Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Loneliness is adversely associated with physical and mental health and lifestyle factors: Results from a Swiss national survey

  • Aline Richard,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Prevention Institute, Division of Chronic Disease Epidemiology, University of Zurich, Hirschengraben 84, Zurich, Switzerland

  • Sabine Rohrmann ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    sabine.rohrmann@uzh.ch

    Affiliation Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Prevention Institute, Division of Chronic Disease Epidemiology, University of Zurich, Hirschengraben 84, Zurich, Switzerland

  • Caroline L. Vandeleur,

    Roles Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Centre for Research in Psychiatric Epidemiology and Psychopathology, Department of Psychiatry, University Hospital of Lausanne, Site de Cery, Prilly, Switzerland

  • Margareta Schmid,

    Roles Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Prevention Institute, Division of Chronic Disease Epidemiology, University of Zurich, Hirschengraben 84, Zurich, Switzerland

  • Jürgen Barth,

    Roles Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Institute for Complementary and Integrative Medicine, University Hospital and University of Zurich, Sonneggstr. 6, Zurich, Switzerland

  • Monika Eichholzer

    Roles Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Prevention Institute, Division of Chronic Disease Epidemiology, University of Zurich, Hirschengraben 84, Zurich, Switzerland

Abstract

Introduction

Loneliness is a common, emotionally distressing experience and is associated with adverse physical and mental health and an unhealthy lifestyle. Nevertheless, little is known about the prevalence of loneliness in different age groups in Switzerland. Furthermore, the existing evidence about age and gender as potential effect modifiers of the associations between loneliness, physical and mental health and lifestyle characteristics warrants further investigation. Thus, the aim of the study was to examine the prevalence of loneliness among adults in Switzerland and to assess the associations of loneliness with several physical and mental health and behavioral factors, as well as to assess the modifying effect of sex and age.

Methods

Data from 20,007 participants of the cross-sectional population-based Swiss Health Survey 2012 (SHS) were analyzed. Logistic regression analyses were used to assess associations of loneliness with physical and mental health or lifestyle characteristics (e.g. diabetes, depression, physical activity). Wald tests were used to test for interactions.

Results

Loneliness was distributed in a slight U-shaped form from 15 to 75+ year olds, with 64.1% of participants who had never felt lonely. Lonely individuals were more often affected by physical and mental health problems, such as self-reported chronic diseases (Odds ratio [OR] 1.41, 95% confidence interval [CI] 1.30–1.54), high cholesterol levels (OR 1.31, 95% CI 1.18–1.45), diabetes (OR 1.40, 95% CI 1.16–1.67), moderate and high psychological distress (OR 3.74, 95% CI 3.37–4.16), depression (OR 2.78, 95% CI 2.22–3.48) and impaired self-perceived health (OR 1.94, 95% CI 1.74–2.16). Loneliness was significantly associated with most lifestyle factors (e.g. smoking; OR 1.13, 95% 1.05–1.23). Age, but not sex, moderated loneliness’ association with several variables.

Conclusion

Loneliness is associated with poorer physical and mental health and unhealthy lifestyle, modified by age, but not by sex. Our findings illustrate the importance of considering loneliness for physical and mental health and lifestyle factors, not only in older and younger, but also in middle-aged adults. Longitudinal studies are needed in Switzerland to elucidate the causal relationships of these associations.

Introduction

Throughout decades loneliness has commonly been described by philosophers, psychologists and in the literature as ubiquitous and even as an essence of human being [1]. Loneliness is defined as an emotional distress experience that goes along with the perception of unsatisfying social relationships. Perceived unsatisfying relationships, thus, are independent of the quantity of social interactions, but are caused by the felt sense of social isolation and unsatisfied need for affection in current relationships [2, 3]. Nevertheless, a low quantity and diminished meaning of social contacts has also been related to loneliness.

Sociodemographic characteristics are partly associated with loneliness. In general women seem to report loneliness more frequently than men [46], although the gender differences may disappear when controlling for other factors such as widowhood, depression, mobility problems, age, education, and social contacts [7]. Concerning age, some authors have described a U-shaped prevalence with more lonely subjects in younger and older adults than in middle-aged subjects [3, 8, 9]. However, other authors suggested that loneliness is more common in older age [1013]. Luhmann and Hawkley [14] observed an even more complex association in their nationally representative German sample with an age range from 18 to 103 years. Loneliness levels (measured by a short version of the UCLA Loneliness Scale [15]) showed two peaks around ages 30 and 60 and two dips around ages 40 and 75. There was a general downward trend in loneliness in young and middle-aged adults. The lowest level was found around age 75 after which the levels rose continuously into old age. Overall, most studies focused on older age [7, 1622], children [23], adolescents and young adults [14, 24], and less is known about the prevalence of loneliness in middle-aged adults [14, 25, 26].

Other sociodemographic factors have been suggested to be risk factors for loneliness, such as being single and/or living alone [8, 25, 27], low educational or low-income levels [16, 28, 29], immigration status, especially immigration in women [30], as well as low social support [31]. In contrast, the fact of living in an urban or rural area as associated with loneliness has yielded contradictory results [16, 28, 32].

Loneliness can be a transient state, but more importantly, it can also be a long-lasting perception [9] with negative health and behavioral outcomes [3335]. Loneliness is associated with increased morbidity and mortality risk [19, 36]. There is increasing evidence that lonely individuals feel less healthy [37], have a higher risk for hypertension [38], hypercholesterolemia, the metabolic syndrome [19], and coronary heart disease [39]. Furthermore, loneliness has been related to mental health problems, such as psychosis [40, 41], suicide [42], and depression [3, 43]. Loneliness is associated with a lower capacity for self-regulation [3]. As a consequence, lower self-regulation goes along with a worsened lifestyle: Lonely individuals are less physically active [44], are more often affected by alcohol abuse [45] and are more often obese [46] than non-lonely persons.

In 2011, a study compared loneliness in 25 European countries and a north-east gradient for self-reported loneliness was observed [13]. In Northern Europe, including Switzerland, loneliness was under 6%, in contrast to higher prevalences in Russia and Eastern European (about 10–34% for different age groups). In Switzerland, an updated population-based estimate of loneliness frequencies is nevertheless necessary.

In relation to studies evaluating the associations of loneliness with health factors, many of them are of North American origin, but there are also a number of studies which were carried out in Asia, Europe, and New Zealand [3, 35]. In contrast, in Switzerland, the evidence for the associations of loneliness with health and lifestyle factors is very limited [47]. In contrast to several other countries, a rather wealthy country with a universal health care system. Thus, associations between loneliness and health outcomes may differ from other Western countries with different health care systems such as the US. Furthermore, in regard to implement prevention programs in Switzerland, which aim to address loneliness, it is of importance to provide data also for Switzerland. Against this background, we aimed to determine the prevalence of loneliness in a large representative sample of the Swiss population aged 15 years and older, to evaluate loneliness and its associations with health and lifestyle outcomes, with a special emphasis on sex and age as modifiers of these associations.

Methods

Study population and data

Data were obtained from the Swiss Health Survey (SHS) conducted in 2012/2013 by the Swiss Federal Bureau of Statistics (SFSO) (Legal basis: Ordinance of the Conduct of Federal Statistical Surveys of 30 June 1993). This cross-sectional, population-based nationwide survey focuses on health status, several lifestyle and demographic factors, and healthcare use and has been carried out every five years since 1992. It uses a stratified random sampling technique based on registries of inhabitants, and with the cantons of Switzerland as strata. Out of an initial sample of 41,008 individuals a total number of 21,597 individuals aged 15 years or older and living in a private household agreed to participate in the 2012 SHS (i.e. participation rate 54%). They participated in a computer-assisted telephone interview (CATI). In a next step, a written questionnaire was provided (paper or online) upon approval from the participants (n = 18,357). The written questionnaire was an extension of the basic telephone interview, with the aim to gather more detailed information on some topics, such as dental care, clinical depression, etc. The representativeness of the Swiss population was ensured by the multistage probability sampling and the appropriate weighting factors provided by the SFSO. The sampling weights provided by the SFSO were used which allows a comparison with the permanent 2012 Swiss population regarding sex, age, geographic region and nationality (Swiss vs. others).

From the telephone interview, 20,841 individuals had information available on loneliness. After excluding individuals with missing information on health outcomes listed in Table 1, data on 20,405 individuals were available. In a further step, we excluded individuals with missing information on lifestyle factors (n = 398), and our final sample therefore consisted of n = 20,007 individuals. One exception was the outcome variable “depression” assessed by the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9), which stems from the written questionnaire (n = 18,357). Out of the 20,007 included individuals only 16,114 individuals had available information on depression status. Thus, results concerning depression included less individuals than the other results.

thumbnail
Table 1. Characteristics of the sample of the 2012 Swiss Health Survey1.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0181442.t001

The SHS was conducted by the Swiss Federal Bureau of Statistics and does not require formal approval by an ethics committee. This data collection is specifically permitted under Swiss la (SR 431.012.1 and SR 431.112.1).

Measurements

Loneliness was used as a predictor variable and assessed by one item of the SHS asking the participants: “How often do you feel lonely? “Very often”, “quite often”, “sometimes”, “never”. Answers were dichotomized into never vs. sometimes, quite often and very often.

Outcome variables were divided into two parts, first into physical and mental health, and second into lifestyle factors. Physical and mental health included self-reported non-specified chronic diseases (self-reported ongoing disease or health problem lasting for at least 6 months or expected to last for longer than 6 months vs. none). Further health outcomes were self-reported diagnoses of hypertension, and high cholesterol concentration (yes vs. no), diabetes, and psychological distress (low vs. moderate and high) during the previous 4 weeks (assessed by the 5-item mental health index MHI-5). The MHI-5 has been shown to be a valid tool for detecting psychological distress in the general population and in psychiatric surveys [4851]. From the written questionnaire the validated PHQ-9 was used to detect depression (yes vs. no). We used a cut-off point of ≥10 vs. less, which has been shown to have a sensitivity of 88% and a specificity of 88% for the diagnosis of major depression [52, 53]. Additional health outcomes were self-perceived health (good and very good vs. fair, poor and very poor), having visited a medical doctor within the last year (yes vs. no), and body mass index (BMI) dichotomized into overweight and obese (BMI ≥25.0 kg/m2) vs. less. For adolescents aged 15 to 18 years, tables provided by Cole and colleagues were used to define overweight and obesity [54].

Lifestyle included smoking status (never and former vs. current), chronic alcohol consumption associated with a health risk (women ≥20 g, men ≥40 g ethanol daily vs. less), binge drinking (≥ 6 glasses of alcohol at one occasion within the last year vs. less often), physical activity (≥ 150 minutes per week vs. less) [55], diet awareness ("Do you pay attention to specific aspects of your diet?"; yes vs. no), and keeping to the 5-a-day recommendation of fruit and vegetable consumption according to the recommendations of the Swiss Society for Nutrition [56] (yes vs. no).

Confounders were chosen a priori from the known literature and included sex, age (categorized into 5-year age groups), area of residence (urban vs. rural), nationality (Swiss vs. non-Swiss), educational level (low: compulsory education or less vs. middle: secondary education vs. high: tertiary education), marital status (married/registered partnership vs. single, divorced/dissolved, separated, widowed), household size (living alone vs. not living alone), and level of social support received (high, middle, low; based on the Oslo-3-social support scale [57]).

Statistical analyses

All statistical analyses were conducted using STATA software version 13.1 (College Station, Texas). Weighted percentages were used to illustrate the prevalence of loneliness, socio-demographics, health and lifestyle outcomes. The prevalence of loneliness was shown in 5-year age groups. For further analyses, three age groups (15–29, 30–59, and 60+ years) were built, based on the prevalence results, which showed peaks of loneliness at the ages of 30 and 60 years.

Logistic regression analyses were used to determine associations of loneliness with physical and mental health and lifestyle factors in three models (model 1 unadjusted, model 2 age- and sex adjusted, and model 3 adjusted for age, sex, area of residence, nationality, educational level, marital status, household size, and social support). Odds ratios (OR) with the corresponding 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) were computed. Sampling weights were used for all analyses. Furthermore, a sub-analysis with loneliness dichotomized into extreme groups (feeling never vs. quite/very often lonely) was performed and associations with physical and mental health and lifestyle factors were analyzed.

As for “marital status” and “living alone” no categorization based on recommendations or validated instruments are known, we performed additional sub-analyses with different categorizations, such as without grouping different marital status. This did not affect the results (data not shown). Therefore, we used our dichotomized variables.

P-values < 0.05 were considered to be statistically significant.

We tested for multiplicative interactions by including a cross-product term for indicators of physical and mental health or lifestyle factors, and sex or age (in 3 groups) in the logistic regression models, along with the main effect terms for each. The statistical significance of the coefficient for each cross-product term was evaluated using the Wald test.

Results

Description of the sample and frequency of loneliness

Characteristics of the sample as well as the frequencies of loneliness are provided in Table 1. A description of the perceived frequency of loneliness in 5-year age steps from 15 to 80 years or older is shown in Fig 1. In all age groups, between 26% and 47% of individuals felt sometimes, quite or very often lonely. Furthermore, feeling lonely was reported more often by younger individuals than by older ones, with a peak at around 25 to 29 years of age, and a decrease from 70 to 74 years of age followed by a small increase thereafter (Fig 1).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Loneliness in different age groups.

Prevalence of perceived frequency of loneliness by 5-year age groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0181442.g001

Loneliness and physical and mental health

Table 2 shows the multivariable adjusted associations between loneliness and physical and mental health or lifestyle factors. Loneliness was statistically significantly associated with self-reported chronic diseases (OR 1.41, 95% CI 1.30–1.54), high cholesterol concentration (OR 1.31, 95% CI 1.18–1.45), diabetes (OR 1.40, 95% CI 1.16–1.67), moderate to high psychological distress (OR 3.74, 95% CI 3.37–4.16), depression (OR 2.78, 95% CI 2.22–3.48), impaired self-perceived health (OR 1.94, 95% CI 1.74–2.16), and with having had contact more often with physicians during the past 12 months (OR 1.29, 95% CI 1.17–1.42), No associations were observed between loneliness and hypertension or loneliness and BMI.

thumbnail
Table 2. Associations between loneliness and lifestyle or health-related factors of the 2012 Swiss Health Survey1.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0181442.t002

Loneliness and lifestyle

Compared to participants who never felt lonely, lonely men and women were statistically significantly more frequently current smokers (OR 1.13, 95% CI 1.05–1.23), less physically active (OR 1.20, 95% CI 1.10–1.31), and adhered less often to the recommendations of fruit and vegetable consumption (OR 1.21, 95% CI 1.07–1.37) (Table 2, multivariable adjusted for sociodemographic and social support variables in Table 1). Loneliness was not associated with chronic alcohol consumption, binge drinking, or diet awareness.

We only observed one statistically significant interaction of loneliness with sex, for diet awareness (p-interaction < 0.001, data not shown). Indeed, lonely men mentioned statistically significantly more awareness to diet than men who never felt lonely, but there was no such association between loneliness and diet in women.

Associations were even stronger in the sub-analysis, in which never lonely individuals were compared to quite and very often lonely individuals, with one exception, i.e. the association of loneliness with smoking was no longer statistically significant (data not shown).

Loneliness, health and lifestyle outcomes by age group

Table 3 shows the multivariable adjusted associations between loneliness and physical and mental health or lifestyle outcomes, stratified by age group. We observed statistically significant effect modification by age group and loneliness for self-perceived health, visiting a physician within the past year, BMI and smoking, but not for any other health or lifestyle characteristic (all other p-interactions ≥ 0.05; data not shown). The association of loneliness with self-perceived health was strongest in the 30 to 59 year age group but was also statistically significant in 15 to 29 year old subjects as well as in subjects aged 60 years or older. Loneliness was not statistically significantly associated with BMI for the total sample, but younger lonely participants (up to 59 years) tended to have lower odds of being overweight or obese than older participants (Table 3). Lonely 15 to 29 and 30 to 59 year old subjects were statistically significantly more often ever or current smokers than subjects who never felt lonely. In the oldest age group, loneliness was most strongly associated with visits to a physician within the past year, followed by the middle-aged group.

thumbnail
Table 3. Multivariable adjusted associations between loneliness and lifestyle and health-related factors stratified by age group of the 2012 Swiss Health Survey1.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0181442.t003

Our sub-group analysis performed by including only the two extremes of loneliness (i.e. feeling never lonely vs. feeling very often lonely) revealed similar results (S1 Table).

Discussion

One third of the Swiss population reported feeling lonely to some extent and about five percent claimed to feel lonely often or very often. Loneliness was more prevalent in young adults and in individuals older than 75 years. Although our results are merely descriptive, they are in accordance with a U-shaped association over the life course reported in earlier studies [3, 13], although we observed a peak of feeling lonely in the age ranges of 30 and 60 years. However, as observed by Heinrich and Gullone [33] many studies on the prevalence of loneliness were not up to date. The higher prevalence of loneliness in all age groups investigated in our study than in the mentioned survey on loneliness in 25 European nations [13] most probably reflect different categorizations of the term “loneliness” used in the two studies (feeling lonely all or almost all the time and most of the time in the European survey versus very often, quite often and sometimes in the SHS).

Loneliness and physical and mental health

We observed in our analyses of the SHS data that participants who felt lonely were significantly more likely to report chronic diseases, hypercholesterolemia, diabetes, distress, and depression, poor self-rated health, and more visits to medical doctors than participants who did not feel lonely.

Participants who felt lonely to some extent in our study on the SHS data reported being affected by chronic diseases more often than individuals who did not feel lonely. Whether the report of chronic diseases is elevated in lonely people or whether chronic diseases are indeed more frequent in lonely people is not obvious in our study, due to the single item question about ongoing diseases or health problems lasting for at least 6 months or expected to last for longer than 6 months. Nevertheless, other studies also revealed a positive association of loneliness with chronic diseases [16, 58].

In contrast to some [21, 38] but not all other studies [59], we observed in the SHS no association between loneliness and hypertension. In a study of young adults [60], loneliness was associated with higher total peripheral resistance, a mechanism by which loneliness may contribute to the development of hypertension [59].

Loneliness was also associated with psychological distress in the present SHS data. Other studies found similar results, going into even more detail, finding for example that the association of loneliness with distress depended on the duration of experienced loneliness or that associations were only observed for specific dimensions of distress [6163].

Loneliness was additionally associated with depression in our analyses of Swiss data. In the literature, depression has often been described to be accompanied by loneliness, but loneliness does not inevitably end in depression. Thus, loneliness may be both a risk factor for or a consequence of depression. Our findings are in line with those of other studies that found positive associations between loneliness and depression [43, 64, 65]. Although there are not only cross-sectional but also longitudinal studies that found an increased risk of depression with precedent loneliness, many of these studies included only older adults [43, 66, 67]. There are also a number of studies on children [6870], adolescents [7173] and younger adults [74, 75]. But less studies explored the association of loneliness with depression in middle-aged adults [47]. Thus, our results contribute to a better awareness of an underestimated health risk factor for all ages, and not only among older adults.

Similar to the present study on SHS data, several other studies found loneliness to be associated with poor self-rated health [16, 26, 28, 76]. In a longitudinal study in Finland, never or seldom feeling lonely predicted good self-rated health over time [37]. The association was stronger among women than among men. The authors of the study hypothesized that, among men, voluntary work and the associated social support might reflect pathways between loneliness and self-reported health. Among women, the initial significant association disappeared after adjusting for baseline health conditions. In our study, the association was not modified by sex but it was by age. Even though a significant association was observed in all three age groups, the association of loneliness with self-perceived health was surprisingly strongest in 30 to 59 year olds.

In our study, lonely individuals visited a medical doctor more often during the past year than participants who did not feel lonely. This association was observed in middle-aged and older but not younger individuals, which confirms results of previous studies [7779]. Overall, lonely individuals might need to see a medical doctor more often because of their poor health. Furthermore, as Ellaway et al. [79] pointed out medical doctors possibly fulfill a social role for those who need someone to talk to.

Furthermore, our analyses of the SHS data, revealed no overall association between loneliness and BMI although the association was modified by age, pointing towards a tendency for lower odds of overweight and obesity in lonely younger but not in older participants. Our findings are in contrast to evidence from the literature of a positive association between loneliness and overweight, obesity and central obesity. Accordingly, in a cross-sectional study on 1289 adults in Australia a higher proportion of lonely than non-lonely individuals were overweight and obese [46]. In a population-based study in England, loneliness was associated with an increased likelihood for meeting criteria of the metabolic syndrome and with the individual criteria of central obesity and elevated fasting blood glucose, but not of high blood pressure and dyslipidemia [19]. As pointed out in the literature, higher food consumption might partially explain the association of loneliness with central obesity [19, 59, 80]. It was hypothesized by Jaremka et al. [81] that loneliness might predict higher post-brandial ghrelin concentration and hunger in women with a lower BMI. Ghrelin, a hormone which increases appetite, may thus link loneliness to weight gain. One explanation for our negative results in relation to loneliness and high body weight in the SHS may be the more frequent visits to medical doctors by lonely than by not lonely individuals. Indeed, medical doctors may have given advice to overweight patients to lose weight.

Loneliness and lifestyle factors

In our analyses of the SHS data, feeling lonely was associated with smoking, physical inactivity, and non-adherence to the recommendation of fruit and vegetable consumption. Sex did not–with one exception–modify these associations. Age, on the other hand, had a modifying effect for smoking.

Our results in relation to smoking are partly in line with a newer systematic review, which found half significant and another half non-significant associations between loneliness and smoking [82]. In our study, lonely young (15 to 29 year old) and middle-aged (30 to 59 year old) participants, but not those of 60 years and older, smoked more often than participants who did not feel lonely. As DeWall and Pond [83] pointed out, young individuals may start smoking in an attempt to connect with others and gain social acceptance.

We observed no association between loneliness and neither chronic alcohol consumption nor binge drinking in our analyses of the SHS data. A population-based study in the US found an association of loneliness with reduced alcohol use, and not with binge drinking in individuals of 50 years or older [84]. In contrast, loneliness was considered to be a contributing, maintaining and poor prognostic factor in the development of alcohol abuse and a risk factor for all stages of alcoholism in a recent review [85]. Another study tried to explain these inconsistent findings by assessing transient loneliness and individual drinking behaviors, revealing that loneliness was related to an increase in solitary consumption and a decrease in social alcohol consumption [86]. Unfortunately, we were not able to confirm this potential explanation for the inconsistent association between loneliness and alcohol consumption because the SHS does not include data on social and solitary alcohol consumption.

For physical activity, we observed an association between loneliness and physical inactivity, independently of age. To our knowledge, no study had yet examined the association of loneliness with physical activity in the age range of total adulthood. But our findings are in accordance with previous literature on older adults, such as an Israeli study with a random sample [87] and a Canadian longitudinal study [88], in which loneliness was associated with a lower odds of engaging in physical activity. Similarly, in the study of Hawkley et al. [44] loneliness was an independent risk factor for physical inactivity among adults aged 50 years and above. As Hawkley & Cacioppo pointed out, loneliness is associated with poor emotional self-regulation [3], which may contribute to reduced motivation to engage in physical activity. Further studies, especially with samples of younger individuals, are needed to elucidate these associations.

We observed no overall association between feeling lonely and diet awareness in our analyses of the SHS data, but lonely men did mention diet awareness more often than male participants who did not feel lonely. Adherence to the recommendation of fruit and vegetable consumption, on the other hand, which can be interpreted as a proxy for “healthy” diet [56], was reported less frequently among lonely individuals in our study. To our knowledge the association of loneliness with healthy or unhealthy diet has scarcely been examined, with the exception of the effect of loneliness on malnutrition in the elderly [89] and in relation to eating disorders [90, 91]. However, our data were too limited to further evaluate these associations.

Overall, Hawkley and Cacioppo [3] proposed a model that explained how loneliness has health consequences and how it influences health behaviors. According to this model, loneliness is accompanied by feelings of hostility, stress, pessimism, anxiety, etc. and represents a dispositional tendency that activates or modifies genetic, neurobiological and immune functioning. In addition, loneliness is associated with a diminished capacity for self-regulation, which is of importance in relation to lifestyle behaviors. All of this contributes to adverse physical and mental health outcomes and adverse health behaviors.

Strengths and limitations

Our study had two major strengths: First, we included all adults in our large population-based survey, stratifying them by age, as most of the existing studies focused on adolescents or older adults [7, 14, 1622, 24] rather than on the entire adult age range. Second, the associations of loneliness with various physical and mental health aspects were examined. Research on these associations, especially with behavioral factors, is still rare and only scarce research exists for younger adults. Furthermore, we tested for age and sex interactions, which have rarely been evaluated before [8, 92]. Unfortunately, we could not analyze the adolescents separately because of their limited number, and thus, included them in the group of younger adults. A further strength is the use of a validated scale for depression and psychological distress as well as the multivariable adjustment for our large sample, which gives a good population based estimate of distress levels and depression status in Switzerland. Last but not least, our study is representative of the Swiss population, although individuals living in institutions (e.g. nursing homes, hospital, prisons) were not included and thus, we missed a group of individuals with worse health, which are more frequent in older age groups. A further strength were the additional analyses including only the two extremes of feeling lonely (i.e. never vs. very often), which confirmed our results.

Results of our study also entail several limitations. First, we were limited by the cross-sectional study design. No causal conclusions could be derived since the direction of the associations cannot be determined. Thus, it is difficult to determine whether loneliness followed exposure in time or exposure resulted from loneliness. However, our results support prospective studies and are in line with theoretical frameworks on the association of loneliness and health. Due to the lack of longitudinal data in Switzerland and the limited research on loneliness and its overall consequences, it is still worthwhile to present population-based cross-sectional data. Furthermore, we dichotomized loneliness due to the fact that only 2.7% and 1.5% reported to feel lonely quite often or very often in, respectively, in two categories. However, for the comprehensibility a dichotomization is advantageous and also used in studies frequently [7, 23]. A further limitation is that the study was based on self-reported data, assessed only once. Inaccurate self-reporting can be caused by recall bias, social desirability bias and errors in self-observation. There might also be bias with regard to a healthy participant effect in the Swiss Health Survey. Furthermore, as Luhmann & Hawkley [14] pointed out, in the case of age, loneliness may be more readily reported in older age when it is considered age-typical than in younger age. Indirect measures reduce this bias by avoiding the use of the term “loneliness” etc. Indirect measures are also invariably multi-item measures that offer greater reliability than single-item measures. In the present study, loneliness was assessed by one single-item question, which included the word “lonely”. Single-item questions were considered valid compared to multi-item questions for the same construct, in some, but not in all studies. [9395]. The use of a direct single-item question which included the word “lonely” may have resulted in under-reporting due to the social stigma of loneliness. Another consequence of this phenomenological approach is that participants may have understood loneliness in different ways. Other variables in our study, such as physical activity and body weight, were based on self-reported information, which is prone to recall bias. Lastly, due to the low overall response rate of 54% in the SHS, the possibility of response bias cannot be ruled out. The use of weighting factors, nevertheless, allows for the extrapolation of the results in relation to age, sex, region and nationality from the sample to the total population living in Switzerland [96].

Conclusion

Loneliness was apparent throughout the total age range of adulthood and was associated with poorer physical and mental health and unhealthy behavior. Furthermore, loneliness was modified by age, but not sex (with one exception), for some but not all physical and mental health aspects. Our findings illustrate the importance of considering loneliness for physical and mental health and lifestyle factors, not only in older and younger, but also in middle adults. Due to our cross-sectional study design no causal relationship could be inferred and, thus, evaluating loneliness in a longitudinal perspective is needed to contribute to a better understanding of the importance of loneliness as an independent risk factor for physical and mental health in Switzerland.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The data collection and data storage for the Swiss Health Survey (Schweizerische Gesundheitsbefragung) does not require formal approval by an ethics committee. This data collection is specifically permitted under Swiss law (SR 431.012.1 and SR 431.112.1). Individuals invited to participate received a brief description of the study and could decline to participate or withdraw at any time. Participants’ responses were treated confidentially and aggregated anonymous responses were utilized for the analyses presented herein.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Associations between loneliness dichotomized into extreme groups (feeling never vs. quite/very often lonely) and lifestyle or health-related factors of the 2012 Swiss Health Survey1.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0181442.s001

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the Swiss Federal Office of Statistics for permission to use the data of the 2012 Swiss Health Survey.

References

  1. 1. Mijuskovic BL. Loneliness in Philosophy, Psychology, and Literature. 3rd ed. Bloomington2012.
  2. 2. Cacioppo S, Capitanio JP, Cacioppo JT. Toward a neurology of loneliness. Psychol Bull. 2014;140(6):1464–504. pmid:25222636.
  3. 3. Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness matters: a theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Ann Behav Med. 2010;40(2):218–27. pmid:20652462; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3874845.
  4. 4. De Jong Gierveld J, Van Tilburg T. The De Jong Gierveld short scales for emotional and social loneliness: tested on data from 7 countries in the UN generations and gender surveys. Eur J Ageing. 2010;7(2):121–30. pmid:20730083; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2921057.
  5. 5. Flood M. Mapping loneliness in Australia. Canberra, Australia: The Australia Institute, 2005.
  6. 6. Pinquart M, Sorensen S. Gender differences in self-concept and psychological well-being in old age: a meta-analysis. The journals of gerontology Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences. 2001;56(4):P195–213. pmid:11445606.
  7. 7. Dahlberg L, Andersson L, McKee KJ, Lennartsson C. Predictors of loneliness among older women and men in Sweden: A national longitudinal study. Aging Ment Health. 2015;19(5):409–17. pmid:25126996.
  8. 8. Victor CR, Yang K. The prevalence of loneliness among adults: a case study of the United Kingdom. J Psychol. 2012;146(1–2):85–104. pmid:22303614.
  9. 9. Qualter P, Vanhalst J, Harris R, Van Roekel E, Lodder G, Bangee M, et al. Loneliness across the life span. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015;10(2):250–64. pmid:25910393.
  10. 10. Dykstra PA. Older adult loneliness: myths and realities. Eur J Ageing. 2009;6(2):91–100. pmid:19517025; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2693783.
  11. 11. Jylha M. Old age and loneliness: cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses in the Tampere Longitudinal Study on Aging. Can J Aging. 2004;23(2):157–68. pmid:15334815.
  12. 12. Tijhuis MA, De Jong-Gierveld J, Feskens EJ, Kromhout D. Changes in and factors related to loneliness in older men. The Zutphen Elderly Study. Age Ageing. 1999;28(5):491–5. pmid:10529046.
  13. 13. Yang KM, Victor C. Age and loneliness in 25 European nations. Ageing Soc. 2011;31:1368–88.
  14. 14. Luhmann M, Hawkley LC. Age differences in loneliness from late adolescence to oldest old age. Dev Psychol. 2016;52(6):943–59. pmid:27148782.
  15. 15. Russell DW. UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version 3): reliability, validity, and factor structure. J Pers Assess. 1996;66(1):20–40. pmid:8576833.
  16. 16. Ferreira-Alves J, Magalhaes P, Viola L, Simoes R. Loneliness in middle and old age: demographics, perceived health, and social satisfaction as predictors. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2014;59(3):613–23. pmid:25109808.
  17. 17. Coyle CE, Dugan E. Social isolation, loneliness and health among older adults. J Aging Health. 2012;24(8):1346–63. pmid:23006425.
  18. 18. Shankar A, McMunn A, Banks J, Steptoe A. Loneliness, social isolation, and behavioral and biological health indicators in older adults. Health Psychol. 2011;30(4):377–85. pmid:21534675.
  19. 19. Whisman MA. Loneliness and the metabolic syndrome in a population-based sample of middle-aged and older adults. Health Psychol. 2010;29(5):550–4. pmid:20836610.
  20. 20. Victor CR, Bowling A. A longitudinal analysis of loneliness among older people in Great Britain. J Psychol. 2012;146(3):313–31. pmid:22574423.
  21. 21. Momtaz YA, Hamid TA, Yusoff S, Ibrahim R, Chai ST, Yahaya N, et al. Loneliness as a risk factor for hypertension in later life. J Aging Health. 2012;24(4):696–710. pmid:22422758.
  22. 22. Luo Y, Hawkley LC, Waite LJ, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness, health, and mortality in old age: a national longitudinal study. Soc Sci Med. 2012;74(6):907–14. pmid:22326307; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3303190.
  23. 23. Bartels M, Cacioppo JT, Hudziak JJ, Boomsma DI. Genetic and environmental contributions to stability in loneliness throughout childhood. American journal of medical genetics Part B, Neuropsychiatric genetics: the official publication of the International Society of Psychiatric Genetics. 2008;147(3):385–91. pmid:17918194.
  24. 24. Hawthorne G. Perceived social isolation in a community sample: its prevalence and correlates with aspects of peoples' lives. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2008;43(2):140–50. pmid:17994175.
  25. 25. Nicolaisen M, Thorsen K. Who are lonely? Loneliness in different age groups (18–81 years old), using two measures of loneliness. Int J Aging Hum Dev. 2014;78(3):229–57. pmid:25265679.
  26. 26. Stickley A, Koyanagi A, Roberts B, Richardson E, Abbott P, Tumanov S, et al. Loneliness: its correlates and association with health behaviours and outcomes in nine countries of the former Soviet Union. PloS one. 2013;8(7):e67978. pmid:23861843; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3701665.
  27. 27. Fokkema T, De Jong Gierveld J, Dykstra PA. Cross-national differences in older adult loneliness. J Psychol. 2012;146(1–2):201–28. pmid:22303621.
  28. 28. Savikko N, Routasalo P, Tilvis RS, Strandberg TE, Pitkala KH. Predictors and subjective causes of loneliness in an aged population. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2005;41(3):223–33. pmid:15908025.
  29. 29. Hawkley LC, Hughes ME, Waite LJ, Masi CM, Thisted RA, Cacioppo JT. From social structural factors to perceptions of relationship quality and loneliness: the Chicago health, aging, and social relations study. The journals of gerontology Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences. 2008;63(6):S375–84. pmid:19092047; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2769562.
  30. 30. Povlsen L. Immigrant women's clubs in a health-promotion perspective. Scand J Public Health. 2012;40(4):355–9. pmid:22786920.
  31. 31. Merz EM, Jak S. The long reach of childhood. Childhood experiences influence close relationships and loneliness across life. Adv Life Course Res. 2013;18(3):212–22. pmid:24796560.
  32. 32. Routasalo PE, Savikko N, Tilvis RS, Strandberg TE, Pitkala KH. Social contacts and their relationship to loneliness among aged people—a population-based study. Gerontology. 2006;52(3):181–7. pmid:16645299.
  33. 33. Heinrich LM, Gullone E. The clinical significance of loneliness: a literature review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2006;26(6):695–718. pmid:16952717.
  34. 34. Cacioppo JT, Cacioppo S, Cole SW, Capitanio JP, Goossens L, Boomsma DI. Loneliness across phylogeny and a call for comparative studies and animal models. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015;10(2):202–12. pmid:25910390.
  35. 35. Rico-Uribe LA, Caballero FF, Olaya B, Tobiasz-Adamczyk B, Koskinen S, Leonardi M, et al. Loneliness, Social Networks, and Health: A Cross-Sectional Study in Three Countries. PloS one. 2016;11(1):e0145264. pmid:26761205; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4711964.
  36. 36. Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Baker M, Harris T, Stephenson D. Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015;10(2):227–37. pmid:25910392.
  37. 37. Nummela O, Seppanen M, Uutela A. The effect of loneliness and change in loneliness on self-rated health (SRH): a longitudinal study among aging people. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2011;53(2):163–7. pmid:21093070.
  38. 38. Hawkley LC, Thisted RA, Masi CM, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness predicts increased blood pressure: 5-year cross-lagged analyses in middle-aged and older adults. Psychol Aging. 2010;25(1):132–41. pmid:20230134; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2841310.
  39. 39. Thurston RC, Kubzansky LD. Women, loneliness, and incident coronary heart disease. Psychosom Med. 2009;71(8):836–42. pmid:19661189; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2851545.
  40. 40. DeNiro DA. Perceived alienation in individuals with residual-type schizophrenia. Issues Ment Health Nurs. 1995;16(3):185–200. pmid:7759231.
  41. 41. Neeleman J, Power MJ. Social support and depression in three groups of psychiatric patients and a group of medical controls. Social psychiatry and psychiatric epidemiology. 1994;29(1):46–51. pmid:8178222.
  42. 42. Stravynski A, Boyer R. Loneliness in relation to suicide ideation and parasuicide: a population-wide study. Suicide Life Threat Behav. 2001;31(1):32–40. pmid:11326767.
  43. 43. Cacioppo JT, Hughes ME, Waite LJ, Hawkley LC, Thisted RA. Loneliness as a specific risk factor for depressive symptoms: cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Psychol Aging. 2006;21(1):140–51. pmid:16594799.
  44. 44. Hawkley LC, Thisted RA, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness predicts reduced physical activity: cross-sectional & longitudinal analyses. Health Psychol. 2009;28(3):354–63. pmid:19450042; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2791498.
  45. 45. Akerlind I, Hornquist JO. Loneliness and alcohol abuse: a review of evidences of an interplay. Soc Sci Med. 1992;34(4):405–14. pmid:1566121.
  46. 46. Lauder W, Mummery K, Jones M, Caperchione C. A comparison of health behaviours in lonely and non-lonely populations. Psychol Health Med. 2006;11(2):233–45. pmid:17129911.
  47. 47. Barger SD, Messerli-Burgy N, Barth J. Social relationship correlates of major depressive disorder and depressive symptoms in Switzerland: nationally representative cross sectional study. BMC public health. 2014;14:273. pmid:24656048; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3994328.
  48. 48. Berwick DM, Murphy JM, Goldman PA, Ware JE Jr., Barsky AJ, Weinstein MC. Performance of a five-item mental health screening test. Med Care. 1991;29(2):169–76. Epub 1991/02/11. pmid:1994148.
  49. 49. Rumpf HJ, Meyer C, Hapke U, John U. Screening for mental health: validity of the MHI-5 using DSM-IV Axis I psychiatric disorders as gold standard. Psychiatry Res. 2001;105(3):243–53. Epub 2002/01/30. pmid:11814543.
  50. 50. Cuijpers P, Smits N, Donker T, ten Have M, de Graaf R. Screening for mood and anxiety disorders with the five-item, the three-item, and the two-item Mental Health Inventory. Psychiatry Res. 2009;168(3):250–5. Epub 2009/02/03. pmid:19185354.
  51. 51. Veit CT, Ware JE Jr. The structure of psychological distress and well-being in general populations. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1983;51(5):730–42. pmid:6630688.
  52. 52. Manea L, Gilbody S, McMillan D. Optimal cut-off score for diagnosing depression with the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9): a meta-analysis. CMAJ. 2012;184(3):E191–6. Epub 2011/12/21. pmid:22184363; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3281183.
  53. 53. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB. The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. Journal of general internal medicine. 2001;16(9):606–13. PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1495268. pmid:11556941
  54. 54. Cole TJ, Bellizzi MC, Flegal KM, Dietz WH. Establishing a standard definition for child overweight and obesity worldwide: international survey. BMJ. 2000;320(7244):1240–3. pmid:10797032; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC27365.
  55. 55. Bundesamt für Sport BASPO. Gesundheitswirksame Bewegung. Magglingen: BASPO. 2013.
  56. 56. Schweizerische Gesellschaft für Ernährung SGE. Schweizer Lebensmittelpyramide. available: http://www.sge-ssn.ch/de/ich-und-du/essen-und-trinken/ausgewogen/lebensmittelpyramide/. Last access 11.04.2016. 2013.
  57. 57. Meltzer H. Development of a common instrument for mental Health. In: Nosikov A, Gudex C (eds) EUROHIS: developing common istruments for health surveys. Amsterdam, IOS Press2003.
  58. 58. Arslantas H, Adana F, Abacigil Ergin F, Kayar D, Acar G. Loneliness in Elderly People, Associated Factors and Its Correlation with Quality of Life: A Field Study from Western Turkey. Iran J Public Health. 2015;44(1):43–50. pmid:26060775; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4450013.
  59. 59. Theeke LA, Mallow J. Loneliness and quality of life in chronically ill rural older adults. Am J Nurs. 2013;113(9):28–37; quiz 8. pmid:23958674; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4323171.
  60. 60. Hawkley LC, Burleson MH, Berntson GG, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness in everyday life: cardiovascular activity, psychosocial context, and health behaviors. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;85(1):105–20. pmid:12872887.
  61. 61. Paul C, Ayis S, Ebrahim S. Psychological distress, loneliness and disability in old age. Psychol Health Med. 2006;11(2):221–32. pmid:17129910.
  62. 62. DeBerard MS, Kleinknecht RA. Loneliness, duration of loneliness, and reported stress symptomatology. Psychol Rep. 1995;76(3 Pt 2):1363–9. pmid:7480506.
  63. 63. Jackson J, Cochran SD. Loneliness and psychological distress. J Psychol. 1991;125(3):257–62. pmid:1880752.
  64. 64. Alpass FM, Neville S. Loneliness, health and depression in older males. Aging Ment Health. 2003;7(3):212–6. pmid:12775403.
  65. 65. Jaremka LM, Andridge RR, Fagundes CP, Alfano CM, Povoski SP, Lipari AM, et al. Pain, depression, and fatigue: loneliness as a longitudinal risk factor. Health Psychol. 2014;33(9):948–57. pmid:23957903; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3992976.
  66. 66. Holvast F, Burger H, de Waal MM, van Marwijk HW, Comijs HC, Verhaak PF. Loneliness is associated with poor prognosis in late-life depression: Longitudinal analysis of the Netherlands study of depression in older persons. Journal of affective disorders. 2015;185:1–7. pmid:26142687.
  67. 67. Jaremka LM, Fagundes CP, Glaser R, Bennett JM, Malarkey WB, Kiecolt-Glaser JK. Loneliness predicts pain, depression, and fatigue: understanding the role of immune dysregulation. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38(8):1310–7. pmid:23273678; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3633610.
  68. 68. Qualter P, Brown SL, Munn P, Rotenberg KJ. Childhood loneliness as a predictor of adolescent depressive symptoms: an 8-year longitudinal study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;19(6):493–501. pmid:19777287.
  69. 69. Nangle DW, Erdley CA, Newman JE, Mason CA, Carpenter EM. Popularity, friendship quantity, and friendship quality: interactive influences on children's loneliness and depression. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2003;32(4):546–55. pmid:14710463.
  70. 70. Lau S, Chan DW, Lau PS. Facets of loneliness and depression among Chinese children and adolescents. The Journal of social psychology. 1999;139(6):713–29. pmid:10646306.
  71. 71. Stickley A, Koyanagi A, Koposov R, Blatny M, Hrdlicka M, Schwab-Stone M, et al. Loneliness and its association with psychological and somatic health problems among Czech, Russian and U.S. adolescents. BMC Psychiatry. 2016;16:128. pmid:27146137; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4857285.
  72. 72. Shevlin M, Murphy S, Mallett J, Stringer M, Murphy J. Adolescent loneliness and psychiatric morbidity in Northern Ireland. Br J Clin Psychol. 2013;52(2):230–4. pmid:24215150.
  73. 73. Witvliet M, Brendgen M, van Lier PAC, Koot HM, Vitaro F. Early Adolescent Depressive Symptoms: Prediction from Clique Isolation, Loneliness, and Perceived Social Acceptance. J Abnorm Child Psych. 2010;38(8):1045–56. pmid:20499155
  74. 74. Matthews T, Danese A, Wertz J, Odgers CL, Ambler A, Moffitt TE, et al. Social isolation, loneliness and depression in young adulthood: a behavioural genetic analysis. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2016;51(3):339–48. pmid:26843197; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4819590.
  75. 75. Hagerty BM, Williams RA. The effects of sense of belonging, social support, conflict, and loneliness on depression. Nursing research. 1999;48(4):215–9. pmid:10414684
  76. 76. Stickley A, Koyanagi A, Leinsalu M, Ferlander S, Sabawoon W, McKee M. Loneliness and health in Eastern Europe: findings from Moscow, Russia. Public Health. 2015;129(4):403–10. pmid:25744109.
  77. 77. Taube E, Kristensson J, Sandberg M, Midlov P, Jakobsson U. Loneliness and health care consumption among older people. Scand J Caring Sci. 2014. pmid:24826811.
  78. 78. Cheng ST. Loneliness-distress and physician utilization in well-elderly females. J community psychol 1992;20(1):43–56.
  79. 79. Ellaway A, Wood S, Macintyre S. Someone to talk to? The role of loneliness as a factor in the frequency of GP consultations. Br J Gen Pract. 1999;49(442):363–7. pmid:10736886; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1313421.
  80. 80. Rotenberg KJ, Flood D. Loneliness, dysphoria, dietary restraint, and eating behavior. Int J Eat Disord. 1999;25(1):55–64. pmid:9924653.
  81. 81. Jaremka LM, Fagundes CP, Peng J, Belury MA, Andridge RR, Malarkey WB, et al. Loneliness predicts postprandial ghrelin and hunger in women. Horm Behav. 2015;70:57–63. pmid:25725426; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4409518.
  82. 82. Dyal SR, Valente TW. A Systematic Review of Loneliness and Smoking: Small Effects, Big Implications. Subst Use Misuse. 2015:1–20. pmid:26555089.
  83. 83. DeWall CN, Pond RS. Loneliness and smoking: The costs of the desire to reconnect. Self and Identity. 2011;10(3):375–85.
  84. 84. Canham SL, Mauro PM, Kaufmann CN, Sixsmith A. Association of Alcohol Use and Loneliness Frequency Among Middle-Aged and Older Adult Drinkers. J Aging Health. 2015. pmid:26082130.
  85. 85. Mushtaq R, Shoib S, Shah T, Mushtaq S. Relationship between loneliness, psychiatric disorders and physical health? A review on the psychological aspects of loneliness. J Clin Diagn Res. 2014;8(9):WE01–4. pmid:25386507; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4225959.
  86. 86. Arpin SN, Mohr CD, Brannan D. Having friends and feeling lonely: a daily process examination of transient loneliness, socialization, and drinking behavior. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2015;41(5):615–28. pmid:25758705.
  87. 87. Netz Y, Goldsmith R, Shimony T, Arnon M, Zeev A. Loneliness is associated with an increased risk of sedentary life in older Israelis. Aging Ment Health. 2013;17(1):40–7. pmid:22913477.
  88. 88. Newall NE, Chipperfield JG, Bailis DS, Stewart TL. Consequences of loneliness on physical activity and mortality in older adults and the power of positive emotions. Health Psychol. 2013;32(8):921–4. pmid:22888819.
  89. 89. Ramic E, Pranjic N, Batic-Mujanovic O, Karic E, Alibasic E, Alic A. The effect of loneliness on malnutrition in elderly population. Med Arh. 2011;65(2):92–5. pmid:21585182.
  90. 90. Levine MP. Loneliness and eating disorders. J Psychol. 2012;146(1–2):243–57. pmid:22303623.
  91. 91. Southward MW, Christensen KA, Fettich KC, Weissman J, Berona J, Chen EY. Loneliness mediates the relationship between emotion dysregulation and bulimia nervosa/binge eating disorder psychopathology in a clinical sample. Eat Weight Disord. 2014;19(4):509–13. pmid:24235091.
  92. 92. Griffin J. The lonely society? London: The Mental Health Foundation, 2010.
  93. 93. Gliem J, Gliem R. Calculating, Interpreting, and Reporting Cronbach's Alpha Reliability Coefficient for Likert-Type Scales. Midwest Research to Practice Conference in Adult, Continuing, and Community Education: 2003.
  94. 94. Bergkvist L, Rossiter JR. The predicitive validity of multiple-item versus single-item measures of the same construct. Journal of Marketing Research. 2007;44(2):175–84.
  95. 95. DeSalvo KB, Fisher WP, Tran K, Bloser N, Merrill W, Peabody J. Assessing measurement properties of two single-item general health measures. Quality of life research: an international journal of quality of life aspects of treatment, care and rehabilitation. 2006;15(2):191–201. pmid:16468076.
  96. 96. Swiss Federal Statistical Office. Swiss Health Survey—Indices. Neuchâtel: Swiss Federal Statistical Office; 2012.