Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Naturally-occurring changes in social-cognitive factors modify change in physical activity during early adolescence

Abstract

Purpose

To determine whether naturally-occurring changes in children’s motives and beliefs are associated with the steep decline in physical activity observed from childhood to early adolescence.

Methods

Latent growth modeling was applied in longitudinal tests of social-cognitive influences, and their interactions, on physical activity in a large cohort of boys and girls evaluated annually between 5th and 7th grades.

Results

Measurement equivalence of motives and beliefs was confirmed between boys and girls. After adjustment for gender and maturity differences, physical activity declined less in children who reported the least decreases in self-efficacy for overcoming barriers to activity and perceived parental support. Physical activity also declined less in students who persistently felt they had more parental and friend support for activity compared to those who reported the largest decrease in support from friends. After further adjustment for race, the decline in physical activity was less in those who had the largest decrease in perceived barriers and maintained a favorable perception of their neighborhood environment. Changes in enjoyment and social motives were unrelated to change in physical activity.

Conclusion

Using an objective measure of physical activity, we confirm that naturally-occurring changes in children’s beliefs about barriers to physical activity and their ability to overcome them, as well as perceptions of their neighborhood environment and social support, are concurrent with age-related declines in children’s physical activity. The longitudinal findings confirm these putative social-cognitive mediators as plausible, interacting targets of interventions designed to mitigate the marked decline in physical activity that occurs during the transition between elementary and middle schools.

Introduction

Physical inactivity is regarded worldwide as a public health burden during adolescence, which is a foundational period for future health [1]. Physical activity among U.S. youths is below recommended levels [2, 3], and it decreases steeply in children and youths between ages 9 to 15 years in the U.S. [4, 5] and in other nations [68]. This age-related decline in physical activity is thought to be a major contributor to the increased rates of overweight and obesity observed during this developmental period [9]. Accordingly, many authorities have recommended that public health interventions be launched to promote higher levels of physical activity in young people [10, 11]. An important goal of such interventions is to reduce the pronounced age-related decline in physical activity during the transition from childhood to early adolescence.

Interventions to increase physical activity in children and youths have achieved only modest success, on the whole, in sustaining meaningful change [1216]. The Physical Activity Guidelines for Americans Midcourse Report[12] and others [17] recommended that long-term longitudinal assessment of youth physical activity, which is the focus of the study we report here, is a high priority for future research. We measured physical activity using accelerometry, which is a preferred method for the objective measurement of physical activity in children and youths [18].

One explanation for the limited success of past physical activity interventions is that most of them did not target or alter child-level factors that are putative mediators (i.e., causal explanations) of physical activity change (e.g., children’s motives and beliefs that theoretically transmit or modify an intervention effect) [19]. This has mainly resulted from a dearth of evidence confirming that change in these presumptive determinants of physical activity in children and adolescents is, in fact, prospectively associated with measured change in physical activity [20]. Of equal importance, it is not known how these candidate mediators of physical activity interact to moderate each other’s influence on change in physical activity. Furthermore, previous studies have not examined how change in physical activity and its mediating or moderating influences by children’s beliefs and motives depend on maturity, which accelerates during early adolescence. Prior evidence suggests that earlier maturation partly accounts for lower physical activity in girls compared to boys [2124] and, hence, might obscure the relationship of physical activity with change in social-cognitive factors that can also be influenced by maturity during early adolescence [25].

In this study, we built on our prior studies of modifiable motives (i.e., social and enjoyment) and beliefs (i.e., self-efficacy and perceptions of barriers, support from parents and friends, and the perceived neighborhood environment) about physical activity among adolescent girls [2629] by observing prospective associations of these social-cognitive variables with change in objectively measured physical activity in a cohort of boys and girls as they transitioned from elementary school to middle school. We examined direct and moderated (i.e., interactions) effects of change in the variables on age-related decline in physical activity, while adjusting for maturation. Although motives and beliefs are formed in large part by social learning and self-appraisal as well as reinforcement history [30], whether they change naturally during the transition from elementary to middle school had not been determined by prior evidence. Based on theory elaborated in our prior studies [2629] and elsewhere [19], we hypothesized that changes in the social-cognitive variables, and their interactions, would be related to change in physical activity.

Methods

Study design

The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the University of South Carolina (approval #PRO00003730). The Transitions and Activity Changes in Kids (TRACK) study employed a prospective cohort design. Students and their parents were enrolled in the study when the children were 5th graders. A data collection protocol was administered when the students were in the 5th, 6th and 7th grades, thereby providing observations across a period that spanned the transition from elementary to middle school. Consistent with the Social Ecological Model [31], the overarching goal of TRACK was to identify factors ranging from intra-personal to physical environmental that influence change in children’s physical activity levels during a critical developmental period. The overall study protocol included collection of data from the children, their parents, and school personnel, as well as characterization of the physical environment of the communities in which the families resided. The analyses presented here are based on information provided by the children, and latent growth modeling procedures were used to identify changing factors that were related to changes in physical activity levels across the two-year observation period.

Participants

The multi-ethnic sample included 857 5th grade boys and girls recruited in Spring and Fall 2010 from elementary schools located in two school districts in South Carolina, as described in detail elsewhere [32]. Fourteen of the 17 elementary schools and subsequently all seven middle schools in one district and all seven elementary and all six middle schools in the other district agreed to participate. Active consent and assent forms approved by the IRB were sent home with students, and written forms completed by parents or legal guardians were returned to the schools. The race/ethnicity proportions were approximately: 36% non-Hispanic black, 37% non-Hispanic white, 9.5% Hispanic/Latino, 3% Asian/Pacific Islander, 3% American Indian, and 11% multi-racial. The samples were not randomly selected, but their gender and race/ethnicity were generally representative of the school populations. The median (low to high quartiles) percentage of students below the Federal poverty level was 14.7% (11.1% to 19.3%). Fifty-seven percent of parents had attended or graduated from college or technical school, another 11% had attended or graduated from graduate school, and seven percent had not completed high school. Body Mass Index (BMI, kg/m2) (mean ± SD) was higher in girls (p< .001); 46–49% of girls and 43–46% of boys had a BMI ≥ 85th percentile based on sex-specific BMI-for-age growth charts published by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) [33]. Table 1 presents participant characteristics according to gender.

thumbnail
Table 1. Participant characteristics according to gender.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172040.t001

Data collection procedures

Data collection procedures were carried out at each school according to a manual of procedures by a trained measurement team over two visits with each participant. During the first visit, participants completed the questionnaires by entering their responses into a survey software database on laptop computers, had anthropometric measurements taken, and received an accelerometer. Participants completed the measures in small groups (≤ 24 students), at times and places determined by each school. Participants returned the accelerometer at the second visit.

Anthropometric variables.

Height and weight were each assessed with two trials using a Seca height board and a Seca Model 880 weight scale. Standing and seated height measurements were repeated and averaged if the difference between the two measurements was ≥ 0.5 cm. Weight measurements were repeated if the difference was ≥ 0.5 kg. BMI was expressed as kg/m2 and as standardized scores (BMz) based on CDC growth charts [34]. Maturity was assessed by years from estimated peak height velocity using a longitudinally validated prediction equation for boys and girls [34, 35]. Each student responded to two questions about race/ethnicity. The first asked whether the student thought of himself or herself as Hispanic or Latino. The second asked about student’s self-identification as American Indian or Alaskan Native, Black/African American, Native Hawaiian or other Pacific Islander, White, Asian or other (e.g., multi-racial).

Socio-economic variables.

Parents reported their highest level of education (1 = attended high school, 2 = completed high school, 3 = attended college or technical school, 4 = completed college or technical school, 5 = attended graduate school, 6 = completed graduate school). Percent poverty was calculated using the US Census American Community Survey variable “Poverty status in the past 12 months” based on the Census tract of each child’s place of residence [36].

Physical activity.

Each child wore an Actigraph accelerometer (models GT1M and GT3X, Pensacola, FL) during waking hours for 7 consecutive days, except while bathing, swimming or sleeping. Accelerometer counts in the vertical plane were collected and stored in 60-sec epochs and reduced using methods previously described [36]. Total physical activity (light + moderate + vigorous physical activity (PA) was expressed as mean daily minutes per hour (min/hour) of wear time. Data for Sunday were excluded from analysis because of poor wear rates and low reliability. Eighty percent of children provided accelerometer data for eight or more hours of daily wear on four or more days, representing 77% of the total records possible on Monday through Saturday. Missing values for children with at least two days of 8 or more hours of wear each day were estimated using Proc MI in SAS (Version 9.3, SAS Institute, Inc., Cary, NC). There was a quadratic drop in wear time (mean ± SD) of 0.7 hours (95% CI = 0.5 to 0.9) from 5th grade (12.5 ± 0.9 hours) to 6th grade (11.8 ± 1.0) and 0.2 hours (95% CI = 0.18 to 0.22) to 7th grade (11.6 ± 1.0), P< .001, that differed by elementary school, P = .012, so physical activity was expressed as min/hour of wear time. Reliability across the 6 days in the samples reported here was 0.84 in 5th grade, 0.88 in 6th grade and 0.88 in 7th grade. Stability coefficients were .64, .61, and .52 between 5th and 6th grades, 6th and 7th grades and 5th and 7th grades, respectively. Physical activity data were available for a cohort of 857 children assessed in 5th grade and in either or both of 6th (N = 778) or 7th (N = 686) grades.

Social-cognitive variables.

A student questionnaire was developed for the purpose of measuring the putative mediators and moderators of change in physical activity consistent with prior validation studies of 5th grade boys and girls and 6th grade girls [37] and 6th and 8th grade girls[27, 3840]. The questionnaire with evidence for the validity of the scales is available elsewhere [32, 41]. Unless otherwise noted, a four-point ordered scale format was used. Results are reported as mean scores per item for each scale. Missing responses to items on the questionnaires ranged from 0% to 3%.

Self-efficacy.

Efficacy beliefs about overcoming barriers to physical activity was measured using 8 items developed for use with 5th grade boys and girls and re-specified for use with 6th and 8th grade girls [38, 3942].

Perceived barriers.

The 9-item version of a measure developed for the Trial of Activity in Adolescent Girls (TAAG) study was used. It is comprised of three 3-item scales for assessing obstacles, evaluation, and outcomes as barriers to physical activity that are subordinate to a single secondary factor [41].

Enjoyment motivation and social motivation for physical activity.

Two Likert-type scales assessed presumably intrinsic (i.e., Enjoyment; 7 items) and extrinsic (i.e., Social; 5 items) incentives for participation in physical activities [43], consistent with self-determination theory [44] and modified for reading level [38].

Perceived neighborhood environment.

The children were asked to respond on a 5-point Likert-type scale to 12 items derived from previously validated questionnaires.[45]

Perceived parent support.

Four items from the student survey of the Amherst Health and Activity Study [46], previously validated for use in adolescent girls, were used to assess children’s perception of support for physical activity provided by parents or guardians [42].

Perceived friend support.

Three items from the student survey of the Amherst Health and Activity Study,[46] previously validated for use in adolescent girls [27], were used to assess children’s perception of support for physical activity provided by friends.

Statistical analysis

Latent growth modeling.

Trajectories of change in physical activity and the putative mediators or moderators were estimated using latent growth modeling in Mplus 7.4 [47] with robust maximum likelihood estimation of parameters with full information imputation, which is robust with up to 25% missing data [48]. Multi-level models were used to estimate between-school differences in physical activity and the putative mediators and moderators [47]. Adjustment was made for nesting effects of students within schools by correcting the standard errors of parameter estimates for between-school variance using the Huber-White sandwich estimator, which is robust to heteroscedasticity and group-correlated responses [47]. Parameters and their standard errors were estimated for initial status (i.e., mean at 5th-grade baseline), change (i.e., slope of differences across the 3 time points from 5th to 7th grades), and the variances (i.e., inter-individual differences) of initial status and change. Critical z-scores (parameter estimate/SE) were used to test significance.

Prediction of change in physical activity was tested by including initial status and change in physical activity regressed on initial status and change in each social-cognitive mediator, while setting the regression of change in the mediator on initial status at zero (see Fig 1 for self-efficacy as an example). Regression coefficients were compared between boys and girls in a multi-group model using the Wald test. Interactions between changes in social-cognitive variables (or between change in a variable with 5th grade level in another variable that did not change) were tested using standard procedures [47, 49].

To account for effects of gender, race/ethnicity (black, Hispanic, and all others vs. white or vs. black), parental education, and poverty level on change, those variables were added to the LGM models as covariates. Maturity was tested as a time-varying covariate. Growth models (initial values in 5th grade and change between 5th and 7th grades) were otherwise compared between boys and girls in a multi-group model using on the Wald test. The longitudinal measurement equivalence/ invariance (MEI) of the model for each scale was examined between boys and girls by testing the equality of item variance and covariance between boys and girls across the three measurement periods [50].

Model fit was evaluated with multiple indices. The chi-square statistic assessed absolute fit of the model to the data. However, the test is almost always significant when samples approximate or exceed 400 cases, so other fit indices were also used, as recommended elsewhere [51]. Values of the Comparative Fit Index (CFI) ≥ 0.90 and 0.95 were used to indicate acceptable and good fit. Values ≤0.06 of the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) were used to represent close fit. Values ≥ 0.96 for CFI in combination with values of the SRMR ≤ 0.10 results in the least sum of type I and type II error rates [51]. The sample size was adequate for model tests. Statistical power exceeded .90 at an alpha of .05 for rejecting good fit at a RMSEA of .06 and a conservative estimate of model complexity at 10 df [52, 53]. Model fit was acceptable for all growth models reported here (CFI ≥ 0.960, RMSEA ≤ 0.051, SRMR ≤ .052).

Results

Between-school variance

Multi-level analysis indicated small and non-significant variance between schools (intra-class correlation coefficient; ICC) in physical activity in 5th (.058, p = .076), 6th (.025, p = .233), and 7th (.027, p = .140) grades. Between-school variance in the social-cognitive variables was also small (ICC ≤ .048) and was significant only for perceived environment in 6th grade (p = .049). Between-school variance in maturity was similarly small (ICC ≤ .045) and reached significance only in 5th grade (p = .019). Hence, tests of cross-level influences of school features on child-level associations between those variables were not conducted. Nonetheless, elementary school was retained as a cluster variable in all models to provide precise and conservative parameter estimates to adjust for nesting effects within schools [47].

Measurement equivalence/invariance

Longitudinal tests of equal variance and covariance of scale items between boys and girls across the three years between 5th and 7th grades supported MEI for all social-cognitive scales (CFI ≥ 0.955, RMSEA ≤ 0.071, SRMR ≤.100). Table 2 shows tests and fit indices for each measurement scale.

thumbnail
Table 2. Longitudinal tests of measurement equivalence/invariance (equal variance and covariance) between boys and girls across 5th through 7th grades.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172040.t002

Growth models

Physical activity.

Boys and girls differed on initial values and change trajectories (p < .001). See Fig 2. Mean physical activity in the 5th grade was 29.3 min/hour (95% CI = 28.4 to 30.2) in boys and 27.37 min/hour (95% CI = 26.8 to 27.9) in girls. There was a decline from 5th grade to 6th grade for boys (-3.31 min/hour (95% CI = -4.14 to -2.47, P < .001)) and girls (-4.39 min/hour (95% CI = -4.99 to -3.79) P < .001)). Thereafter, negatively accelerated declines were observed between 6th and 7th grades of -1.61 min/hour (95% CI = -1.84 to -1.38) for boys and -1.68 min/hour (95% CI = -1.82 to -1.54) for girls. Based on wear time, physical activity dropped 10 min/day (95% CI = -11.2 to -8.8) for boys and 11.8 min/day (95% CI = -13.2 to -10.4) for girls between 5th to 6th grades; drops of 16% and 21%.

Maturity.

Boys and girls also differed on initial peak height velocity offset (< .001) and change trajectories (p < .002). Mean peak height velocity offset in the 5th grade was -2.38 years (95% CI = -2.46 to -2.29) in boys and -0.56 years (95% CI = -0.67 to -0.44) in girls. The decrease was 0.69 (95% CI = 0.64 to 0.74) years in boys and 0.75 (95% CI = 0.71 to 0.79) years in girls between 5th and 6th grades and 1.26 (95% CI = 0.90 to 1.62) years in boys and 0.87 (95% CI = 0.71 to 1.02) years in girls between 6th and 7th grades.

Physical activity levels in 5th grade no longer differed significantly between boys and girls (P = .770) after adjustment for maturity. Adjusted physical activity level (± 95% CI) was 26.80 (24.77 to 28.76) min/hour in boys and 27.06 (26.48 to 27.63) min/hour in girls. See Fig 2. Similarly, the difference in physical activity decline between 5th and 7th grades also was no longer significantly different after adjusting for maturity each year (Wald test (1) = 3.2, P = .073). The adjusted decline was -2.19 (-3.75 to -0.62) min/hour in boys and -3.84 (-4.44 to -.3.23) min/hour in girls between 5th and 6th grades and -0.26 min/hour (-0.97 to 0.45) in boys and -1.76 minutes (-1.96 to -1.56) in girls between 6th and 7th grades. The decline in physical activity was less in black boys and girls than other children (p< .019) but was unrelated to parental education or poverty (p≥ .912).

BMI.

Boys and girls also differed on initial BMI (p = .007) and change trajectories (p < .002). Mean BMI (kg/m2) in the 5th grade was 20.65 (95% CI = 20.14 to -21.16) in boys and 21.55 (95% CI = 21.04 to 22.06) in girls. The increase in BMI (kg/m2) per year was 0.63 (95% CI = 0.52 to 0.75) in boys and 0.83 (95% CI = 0.72 to 0.94) in girls between 5th and 7th grades. The differences between boys and girls in physical activity at 5th grade and change in physical activity between 5th and 7th grades remained (P-values < .005) after adjustment for BMI each year.

Boys and girls no longer differed significantly on BMI or BMIz in 5th grade (P-values .104) or on change in BMI or BMIz (P-values .125) after adjustment for maturity each year, so further results for physical activity and the social-cognitive variables were adjusted for maturity each year rather than BMI. Gender was retained with maturity in the reported models in order to improve the precision of parameter estimates. Growth models were not substantively different after further adjustment for race/ethnicity and the socio-economic variables, unless otherwise noted.

Social-cognitive variables.

There were declines (SD, P-value) each year in all social-cognitive variables: self-efficacy (- 0.41, P = .002); perceived barriers (- 0.47, P = .004); enjoyment motivation (- 0.24, P = .004); social motivation (- 0.22, P = .021); perceived neighborhood environment (- 0.17, P = .006); perceived parental support (-.19, P = .027); and perceived friend support (- 0.26, P< .001). Growth model results according to gender are shown in Table 3. Boys and girls differed on initial values (P ≤ = .05) for all variables except perceived neighborhood environment. Girls had a bigger decline in enjoyment motivation, while boys had a bigger decline in friend support. Only social motivation differed between boys and girls, after adjustment for maturity each year. Boys had higher adjusted levels in 5th grade (2.97, 95% CI = 2.81 to 3.13) and greater decline each year (-0.11, 95% CI = -0.20 to -0.02) than did girls (2.47, 95% CI = 2.38 to 2.55) and (0.049, 95% CI = -0.20 to 0.12).

thumbnail
Table 3. Growth models for social-cognitive variables according to gender.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172040.t003

After adjustment for maturity each year and gender, the decline in physical activity was related to decline in self-efficacy (β = .268, 95% CI .012 to .524, P = .041) and perceived neighborhood environment (β = .200, 95% CI .018 to .383, P = .031) but not related to declines in enjoyment motivation, P = .130; social motivation, P = .324; perceived parental support, P = .959; perceived friend support, P = .102; or perceived barriers, P = .136. After further adjustment for race, the decline in physical activity was inversely related to decline in perceived barriers (β = -.237, 95% CI -.414 to -.060, P = .009).

Moderated (i.e., interaction) effects

Self-efficacy with perceived parental support.

After adjustment for maturity each year and gender, there was an interaction effect between change in self-efficacy and change in perceived parental support on decline in physical activity (z-value = 6.1, p < .001, R2 = .54). Physical activity declined least in students who had less of a decline in self-efficacy and less of a decline in parental support compared to other students (Fig 3).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Interaction of change in self-efficacy with change in perceived parental support.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172040.g003

Perceived neighborhood environment with perceived barriers.

After adjustment for maturity each year, gender and race, there was an interaction effect between change in the perceived environment and change in perceived barriers on decline in physical activity after further adjustment for race (z-value = 2.21, p = .027, R2 = .47). Physical activity declined least in students who maintained more favorable perceptions of their neighborhood environment and had the largest decline in barriers compared to other students (Fig 4). The effect was no longer significant after further adjustment for parental education and poverty (P = .828).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Interaction of change in perceived neighborhood environment with change in perceived barriers.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172040.g004

Perceived parental support with perceived friend support.

After adjustment for maturity each year and gender, there was an interaction effect between change in perceived parental support and change in perceived friend support on decline in physical activity (z-value = 3.12, p = .002, R2 = .53). Physical activity declined least in students who maintained the highest perceptions of both parental and friend support compared to students who had the greatest decline in friend support regardless of parental support (Fig 5).

thumbnail
Fig 5. Interaction of change in perceived parental support with change in perceived friend support.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172040.g005

Discussion

Naturally-occurring changes in children’s beliefs about barriers to physical activity and their self-efficacy for overcoming them, as well as perceptions of their neighborhood environment and social support from parents and friends, correlated with the age-related decline in children’s physical activity during the transition from childhood to early adolescence. The correlations were direct only for self-efficacy, perceived barriers, and perceived neighborhood environment. Moderating interactions between changes in the variables better accounted for change in physical activity. Physical activity declined least in students who had less of a decline in self-efficacy for overcoming barriers to activity concurrent with less of a decline in perceived parental support. Physical activity also declined least in students who maintained more favorable perceptions of their neighborhood environment and had the largest decline in perceived barriers, as well as less in students who persistently perceived more parental support or friend support for physical activity, compared to those who reported the largest decrease in perceived support from friends. In contrast to our prior finding that enjoyment motivation was related to physical activity in 5th and 6th grades [38], change in enjoyment motivation, like change in social motivation, were unrelated to change in physical activity during the transition from elementary school to middle school.

The average daily decline in total physical activity of ten minutes for boys (16%) and twelve minutes for girls (21%) seen here between 5th and 6th grades is consistent with, though a little less than, the 25% age-related decline in moderate-to-vigorous physical activity observed for boys and girls between ages 11 and 12 years in the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development cohort study [4]. Here and there, the declines could account for a substantial part of the large drop for US children in the rate of sufficient physical activity (from near 50% to just 12%) during their transition to early adolescence [5]. Here, however, interactions between changes in social-cognitive variables (self-efficacy with perceived parental support; perceived neighborhood with perceived barriers; perceived parental support with perceived friend support) had strong effects (approximating 1.5 to 2.5 SD), which accounted for about half the decline in physical activity.

The results are new, because they confirm that changes in these social-cognitive influences on physical activity, which were known to have cross-sectional or predictive associations with physical activity measured mostly by self-report in children and youths [20], are in fact prospectively associated with objectively measured change in physical activity. The results here for self-efficacy and perceived support from parents and friends, based on changes observed across two years, extend prior longitudinal evidence that showed initial levels on similar constructs were predictive of change in physical activity measured by accelerometry among boys and girls across one year from age 9 to 10 years [54]. The findings extend the evidence from our prior studies of modifiable motives (i.e., social and enjoyment) and beliefs (i.e., self-efficacy and perceived barriers, support from parents and friends, and the neighborhood environment) about physical activity among adolescent girls [2629].

Strengths of the study are the use of an objective measure of physical activity and the repeated observations in a large cohort of boys and girls followed for two years, from the 5th grade through the 7th grade. An additional strength of the study is the application of latent growth modeling, which uses each student’s trajectory of change to estimate the typical change across students in physical activity and the social-cognitive determinants, as well as the variance of those changes, while also adjusting for initial values observed in the 5th grade. This approach permits a fuller test of correlated changes across time than prior longitudinal approaches, which may have failed to detect significant associations among similar variables when analysis was limited to less precise estimates of change [27, 55]. A novel aspect of the growth modeling was the adjustment for maturational differences between boys and girls [21, 25]. The measure of biological maturation was limited to an estimate based on stature, which is nonetheless a preferred method that is practicable in large cohort studies [24, 56].

Novel findings of the study are the moderating effects of concurrent changes in social-cognitive factors, which interacted to influence change in physical activity. Prior observational studies that used a longitudinal design to test social-cognitive theories of adolescent physical activity relied on a self-report of physical activity and did not test whether changes in social-cognitive variables interacted [57]. The absence or presence of main effects on physical activity in those studies might have obscured more complex, moderated (i.e., interactive) effects on physical activity among key social-cognitive variables. Here, for example, changes in perceived support from parents or friends were not directly related to change in physical activity, but change in perceived parental support interacted with changes in self-efficacy and perceived friend support to influence change in physical activity. Less than 20% of about 100 published observational studies on social support by parents or friends of youths’ physical activity used a longitudinal design [58, 59], and just a handful assessed the association of change in support with change in physical activity, measured by self-report or accelerometry [58]. Those studies of change reported varying positive and null effects, but they collectively did not examine whether change in social support had indirect effects on physical activity by mediating or moderating (i.e., interacting with) the effects of other social-cognitive factors [26]. Involving parents in interventions based on social-cognitive theories has typically had little or no effect on children’s physical activity [60], but to our knowledge, tests of moderators of physical activity change after intervention have been limited mainly to stable characteristics of students or their environment not practically amenable to change by intervention [61, 62].

The findings also extend evidence for the longitudinal measurement equivalence/ invariance of the social-cognitive measures, previously validated in 6th and 8th grade girls, to 5th graders and to boys from the 5th to 7th grades. The scales can be tested in other large samples of boys and girls and used to measure change or test the variables’ putative roles as mediators or moderators of physical activity change in interventions during middle school. Results were similar for boys and girls. Findings were not influenced by race/ethnicity, except for the influence on physical activity change by the interaction of perceived environment with perceived barriers. Sample size was not big enough to statistically compare the measurement equivalence of change and relationships with change according to race or ethnic group.

Multi-level intra-class correlations indicated that the variation between schools in both physical activity and the social-cognitive factors was insufficient to test cross-level effects between school features and student variables. Nonetheless, errors of the parameter estimates were properly adjusted to account for nesting of students within schools. The findings are limited to students attending two school districts in South Carolina, so it is not known how well they generalize to other parts of the nation or to similarly aged children educated in private schools or alternative settings such as home-schooling or online education, where social influences on physical activity may differ. Except for extinguishing the moderator effect of the interaction between perceived neighborhood environment and perceived barriers, results were not influenced by socio-economic variables. However, the socio-economic measures were limited to parents’ reports of education level and the percentage of families at or below poverty level based on US census tract data.

The ages studied represent a key period of biological maturation when growth is accelerated. Past studies of the association between biological maturity and physical activity have yielded conflicting results but suggested that the lower physical activity observed among early-maturing girls is explainable by increasing BMI [24]. The results we report here indicate that biological maturity indexed by the growth spurt are inversely related to physical activity and explain higher physical activity among boys in the 5th grade and part of the steeper decline in physical activity among girls from the 5th to the 6th grade, after which most boys have caught up to girls in maturity. In the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children birth cohort study, maturity (similarly defined as percentage of predicted adult stature) was inversely related to physical activity measured by an accelerometer at age 11 years, but maturity at age 11 did not predict physical activity at age 13 years in either sex [56]. However, the time-varying influence of maturity was not modeled each year in that study, as was done here. Additional longitudinal research is needed to examine how biological maturation influences physical activity [21], as well as whether it determines or modifies youths’ motivation for physical activity within the changing social context of middle school [24, 25].

In conclusion, these findings indicate that modifiable beliefs (i.e., self-efficacy and perceptions of barriers, perceived support from parents and friends, and the perceived neighborhood environment) about physical activity are plausible, interacting targets of interventions designed to mitigate the marked decline in physical activity that occurs during the transition between elementary and middle schools.

Acknowledgments

This study was funded by grant number R01HL091002 from the National Heart, Lung and Blood Institute. The authors thank Gaye Groover Christmus, MPH for editorial assistance in the preparation of the manuscript.

Data are available from the Dryad Digital Repository http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.6g7j6

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: RKD RRP RPS.
  2. Data curation: MD KLM.
  3. Formal analysis: RKD.
  4. Funding acquisition: RRP RKD.
  5. Investigation: KLM.
  6. Methodology: RKD.
  7. Project administration: RRP KLM.
  8. Supervision: RRP RKD.
  9. Validation: RKD MD.
  10. Visualization: RKD.
  11. Writing – original draft: RKD.
  12. Writing – review & editing: RKD RRP MD RPS KLM.

References

  1. 1. Sawyer SM, Afifi RA, Bearinger LH, Blakemore SJ, Dick B, Ezeh AC, et al. Adolescence: a foundation for future health. Lancet. 2012;379(9826):1630–40. pmid:22538178
  2. 2. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. 2008 Physical Activity Guidelines for Americans. USDepartment of Health and Human Services [Internet]. 2008. Available from: http://www.health.gov/paguidelines/
  3. 3. Strong WB, Malina RM, Blimkie CJ, Daniels SR, Dishman RK, Gutin B, et al. Evidence based physical activity for school-age youth. J Pediatr. 2005;146(6):732–7. pmid:15973308
  4. 4. Nader PR, Bradley RH, Houts RM, McRitchie SL, O'Brien M. Moderate-to-vigorous physical activity from ages 9 to 15 years. JAMA. 2008;300(3):295–305. pmid:18632544
  5. 5. Troiano RP, Berrigan D, Dodd KW, Masse LC, Tilert T, McDowell M. Physical activity in the United States measured by accelerometer. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2008;40(1):181–8. pmid:18091006
  6. 6. Corder K, Sharp SJ, Atkin AJ, Griffin SJ, Jones AP, Ekelund U, et al. Change in objectively measured physical activity during the transition to adolescence. Br J Sports Med. 2015;49(11):730–6. PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4453714. pmid:24273308
  7. 7. Dumith SC, Gigante DP, Domingues MR, Hallal PC, Menezes AM, Kohl HW III. A longitudinal evaluation of physical activity in Brazilian adolescents: tracking, change and predictors. PediatrExercSci. 2012;24(1):58–71.
  8. 8. Kwon S, Janz KF, International Children's Accelerometry Database C. Tracking of accelerometry-measured physical activity during childhood: ICAD pooled analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2012;9:68. PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3447713. pmid:22676230
  9. 9. Ramires VV, Dumith SC, Goncalves H. Longitudinal association between physical activity and body fat during adolescence: A systematic review. J Phys Act Health. 2015;12(9):1344–58. pmid:25409296
  10. 10. Pate RR. An inside view of the U.S. National Physical Activity Plan. J Phys Act Health. 2014;11(3):461–2. pmid:24714331
  11. 11. Kraus WE, Bittner V, Appel L, Blair SN, Church T, Despres JP, et al. The National Physical Activity Plan: a call to action from the American Heart Association: a science advisory from the American Heart Association. Circulation. 2015;131(21):1932–40. pmid:25918126
  12. 12. Physical Activity Guidelines for Americans Midcourse Report Subcommittee, President's Council on Fitness Sports and Nutrition. Physical Activity Guidelines for Americans Midcourse Report: Strategies to Increase Physical Activity Among Youth. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services 2012.
  13. 13. Kriemler S, Meyer U, Martin E, van Sluijs EM, Andersen LB, Martin BW. Effect of school-based interventions on physical activity and fitness in children and adolescents: a review of reviews and systematic update. BrJ Sports Med. 2011;45(11):923–30.
  14. 14. Metcalf B, Henley W, Wilkin T. Effectiveness of intervention on physical activity of children: Systematic review and meta-analysis of controlled trials with objectively measured outcomes (EarlyBird 54). BMJ. 2012;345:e5888. pmid:23044984
  15. 15. O'Connor TM, Jago R, Baranowski T. Engaging parents to increase youth physical activity: A systematic review. Am J Prev Med. 2009;37(2):141–9. pmid:19589450
  16. 16. van Sluijs EM, Kriemler S, McMinn AM. The effect of community and family interventions on young people's physical activity levels: a review of reviews and updated systematic review. Br J Sports Med. 2011;45(11):914–22. PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3736309. pmid:21836175
  17. 17. Atkin AJ, van Sluijs EM, Dollman J, Taylor WC, Stanley RM. Identifying correlates and determinants of physical activity in youth: How can we advance the field? Prev Med. 2016;87:167–9. PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4893019. pmid:26940254
  18. 18. Strath SJ, Pfeiffer KA, Whitt-Glover MC. Accelerometer use with children, older adults, and adults with functional limitations. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise. 2012;44:S77–S85. pmid:22157778
  19. 19. Lubans DR, Foster C, Biddle SJ. A review of mediators of behavior in interventions to promote physical activity among children and adolescents. Prev Med. 2008;47(5):463–70. pmid:18708086
  20. 20. Craggs C, Corder K, van Sluijs EM, Griffin SJ. Determinants of change in physical activity in children and adolescents: A systematic review. Am J PrevMed. 2011;40(6):645–58.
  21. 21. Cairney J, Veldhuizen S, Kwan M, Hay J, Faught BE. Biological age and sex-related declines in physical activity during adolescence. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2014;46(4):730–5. pmid:24056271
  22. 22. Machado Rodrigues AM, Coelho e Silva MJ, Mota J, Cumming SP, Sherar LB, Neville H, et al. Confounding effect of biologic maturation on sex differences in physical activity and sedentary behavior in adolescents. Pediatr Exerc Sci. 2010;22(3):442–53. pmid:20814039
  23. 23. Metcalf BS, Hosking J, Jeffery AN, Henley WE, Wilkin TJ. Exploring the adolescent fall in physical activity: A 10-year cohort study (EarlyBird 41). Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2015;47(10):2084–92. pmid:25706294
  24. 24. Sherar LB, Cumming SP, Eisenmann JC, Baxter-Jones AD, Malina RM. Adolescent biological maturity and physical activity: biology meets behavior. Pediatr Exerc Sci. 2010;22(3):332–49. pmid:20814031
  25. 25. Labbrozzi D, Robazza C, Bertollo M, Bucci I, Bortoli L. Pubertal development, physical self-perception, and motivation toward physical activity in girls. J Adolesc. 2013;36(4):759–65. pmid:23849670
  26. 26. Dishman RK, Saunders RP, Motl RW, Dowda M, Pate RR. Self-efficacy moderates the relation between declines in physical activity and perceived social support in high school girls. J Pediatr Psychol. 2009;34(4):441–51. PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2671981. pmid:18812410
  27. 27. Dishman RK, Dunn AL, Sallis JF, Vandenberg RJ, Pratt CA. Social-cognitive correlates of physical activity in a multi-ethnic cohort of middle-school girls: Two-year prospective study. J Pediatr Psychol. 2010;35(2):188–98. PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2902830. pmid:19468040
  28. 28. Dowda M, Dishman RK, Pfeiffer KA, Pate RR. Family support for physical activity in girls from 8th to 12th grade in South Carolina. Prev Med. 2007;44(2):153–9. pmid:17157371
  29. 29. Dowda M, Dishman RK, Porter D, Saunders RP, Pate RR. Commercial facilities, social cognitive variables, and physical activity of 12th grade girls. Ann Behav Med. 2009;37(1):77–87. pmid:19229664
  30. 30. Bandura A. 1986. Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice- Hall, pp 1–607.
  31. 31. Elder JP, Lytle L, Sallis JF, Young DR, Steckler A, Simons-Morton D, Stone E, Jobe JB, Stevens J, Lohman T, Webber L, Pate R, Saksvig BI, Ribisl K. A description of the social-ecological framework used in the trial of activity for adolescent girls (TAAG). Health Educ Res. 2007;22(2):155–65 pmid:16855014
  32. 32. Dishman RK, Saunders RP, McIver KL, Dowda M, Pate RR. Construct validity of selected measures of physical activity beliefs and motives in fifth and sixth grade boys and girls. J Pediatr Psychol. 2013;38(5):563–76. PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3716273. pmid:23459310
  33. 33. Kuczmarski RJ, Ogden CL, Grummer-Strawn LM, Flegal KM, Guo SS, Wei R, et al. CDC Growth Charts: United States. Adv Data. 2000;(314):1–27. pmid:11183293
  34. 34. Malina RM, Koziel SM. Validation of maturity offset in a longitudinal sample of Polish boys. J Sports Sci. 2014;32(5):424–37. pmid:24016098
  35. 35. Mirwald RL, Baxter-Jones AD, Bailey DA, Beunen GP. An assessment of maturity from anthropometric measurements. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2002;34(4):689–94. pmid:11932580
  36. 36. American Community Survey. 2006–2010 American Community Survey Selected Population Tables: Poverty Status in the Past 12 Months by Sex and Age. Washington, DC: US Census Bureau, 2010.
  37. 37. Catellier DJ, Hannan PJ, Murray DM, Addy CL, Conway TL, Yang S, et al. Imputation of missing data when measuring physical activity by accelerometry. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2005;37((11 Suppl)):S555–S62.
  38. 38. Dishman RK, Motl RW, Saunders RP, Felton G, Ward DS, Dowda M, et al. Enjoyment mediates effects of a school-based physical activity intervention. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2005;37(3):478–87. pmid:15741848
  39. 39. Saunders RP, Pate RR, Felton GM, Dowda M, Weinrich MC, Ward DS, et al. Development of questionnaires to measure psychosocial influences on children's physical activity. Prev Med. 1997;26:241–47. pmid:9085394
  40. 40. Dishman RK, Motl RW, Saunders RP, Dowda M, Felton G, Ward DS, et al. Factorial invariance and latent mean structure of questionnaires measuring social-cognitive determinants of physical activity among black and white adolescent girls. Prev Med. 2002;34(1):100–8. pmid:11749102
  41. 41. Dishman RK, Hales DP, Sallis JF, Saunders R, Dunn AL, Bedimo-Rung AL, et al. Validity of social-cognitive measures for physical activity in middle-school girls. J Pediatr Psychol. 2010;35(1):72–88. PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2910934. pmid:19433571
  42. 42. Motl RW, Dishman RK, Trost SG, Saunders R, Dowda M, Felton G, et al. Factorial validity and invariance of questionnaires measuring social-cognitive determinants of physical activity among adolescent girls. Prev Med. 2000;31:584–94. pmid:11071840
  43. 43. Ryan RM, Frederick CM, Lepes D, Rubio N, Sheldon KM. Intrinsic motivation and exercise adherence. International Journal of Sport Psychology. 1997;28(4):335–54.
  44. 44. Ryan RM, Deci EL. Active human nature: Self-determination theory and the promotion and maintenance of sport, exercise, and health. In: Hagger MS, Chatzisarantis NLD, editors. Intrinsic Motivation and Self-determination in Exercise and Sport. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics; 2007. p. 1–19.
  45. 45. Evenson KR, Birnbaum AS, Bedimo-Rung AL, Sallis JF, Voorhees CC, Ring K, et al. Girls' perception of physical environmental factors and transportation: Reliability and association with physical activity and active transport to school. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2006;3:28. pmid:16972999
  46. 46. Sallis JF, Taylor WC, Dowda M, Freedson PS, Pate RR. Correlates of vigorous physical activity for children in grades 1 through 12: Comparing parent-reported and objectively measured physical activity. Pediatr Exerc Sci. 2002;14(1):30–44.
  47. 47. Muthen LK, Muthen BO. Mplus: Statistical Analysis with Latent Variables (edition 7.0). Los Angeles: Muthen and Muthen; 2012 2006.
  48. 48. Enders CK, Bandalos DL. The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling. 2001;8:430–57.
  49. 49. Aiken LS, West SG. Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 1991.
  50. 50. Meredith W, Teresi JA. An essay on measurement and factorial invariance. Med Care. 2006;44(11):S69–S77.
  51. 51. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indices in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling. 1999;6:1–55.
  52. 52. MacCallum RC, Browne MW, Sugawara HM. Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychol Methods. 1996;1:130–49.
  53. 53. MacCallum RC, Browne MW, Cai L. Testing differences between nested covariance structure models: Power analysis and null hypotheses. Psychol Methods. 2006;11(1):19–35. pmid:16594765
  54. 54. Corder K, Craggs C, Jones AP, Ekelund U, Griffin SJ, van Sluijs EM. Predictors of change differ for moderate and vigorous intensity physical activity and for weekdays and weekends: a longitudinal analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2013;10:69. PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3672092. pmid:23714688
  55. 55. Hearst MO, Patnode CD, Sirard JR, Farbakhsh K, Lytle LA. Multilevel predictors of adolescent physical activity: A longitudinal analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2012;9:8. pmid:22309949
  56. 56. Cumming SP, Sherar LB, Esliger DW, Riddoch CJ, Malina RM. Concurrent and prospective associations among biological maturation, and physical activity at 11 and 13 years of age. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2014;24(1):e20–8. pmid:24102894
  57. 57. Plotnikoff RC, Costigan SA, Karunamuni N, Lubans DR. Social cognitive theories used to explain physical activity behavior in adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Prev Med. 2013;56:245–53. pmid:23370047
  58. 58. Laird Y, Fawkner S, Kelly P, McNamee L, Niven A. The role of social support on physical activity behaviour in adolescent girls: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2016;13:79. pmid:27387328
  59. 59. Yao CA, Rhodes RE. Parental correlates in child and adolescent physical activity: a meta-analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2015;11;12:10.
  60. 60. O'Connor TM, Jago R, Baranowski T. Engaging parents to increase youth physical activity a systematic review. Am J Prev Med. 2009 Aug;37(2):141–9. pmid:19589450
  61. 61. Kremers SP, de Bruijn GJ, Droomers M, van LF, Brug J. Moderators of environmental intervention effects on diet and activity in youth. Am J Prev Med. 2007;32(2):163–72. pmid:17197152
  62. 62. Prins RG, van Empelen P, Te Velde SJ, Timperio A, van Lenthe FJ, Tak NI, Crawford D, Brug J, Oenema A. Availability of sports facilities as moderator of the intention-sports participation relationship among adolescents. Health Educ Res. 2010; 25:489–97. pmid:20382675