Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Openness to experience and stress responsivity: An examination of cardiovascular and underlying hemodynamic trajectories within an acute stress exposure

Abstract

The personality trait openness to experience has been implicated in health, and in particular cardiovascular wellbeing. In a sample of 62 healthy young female adults, the role of openness in cardiovascular responsivity during a stress exposure was examined. Traditionally, methodologies have averaged a stress exposure into a single reading. This may be limited in that it does not consider patterns of cardiovascular adaptation within a stress exposure. Continuous cardiovascular data were reduced to mean 10 second readings, with phases determined through examinations of shifts in responsivity between each 10 second pairing. Analyses revealed a significant linear interaction for openness across the entire exposure for systolic blood pressure, and cardiac output. A significant between-subjects effect for heart rate also emerged. Contrary to their lower counterparts, those highest in openness exhibited an increasingly myocardial hemodynamic response profile throughout the exposure. Comparisons of responsivity suggests adaptive stress response trajectories for those highest in openness. This study also provides evidence that an attenuation of myocardial responsivity may underpin blunted responsivity. This study provides a potential mechanism in reported openness-health associations.

Introduction

Accumulating research indicates the relevance of the personality trait of openness to experience as a predictor of health outcomes and in particular, cardiovascular wellbeing [19]. Openness refers to an individual’s propensity to be open to a variety of experiences, with a need to enlarge and examine experience [10, 11]. A meta-analysis, in addition to a 10-year follow-up study has found that higher openness is protective with respect to all-cause mortality [2, 8]. Openness has also been found to be implicated in cardiovascular wellbeing, such that higher openness has been observed as a protective factor. More specifically, data pertaining to a 10.5 year follow-up study found that openness was associated with coronary heart disease (CHD), with higher openness being observed as being an independent protective risk factor [12]. Openness was found to be the sole personality trait predictive of CHD [12]. A further study drawn from the Health and Retirement Study found that higher openness reduced the odds of diagnosis of multiple cardiovascular health associations; namely stroke by 31%, high blood pressure by 29%, and heart conditions (myocardial infarction, CHD, angina, cardiac heart failure, or other heart problems) by 17% [13]. Collectively, the aforementioned authors concluded potential mechanisms which may account for these associations as being unclear. Openness constitutes a trait which accounts for an individual’s motivation and receptiveness for experiences. As such, a naturally selected trait such as openness should be of crucial importance to an individual’s responsivity to stress experiences. As suggested by Ó Súilleabháin and colleagues [14] persons highest in openness should possess the required ability to stimulate short-term stress responsivity, while demonstrating an ability to habituate across time.

Metabolically inappropriate cardiovascular reactivity (difference between stress elevation and baseline) to psychological stress is thought to disrupt homeostasis in ways which are detrimental to health [15]. Indeed, as reviewed by Phillips and Hughes [16], extensive prospective and cross-sectional research supports the association between heightened cardiovascular reactivity and increased risk of cardiovascular disease (CVD); including hypertension, atherosclerosis, myocardial infarction (MI), increased left ventricular mass (LVM), and CHD mortality [1726]. While most research has examined the adverse implications of elevated cardiovascular responsivity, significant caveats have emerged [27]. While elevated cardiovascular responsivity to stress over prolonged periods can be considered as leading to negative outcomes, they may also be adaptive in the short-term when responding to acute stress [27]. As highlighted by Hughes [27], research has observed that acute stress can stimulate immune effectiveness, and that cardiovascular stress responding is positively associated with enhanced immune responding [28]. The potential negative implications of sustained responsivity may also be diminished due to the habituation of responsivity across time (e.g. [29]). Indeed, when individuals are presented with similar stress exposures, patterns of cardiovascular stress adaptation have been observed (e.g., [29,30]). Recent research has also observed that patterns of cardiovascular adaptation can occur across a change in stress exposures [14].

Research examining the potential associations between openness and cardiovascular stress responding is limited. Recently Ó Súilleabháin and colleagues [14] reported that higher openness stimulates short-term stress responsivity, while ensuring cardiovascular habituation to change in stress across time. Further research has also reported higher openness as associated with lesser heart rate (HR) reactivity across repeated social stress exposures, in addition to lesser SBP reactivity to the repeated stress exposure [31]. In addition, Williams and colleagues [32] found persons higher in openness to exhibit lower SBP and DBP to stress tasks involving the recall of stressful experiences. Further research has reported a positive association between openness and HR reactivity in a midlife sample [33]. These aforementioned studies, and indeed wider stress research have traditionally employed methodologies which quantify a stress response as an averaged reading across an entire stress exposure. While this strategy has provided significant contributions, it may be limited in that it does not consider trajectories within an exposure.

The examination of responsivity during a stress exposure has the potential to uncover highly relevant associations which may otherwise be masked by reducing the entire stress experience to a single measurement. In other words, responsivity which may appear elevated or diminished when reducing the exposure to a single measurement may mask both adaptive and maladaptive trajectories which have significant health implications. As such, the present study sought to examine if openness is associated with shifts in cardiovascular responsivity within a stress exposure. Given the potential relevance of openness as a facilitator for adjustment to stress experiences, the present study examined if openness is related to cardiovascular and underlying hemodynamic response patterns during a stress exposure.

Methods

Participants

Participants were healthy female college students (N = 62; M ± SD = 19.15 ± 1.32 years; range = 17–24 years; BMI, M ± SD = 22.96 ± 2.95 kg/m2). Females were examined due to a lack of availability of a biometrically comparable sample of males within the college population in psychology. Each participant provided written informed consent. Persons below the age of 18 years provided parental consent to participate. Given the accumulative nature of the health associations implicated with openness, the examination of healthy individuals was of importance. As such, participants reported not consuming cardioactive medication or suffering from any cardiovascular illness. Sample size is comparable to those of similar research (e.g., [34]).

Psychometric assessment

Openness (M ± SD = 27.60 ± 7.60) was assessed using the NEO Five Factor Inventory (NEO FFI-3; [11]). Current mean openness levels are within the average range for female adults within published norms [35]. Openness reliability alpha (Cronbach’s) in the sample was excellent (α = .82).

Physiological assessment

A Finometer (Finapres Medical Systems BV, BT Arnhem, The Netherlands) was used to examine cardiovascular function. The Finometer is a continuous hemodynamic monitor that assesses beat-to-beat blood pressure and heart rate. Using the volume-clamp method [36], the Finometer obtains measurements by photophethysmography via a finger cuff. The Finometer has been shown to successfully assess accurate blood pressure measurements in a variety of samples [37, 38]. Beat-to-beat measures were obtained continuously at a sampling rate of 200 Hz. While the Finometer maintains a low sensitivity to motion artifacts, the sensor is also securely fastened to each participants wrist to further minimise potential artifacts. Hand position was maintained at the correct level throughout. Calibration using the Finometer’s patented Return-to-Flow technology was conducted on each participant. This results in the Finometer achieving a standard of absolute blood pressure measurement for each participant in meeting the validation criteria of both the British Hypertension Society, and the Association for the Advancement of Medical Instrumentation [37].

Procedure

In order to limit the impact of circadian rhythms testing took place between 09:00 and 13:00. In addition, in order to limit the impact of any environmental variation on physiological responsivity [39], all testing took place in the same laboratory. Participants received instructions not to partake in exercise for 2 hours prior to attending and to not consume any caffeinated products. Participants were greeted by the researcher on arrival, and their height and weight were digitally recorded. Following being seated in a comfortable chair, the Finometer was attached to the middle finger of their non-dominant hand. A 30-minute acclimatization period followed prior to the commencement of the experimental protocol. To facilitate relaxation and establishment of cardiovascular baseline levels, participants were provided with non-emotive reading material [40]. Following this period, the formal protocol commenced with a baseline period of 10 minutes; followed by a 5-minute exposure to a mental arithmetic task. The mental arithmetic stressor employed was presented on a computer screen where participants were required to solve on-screen subtraction problems to which participants entered their responses via a keyboard. This is a standard stress procedure which has been repeatedly employed in existing literature (e.g. [14]). Given the associations between openness and an ability to perform tasks (e.g., [41]), the task controlled for mathematical ability where subtraction items became more challenging or easier when three consecutive correct/incorrect responses were entered. This has also been shown to be effective in the maintenance of engagement and stressfulness during cardiovascular stress research [4244]. Beat-to-beat measures of systolic blood pressure (SBP), diastolic blood pressure (DBP), heart rate (HR), cardiac output (CO), and total peripheral resistance (TPR) were obtained continuously throughout.

Phase reduction

SBP, DBP, HR, CO and TPR continuous measurements were computed as the mean 10 second readings throughout the stress exposure. In order to determine potential phases of shifts in responsivity; paired samples t-tests were conducted between each incremental 10 second period for each cardiovascular parameter. Thus, SBP, DBP, HR, CO and TPR were each examined individually for changes between each 10 second pairing. It was determined that a phase would be constituted if the paired samples t-test was significant.

SBP, paired samples t-tests t(61) = -2.154, p = .035 between 70 and 80 seconds; t(61) = 2.024, p = .047 and 190 and 200 seconds. DBP, t(61) = -2.167, p = .034 between 10 and 20 seconds; t(61) = 2.425, p = .018 between 190 and 200 seconds. HR, t(61) = -3.084, p = .003 between 10 and 20 seconds; t(61) = 2.855, p = .006 between 50 and 60 seconds; t(61) = 3.317, p = .002 between 60 and 70 seconds; t(61) = 2.703, p = .009 between 230 and 240 seconds. CO, t(61) = -2.528, p = .014 between 40 and 50 seconds. TPR, t(61) = 2.95, p = .005 between 40 and 50 seconds. For graphical illustration of created phases, see Fig 1. Excellent internal reliability consistency for each measure was observed (Cronbach’s α; SBP = .92, DBP = .90, HR = .96, CO = .96, and TPR = .89).

thumbnail
Fig 1. SBP, DBP, HR, CO and TPR mean function across the entire stress exposure delimited by created phases.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199221.g001

As the multiple comparisons may increase the familywise error rate, the established phases were examined using ANOVA for each parameter. Within-subjects ANOVA confirmed a main effect for phase, for SBP, Wilk’s λ = .88, F(2, 60) = 4.05, p = .022, DBP, Wilk’s λ = .73, F(2,60) = 11.00, p < .001, and HR, Wilk’s λ = .66, F(4, 58) = 7.45, p < .001. Paired-sample t-test confirmed a difference in the two CO phases, t(61) = 2.18, p = .034, but not the two TPR phases, p = .792, indicating clear difference across the established phases for all parameters except TPR.

Overview of analyses

To examine the potential impact of openness throughout the stress exposure, a series of mixed factorial ANCOVAs were conducted using PASW Statistics 22.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL; S1 File). The within-subjects variable was phase, namely the computed mean of the identified phases for each cardiovascular variable. For each corresponding cardiovascular parameter, the first data points at the commencement of the exposure and relevant baseline period were included as covariates. Given the inclusion of both aforementioned covariates, openness was examined through its inclusion as a between-subjects factor in tertiles; lowest (n = 22), middle (n = 20), and highest (n = 20). In other words, given the importance of including both covariates due to their potential in driving response trajectories, openness was examined as a between-subjects factor for clarity of interpretation. This approach to examining personality traits has been frequently used and indeed is recommended to guard against the violation of the homogeneity of regression that would likely occur should openness be treated as a covariate [45, 46]. Both covariates were significantly correlated with the dependent variables in each instance, thus reducing the potential of hampering power by their inclusion through reducing degrees of freedom. Significant findings are graphically represented by tertiles of openness and reactivity trajectories following stress commencement. Where sphericity assumptions were violated, Greenhouse-Geisser results were reported. The assumption of homogeneity of variance was not violated. Partial η2 values of .04, .25, and .64 were taken to represent small, medium and large effect sizes respectively [47, 48]. Descriptive statistics for all parameters are outlined in Table 1.

thumbnail
Table 1. Mean and standard deviations for all cardiovascular measures across each phase of the experiment.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199221.t001

Results

Elicitation of stress response

A series of one-way repeated-measures ANOVAs were conducted to examine if the task employed was successful in eliciting a stress response. Consistent with the increase from baseline to task, significant linear effects were observed across time (SBP, F(1, 61) = 111.14, p < .001, partial η2 = .646; DBP, F(1, 61) = 149.42, p < .001, partial η2 = .71; HR, F(1, 61) = 49.68, p < .001, partial η2 = .449; CO, F(1, 61) = 50.596, p < .001, partial η2 = .453; TPR, p = .221).

Cardiovascular responses and personality

SBP, DBP, HR.

For SBP, while there was no significant within-subjects effects for phase (p = .442), a significant phase × openness interaction effect was observed F(3.12, 88.85) = 4.40, p = .006, partial η2 = .134. Examination of within-subjects contrasts revealed a significant phase × openness linear interaction F(2, 57) = 5.06, p = .010, partial η2 = .151 (see Fig 2), with those highest in openness displaying an elevation of responsivity compared to their lower counterparts. The observed between-subjects effect was not significant (p = .084).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Patterns of mean SBP function across each phase of the experiment by tertiles of openness.

Note: Error bars denote ± 1 standard error of the mean.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199221.g002

For DBP, no significant within-subjects effect for phase or phase × openness interaction emerged (all ps > .077). No significant within-subjects contrasts effects emerged (all ps > .064). Additionally, a significant between-subjects effect was also not observed (p = .093).

For HR, the within-subjects main effect for phase and phase × openness interaction did not emerge as significant (p = .165). In addition, no significant within-subjects contrasts phase × openness effect was observed (all ps > .089). However, a significant between-subjects effect emerged for openness F(2, 57) = 3.33, p = .043, partial η2 = .105 (see Fig 3). Pairwise comparisons revealed that the effect for openness reflected higher HR for those in the middle tertile of openness compared to their lower counterparts (mean difference ± SE = 5.11 ± 1.99 bpm; p = .039 [Bonferroni corrected]). No significant pairwise comparison was observed between persons higher in openness and their lower (p = .47) and middle (p = .83) counterparts.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Patterns of mean HR function across each phase of the experiment by tertiles of openness.

Note: Error bars denote ± 1 standard error of the mean.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199221.g003

CO, TPR.

For CO, the main within-subjects effect for phase did not emerge as significant (p = .179). A significant phase × openness within-subjects effect was observed F(1, 57) = 4.15, p = .021, partial η2 = .127. Analyses of the within-subjects contrasts effects revealed a significant phase × openness linear interaction F(2, 57) = 4.15, p = .021, partial η2 = .127, with those in the middle and lowest tertile of openness exhibiting an attenuation of responsivity across phases (see Fig 4). No significant between-subjects effect emerged (p = .313).

thumbnail
Fig 4. CO and TPR reactivity across both phases of the experiment by tertiles of openness.

Note: Error bars denote ± 1 standard error of the mean.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199221.g004

For TPR, a significant main within-subjects effect, phase × openness within-subjects effect, phase × openness within-subjects contrasts effect, or between-subjects effect did not emerge (all ps > .115). As previously outlined, TPR phases were not confirmed with the within-subjects ANOVA.

Discussion

The present study provides evidence that openness is relevant to consider when seeking to predict responsivity during an acute stress exposure. Openness was observed as being associated with SBP throughout the exposure. Persons highest in openness were found to exhibit what appears to represent adaptive response trajectories to the stress experience. Indeed, this is line with existing research suggesting that higher openness facilitates short term stress responsivity while ensuring habituation across time [14]. As previously highlighted, existing literature suggests healthful associations with short-term stress responsivity [27, 28]. Comparatively, those within the middle and lowest tertiles were found to exhibit low SBP stress responsivity. Compared to the remaining tertiles, individuals lowest in openness were also found to display low HR responsivity. Openness was also found to be relevant within CO response trajectories throughout the stress exposure. Persons highest in openness exhibited an increasing CO hemodynamic response profile which is thought to constitute an adaptive response profile. Comparatively, those within the middle and lowest tertiles both exhibited an attenuating trend from CO responsivity during the stressor. Collectively, the aforementioned findings suggest that the immediate adjustment to stress was tolerated in a more adaptive manner by those in the highest tertile of openness.

As discussed previously, the motivation and capacity to be receptive to experiences is central to openness. As such, it is unsurprising that those highest in openness produce a stimulation of responsivity to a stressor requiring active engagement. In other words, persons highest in openness appear to possess a flexibility to responds to the presented stressful experience. Interestingly, a distinct difference between those highest in openness and their lower counterparts with respect to their hemodynamic profile (the reciprocal relationship between CO and TPR) emerged. Those highest in openness were observed to display an increasingly myocardial (CO) dominated hemodynamic profile during the stress exposure. Both of the remaining tertiles of openness were observed to exhibit a differing profile, characterised by an attenuation of myocardial responsivity. A myocardial dominated profile is thought to be more adaptive and less atherosclerotic than a vascular-dominated profile [49]. As such, the observed tendency for those highest in openness to mount an increasingly myocardial response profile within the stress exposure may indicate a protective effect to the more negative health associated vascular orientation (TPR). Indeed, the protective implications of higher openness in cardiovascular health associations have been documented [13].

As previously discussed, research investigating cardiovascular responses to stress typically examine stress as an averaged reading across an entire exposure. While research have examined cardiovascular trajectories during acute stress (e.g. [50, 51]), this research is the first to examine the association between a stress exposure and personality in this manner. It was unclear what, if any, associations may emerge once the exposure was magnified. The present findings significantly add to existing literature examining openness and stress responsivity. It highlights that higher openness appears to facilitate a distinctly adaptive cardiovascular response profile during a stress exposure. The adaptive and healthful value of adequately responding to acute stress in the short-term has been previously outlined [28]. Aside from implications observed with openness, these findings make a significant contribution to broader research examining cardiovascular stress responses. The present study has observed that a personality trait is associated with differing cardiovascular and hemodynamic response trajectories can be observed within a stress exposure. Indeed, the more adaptive myocardial-dominated response can be seen to increase for some (those highest in openness), while attenuating for others (those in the middle and lowest tertiles of openness). As suggested by James and colleagues [52], differing hemodynamic response profiles may result in blunted blood pressure responsivity, such that blunting may be reflective of vascular response tendencies. Indeed, the present study found that those in the lowest tertile of openness who could be characterised as displaying blunted SBP responsivity exhibited a decreasingly myocardial response. Thus, the blood pressure responses and underlying increasingly-myocardial responsivity during a stress exposure would appear to signify an adaptive response for those highest in openness. Contrarily, the low SBP responses, in addition to underlying attenuation of myocardial responsivity for their lower counterparts, may indicate a maladaptive response profile which may be implicated in negative health associations [53].

While the present study consists of a number of strengths, limitations must be noted. While the present sample size is comparable to existing research, employing a larger sample size may have detected smaller effects. Future research also needs to employ a biometrically comparable sample of males given research has also implicated differing stress responsivity across sexes. In addition, the incorporation of further measures of stress and task engagement both prior to and following the task would add further value to future research. The examination of various other stress tasks would also benefit the current literature, as would rates of recovery following stress exposure. Future research may also benefit from the investigating various approaches to continuous stress data, such as through the employment of correlational or regression statistical procedures. While the lack of significance with respect to TPR is consistent with existing research indicating active stressors eliciting myocardial dominated responsivity, it would be worthwhile for future research to examine differing stress exposure types. Particularly those of theoretical relevance to the personality trait or individual difference under examination [54]. Future research would benefit from examining the effects of differing personality traits and coping mechanisms on cardiovascular trajectories within an acute stress exposure [55]. In addition, recent research suggests cardiovascular mechanisms may be responsible for the association between personality and mortality [56], and as such future research should also seek to examine the effects of personality across stress responsivity and resulting mortality effects. Future research may also find wish to examine how phases of shifts in responsivity in hemodynamic may directly map to shifts in changes in both blood pressure and heart. This would provide further linkages between underlying hemodynamic mechanisms, and both blood pressure and heart rate.

The results from the present study demonstrate that openness is associated with cardiovascular and underlying hemodynamic response trajectories within a stress exposure. In line with theoretical implications of this traits relevance in experiences, persons highest in openness appear to possess an ability to respond in an adaptive manner to a newly presented stress experience. Aside from highlighting the relevance of examining response trajectories within a stress exposure, the observed findings do suggest a distinct flexibility for those highest in openness. Personality traits have been suggested as having evolved as strategies to solve adaptive problems, such that scoring highly on a trait is adaptive in one context and not in another [57]. In the case of openness, being higher in openness may facilitate the organism in responding to a newly presented stressor in ways which are adaptive. Certainly, the present study would suggest persons lower in openness as not being characterised by a required stimulation of physiological responsivity within the stress experience. In addition to highlighting the importance for future stress research to examine shifts in responsivity during acute stress, the present study implicates openness in the context of responsivity during a stress exposure.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank all participants who took part in this study.

References

  1. 1. Feldman PJ, Cohen S, Doyle WJ, Skoner DP, Gwaltney JM Jr. The impact of personality on the reporting of unfounded symptoms and illness. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1999 Aug;77(2):370. pmid:10474212
  2. 2. Ferguson E, Bibby PA. Openness to experience and all‐cause mortality: A meta‐analysis and requivalent from risk ratios and odds ratios. British journal of health psychology. 2012 Feb 1;17(1):85–102. pmid:22107453
  3. 3. Goodwin RD, Friedman HS. Health status and the five-factor personality traits in a nationally representative sample. Journal of health psychology. 2006 Sep;11(5):643–54. pmid:16908463
  4. 4. Ironson GH, O'Cleirigh C, Schneiderman N, Weiss A, Costa Jr PT. Personality and HIV disease progression: role of NEO-PI-R openness, extraversion, and profiles of engagement. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2008 Feb;70(2):245. pmid:18256349
  5. 5. Iwasa H, Masui Y, Gondo Y, Inagaki H, Kawaai C, Suzuki T. Personality and all-cause mortality among older adults dwelling in a Japanese community: A five-year population-based prospective cohort study. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2008 May 31;16(5):399–405. pmid:18403571
  6. 6. Jonassaint CR, Boyle SH, Kuhn CM, Siegler IC, Copeland WE, Williams R. Personality and inflammation: the protective effect of openness to experience. Ethnicity & disease. 2010;20(1):11.
  7. 7. Jonassaint CR, Boyle SH, Williams RB, Mark DB, Siegler IC, Barefoot JC. Facets of openness predict mortality in patients with cardiac disease. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2007 May 1;69(4):319–22. pmid:17510289
  8. 8. Taylor MD, Whiteman MC, Fowkes GR, Lee AJ, Allerhand M, Deary IJ. Five Factor Model personality traits and all-cause mortality in the Edinburgh Artery Study cohort. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2009 Jul 1;71(6):631–41. pmid:19483118
  9. 9. Turiano NA, Spiro A III, Mroczek DK. Openness to experience and mortality in men: Analysis of trait and facets. Journal of Aging and Health. 2012 Jun;24(4):654–72. pmid:22219209
  10. 10. McCrae RR, Costa PT Jr. Adding Liebe und Arbeit: The full five-factor model and well-being. Personality and social psychology bulletin. 1991 Apr;17(2):227–32.
  11. 11. Costa PT, MacCrae RR. Revised NEO personality inventory (NEO PI-R) and NEO five-factor inventory (NEO-FFI): Professional manual. Psychological Assessment Resources, Incorporated; 1992.
  12. 12. Lee HB, Offidani E, Ziegelstein RC, Bienvenu OJ, Samuels J, Eaton WW, et al. Five-Factor Model Personality Traits as predictors of incident coronary heart disease in the community: A 10.5-year cohort study based on the Baltimore Epidemiologic Catchment Area Follow-Up Study. Psychosomatics. 2014 Aug 31;55(4):352–61. pmid:24751113
  13. 13. Weston SJ, Hill PL, Jackson JJ. Personality traits predict the onset of disease. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2015 Apr;6(3):309–17.
  14. 14. Ó Súilleabháin PS, Howard S, Hughes BM. Openness to experience and adapting to change: Cardiovascular stress habituation to change in acute stress exposure. Psychophysiology. 2018 May 55: e13023. pmid:29044555
  15. 15. Obrist PA. Cardiovascular psychophysiology. New York: Plenum Press; 1981.
  16. 16. Phillips AC, Hughes BM. Introductory paper: Cardiovascular reactivity at a crossroads: where are we now?. Biological psychology. 2011 Feb 28;86(2):95–7. pmid:20226227
  17. 17. Matthews KA, Woodall KL, Allen MT. Cardiovascular reactivity to stress predicts future blood pressure status. Hypertension. 1993 Oct 1;22(4):479–85. pmid:8406652
  18. 18. Carroll D, Smith GD, Sheffield D, Shipley MJ, Marmot MG. Pressor reactions to psychological stress and prediction of future blood pressure: data from the Whitehall II study. Bmj. 1995 Mar 25;310(6982):771–5. pmid:7711581
  19. 19. Everson SA, Kaplan GA, Goldberg DE, Salonen JT. Anticipatory blood pressure response to exercise predicts future high blood pressure in middle-aged men. Hypertension. 1996 May 1;27(5):1059–64. pmid:8621197
  20. 20. Markovitz JH, Raczynski JM, Wallace D, Chettur V, Chesney MA. Cardiovascular reactivity to video game predicts subsequent blood pressure increases in young men: The CARDIA study. Psychosomatic medicine. 1998 Mar 1;60(2):186–91. pmid:9560868
  21. 21. Newman JD, McGarvey ST, Steele MS. Longitudinal association of cardiovascular reactivity and blood pressure in Samoan adolescents. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1999 Mar 1;61(2):243–9. pmid:10204978
  22. 22. Treiber FA, Turner JR, Davis H, Strong WB. Prediction of resting cardiovascular functioning in youth with family histories of essential hypertension: a 5-year follow-up. International journal of behavioral medicine. 1997 Dec 1;4(4):278–91. pmid:16250719
  23. 23. Carroll D, Smith GD, Shipley MJ, Steptoe A, Brunner EJ, Marmot MG. Blood pressure reactions to acute psychological stress and future blood pressure status: a 10-year follow-up of men in the Whitehall II study. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2001 Sep 1;63(5):737–43. pmid:11573021
  24. 24. Chida Y, Steptoe A. Greater cardiovascular responses to laboratory mental stress are associated with poor subsequent cardiovascular risk status. Hypertension. 2010 Apr 1;55(4):1026–32. pmid:20194301
  25. 25. Carroll D, Ginty AT, Der G, Hunt K, Benzeval M, Phillips AC. Increased blood pressure reactions to acute mental stress are associated with 16‐year cardiovascular disease mortality. Psychophysiology. 2012 Oct 1;49(10):1444–8. pmid:22958235
  26. 26. Carroll D, Ginty AT, Painter RC, Roseboom TJ, Phillips AC, de Rooij SR. Systolic blood pressure reactions to acute stress are associated with future hypertension status in the Dutch Famine Birth Cohort Study. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2012 Aug 31;85(2):270–3. pmid:22522348
  27. 27. Hughes BM. Blood Pressure Reactivity or Responses. InEncyclopedia of Behavioral Medicine 2013 (pp. 235–239). Springer New York.
  28. 28. Phillips AC, Carroll D, Burns VE, Drayson M. Cardiovascular activity and the antibody response to vaccination. Journal of psychosomatic research. 2009 Jul 31;67(1):37–43. pmid:19539817
  29. 29. Hughes BM, Howard S, James JE, Higgins NM. Individual differences in adaptation of cardiovascular responses to stress. Biological Psychology. 2011 Feb 28;86(2):129–36. pmid:20347005
  30. 30. Frankish J, Linden W. Is response adaptation a threat to the high-low reactor distinction among female college students?. Health Psychology. 1991;10(3):224. pmid:1879395
  31. 31. Lü W, Wang Z, Hughes BM. The association between openness and physiological responses to recurrent social stress. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2016 Aug 31;106:135–40. pmid:27181704
  32. 32. Williams PG, Rau HK, Cribbet MR, Gunn HE. Openness to experience and stress regulation. Journal of Research in Personality. 2009 Oct 31;43(5):777–84.
  33. 33. Bibbey A, Carroll D, Roseboom TJ, Phillips AC, de Rooij SR. Personality and physiological reactions to acute psychological stress. International journal of psychophysiology. 2013 Oct 31;90(1):28–36. pmid:23147393
  34. 34. Lee EM, Hughes BM. Trait dominance is associated with vascular cardiovascular responses, and attenuated habituation, to social stress. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2014 May 31;92(2):79–84. pmid:24632103
  35. 35. McCrare RR, Costa PT. NEO Inventories for the NEO Personality Inventory-3 (NEO PI-3), NEO Five-Factor Inventory-3 (NEO-FFI-3) and NEO Personality Inventory-revised (NEO PI-R): Professional Manual. Psychological Assessment Resources; 2010.
  36. 36. Peňáz J. Photoelectric measurement of blood pressure, volume and flow in the finger. InDigest of the 10th international conference on medical and biological engineering 1973 Aug 13 (Vol. 104). International Federation for Medical and Biological Engineering, Publishers New York.
  37. 37. Guelen I, Westerhof BE, van der Sar GL, van Montfrans GA, Kiemeneij F, Wesseling KH, et al. Finometer, finger pressure measurements with the possibility to reconstruct brachial pressure. Blood pressure monitoring. 2003 Feb 1;8(1):27–30. pmid:12604933
  38. 38. Schutte AE, Huisman HW, van Rooyen JM, Oosthuizen W, Jerling JC. Sensitivity of the Finometer device in detecting acute and medium-term changes in cardiovascular function. Blood pressure monitoring. 2003 Oct 1;8(5):195–201. pmid:14624169
  39. 39. Christenfeld N, Glynn LM, Kulik JA, Gerin W. The social construction of cardiovascular reactivity. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 1998 Dec 1;20(4):317–25. pmid:10234426
  40. 40. Jennings JR, Kamarck T, Stewart C, Eddy M, Johnson P. Alternate cardiovascular baseline assessment techniques: Vanilla or resting baseline. Psychophysiology. 1992 Nov 1;29(6):742–50. pmid:1461961
  41. 41. Sutin AR, Terracciano A, Kitner-Triolo MH, Uda M, Schlessinger D, Zonderman AB. Personality traits prospectively predict verbal fluency in a lifespan sample. Psychology and aging. 2011 Dec;26(4):994. pmid:21707179
  42. 42. Hughes BM. Memory and arithmetic as laboratory stressors for analyses of cardiovascular reactivity: A cursory assessment. Studia Psychologica. 2001 Jan 1;43(1):3–12.
  43. 43. Turner JR, Hewitt JK, Morgan RK, Sims J, Carroll D, Kelly KA. Graded mental arithmetic as an active psychological challenge. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 1986 Mar 31;3(4):307–9. pmid:3754545
  44. 44. Turner JR. Cardiovascular reactivity and stress: Patterns of physiological response. Springer Science & Business Media; 1994 Jan 31.
  45. 45. Higgins NM, Hughes BM. Individual differences in the impact of attentional bias training on cardiovascular responses to stress in women. Anxiety, Stress & Coping. 2012 Jul 1;25(4):381–95.
  46. 46. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using Multivariate Statistics. Pearson Education. Boston, MA. 2007.
  47. 47. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences Lawrence Earlbaum Associates. Hillsdale, NJ. 1988:20–6.
  48. 48. Cohen J. A power primer. Psychological bulletin. 1992 Jul;112(1):155. pmid:19565683
  49. 49. Palatini P, Julius S. The role of cardiac autonomic function in hypertension and cardiovascular disease. Current hypertension reports. 2009 Jun 1;11(3):199–205. pmid:19442329
  50. 50. Jayasinghe SU, Torres SJ, Hussein M, Fraser SF, Lambert GW, Turner AI. Fitter women did not have attenuated hemodynamic responses to psychological stress compared with age-matched women with lower levels of fitness. PLOS ONE. 2017 Jan 12;12(1):e0169746. pmid:28081200
  51. 51. Torres SJ, Turner AI, Jayasinghe SU, Reynolds J, Nowson CA. The effect of overweight/obesity on cardiovascular responses to acute psychological stress in men aged 50–70 years. Obesity facts. 2014;7(6):339–50. pmid:25428119
  52. 52. James JE, Gregg ME, Matyas TA, Hughes BM, Howard S. Stress reactivity and the Hemodynamic Profile–Compensation Deficit (HP–CD) Model of blood pressure regulation. Biological psychology. 2012 May 31;90(2):161–70. pmid:22414744
  53. 53. Phillips AC, Ginty AT, Hughes BM. The other side of the coin: Blunted cardiovascular and cortisol reactivity are associated with negative health outcomes. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2013 Oct 31;90(1):1–7. pmid:23454029
  54. 54. Ó Súilleabháin P. Experimental stress methodology, personality and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical (HPA) axis associations: A commentary on ‘Global stress response during a social stress test: impact of alexithymia and its subfactors’. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2014 Dec 1;50:209. pmid:25240205
  55. 55. Sesker AA, Ó Súilleabháin P, Howard S, Hughes BM. Conscientiousness and mindfulness in midlife coping: An assessment based on MIDUS II. Personality and mental health. 2016 Feb 1;10(1):29–42. pmid:26593128
  56. 56. O’Súilleabháin PS., Hughes BM. Neuroticism predicts all-cause mortality over 19-years: The moderating effects on functional status, and the angiotensin-converting enzyme. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2018 July 1:110:32–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2018.04.013 pmid:29764603
  57. 57. Buss DM. How can evolutionary psychology successfully explain personality and individual differences?. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2009 Jul 1;4(4):359–66. pmid:26158983