Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Association of loneliness with all-cause mortality: A meta-analysis

  • Laura Alejandra Rico-Uribe,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain, Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental, CIBERSAM, Madrid, Spain, Department of Psychiatry, Hospital Universitario de La Princesa, Instituto de Investigación Sanitaria Princesa (IIS-Princesa), Madrid, Spain

  • Francisco Félix Caballero,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain, Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental, CIBERSAM, Madrid, Spain, Department of Psychiatry, Hospital Universitario de La Princesa, Instituto de Investigación Sanitaria Princesa (IIS-Princesa), Madrid, Spain

  • Natalia Martín-María,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain, Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental, CIBERSAM, Madrid, Spain, Department of Psychiatry, Hospital Universitario de La Princesa, Instituto de Investigación Sanitaria Princesa (IIS-Princesa), Madrid, Spain

  • María Cabello,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain, Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental, CIBERSAM, Madrid, Spain, Department of Psychiatry, Hospital Universitario de La Princesa, Instituto de Investigación Sanitaria Princesa (IIS-Princesa), Madrid, Spain

  • José Luis Ayuso-Mateos,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain, Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental, CIBERSAM, Madrid, Spain, Department of Psychiatry, Hospital Universitario de La Princesa, Instituto de Investigación Sanitaria Princesa (IIS-Princesa), Madrid, Spain

  • Marta Miret

    Roles Conceptualization, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    marta.miret@uam.es

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain, Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental, CIBERSAM, Madrid, Spain, Department of Psychiatry, Hospital Universitario de La Princesa, Instituto de Investigación Sanitaria Princesa (IIS-Princesa), Madrid, Spain

Abstract

Introduction

Loneliness has social and health implications. The aim of this article is to evaluate the association of loneliness with all-cause mortality.

Methods

Pubmed, PsycINFO, CINAHL and Scopus databases were searched through June 2016 for published articles that measured loneliness and mortality. The main characteristics and the effect size values of each article were extracted. Moreover, an evaluation of the quality of the articles included was also carried out. A meta-analysis was performed firstly with all the included articles and secondly separating by gender, using a random effects model.

Results

A total of 35 articles involving 77220 participants were included in the systematic review. Loneliness is a risk factor for all-cause mortality [pooled HR = 1.22, 95% CI = (1.10, 1.35), p < 0.001] for both genders together, and for women [pooled HR = 1.26, 95% CI = (1.07, 1.48); p = 0.005] and men [pooled HR = 1.44; 95% CI = (1.19, 1.76); p < 0.001] separately.

Conclusions

Loneliness shows a harmful effect for all-cause mortality and this effect is slightly stronger in men than in women. Moreover, the impact of loneliness was independent from the quality evaluation of each article and the effect of depression.

Introduction

Loneliness has been conceptualized as an individual’s subjective experience about the generalized lack of satisfying human relationships [1]. A recent article has shown that the prevalence of loneliness in European countries ranks from 10% in Western and Northern countries to 55% in Eastern countries [2]. Moreover, a report conducted in the United Kingdom suggested that if loneliness is not considered as a relevant priority, in 2030 depression and other health problems may increase, given their association with loneliness [3].

According to previous articles, loneliness has significant implications with several physical and mental health issues such as: depression [4, 5], alcoholism [6], cardiovascular problems [7], sleep difficulties [7], alteration in the immunological system [8], Alzheimer´s disease [9], and health status in general [10]. Moreover, an increasing body of research has shown that loneliness is also associated with early mortality [1114].

In order to have a better understanding of the association of loneliness with mortality, gender analyses should be carried out for several reasons. Firstly, women live generally longer than men [15]. Secondly, some studies have shown that feelings of loneliness might be more prevalent in women than in men [16]. Thirdly, women and men build social networks in a different way, as an example, men experience smaller social networks [17] and less intimate relationships [18]. Fourthly, it is culturally less acceptable for men to express their emotions than it is for women [19]. And last but not least, some common risk factors for loneliness are also gender specific, i.e.; depression is more prevalent in women than men [20] whereas alcoholism is more frequent in men [21]. Moreover, the longer life expectancy of women entails that some risk factors for loneliness such as living alone and widowhood, occur earlier in women than in men [22].

There is a lack of research on the gendered aspects of the association of loneliness with all-cause mortality. To our knowledge, no meta-analysis that studies this association by gender has been carried out yet, and this is problematic because what could be associated with mortality for the whole sample might not be for men or women separately. One recent meta-analysis analyzed the impact of loneliness on mortality in men and women together [23]. This meta-analysis, though valuable, did not conduct sensitivity analyses for the quality of the studies, covered only a specific range of years and was limited to studies published in English. Therefore, the main aim of this meta-analysis is to determine whether loneliness is associated with all-cause mortality, considering all populations (including general and clinical populations). A secondary aim is to check whether this association is the same in women and men. Additionally, this meta-analysis has been conducted with no languages and time restrictions.

Methods

Pubmed, PsycINFO, CINAHL and Scopus databases were searched for articles that measured loneliness and mortality published until June 27th, 2016. In these databases, all the abstracts of the articles are provided in English, even though the articles might be in other language. The following terms were used to search all articles in the databases: (("Loneliness"[Mesh]) OR Lone*[Title/Abstract]) OR Forlorn*[Title/Abstract]) OR Desol*[Title/Abstract]) OR ("Social Isolation"[Majr] OR "Feeling isolated"[Title/Abstract]) AND ("Mortality"[MESH] OR "Death"[Mesh] OR Decease*[Title/Abstract] OR Die[Title/Abstract] OR Dead[Title/Abstract] OR Remain alive[Title/Abstract] OR Remained alive[Title/Abstract] OR "Longevity"[Mesh] OR "Survival"[Mesh]) AND (Humans[Mesh]) AND (adult[MeSH]) NOT ("Cross-Sectional Studies"[Mesh]) NOT ("Books"[Mesh]) NOT ("Validation Studies" [Publication Type])). Search terms were tailored to each database. In addition, in order to minimize omissions, the reference sections of past reviews and meta-analysis were examined to locate articles not identified in the search.

The inclusion criteria were: articles with longitudinal observational design, prospective cohort design, meta-analysis, and systematic reviews. Articles that selected participants older than 18 years, and that used loneliness and mortality as measures of interest were included too. Articles in which mortality was the outcome measure, and loneliness was the independent variable defined as a subjective feeling that accompanies the perception that one’s social needs are not being met by the quantity or especially the quality of one’s social relationships, were also included.

The exclusion criteria were psychometric studies (development or validation of questionnaires or scales), articles of phase-I/II clinical trials, cross-sectional, primary prevention, ecologic, case report/case series, retrospective, and case-control studies. Non-human population, articles that did not analyze loneliness and mortality, articles that did not evaluate loneliness or perceived feelings of social isolation but other constructs such as size of the network, articles that did not consider loneliness as an independent variable, and articles that did not consider mortality as a dependent variable were also excluded. Since the aim of this study was to analyze the association of loneliness with mortality through physical disease, articles investigating death by suicide, injury, or accidents were not included. Thesis and books or book sections were excluded as well.

Three subsequent steps were performed to select the articles and collect the data. In the first step, articles with prospective and longitudinal design that addressed the effect of loneliness on mortality were identified and selected. A software package for managing bibliographies was used to eliminate duplicates. A researcher (LARU) checked the titles and abstracts of all the articles for inclusion or exclusion. In case the article was excluded the reason was provided. A random sample of 337 (20%) of the articles was double-checked independently by a second researcher (NMM). This 20% was selected with the statistical software SPSS. Initial disagreements between reviewers were solved by discussion; if no agreement could be reached a third researcher (MM) was consulted. In the second step, all included articles were fully read to confirm that they fulfilled all inclusion criteria. In the third step, objective and verifiable characteristics of each included article were extracted. In articles that presented more than one analysis, the one that adjusted by more confounders and the one that reported more causes of mortality was selected. When multiple effect sizes were reported across different levels of loneliness, the effect that was reported as “often lonely” or “severe/chronic loneliness” was extracted. Also, when effect sizes by different type of loneliness were reported, the emotional loneliness value was selected. Throughout this work the term “articles” will be used for papers found in the systematic review, while the word “studies” will be employed for papers included in the meta-analysis where the analysis and the effect sizes are provided separately for men and women or for different age groups. If there were doubts about the analyses or if the methodology was not clear, authors were contacted by e-mail.

The articles included in the meta-analysis were assessed for quality using The Cochrane Risk of Bias Assessment Tool for Non-Randomized Studies of Interventions (ACROBAT-NSRI) [24]. This tool includes seven domains: 1) Bias due to confounding, 2) Bias in selection of participants, 3) Bias in measurement of interventions, 4) Bias due to departures from intended interventions, 5) Bias due to missing data, 6) Bias in measurement of outcomes (in this case mortality), and 7) Bias in selection of the reported result. Since this meta-analysis did not include articles of interventions items three, four, and five were omitted. The item related to bias due to missing data was not considered because the dependent variable was mortality and the majority of the articles evaluated it with death registries, so they did not have missing data, and furthermore the potential bias related to the cause of death was already evaluated in item 6. Additionally, a new item that evaluated bias in the measurement of the independent variable (loneliness) was added. The response options for an overall judgment are: low risk of bias, moderate risk, serious risk, critical risk and no information. To consider an article with low risk of bias it is necessary to score in all items low risk. If at least one item was evaluated with moderate risk, the article was evaluated as presenting moderate risk of bias, the same if at least one item was considered as serious risk of bias. For bias due to confounding it was considered low risk if the article adjusted for age, sex, health status (considering chronic diseases as a possible indicator), socioeconomic status (considering education and occupation as proxy variables), smoking, and depression or anxiety; for bias in selection of participants, it was observed whether it counted with consecutive or random recruitment of participants or representative populations; for bias in measurement of mortality it was checked if the information was retrieved from a complete assessment of vital status or from a national death registry; for bias in measurement of the independent variable it was checked if the ascertainment of loneliness was done with a validated instrument; finally, it was evaluated if there was no bias in the selection of the reported result. In order to obtain complete information of the quality of the article, if it was part of a survey or referred to another article, the citations were consulted.

Statistical analysis

The inter-rater agreement between the two researchers was estimated using the Kappa coefficient [25] with a confidence interval of the 95% and based on an analytical method [26]. The kappa value can be interpreted as follows: <0.20, poor; 0.21–0.40, fair; 0.41–0.60, moderate; 0.61–0.80, good; and 0.81–1.00, very good [27].

From the total articles included, those reporting a survival effect were used to conduct a meta-analysis. The effect size measures used from each article included were Hazard Ratios (HRs) and 95% CIs. Articles reporting Relative Risks (RRs) were also considered and combined with those reporting HRs. If the article did not report the confidence interval, it was calculated using the standard error. For each article included, the reported effect size (HRs or RRs) was transformed to the natural logarithms. The model used to meta-analyze the articles included was based on a random effects modeling, since it provides more conservative results than a fixed effects model [28] and assumes that each sample comes from a different population and that the effects in these populations may also differ [29]. In addition, the inverse variance weighted method was used to obtain an overall effect size and 95% CI. To evaluate if the association of loneliness with all-cause mortality is the same in women and in men, a meta-analysis was conducted by gender.

Different sensitivity analyses were carried out. Firstly, the magnitude of the effect of loneliness on all-cause mortality was assessed through a meta-analysis dividing articles with low or moderate risk of bias and articles with serious risk of bias. Secondly, a separate meta-analysis using the methodology described above was conducted over the articles reporting Odds Ratios (ORs) as an effect size measure, since ORs cannot be comparable with HRs or RRs [30]. And thirdly, in order to assess specifically the loneliness-mortality relationship independently from depression, a meta-analysis was done with the studies that controlled for depression.

The heterogeneity was evaluated by means of Cochran´s Q test at significant level of p < 0.10 [31] and quantified by the I2 statistic, considering a substantial level of heterogeneity to be 50% or more [32]. The I2 statistic indicates the proportion of the total variation due to that heterogeneity, while Cochran's Q measures whether the between-study variability in effect size exceeds that expected from corresponding within-study variability. Moreover, to identify potential sources of heterogeneity and characteristics related to the association of loneliness with all-cause mortality, a random effects meta-regression was employed. The characteristics considered in this analysis were: sample size (in thousands), gender of the sample (male, female or both), publication year, follow-up duration (in years), number of items of the instrument used to assess loneliness (only one item and more than one item) and quality (low or moderate risk of bias vs. serious risk of bias).

Finally, to detect publication bias, the degree of asymmetry was measured with Egger’s linear regression test [33] and Begg’s rank correlation test [34]. The former evaluates whether the association between estimated intervention effects and a measure of study size is greater than might be expected to occur by chance; and the latter assesses the correlation between test accuracy estimates and their variances. A funnel plot was done plotting the effect measure against the inverse of its standard error and included the fitted regression line from the Egger’s test for small study effects. It was considered likely publication bias if there was an asymmetric plot and p < 0.05. Data analysis was performed with Stata version 11 [35] using the commands metan, metabias and metareg.

Results

All databases provided 1907 articles. The number of records was reduced to 1684 after duplicates were removed. After reading titles and abstracts, 1608 were excluded because they did not meet all inclusion criteria. A full-text review of 80 articles was carried out; 76 came from the databases and 4 (code: 5, 6, 16, 25) were found after examining the meta-analysis of Holt-Lunstad et al. [23]. In total, 35 articles were included in the systematic review. One of them was published in Spanish [36] and the rest were written in English. Fig 1 shows the flow diagram containing the details of the articles included and excluded.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews (PRISMA) flow diagram.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190033.g001

A detailed description of the characteristics of the 35 articles included in the systematic review is reported in Table 1. From the 35 articles of the systematic review, a total of 43 studies were analyzed given that the articles that reported different effect sizes by gender (codes: 9a, 9b, 10a, 10b, 11a, 11b, 17a, 17b, 20a, 20b, 32a, 32b) or by age group (codes: 29a, 29b, 29c) were considered as different studies. For the general meta-analysis, only 24 articles were included from which 31 studies were analyzed. Twelve studies were excluded from the meta-analyses for several reasons: 5 of them reported ORs and were meta-analyzed separately (codes: 1, 5, 19, 21, 25); 6 more did not present the effect size data needed and it was not possible to obtain it even after contacting the authors (codes: 10a, 10b, 13, 15, 18, 30); and one was a meta-analysis (code: 8), whose studies are described in Table 1. Regarding the studies included in the meta-analyses, 29 were carried out in the general population (code: 3, 4, 6, 9a, 9b, 11a, 11b, 12, 14, 16, 17a, 17b, 20a, 20b, 22, 23, 24, 26, 27, 28, 29a, 29b, 29c, 31, 32a, 32b, 33, 34, 35) and 2 analyze clinical or institutionalized population (code: 2, 7).

thumbnail
Table 1. Overview of studies included in the systematic review.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190033.t001

Study characteristics and quality of the studies included in the meta-analysis

The percentage of agreement between the two independent researchers regarding whether to include or exclude each article was 98.4%, and the Kappa coefficient was 0.85 [95% CI = (0.72, 0.98)], showing a high agreement.

The characteristics of the studies included in the meta-analyses are reported in Table 2. From the 31 studies included in the meta-analysis, more than half considered both genders (61.29%), 6 analyzed only men (19.35%) and other 6 analyzed women (19.35%). The association of loneliness with mortality was evaluated in 51387 participants. A total of 12 (48.00%) studies had a follow-up longer than 10 years. Most of the studies reported all-cause mortality (80.65%) rather than a specific cause. Twenty studies (64.52%) evaluated loneliness with a single item, while eleven (35.48%) used an instrument with more than one item. Regarding the effect of loneliness on mortality, 58.06% reported null effect while 41.94% reported a harmful effect.

thumbnail
Table 2. Characteristics of the studies included in the general meta-analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190033.t002

Table 3 describes the five items and the overall score that evaluates the quality of each article included in the meta-analysis according to the ACROBAT-NSRI tool of the Cochrane group. Only 2 articles (8.33%) were qualified with a low risk of bias, 10 articles obtained a moderate risk of bias (41.67%), and 12 presented a serious risk of bias (50%).

thumbnail
Table 3. Quality evaluation of the articles included in the meta-analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190033.t003

Results of the meta-analysis

The association of loneliness with all-cause mortality of all the studies included in the meta-analysis is reported in Fig 2. This general meta-analysis analyzed 31 studies that came from 24 articles. The main characteristics, the effect size, the confidence interval and the percentage of weight of each study are displayed in that figure. A box has been assigned to each study; representing the weight that the study contributed to the meta-analysis. The overall combined HR were 1.22 [95% CI = (1.10, 1.35); p < 0.001], indicating a harmful effect of loneliness on all-cause mortality. In addition, a high heterogeneity between studies has been found (I2 = 94.4%), and the Cochran's Q test was significant (χ2(30) = 539.15, p < 0.001). Even excluding the six studies that only analyzed cardiovascular mortality, cancer mortality, and respiratory mortality (as can be observed in Table 2), the pooled HR associated to the remaining 25 studies was significant [HR = 1.21, 95% CI = (1.08, 1.35); p = 0.001] with a significant Cochran's Q test (χ2 (24) = 491.77, p < 0.001) and I2 = 95.1%.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Forest plot of the studies included in the meta-analysis.

Note: Forest plot displaying an inverse-variance weighted random-effect meta-analysis. The codes of this figure correspond to the codes of Table 1.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190033.g002

A similar result was obtained when a separate meta-analysis was conducted over the five studies reporting ORs as effect size measure (code: 1, 5, 19, 21, 25, according to the notation in Table 1). The pooled OR associated with the effect of loneliness on all-cause mortality was 1.15 [95% CI = (1.03, 1.28); p = 0.011], indicating that loneliness was a risk factor for all-cause mortality. In this sensitivity analysis, the Cochran's Q test was not significant (χ2(4) = 5.68, p = 0.23) and the level of heterogeneity was moderate (I2 = 29.5%). Moreover, when the meta-analysis was restricted to the 11 studies that included depression as a covariate, loneliness was also a risk factor for mortality [HR = 1.32, 95% CI = (1.06, 1.62); p < 0.001], with a significant Cochran's Q test (χ2 (10) = 451.55, p < 0.001) and I2 = 97.8%.

In reference to the meta-analysis carried out by gender (Fig 3), the overall HRs were 1.26 [95% CI = (1.07, 1.48); p = 0.005] for women and 1.44 [95% CI = (1.19, 1.76); p < 0.001] for men. In both groups loneliness was a risk factor for all-cause mortality. Although significant differences in the association with mortality were not found by gender, according to the overlapping of confidence intervals, the strength of the association was slightly higher in men than in women. The heterogeneity was high in both subgroups: I2 = 66.8% and a significant Cochran's Q test (χ2(6) = 18.06, p = 0.006) for women, and I2 = 71.5% and a significant Cochran's Q test (χ2(6) = 17.59, p = 0.007) for men.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Forest plot of the studies included in the meta-analysis by gender.

Note: Forest plot displaying an inverse-variance weighted random-effect meta-analysis. The codes of this figure correspond to the codes of Table 1.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190033.g003

Considering the risk of bias of the studies, low and moderate risk versus serious risk, loneliness was also a risk factor for all-cause mortality. In the group of serious risk of bias this effect was more significant [HR = 1.20, 95% CI = (1.12, 1.29); p < 0.001] than in the group of low and moderate [HR = 1.17, 95% CI = (1.00, 1.38); p = 0.050] risk. The Cochran's Q test was χ2 (15) = 516.24, p < 0.001 for low and moderate risk and χ2 (14) = 22.83, p = 0.063 for serious risk, I2 values were 97.1% and 38.7% respectively (Fig 4).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Forest plot of the studies included according to the risk of bias.

Note: Forest plot displaying an inverse-variance weighted random-effect meta-analysis. The codes of this figure correspond to the codes of Table 1.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190033.g004

In order to explore potential causes of heterogeneity and to analyze significant characteristics of the studies associated with the effect sizes obtained, a meta-regression was carried out to assess potential variables influencing the association between loneliness and all-cause mortality. However, none of the variables was found significant: the lowest p-value was found for follow-up [coef. = -0.02, 95% CI = (-0.04, 0.01); p = 0.08].

Based on the 31 studies included in the general analysis, potential publication bias was assessed. The publication bias is illustrated in Fig 5, where Begg's rank correlation test indicated no publication bias (p = 0.31), as well as Egger's linear regression: the estimated intercept for the fitted regression model was 0.60 with a standard error of 0.71, giving a p-value of 0.40. In Fig 5, the funnel plot appears symmetric with a distribution of the effect sizes mainly in the top and in the right side of the graph, suggesting no publication bias.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Funnel plot depicting the relationship between effect size and standard error of effect for the studies in the meta-analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190033.g005

Discussion

The overall meta-analysis shows that loneliness is a risk factor for all-cause mortality. This outcome is in line with a recently published meta-analysis that also analyzes the association of loneliness with mortality [23]. This result is consistent even after stratifying by gender, quality, and by studies that controlled for the effect of depression.

In the meta-analysis by gender, loneliness shows a tendency to be associated with all-cause mortality both in men and women. However, this effect was slightly higher in men than in women. An explanation of this difference could be that men are more reluctant to admit feelings of loneliness than women for cultural reasons [16]. Consequently, it might be that men report loneliness when its severity is high and consequently its impact is stronger. Moreover, women tend to associate loneliness with an evaluation of their overall network or relationships whereas men tend to associate this feeling with an evaluation of the relationship with their partner [19], and during the aging process the probabilities of becoming a widow increase, which might contribute to loneliness. Furthermore, widowhood has more adverse effects in men than in women and this might be because when men become widowed they have to readapt to new roles that could represent difficulties for them, like domestic tasks and assisting children [66]. The association of loneliness with health [9, 67, 68] and the fact that men generally have more negative attitudes towards care seeking [69] may also be implicated in this relationship. In addition, previous articles have shown that lonely men are more likely to suffer lower life satisfaction and higher depression, and are less resilient than lonely women [70]. In this line, some authors have suggested that the impact of social isolation on mortality might be greater in men because they experience increased inflammatory responses when they are alone than women [71]. Moreover, unhealthy lifestyles (i.e. tobacco and alcohol problems) have been associated with loneliness [72], and also more frequent in men [21], which could also explain the stronger loneliness-mortality connection in men than in women. However, the interaction of environmental and biological factors and their role needs to be further explored.

The effect of loneliness on mortality was independent from the quality of each article and the effect of depression. In spite of the frequent coexistence of loneliness with depression, particularly among older people, and the possible potential source of bias that this variable could be, it was observed that loneliness had a harmful effect on mortality in studies that controlled for depression and in studies that did not. The effect of loneliness goes beyond its associations with several health problems. Holt-Lunstad et al. [23] suggested that loneliness can be comparable with well-established risk factors for mortality. In view of the results found in this and in the previous meta-analysis, as well as in the literature that showed the harmful effect of loneliness on health [65, 7377], it seems that it is important to consider loneliness a topic of interest for public health. There is substantial body of literature that raises the warning regarding the effects of loneliness [3, 23, 7881].

The strengths of this work include its high sensitive search that covered all years and languages, and the inclusion of a revision of references of previous reviews and meta-analyses related to the topic of interest. Moreover, this meta-analysis updates the data regarding the association of loneliness with all-cause mortality and includes a higher number of articles and a higher number of participants compared with the previous meta-analysis. Additionally, an evaluation of the quality of each included article was done in order to analyze if the association of loneliness with all-cause mortality differs according to the quality of the studies. Furthermore, good agreement between the reviewers who did the double-check of the articles was found. To our knowledge, this is the first meta-analysis that evaluates this association of loneliness with all-cause mortality in both genders independently.

A number of limitations should be born in mind when interpreting the results. First, even though the included articles are longitudinal, causality cannot be inferred since all of them are observational. Second, only articles published in peer-reviewed journals were included, “grey literature” was excluded, which may have limited the findings. Third, the systematic review was done only in four databases (CINAHL, Pubmed, PsycINFO, and Scopus). These databases comprise a high number of articles focused in our area of interest and were chosen after consultation with an information specialist and carefully reading the descriptions of the databases. Fourth, high levels of heterogeneity, mainly in the analysis of low-moderate risk of bias studies, were obtained. Some reasons that might explain this high heterogeneity are: a high diversity of instruments used to measure loneliness, a large variety of covariates used in each study to control their effect in the association between loneliness and mortality, the wide range of publication year, the age differences analyzed in each study, and the contrast between the sample sizes. Fifth, in some cases it was not possible to obtain the necessary information to include some studies in the meta-analysis (e.g. standard deviation, confidence intervals, a measure of the effect size of loneliness, the sample size, or a comparable effect size value) even after contacting the authors. Sixth, the double-checked was done only for the 20% of the articles after removing the duplicates. Previous studies also re-inspected 20% [8286].

Despite these limitations, some conclusions can be drawn from this article. Loneliness is a risk for all-cause mortality and this effect is slightly stronger in men than in women. The harmful of effect of loneliness on mortality was consistent across studies with different quality as well as when depression was considered as a covariate. Qualitative studies that help to understand the differential experience and the possible related factors to loneliness in men and women, and articles that use validated questionnaires for loneliness, are needed. Further studies should evaluate the association of loneliness with all-cause mortality across age, especially in the young population. Only five articles analyzed in this work had a sample younger than 50 years [37, 40, 58, 61, 63]. Besides, more articles with clinical or institutionalized population are needed since only two studies [11, 40] of those included in the meta-analysis performed analyses with this population.

Understanding the differential impact of loneliness in women and men is crucial to develop a better understanding of the nature of these feelings and approach the circumstances of the risk group. More research by gender is required to clarify and fully explore the possible association of loneliness with all-cause mortality and to suggest future recommendations in relation to prevention and treatment.

Supporting information

S1 Dataset. Dataset providing the information of all the articles included in the meta-analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190033.s001

(DTA)

S1 Text. Detailed protocol followed to carry out the meta-analyses.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0190033.s002

(DOC)

References

  1. 1. Andersson L. Loneliness research and interventions: a review of the literature. Aging & Mental Health. 1998;2(4):264–74.
  2. 2. Hansen T, Slagsvold B. Late-Life Loneliness in 11 European Countries: Results from the Generations and Gender Survey. Social Indicators Research. 2016:445–64.
  3. 3. Linehan T, Bottery S, Kaye A, Millar L, Sinclair D, Watson J. 2030 vision: The best and worst futures for older people in the UK 2014. http://www.ilcuk.org.uk/index.php/publications/publication_details/2030_vision_the_best_and_worst_futures_for_older_people_in_the_uk.
  4. 4. Cacioppo JT, Hughes ME, Waite LJ, Hawkley LC, Thisted RA. Loneliness as a specific risk factor for depressive symptoms: cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Psychology and aging. 2006;21(1):140–51. pmid:16594799.
  5. 5. Cacioppo JT, Hawkley LC, Thisted RA. Perceived social isolation makes me sad: 5-year cross-lagged analyses of loneliness and depressive symptomatology in the Chicago Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study. Psychology and aging. 2010;25(2):453–63. pmid:20545429.
  6. 6. Åkerlind I, Hörnquist J. Loneliness and alcohol abuse: A review of evidences of an interplay. Social science & medicine. 1992;34(4):405–14.
  7. 7. Cacioppo JT, Hawkley LC, Crawford LE, Ernst JM, Burleson MH, Kowalewski RB, et al. Loneliness and Health: Potential Mechanisms. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2002;64:407–17. pmid:12021415
  8. 8. Pressman SD, Cohen S, Miller GE, Barkin A, Rabin BS, Treanor JJ. Loneliness, social network size, and immune response to influenza vaccination in college freshmen. Health Psychol. 2005;24(3):297–306. pmid:15898866.
  9. 9. Wilson RS, Krueger KR, Arnold SE, Schneider JA, Kelly JF, Barnes LL, et al. Loneliness and risk of Alzheimer disease. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(2):234–40. pmid:17283291.
  10. 10. Rico-Uribe LA, Caballero FF, Olaya B, Tobiasz-Adamczyk B, Koskinen S, Leonardi M, et al. Loneliness, Social Networks, and Health: A Cross-Sectional Study in Three Countries. PLoS One. 2016;11(1):e0145264. pmid:26761205.
  11. 11. Drageset J, Eide GE, Kirkevold M, Ranhoff AH. Emotional loneliness is associated with mortality among mentally intact nursing home residents with and without cancer: a five-year follow-up study. J Clin Nurs. 2013;22(1–2):106–14. pmid:22882807.
  12. 12. Perissinotto CM, Stijacic Cenzer I, Covinsky KE. Loneliness in older persons: a predictor of functional decline and death. Arch Intern Med. 2012;172(14):1078–83. pmid:22710744.
  13. 13. Luo Y, Waite LJ. Loneliness and mortality among older adults in China. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2014;69(4):633–45. pmid:24550354.
  14. 14. Tilvis RS, Laitala V, Routasalo PE, Pitkala KH. Suffering from loneliness indicates significant mortality risk of older people. J Aging Res. 2011;2011:534781. pmid:21423600.
  15. 15. World Health Organization. Life expectancy Data by WHO region 2016. http://apps.who.int/gho/data/view.main.SDG2016LEXREGv?lang=en.
  16. 16. Dahlberg L, Andersson L, McKee KJ, Lennartsson C. Predictors of loneliness among older women and men in Sweden: A national longitudinal study. Aging Ment Health. 2015;19(5):409–17. pmid:25126996.
  17. 17. Pinquart M, Sorensen S. Influences on Loneliness in Older Adults: A Meta-Analysis. Basic and applied social psychology. 2001;23(4):245–66.
  18. 18. Borys S, Perlman D. Gender Differences in Loneliness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 1985;11(1):63–74.
  19. 19. Tijhuis MA, De Jong-Gierveld J, Feskens EJ, Kromhout D. Changes in and factors related to loneliness in older men. The Zutphen Elderly Study. Age Ageing. 1999;28(5):491–5. pmid:10529046.
  20. 20. Kuehner C. Why is depression more common among women than among men? The Lancet Psychiatry. 2016.
  21. 21. Mendis S. Global status report on noncommunicable diseases 2014. 2014.
  22. 22. Aartsen M, Jylha M. Onset of loneliness in older adults: results of a 28 year prospective study. Eur J Ageing. 2011;8(1):31–8. pmid:21475393.
  23. 23. Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Baker M, Harris T, Stephenson D. Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015;10(2):227–37. pmid:25910392.
  24. 24. Sterne J, Higgins J, Reeves B, on behalf of the development group for ACROBAT-NRSI. A cochrane risk of bias assessment tool: for non-randomized studies of interventions (ACROBAT-NRSI), version 1.0. 2014.
  25. 25. Cohen J. A Coefficient Of Agreement For Nominal Scales. Educ Psychol Meas. 1960;20:37–46.
  26. 26. Fleiss JL. Statistical Methods for Rates and Proportions1981.
  27. 27. Altman DG. Practical statistics for medical research. London: Chapman and Hall 1991.
  28. 28. Jackson D, White IR, Thompson SG. Extending DerSimonian and Laird's methodology to perform multivariate random effects meta-analyses. Stat Med. 2010;29(12):1282–97. pmid:19408255.
  29. 29. Tak LM, Meijer A, Manoharan A, de Jonge P, Rosmalen JG. More than the sum of its parts: meta-analysis and its potential to discover sources of heterogeneity in psychosomatic medicine. Psychosom Med. 2010;72(3):253–65. pmid:20395625.
  30. 30. Twisk J. Causal inference in longitudinal experimental research. In: Menard S, editor. Handbook of Longitudinal Research: Design, Measurement, and Analysis. Amsterdam: Elsevier/Academic Press; 2008. p. 279–94.
  31. 31. Hedges LV, Olkin I. Statistical methods for meta-analysis: Academic press; 2014.
  32. 32. Higgins JP, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. Bmj. 2003;327(7414):557–60. pmid:12958120
  33. 33. Egger M, Smith GD, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. Bmj. 1997;315(7109):629–34. pmid:9310563
  34. 34. Begg CB, Mazumdar M. Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics. 1994:1088–101. pmid:7786990
  35. 35. StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software: Release 11. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP 2010.
  36. 36. Meller I, Fichter MM, Schröppel H. Riesgo de mortalidad en octogenarios y nonagenarios en relación con síntomas psíquicos, causa y lugar de fallecimiento: Resultados longitudinales de un estudio epidemiológico de seguimiento en una muestra comunitaria. The European journal of psychiatry (edición en español). 2004;18(1):45–61.
  37. 37. Eaker ED, Pinsky J, Castelli WP. Myocardial infarction and coronary death among women: psychosocial predictors from a 20-year follow-up of women in the Framingham Study. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1992;135(8):854–64. pmid:1585898
  38. 38. Ellwardt L, van Tilburg T, Aartsen M, Wittek R, Steverink N. Personal networks and mortality risk in older adults: a twenty-year longitudinal study. PLoS One. 2015;10(3):e0116731. pmid:25734570.
  39. 39. Grand A, Grosclaude P, Bocquet H, Pous J, Albaredez J. Disability, psychosocial factors and mortality among the elderly in a rural French population. Journal of clinical epidemiology. 1990;43(8):773–82. pmid:2143526
  40. 40. Herlitz J, Wiklund I, Caidahl K, Hartford M, Haglid M, Karlsson B, et al. The feeling of loneliness prior to coronary artery bypass grafting might be a predictor of short-and long-term postoperative mortality. European Journal of Vascular and Endovascular Surgery. 1998;16(2):120–5. pmid:9728430
  41. 41. Holwerda TJ, Beekman AT, Deeg DJ, Stek ML, van Tilburg TG, Visser PJ, et al. Increased risk of mortality associated with social isolation in older men: only when feeling lonely? Results from the Amsterdam Study of the Elderly (AMSTEL). Psychol Med. 2012;42(4):843–53. pmid:21896239.
  42. 42. Julsing JE, Kromhout D, Geleijnse JM, Giltay EJ. Loneliness and All-Cause, Cardiovascular, and Noncardiovascular Mortality in Older Men: The Zutphen Elderly Study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2016;24(6):475–84. pmid:27066732.
  43. 43. Jylhä M, Aro S. Social ties and survival among the elderly in Tampere, Finland. International Journal of Epidemiology. 1989;18(1):158–64. pmid:2722360
  44. 44. Levy BR, Myers LM. Relationship between respiratory mortality and self-perceptions of aging. Psychology & Health. 2005;20(5):553–64.
  45. 45. Luo Y, Hawkley LC, Waite LJ, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness, health, and mortality in old age: a national longitudinal study. Soc Sci Med. 2012;74(6):907–14. pmid:22326307.
  46. 46. Maier H, Smith J. Psychological predictors of mortality in old age. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 1999;54(1):P44–P54.
  47. 47. Olsen RB, Olsen J, Gunner-Svensson F, Waldstrøm B. Social networks and longevity. A 14 year follow-up study among elderly in Denmark. Social science & medicine. 1991;33(10):1189–95.
  48. 48. Penninx BW, Van Tilburg T, Kriegsman DM, Deeg DJ, Boeke AJP, van Eijk JTM. Effects of social support and personal coping resources on mortality in older age: the Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam. American journal of epidemiology. 1997;146(6):510–9. pmid:9290512
  49. 49. Pitkala K, Laakkonen M-L, Strandberg TE, Tilvis RS. Positive life orientation as a predictor of 10-year outcome in an aged population. Journal of clinical epidemiology. 2004;57(4):409–14. pmid:15135844
  50. 50. Shiovitz-Ezra S, Ayalon L. Situational versus chronic loneliness as risk factors for all-cause mortality. Int Psychogeriatr. 2010;22(3):455–62. pmid:20003631.
  51. 51. Stek ML, Vinkers DJ, Gussekloo J, Beekman AT, van der Mast RC, Westendorp RG. Is depression in old age fatal only when people feel lonely? American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162(1):178–80. pmid:15625218
  52. 52. Steptoe A, Shankar A, Demakakos P, Wardle J. Social isolation, loneliness, and all-cause mortality in older men and women. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2013;110(15):5797–801.
  53. 53. Stessman J, Rottenberg Y, Shimshilashvili I, Ein-Mor E, Jacobs JM. Loneliness, health, and longevity. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2014;69(6):744–50. pmid:24077598.
  54. 54. Tilvis R, Routasalo P, Karppinen H, Strandberg T, Kautiainen H, Pitkala K. Social isolation, social activity and loneliness as survival indicators in old age; a nationwide survey with a 7-year follow-up. European Geriatric Medicine. 2012;3(1):18–22.
  55. 55. Tilvis RS, Laitala V, Routasalo P, Strandberg TE, Pitkala KH. Positive life orientation predicts good survival prognosis in old age. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2012;55(1):133–7. pmid:21764146.
  56. 56. Zhen Z, Feng Q, Gu D. The impacts of unmet needs for long-term care on mortality among older adults in China. Journal of Disability Policy Studies. 2015;25(4):243–51.
  57. 57. Cuijpers P. Mortality and depressive symptoms in inhabitants of residential homes. International journal of geriatric psychiatry. 2001;16(2):131–8. pmid:11241717
  58. 58. Giraldi T, Rodani M, Cartei G, Grassi L. Psychosocial factors and breast cancer: a 6-year Italian follow-up study. Psychotherapy and psychosomatics. 1997;66(5):229–36. pmid:9311026
  59. 59. Iecovich E, Jacobs JM, Stessman J. Loneliness, social networks, and mortality: 18 years of follow-up. The International Journal of Aging and Human Development. 2011;72(3):243–63. pmid:21834390
  60. 60. Ljungquist B, Sundström G. Health and social networks as predictors of survival in old age. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health. 1996;24(2):90–101.
  61. 61. Miller GE, Kemeny ME, Taylor SE, Cole SW, Visscher BR. Social relationships and immune processes in HIV seropositive gay and bisexual men. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 1997;19(2):139–51. pmid:9603689
  62. 62. Newall NE, Chipperfield JG, Bailis DS, Stewart TL. Consequences of loneliness on physical activity and mortality in older adults and the power of positive emotions. Health Psychol. 2013;32(8):921–4. pmid:22888819.
  63. 63. Patterson AC, Veenstra G. Loneliness and risk of mortality: A longitudinal investigation in Alameda County, California. Social science & medicine. 2010;71(1):181–6.
  64. 64. Shahtahmasebi S, Davies R, Wenger GC. A longitudinal analysis of factors related to survival in old age. The Gerontologist. 1992;32(3):404–13. pmid:1500005
  65. 65. Sugisawa H, Liang J, Liu X. Social networks, social support, and mortality among older people in Japan. Journal of Gerontology. 1994;49(1):S3–S13. pmid:8282987
  66. 66. Lee GR, DeMaris A, Bavin S, Sullivan R. Gender differences in the depressive effect of widowhood in later life. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2001;56(1):S56–S61.
  67. 67. Hawkley LC, Preacher KJ, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness impairs daytime functioning but not sleep duration. Health Psychol. 2010;29(2):124–9. pmid:20230084.
  68. 68. Hawkley LC, Thisted RA, Masi CM, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness predicts increased blood pressure: 5-year cross-lagged analyses in middle-aged and older adults. Psychology and aging. 2010;25(1):132–41. pmid:20230134.
  69. 69. Buffel V, Van de Velde S, Bracke P. Professional care seeking for mental health problems among women and men in Europe: the role of socioeconomic, family-related and mental health status factors in explaining gender differences. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2014;49(10):1641–53. pmid:24802317.
  70. 70. Zebhauser A, Hofmann-Xu L, Baumert J, Häfner S, Lacruz M, Emeny R, et al. How much does it hurt to be lonely? Mental and physical differences between older men and women in the KORA-Age Study. International journal of geriatric psychiatry. 2014;29(3):245–52. pmid:23804458
  71. 71. Yang YC, McClintock MK, Kozloski M, Li T. Social isolation and adult mortality: the role of chronic inflammation and sex differences. J Health Soc Behav. 2013;54(2):183–203. pmid:23653312.
  72. 72. Stickley A, Koyanagi A, Roberts B, Richardson E, Abbott P, Tumanov S, et al. Loneliness: its correlates and association with health behaviours and outcomes in nine countries of the former Soviet Union. PLoS One. 2013;8(7):e67978. pmid:23861843.
  73. 73. Domenech-Abella J, Lara E, Rubio-Valera M, Olaya B, Moneta MV, Rico-Uribe LA, et al. Loneliness and depression in the elderly: the role of social network. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2017;52(4):381–90. pmid:28154893.
  74. 74. Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness matters: a theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Ann Behav Med. 2010;40(2):218–27. pmid:20652462.
  75. 75. Stephens C, Alpass F, Towers A, Stevenson B. The effects of types of social networks, perceived social support, and loneliness on the health of older people: accounting for the social context. J Aging Health. 2011;23(6):887–911. pmid:21398571.
  76. 76. Luanaigh CO, Lawlor BA. Loneliness and the health of older people. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2008;23(12):1213–21. pmid:18537197.
  77. 77. Peerenboom L, Collard RM, Naarding P, Comijs HC. The association between depression and emotional and social loneliness in older persons and the influence of social support, cognitive functioning and personality: A cross-sectional study. J Affect Disord. 2015;182:26–31. pmid:25965692.
  78. 78. Cacioppo JT, Fowler JH, Christakis NA. Alone in the crowd: the structure and spread of loneliness in a large social network. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2009;97(6):977–91. pmid:19968414.
  79. 79. Cacioppo JT, Hawkley LC, Ernst JM, Burleson M, Berntson GG, Nouriani B, et al. Loneliness within a nomological net: An evolutionary perspective. Journal of research in personality. 2006;40(6):1054–85.
  80. 80. Cacioppo JT, Cacioppo S. Social Relationships and Health: The Toxic Effects of Perceived Social Isolation. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2014;8(2):58–72. pmid:24839458.
  81. 81. Valtorta N, Hanratty B. Loneliness, isolation and the health of older adults: do we need a new research agenda? J R Soc Med. 2012;105(12):518–22. pmid:23288086.
  82. 82. Martin-Maria N, Miret M, Caballero FF, Rico-Uribe LA, Steptoe A, Chatterji S, et al. The Impact of Subjective Well-being on Mortality: A Meta-Analysis of Longitudinal Studies in the General Population. Psychosom Med. 2017;79(5):565–75. pmid:28033196.
  83. 83. Kamenov K, Twomey C, Cabello M, Prina AM, Ayuso-Mateos JL. The efficacy of psychotherapy, pharmacotherapy and their combination on functioning and quality of life in depression: a meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2017;47(3):414–25. pmid:27780478.
  84. 84. Raggi A, Giovannetti AM, Quintas R, D'Amico D, Cieza A, Sabariego C, et al. A systematic review of the psychosocial difficulties relevant to patients with migraine. J Headache Pain. 2012;13(8):595–606. pmid:23001069.
  85. 85. Shen X, Xia J, Adams CE. Flupenthixol versus placebo for schizophrenia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2012;11:CD009777. pmid:23152280.
  86. 86. Zhao S, Sampson S, Xia J, Jayaram MB. Psychoeducation (brief) for people with serious mental illness. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2015; (4):CD010823. pmid:25854522.