Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Mental toughness latent profiles in endurance athletes

  • Joanna S. Zeiger ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft

    joannazeiger@comcast.net

    Affiliation Race Ready Coaching, Boulder, CO, United States of America

  • Robert S. Zeiger

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Allergy Kaiser Permanente Southern California Region, San Diego, CA, United States of America, Department of Research and Evaluation, Kaiser Permanente Southern California Region, Pasadena, California, United States of America

Abstract

Mental toughness in endurance athletes, while an important factor for success, has been scarcely studied. An online survey was used to examine eight mental toughness factors in endurance athletes. The study aim was to determine mental toughness profiles via latent profile analysis in endurance athletes and whether associations exist between the latent profiles and demographics and sports characteristics. Endurance athletes >18 years of age were recruited via social media outlets (n = 1245, 53% female). Mental toughness was measured using the Sports Mental Toughness Questionnaire (SMTQ), Psychological Performance Inventory-Alternative (PPI-A), and self-esteem was measured using the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSE). A three-class solution emerged, designated as high mental toughness (High MT), moderate mental toughness (Moderate MT) and low mental toughness (Low MT). ANOVA tests showed significant differences between all three classes on all 8 factors derived from the SMTQ, PPI-A and the RSE. There was an increased odds of being in the High MT class compared to the Low MT class for males (OR = 1.99; 95% CI, 1.39, 2.83; P<0.001), athletes who were over 55 compared to those who were 18–34 (OR = 2.52; 95% CI, 1.37, 4.62; P<0.01), high sports satisfaction (OR = 8.17; 95% CI, 5.63, 11.87; P<0.001), and high division placement (OR = 2.18; 95% CI, 1.46,3.26; P<0.001). The data showed that mental toughness latent profiles exist in endurance athletes. High MT is associated with demographics and sports characteristics. Mental toughness screening in athletes may help direct practitioners with mental skills training.

Introduction

Understanding the psychological underpinnings of mental toughness (MT) in endurance athletes is an important issue because training physical attributes within an athlete is finite (i.e. overtraining can lead to injuries, burnout, or performance decrements) [1,2] while detecting and training weaknesses in MT have no such limitations [3,4]. Because athletes represent a high risk population for mental health problems which can impact performance and well-being [5], identifying MT factors that may be underlying such problems can guide early interventions. Indeed, higher levels of MT cross-over from success in sports to parameters of improved sleep quality [6], higher life control and interpersonal confidence [7], high levels of subjective and objective performance [8] and a healthier lifestyle [9]; it has been suggested that individuals with higher MT exhibit greater emotional control which then leads to better lifestyle choices [9]. “Specifically, individuals with higher levels of MT are less likely to believe that the demands imposed by a given situation exceed their available coping resources.”[10]

Despite the understanding that some degree of MT is necessary for effective endurance sports performance [11,12], actually defining MT has vexed researchers, with a clear classification elusive. Initially, MT was viewed in relation to one’s opponents, with more recent work focusing on “subjective or goals-directed dimensions” [13] Often a series of traits or skills (collectively recognized as MT factors) have been used to “define” MT in lieu of a proper delineation [13,14,8]. It is theorized that MT is an umbrella for multiple dimensions, inclusion of which are eight potential factors: generalized self-efficacy, buoyancy, success mindset, optimistic style, context knowledge, emotion regulation, attention regulation [8] MT has thusly been described:

MT [is] a personal capacity to produce consistently high levels of subjective (e.g., personal goals or strivings) or objective performance (e.g., sales, race time, GPA) despite everyday challenges and stressors as well as significant adversities [8]. MT can [further] be defined as a state-like psychological resource that is purposeful, flexible, and efficient in nature for the enactment and maintenance of goal-directed pursuits [13].

Components of overall MT include a combination of attributes, characteristics, and strategies[13]: goal setting, visualization, stress management, emotion control, confidence, persistence, rebounding from failure, and positive-cognition [14,15]. An effort to understand the factors that comprise MT in athletes and how these factors affect performance outcomes led to the development of multi-dimensional questionnaires [12,16,17]. Two such questionnaires are the Psychological Performance Inventory-Alternative (PPI-A) and the Sports Mental Toughness Questionnaire (SMTQ) [12,17,18]. Studies using these two MT inventories have found associations between higher MT and coping, optimism [19], hardiness, successful sports performance [20], and positive energy control [21]. The MTQ48 has also been widely used to examine six subscales of MT [9,19,22], with this questionnaire showing that MT is associated with measures of academic success [9], better problem approach coping [19,23], and improved dispositional flow [24].

“MT is positioned within a broader category of concepts that are centrally valued in their own right (e.g. self-esteem, close attachments, health, and inner peace)”[13]. MT questionnaires do not specifically measure global self-esteem; close comparisons would be confidence or self-belief, however these constructs are more akin to specific self-esteem and is different than global self-esteem [25]. “Global self-esteem does predict behavior and specific self-esteem does predict psychological well-being,” but these effects mediate each other [25]. It seems, then, that measuring global self-esteem in athletes is useful in the context of MT since self-esteem and sports are intertwined. Studies of both elite and amateur athletes use measures of global self-esteem to identify its direct and moderating/mediating effect on performance [2628]. The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSE) measures global self-esteem [29].

Global self-esteem impacts athletes both in their sporting life and general life. Individuals with higher levels of global self-esteem show higher levels of well-being and satisfaction with life [30,31], Self-esteem is higher in those who exercise [32,33]. Self-esteem does seem to cross over between sports and life, as self-esteem mediated the relationship between physical activity and quality of life [34]. These relationships are important, as enjoyment of sport increases participation rates which in turn increases overall happiness [35]. Overall levels of well-being and happiness in athletes are positively related to sports performance [36,37]. In addition, higher levels of self-esteem are related to lower levels of self-handicapping prior to sporting events [38], and self-handicapping has a deleterious effect on performance [38]. Athletes with high self-esteem present with more positive patterns of perfectionism, specifically these athletes showed less concerns over mistakes and fewer doubts about their actions, which in turn relate to performance gains [28]. The addition of a measure of global self-esteem can add to the total picture of an athlete’s ability to adapt in sporting settings, particularly since higher levels of self-esteem are related to lower levels of anxiety [39]. High self-esteem, in the context of performance, is related to better self-regulation; this means that when there is no alternative way to accomplish the task persistence is higher, but, when persistence is a poor strategy those with high self-esteem know when to quit [40]. The distinction between persistence and quitting is important in a sporting context where negative outcomes can occur by persisting in the face of danger.

The study of MT in athletes should extend beyond excelling in one’s chosen sport as “acquiring a mindset of mental toughness might be one way that physical activity and exercise can impact an individuals’ mental health”[41]. This sentiment underscores the notion that development of MT can be beneficial outside the sporting arena, but, the sporting arena can be the anchor for such development. As such, including several markers for MT development is imperative, because an overall MT score from a single questionnaire does not explain the whole picture, revealing only a snapshot of an athlete’s MT strengths and weaknesses, particularly for questionnaires that only measure a few MT factors.

The SMTQ measures three MT traits (confidence, constancy, control), while the PPI-A measures two traits (determination, self-belief) and two practices (visualization, positive-cognition). Using the SMTQ and PPI-A in conjunction with a measure of self-esteem (e.g. the Rosenberg Self-Esteem scale) offers a more comprehensive insight into an athlete’s MT, and through latent profile analysis, an even more complete understanding of MT within and between athletes can be derived. Indeed, many of the studies regarding mental toughness in athletes have focused on elite athletes or the psychometric properties of the measurement tools with only a few studies examining how MT is related to performance, cognitions, or behaviors [7]. Examining the MT latent profile structure in endurance athletes can help expand the knowledge base about MT, namely, whether athletes fall into MT categories in which they excel or need improvement across all of the studied factors or if there are MT factor variations (e.g. high in some factors, low in others) in endurance athletes. Creating latent classes allows for the construction of subgroups characterized by multiple dimensions, a global MT type, and how these MT types differ with respect to important outcomes of performance, psychological characteristics, and demographics.

Cluster analysis has historically been used to identify MT profiles in athletes. Affective intensity and directionality were measured in athletes. Positive affective profiles were associated with better coping [42]. A cluster analysis of MT using the Psychological Performance Inventory and the Task and Ego Orientations in Sport Questionnaire in 40 athletes revealed a three-cluster solution [21]. The clusters differed on total MT, as well as showing mean differences on MT sub-scales and there were significant differences between clusters on energy control [21].

Studies examining MT using cluster analysis have had small sample sizes, [21,43] been skewed toward males [44,45], and mostly conducted in team sports [44,45]. Latent profile analysis, a more robust method than cluster analysis, using mental toughness measures has not been conducted in endurance athletes. Furthermore, previous studies of cluster analysis did not use multiple MT measures to create a comprehensive athlete MT profile nor have previous studies examined MT profiles in relation to demographics, performance, and satisfaction. The creation of MT profiles lends itself to applications of detecting low MT athletes and using interventions to change thoughts and behaviors.

As such, we aimed to identify the number of MT classes using a latent profile analysis (LPA), discern the number of athletes within each class, and characterize the MT profile of each class.

We hypothesized that MT in endurance athletes is comprised of latent classes that can be generated by the clustering of seven mental toughness factors as measured by the SMTQ (confidence, constancy, control), PPI-A (determination, visualization, positive cognition, self-belief), and self-esteem as measured by the RSE; the validity of the MT latent profiles was tested against demographics, sports characteristics, division placement, and race satisfaction.

Methods

Participants

This quantitative, survey study used a convenience sample. The study was approved with waiver of written consent by Solutions IRB (http://www.solutionsirb.com). Participants were assured confidentiality. Implied consent was provided by survey completion. Participants were required to be, (1) ages 18 years or older, (2) a self-declared endurance athlete, and (3) English speaking. There were no other inclusions or exclusions. The survey was administered on SurveyGizmo (https://www.surveygizmo.com) between 29 April 2016 and 12 May 2016.

Social media and email communication were used for subject recruitment, allowing for large scale targeting of potential subjects in a relatively short time. Recruitment was researcher-initiated through social media using direct posting of the recruitment call-to-action posted on Facebook pages and dedicated to various endurance athletic sports (e.g. triathlon, swimming, ultra-running, and cycling). Postings were shared by individual athletes on their personal Facebook pages. Postings were also placed on Twitter, LinkedIn, websites dedicated to endurance sports, and emails sent directly to coaches and athletes.

Measures

Three measurement tools were used to determine the MT latent class profiles: the Sports Mental Toughness Questionnaire (SMTQ) [17], the Psychological Performance Inventory-Alternative (PPPI-A) [12] and the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSE) [46].

The three tools were used in combination because the statistical power to correctly identify the number of classes in the latent profile analysis is increased with a higher number of indicator variables [47,48]. Although the eight indicator variables from the three tools were significantly correlated with correlations ranging from 0.18 to 0.63, there was no evidence of multicollinearity as measured by the variance inflation factors which were all less than 2.39 in a regression model [49]. The SMTQ and PPI-A have been used in combination in other studies [15,18].

Mental toughness.

The SMTQ is a 14-item tool that measures total MT and has three sub-scales: confidence (e.g. “I have an unshakable confidence in my ability”), control (e.g. “I am committed to the tasks I have to do”), and constancy (e.g. “I worry about performing poorly”) [17]. The responses are on a 4-point Likert scale anchored by not at all true and very true. Confirmatory factor analysis has shown excellent psychometric properties [17], and the measurement tool has been highly correlated with other MT scales such as the MTQ48 (r = 0.75) [11].

The PPI-A is a 14-item tool that is based on the original PPI [50]. Four sub-scales were identified in the PPI-A: determination (e.g. “The goals I’ve set for myself as a player keep me working hard”), self-belief (“I lose my confidence very quickly”), visualization (e.g. “Thinking in picture about my sport comes easy for me”), and positive-cognition (“I can clear interfering emotion quickly and regain my focus”). The PPI-A responses are on a 5-point Likert scale anchored by almost always and almost never. Confirmatory factor analysis indicated a good fit and high factor loadings with low standard errors (Golby et al., 2007). The PPI-A has been associated with sports performance [51].

Self-esteem.

The RSE is a 10-item tool that measures global self-esteem with responses on a 4-point Likert scale anchored by strongly agree and strongly disagree (e.g. “On the whole, I am satisfied with myself”)[46]. Higher scores indicate higher levels of self-esteem. The RSE has shown good construct validity with high factor loadings on a single-factor model [52].

Sports characteristics

Sports characteristics were also measured: the participant’s sport (running, triathlon, swimming, cycling, and other), years involved in the sport measured as a categorical variable (1–2 years, 3–5 years, 6–10 years, more than 10 years), hours per week of training (0–5 hours, 6–10 hours, 11–15 hours, 16 or more hours), race placement in the athlete’s division (top 10, 11–20, 21 or higher), and how often the athlete was satisfied with their race results (always, often, sometimes, rarely/never).

Procedures

Descriptive, univariate, and multivariable analyses were conducted using SPSS v23. Scores were created for the factors of the SMTQ, PPI-A, and the RSE by summing the items within the factor. For every factor, a higher score indicated a higher degree of the measured factor. Descriptive statistics, correlations, and Cronbach’s alpha were measured for each of the factors.

Latent profile analysis (LPA), a model-based cluster analysis of the mixture modeling family was used to classify related individuals [53]. The use of categorical indicator variables in a mixture model is a latent class analysis whereas continuous indicator variables, such as those used in these analyses, is a LPA [54]. The advantage of LPA versus clustering methods is the statistical assignment of an individual to a latent class. This allows for comparisons between different LPA models and the computation of a posterior probability of that individual’s membership to that latent class. An average posterior probability for each class can then be calculated with higher average posterior probabilities indicating a better fitting model [53]. Another advantage of LCA is that it is scale independent; therefore, the data does not need to be standardized [53] and the assumptions of linearity and normality of data do not need to be met [55].

A series of models with increasing number of classes, from 1 to 4, was conducted to determine the best fitting model. Akaike’s Information Criteria (AIC) and Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC) were used to compare model fit, with lower values indicating a better fit [56]. The Vuong-Lo-Mendell-Rubin Likelihood ratio test (VLMR test) was also used, which compares n classes with n– 1 classes. A significant test indicates that the n-class solution is better than the n– 1 class solution [56]. Entropy, a measurement of predictive power where 0 indicates no predictive power and 1 indicates perfect prediction, was examined [53]. Finally, the average posterior probabilities for the different class solutions were considered; a model with a good fit would have high individual probabilities to a single class. LPA was conducted in Mplus version 7 [57]. Eight variables were included in the LPA: three factors from the SMTQ, four factors from the PPI-A, and the single factor RSE. Eta squared was calculated as a measure of the effect sizes [58].

After latent class identification, ANOVA tests were conducted to compare mean scores for the eight factors between the classes and a series of chi-square tests of association examined whether class membership differed by the demographic variables (age, gender), sports characteristics (years competing in sports, hours per week of training, primary sport), and sports outcomes (placement in division, satisfaction). Multinomial logistic regression was used to examine the joint relationships between the classes and the demographics, sports characteristics, and performance variables and to test for potential interactions.

Results

Descriptive statistics and preliminary analyses

There was a total of 1,256 respondents, of which 11 were did not complete the entire questionnaire and were excluded from the analyses. The remaining 1,245 participants included 53% female, 23% were 18–34, 34% were 35–44, 27% were 45–54, 16% were older than 55, 84% earned a Bachelor’s degree or higher, and 90% of White ethnicity. The primary sports recorded were triathlon (54%) and running (37%), with 43% of the participants competing for 10 or more years and 45% training 6–10 hours per week. Facebook was the primary recruitment site (54%) (Table 1).

thumbnail
Table 1. Characteristics of the adult endurance athlete participants (n = 1,245).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193071.t001

There was a significant difference between males and females in category placement (χ2(2) = 12.55, P = 0.002), with males more often placing in the top 10 than females (49% vs. 45%). There were also significant gender differences in the number of years competing in their sport (χ2(2) = 38.17, P<0.001). Only 6.4% of males were in their sport for 1–2 years compared to 11.6% of females, and 51.6% of males participated for 10 or more years compared to 34.8% of females. There were no gender differences in the number of hours per week spent training or how often a subject was satisfied with their race performances.

Internal consistency for the factors was measured by McDonald’s omega, using JASP, which is considered a more accurate approximation of a scale’s structure than alpha [59,60]. Omega ranged from 0.60 to 0.82 (Table 2). Males had higher mean scores than females for confidence (18.9 vs. 18.1, P<0.001), control (11.6 vs. 10.7, P<0.001), self-belief (16.3 vs. 15.7, P<0.001) and self-esteem (32.5 vs. 31.6, P<0.001).

thumbnail
Table 2. Means, standard deviations, alpha, and correlations (95% CI) for study variables.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193071.t002

Latent profile analysis

The four LPA model comparisons indicated that a three-class solution was the best fit to the data (Table 3). The four-class model had the lowest AIC and BIC values, however, the VLMR test was not significant, indicating that four classes were not better than three classes. The three-class solution had lower AIC and BIC values than the two-class solution, a significant VLMR test, and an entropy value of 0.80. In the three-class solution, the mean posterior probabilities ranged from 90% to 92%.

thumbnail
Table 3. Latent profile comparisons with fit statistics and average class probabilities for most likely class membership by latent profile class numbers.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193071.t003

The three classes were designated relative to MT as “High MT” (n = 433, 34.9%), “Moderate MT” (n = 579, 46.5%) and “Low MT” (n = 233, 18.7%) based on the factor means for each class, which were significantly different for all eight factors between all three classes tested with ANOVA and a post-hoc Bonferonni test (Table 4). A minimum effect size greater than 0.2 is considered the lower level for practical significance, with 0.5 indicating a moderate effect and 0.8 a strong effect [61]. The smallest effect sizes were 0.18 for visualization and 0.19 for determination. The highest effect sizes were 0.65 for self-belief, 0.52 for positive cognition and confidence.).

thumbnail
Table 4. Means and standard deviations by classes for mental toughness factors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193071.t004

Univariate analyses

Males (χ2(2) = 13.43, P = 0.001), older age groups (χ2(6) = 16.03, P = 0.01), athletes who competed for more years (χ2(6) = 30.35, P<0.001) and participants who trained more hours (χ2(6) = 14.80, P = 0.02) were in the High MT cluster more often than females, younger age groups, newer athletes, and those who trained fewer hours, respectively (Table 4). Satisfaction with performance (χ2(6) = 187.35, P<0.001) and a top 10 category placement (χ2(4) = 35.07, P<0.001) showed a higher prevalence of being in the High MT cluster compared to lower satisfaction and higher category placement, respectively (Table 5). There were no differences in class membership by education or primary sport.

thumbnail
Table 5. Associations between demographics and sports characteristics and MT classes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193071.t005

Multivariable analyses

The results from the most parsimonious multinomial logistic regression model are shown in Fig 1. The Low MT cluster served as the reference group. Satisfaction was dichotomized into High (Always and Often) and Low (Sometimes, Rarely, or Never). There were no significant interaction effects. Males, athletes older than 55 years, High satisfaction, and a top 10 division placement increased the odds of being in the High MT cluster when compared to females (OR 1.99, 95% CI 1.39–2.83, P<0.001), 18–34 year olds (OR 2.52, 95% CI 1.37–4.62, P<0.05), Low satisfaction (OR 8.17, 95% CI 5.63–9.11.87, P<0.001) and placing 21 or higher (OR 2.18, 95% CI, 1.46–3.26, P<0.001). Membership in the Moderate MT cluster compared to the Low MT cluster was significantly associated with gender (males vs. females: OR 1.44, 95% CI, 1.04–2.00, P<0.05), satisfaction (High vs. Low: OR 3.35, 95% CI 2.41–4.65, P<0.001) and placement in one’s division (top 10 vs. 21 or higher: OR 1.74, 95% CI, 1.21–2.51, P<0.001).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Multivariable analyses of associations between mental toughness (MT) latent class membership and sports and demographic variables.

High MT (n = 433), Moderate MT (n = 579), Low MT (n = 233). High satisfaction = always or often; low satisfaction = sometimes, rarely or never.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193071.g001

Conclusions

We examined whether there are identifiable MT latent classes in athletes who participate in endurance sports and if the MT profiles are associated with demographics, sports characteristics, satisfaction with race results, and division placement. Eight factors from three measurement tools were used to improve both statistical power and clinical utility. Three classes emerged that corresponded to High, Medium, and Low MT based on mean scores for the eight measured factors. Multivariable analysis indicated that males, older athletes, high-ranking division placement, and high levels of race satisfaction all predicted membership into the High MT class as compared to the Low MT class.

Enrollment of a large sample size of athletes who participated in endurance sports with an equitable male to female ratio made this study novel. Additionally, until now, it was unknown whether the MT profiles of endurance athletes mirror that of team sport athletes.

These results corroborate the findings which found three MT profiles in adolescent cricket players corresponding to low, medium, and high MT [45]. The inventory in that study was specific to cricket, however, the five sub-scales of self-belief, affective intelligence, resilience, attentional control, and desire overlapped with those used in the current study. Additionally, in that study, the low and high clusters showed low and high levels across all five measured traits, respectively, a finding also observed in the current study. A study of older adolescent Australian football players indicated a two cluster profile, high and low; the effect sizes for the MT factors in that study were similar to those observed in these analyses [44].

The mean scores for the measured traits by MT cluster in this study fit into the competitive standards created by a study of the SMTQ and the PPI-A in 455 athletes (76% male) from 19 different sports (10% endurance sports) competing at the elite and sub-elite level [18]. Sheard concluded that the PPI-A and SMTQ used together represent a valid and reliable means of measuring MT and these measures can be used to evaluate an athlete’s MT over time, particularly if an intervention of mental skills training has been implemented [18].

Inclusion of females was informative in that there were significant gender differences in class membership. Females were less likely to be in the High MT class, and showed lower levels of the specific traits of confidence, self-esteem, self-belief, and control. Studies have shown lower levels of self-esteem in females [34], and confidence and control as measured by the SMTQ was lower in females than males [17]. These results indicate that the PPI-A, SMTQ, and RSE used together adequately distinguish sub-populations of athletes who need targeted mental skills training.

The relationship between MT and satisfaction is one of particular interest and fits in with the Basic Need Theory, which postulates that “humans function and develop effectively as a consequence of the social environment and its potential for basic need satisfaction”[62]. Sports satisfaction has been related to physical well-being [63], pre- and post-practice well-being [64], athletic performance [65], and sports vitality [62].

In addition, long term adherence to sports improves with increased levels of satisfaction [66,67]. A recommendation for a new model of understanding and implementing sports participation focuses on creating higher levels of sports satisfaction through skills development rather than the current paradigm of increasing motivation and changing behaviors [68]; MT factors are such skills that can be developed over time to increase sports satisfaction.

We propose that the formation of MT classes, as determined in this study, broadens the scope of knowledge about MT profiles in endurance athletes by contextualizing the individual MT factors. Further studies of MT using these measures in endurance athletes will be needed to replicate these results. The field of MT has been hindered by the complexity of the concept. Gucciardi postulated that MT can be defined “as a state-like psychological resource that is purposeful, flexible, and efficient in nature for the enactment and maintenance of goal-directed pursuits.”[13] Within this framework, understanding that MT is flexible, exemplifies that MT can be trained. Therefore, using techniques to identify weaknesses, such as easily administered questionnaires used in this study, is important for coaches and practitioners. Additionally, this study suggests that endurance athletes’ MT is consistent across dimensions, so identifying athletes who are at the lower end can potentially improve not only their proficiency in sports, but also improve their general well-being.

Self-belief had the highest effect size. A group of elite level international athletes ranked self-belief as the most important dimension of MT [14,69], with self-belief having four attributes. “The attributes in this subcomponent relate to factors that contribute to performers’ unshakable belief through their awareness and inner arrogance and how this belief results in performers’ reaching their true potential, despite obstacles and barriers that people or organizations put in their path.[69]”

This study had a few limitations. It is unknown whether the participants were answering the questions honestly; however, the anonymity of the questionnaire increased the likelihood of truthful responses. The generalizability of this convenience sample drawn from social media outlets is unknown. However, comparisons to the latest statistics from the governing body of triathlon (USA Triathlon) and the USA Running State of the Sport trends show that the participant demographics in this sample roughly match the overall populations of running and triathlon, the two sports most largely represented in this sample [70,71]. Even though the sample demographics reflect those of the greater population of triathletes and runners, the participants are self-selected, therefore the MT classes may not be representative of endurance athletes in general.

In summary, using the RSE and two validated MT questionnaires, the PPI-A and SMTQ, in a large cross-sectional sample of adult endurance athletes, we identified three latent profiles that corresponded to High, Medium, and Low MT. The MT profiles are associated with satisfaction with race results, gender, age, and race placement. A scoring algorithm from a discriminant analysis will be developed based on the latent profiles results which will allow clinicians and coaches to screen MT in athletes using the RSE, PPI-A, and SMTQ.

References

  1. 1. Slattery K, Bentley D, Coutts AJ. The Role of Oxidative, Inflammatory and Neuroendocrinological Systems During Exercise Stress in Athletes: Implications of Antioxidant Supplementation on Physiological Adaptation During Intensified Physical Training. Sport Med. Springer International Publishing; 2015;45: 453–471. pmid:25398224
  2. 2. Killer SC, Svendsen IS, Jeukendrup AE, Gleeson M. Evidence of disturbed sleep and mood state in well-trained athletes during short-term intensified training with and without a high carbohydrate nutritional intervention. J Sports Sci. Routledge; 2017;35: 1402–1410. pmid:26406911
  3. 3. Osborne MS, Greene DJ, Immel DT. Managing performance anxiety and improving mental skills in conservatoire students through performance psychology training: a pilot study. Psychol Well Being. Springer Berlin Heidelberg; 2014;4: 18.
  4. 4. Faubert J. Professional athletes have extraordinary skills for rapidly learning complex and neutral dynamic visual scenes. Sci Rep. Nature Publishing Group; 2013;3: 1154. pmid:23378899
  5. 5. Rice SM, Purcell R, De Silva S, Mawren D, McGorry PD, Parker AG. The Mental Health of Elite Athletes: A Narrative Systematic Review. Sport Med. Springer International Publishing; 2016;46: 1333–1353. pmid:26896951
  6. 6. Brand S, Gerber M, Kalak N, Kirov R, Lemola S, Clough PJ, et al. Adolescents with greater mental toughness show higher sleep efficiency, more deep sleep and fewer awakenings after sleep onseT. J Adolesc Heal. 2014;54. pmid:23998848
  7. 7. Crust L, Azadi K. Mental toughness and athletes’ use of psychological strategies. Eur J Sport Sci. 2010;10: 43–51.
  8. 8. Gucciardi D, Hanton S, Gordon S. The concept of mental toughness: tests of dimensionality, nomological network, and traitness. J Pers. 2015;83: 26–44. pmid:24428736
  9. 9. Stamp E, Crust L, Swann C. The impact of mental toughness on lifestyle choices in university students. British Association of Sport and Exercise Science Annual Conference. 2015.
  10. 10. Gucciardi DF, Hanton S, Gordon S, Mallett CJ, Temby P. The Concept of Mental Toughness: Tests of Dimensionality, Nomological Network, and Traitness. J Pers. 2015;83: 26–44. pmid:24428736
  11. 11. Crust L, Swann C. Comparing two measures of mental toughness. Pers Individ Dif. 2011;50: 217–221.
  12. 12. Golby J, Sheard M, van Wersch A. Evaluating the factor structure of the psychological performance inventory. Percept Mot Ski. 2007;105: 309–325.
  13. 13. Gucciardi DF. Mental toughness: progress and prospects—Google Scholar. Curr Opin Psychol. 2017;16: 17–23. pmid:28813344
  14. 14. Jones G, Hanton S, Connaughton D. What Is This Thing Called Mental Toughness? An Investigation of Elite Sport Performers. J Appl Sport Psychol. 2002;14: 205–218.
  15. 15. Wieser R, Thiel H. A survey of “mental hardiness” and “mental toughness” in professional male football players. Chiropr Man Ther. 2014;22: 17. pmid:24735867
  16. 16. Roberts GC, Treasure DC, Balagué G. Achievement goals in sport: the development and validation of the Perception of Success Questionnaire. J Sports Sci. 1998;16: 337–347. pmid:9663958
  17. 17. Sheard M, Golby J, Van Wersch A. Progress toward construct validation of the Sports Mental Toughness Questionnaire (SMTQ). Eur J Psychol Assess. 2009;25: 186–193.
  18. 18. Sheard M. Mental Toughness: The mindset behind sporting achievement. N.Y., N.Y.: Routledge; 2013.
  19. 19. Nicholls AR, Polman RCJ, Levy AR, Backhouse SH. Mental toughness, optimism, pessimism, and coping among athletes. Pers Individ Dif. 2008;44: 1182–1192.
  20. 20. Sheard M. a Cross-National Analysis of Mental Toughness and Hardiness in Elite University Rugby League Teams. Percept Mot Skills. 2009;109: 213–223. pmid:19831102
  21. 21. Kuan G, Roy J. Goal profiles, mental toughness and its influence on performance outcomes among Wushu athletes. J Sport Sci Med. 2007;6: 28–33.
  22. 22. Crust L, Earle K, Perry J, Earle F, Clogh A, Clough PJ. Mental toughness in higher education: Relationships with achievement and progression in first-year university sports students. Pers Individ Dif. Pergamon; 2014;69: 87–91.
  23. 23. Kaiseler M, Polman R, Nicholls A. Mental toughness, stress, stress appraisal, coping and coping effectiveness in sport. Pers Individ Dif. 2009;47: 728–733.
  24. 24. Crust L, Swann C. The relationship between mental toughness and dispositional flow. Eur J Sport Sci. 2013;13: 215–220.
  25. 25. Rosenberg M, Schooler C, Schoenbach C, Rosenberg F. Global Self-Esteem and Specific Self-Esteem: Different Concepts, Different Outcomes. Am Sociol Rev. American Sociological Association; 1995;60: 141.
  26. 26. Iso-Ahola S, Verde D La. Perceived competence as a mediator of the relationship between high risk sports participation and self-esteem. J Leis. 1989;21: 32–39.
  27. 27. Reis N, Kowalski K, Ferguson L. Self-compassion and women athletes’ responses to emotionally difficult sport situations: An evaluation of a brief induction. Psychol Sport. 2015;16: 18–25.
  28. 28. Koivula N, Hassmén P, Fallby J. Self-esteem and perfectionism in elite athletes: effects on competitive anxiety and self-confidence. Pers Individ Dif. 2002;35: 865–875.
  29. 29. Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. 1965;
  30. 30. Kang S-M, Shaver PR, Sue S, Min K-H, Jing H. Culture-Specific Patterns in the Prediction of Life Satisfaction: Roles of Emotion, Relationship Quality, and Self-Esteem. Personal Soc Psychol Bull. 2003;29: 1596–1608. pmid:15018689
  31. 31. Paradise AW, Kernis MH. Self-esteem and Psychological Well-being: Implications of Fragile Self-esteem. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2002;21: 345–361.
  32. 32. Sonstroem RJ, Morgan WP. Exercise and self-esteem. Med Sci Sport Exerc. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 1989;21: 329???337.
  33. 33. Bowker A. The Relationship Between Sports Participation and Self—Esteem During Early Adolescence. Can J Behav Sci. 2006;38: 214–229.
  34. 34. Joseph RP, Royse KE, Benitez TJ, Pekmezi DW. Physical activity and quality of life among university students: exploring self-efficacy, self-esteem, and affect as potential mediators. Qual Life Res. 2014;23: 659–667. pmid:23928820
  35. 35. Huang H, Humphreys B. Sports participation and happiness: Evidence from US microdata. J Econ Psychol. 2012;33: 776–793.
  36. 36. Carpentier J, Mageau G. When change-oriented feedback enhances motivation, well-being and performance: A look at autonomy-supportive feedback in sport. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2013;14: 423–435.
  37. 37. Mahoney J, Gucciardi D. Mental toughness in sport: Motivational antecedents and associations with performance and psychological health. J Sport. 2014;36: 281–292.
  38. 38. Finez L, Berjot S, Rosnet E, Cleveland C, Tice DM. Trait self-esteem and claimed self-handicapping motives in sports situations. J Sports Sci. Routledge; 2012;30: 1757–1765. pmid:22963345
  39. 39. Pyszczynski T, Greenberg J, Solomon S, Arndt J, Schimel J. Why Do People Need Self-Esteem? A Theoretical and Empirical Review. Psychol Bull. 2004;130: 435–468. pmid:15122930
  40. 40. Baumeister RF, Campbell JD, Krueger JI, Vohs KD. DOES HIGH SELF-ESTEEM CAUSE BETTER PERFORMANCE, INTERPERSONAL SUCCESS, HAPPINESS, OR HEALTHIER LIFESTYLES? Psychol Sci PUBLIC Interes. 2003;4.
  41. 41. Gerber M, Kalak N, Lemola S, Clough PJ, Pühse U, Elliot C, et al. Adolescents’ exercise and physical activity are associated with mental toughness. Ment Health Phys Act. 2012;5: 35–42.
  42. 42. Martinent G, Nicolas M, Gaudreau P, Campo M. A cluster analysis of affective states before and during competition. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2013;35: 600–11. pmid:24334321
  43. 43. Connaughton D, Wadey R, Hanton S, Jones G. The development and maintenance of mental toughness: perceptions of elite performers. J Sports Sci. 2008;26: 83–95. pmid:17852671
  44. 44. Gucciardi DF, Gordon S, Dimmock J. Towards an understanding of mental toughness in Australian football. J Appl Sport Psychol. 2008;20: 261–281.
  45. 45. Gucciardi D, Jones M. Beyond optimal performance: Mental toughness profiles and developmental success in adolescent cricketers. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2012;34: 16–36. pmid:22356880
  46. 46. Rosenberg M. Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Science. 1965.
  47. 47. Tein J-Y, Coxe S, Cham H. Statistical Power to Detect the Correct Number of Classes in Latent Profile Analysis. Struct Equ Modeling. NIH Public Access; 2013;20: 640–657. pmid:24489457
  48. 48. Wurpts IC, Geiser C. Is adding more indicators to a latent class analysis beneficial or detrimental? Results of a Monte-Carlo study. Front Psychol. Frontiers; 2014;5: 920. pmid:25191298
  49. 49. O’brien RM. A Caution Regarding Rules of Thumb for Variance Inflation Factors. Qual Quant. Springer Netherlands; 2007;41: 673–690.
  50. 50. Golby J, Sheard M, Wersch A V. Psychological Performance Inventory. Percept Mot Skills. 2007;105: 309–325. pmid:17918579
  51. 51. Newland A, Newton M, Finch L, Harbke CR, Podlog L. Moderating variables in the relationship between mental toughness and performance in basketball. J Sport Heal Sci. Elsevier Ltd; 2013;2: 184–192.
  52. 52. Robins RW, Hendin HM, Trzesniewski KH. Measuring Global Self-Esteem: Construct Validation of a Single-Item Measure and the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. Personal Soc Psychol Bull. 2001;27: 151–161.
  53. 53. Valmaggia L, Stahl D, Yung A. Negative psychotic symptoms and impaired role functioning predict transition outcomes in the at-risk mental state: a latent class cluster analysis study. Psychol Med. 2013;43: 2311–2325. pmid:23442767
  54. 54. Oberski D. Mixture models: Latent profile and latent class analysis. Modern Statistical Methods for HCI. Springer International Publishing; 2016.
  55. 55. McLachlan GJ, Peel D, Bean RW. Modelling high-dimensional data by mixtures of factor analyzers. Comput Stat Data Anal. 2003;41: 379–388.
  56. 56. Nylund K, Asparouhov T, Muthen B. Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo simulation study. Struct Equ. 2007;14: 535–569.
  57. 57. Muthen L, Muthen B. Mplus: Statistical analysis with latent variables: User’s guide. Los Angeles; 2005.
  58. 58. Levine TR, Hullett CR. Eta Squared, Partial Eta Squared, and Misreporting of Effect Size in Communication Research. Hum Commun Res. 2002;28: 612–625.
  59. 59. Crutzen R, Peters G-JY. Scale quality: alpha is an inadequate estimate and factor-analytic evidence is needed first of all. Health Psychol Rev. Routledge; 2017;11: 242–247. pmid:26602990
  60. 60. Dunn TJ, Baguley T, Brunsden V. From alpha to omega: A practical solution to the pervasive problem of internal consistency estimation. Br J Psychol. 2014;105: 399–412. pmid:24844115
  61. 61. Ferguson CJ. An effect size primer: A guide for clinicians and researchers. Prof Psychol Res Pract. 2009;40: 532–538.
  62. 62. Adie JW, Duda JL, Ntoumanis N. Autonomy support, basic need satisfaction and the optimal functioning of adult male and female sport participants: A test of basic needs theory. Motiv Emot. 2008;32: 189–199.
  63. 63. Reinboth M, Duda JL. Perceived motivational climate, need satisfaction and indices of well-being in team sports: A longitudinal perspective. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2006;7: 269–286.
  64. 64. Gagné M, Ryan RM, Bargmann K. Autonomy Support and Need Satisfaction in the Motivation and Well-Being of Gymnasts. J Appl Sport Psychol. 2003;15: 372–390.
  65. 65. Gillet N, Vallerand RJ, Amoura S, Baldes B. Influence of coaches’ autonomy support on athletes’ motivation and sport performance: A test of the hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2010;11: 155–161.
  66. 66. Gallan I, Boix R, Medrano MJ, Ramos P, Rivera F, Moreno C. Individual Factors and School-Based Policies Related to Adherence to Physical Activity Recommendations in Spanish Adolescents. Prev Sci. 2014;15: 588–599. pmid:23728581
  67. 67. Kinnafick F-E, Thogersen-Ntoumani C, Duda JL. Physical Activity Adoption to Adherence, Lapse, and Dropout: A Self-Determination Theory Perspective. Qual Health Res. 2014;24: 706–718. pmid:24692183
  68. 68. Thurston M, Green K. Adherence to exercise in later life: How can exercise on prescription programmes be made more effective? Health Promot Int. 2004;19: 379–387. pmid:15306622
  69. 69. Connaughton D, Hanton S, Jones G. A framework of mental toughness in the world’s best performers. Sport Psychol. 2007;21: 243–264.
  70. 70. USA Triathlon Demographics [Internet]. 2014 [cited 20 Jun 2005]. Available: https://www.teamusa.org/usa-triathlon/about/multisport/demographics
  71. 71. Running USA. Running USA State of the Sports [Internet]. 2015 [cited 1 May 2016]. Available: http://www.runningusa.org/2015-state-of-sport-us-trends