri FO. A study of social network addiction among youths in Nigeria. Journal of Social Science and Policy Review. 2012;4:62–71.
  45. 45. Király O, Zsila Á, Demetrovics Z. Viselkedési addikciók. In: Elekes Z, editor. Európai iskolavizsgálat az alkohol- és egyéb drogfogyasztási szokásokról– 2015; Magyarországi eredmények. Budapest: Budapesti Corvinus Egyetem; Társadalomtudományi és Nemzetközi Kapcsolatok Kar; Szociológia és Társadalompolitika Intézet; 2016. p. 75–94.
  46. 46. Rosenberg M. Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1965.
  47. 47. Urbán R, Szigeti R, Kökönyei G, Demetrovics Z. Global self-esteem and method effects: Competing factor structures, longitudinal invariance, and response styles in adolescents. Behavior Research Methods. 2014;46(2):488–98. pmid:24061931
  48. 48. Radloff LS. The CES-D scale a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;1(3):385–401.
  49. 49. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User’s Guide (7th edn). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén and Muthén; 2012.
  50. 50. Barrett P. Structural equation modelling: Adjudging model fit. Personality and Individual differences. 2007;42(5):815–24.
  51. 51. Lt Hu, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 1999;6(1):1–55.
  52. 52. Steiger JH. Understanding the limitations of global fit assessment in structural equation modeling. Personality and Individual Differences. 2007;42(5):893–8.
  53. 53. Byrne BM. Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. London: Routledge; 2013.
  54. 54. Collins LM, Lanza ST. Latent class and latent transition analysis: With applications in the social, behavioral, and health sciences. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons; 2013.
  55. 55. Altman DG, Bland JM. Diagnostic tests. 1: Sensitivity and specificity. British Medical Journal. 1994;308(6943):1552. pmid:8019315
  56. 56. Glaros AG, Kline RB. Understanding the accuracy of tests with cutting scores: The sensitivity, specificity, and predictive value model. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1988;44(6):1013–23. pmid:3216006
  57. 57. Altman DG, Bland JM. Statistics Notes: Diagnostic tests 2: Predictive values. British Medical Journal. 1994;309(6947):102. pmid:8038641
  58. 58. Released IC. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 22.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.; 2013.
  59. 59. Rumpf H-J, Vermulst AA, Bischof A, Kastirke N, Guertler D, Bischof G, et al. Occurence of internet addiction in a general population sample: a latent class analysis. European Addiction Research. 2013;20(4):159–66. pmid:24401314
  60. 60. Lin I-H, Ko C-H, Chang Y-P, Liu T-L, Wang P-W, Lin H-C, et al. The association between suicidality and Internet addiction and activities in Taiwanese adolescents. Comprehensive psychiatry. 2014;55(3):504–10. pmid:24457034
  61. 61. Pontes HM, Kuss D, Griffiths M. Clinical psychology of Internet addiction: A review of its conceptualization, prevalence, neuronal processes, and implications for treatment. Neuroscience and Neuroeconomics. 2015;4:11–23.
  62. 62. Rehbein F, Mößle T. Video game and Internet addiction: is there a need for differentiation? Sucht. 2013;59(3):129–42.
  63. 63. Király O, Nagygyörgy K, Koronczai B, Griffiths MD, Demetrovics Z. Assessment of problematic internet use and online video gaming. In: Aboujaoude E, Starcevic V, editors. Mental health in the digital age: Grave dangers, great promise. New York: Oxford University Press; 2015. p. 46–68.
  64. 64. Choi BC, Pak AW. A catalog of biases in questionnaires. Preventing Chronic Disease. 2005;2(1):A13. pmid:15670466
  65. 65. Maráz A, Király O, Demetrovics Z. The diagnostic pitfalls of surveys: if you score positive on a test of addiction, you still have a good chance not to be addicted. A response to Billieux et al. 2015. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2015;4(5):151–4.
  66. 66. Billieux J, Schimmenti A, Khazaal Y, Maurage P, Heeren A. Are we overpathologizing everyday life? A tenable blueprint for behavioral addiction research. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2015;4(3):119–23. pmid:26014667
  67. 67. Carbonell X, Panova T. A critical consideration of social networking sites’ addiction potential. Addiction Research and Theory. 2016 Epub ahead of print. http://doi.org/10.1080/16066359.2016.1197915
  68. 68. Ryan T, Chester A, Reece J, Xenos S. The uses and abuses of Facebook: A review of Facebook addiction. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2014; 3(3): 133–148. pmid:25317337
  69. 69. Gupta VK, Arora S, Gupta M. Computer-related illnesses and Facebook syndrome: what are they and how do we tackle them. Medicine Update. 2013;23:676–9.